Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 28 January 2021
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychology and Neuropsychology of Perception, Action, and Cognition in Early Life View all 15 articles

The Gestures in 2–4-Year-Old Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder

\r\nQianYing YeQianYing YeLinRu LiuLinRu LiuShaoLi LvShaoLi LvSanMei ChengSanMei ChengHuiLin ZhuHuiLin ZhuYanTing XuYanTing XuXiaoBing ZouXiaoBing ZouHongZhu Deng*HongZhu Deng*
  • Child Development and Behavior Center, Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China

Deficits in gestures act as early signs of impairment in social interaction (SI) and communication in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, the pieces of literature on atypical gesture patterns in ASD children are contradictory. This investigation aimed to explore the atypical gesture pattern of ASD children from the dimensions of quantity, communicative function, and integration ability; and its relationship with social ability and adaptive behavior. We used a semi-structured interactive play to evaluate gestures of 33 ASD children (24–48 months old) and 24 typically developing (TD) children (12–36 months old). And we evaluated the social ability, adaptive behavior, and productive language of ASD and TD children by using the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System version II (ABAS-II) and Chinese Communication Development Inventory (CCDI). No matter the total score of CCDI was corrected or not, the relative frequency of total gestures, behavior regulation (BR) gestures, SI gestures, and joint attention (JA) gestures of ASD children were lower than that of TD children, as well as the proportion of JA gestures. However, there was no significant group difference in the proportion of BR and SI gestures. Before adjusting for the total score of CCDI, the relative frequency of gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration and vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures in ASD children was lower than that in TD children. However, after matching the total score of CCDI, only the relative frequency of gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration was lower. Regardless of the fact that the total score of CCDI was corrected or not, the relative frequency and the proportion of eye-gaze-integrated gestures in ASD children were lower than that in TD children. And the proportion of gestures without eye-gaze integration in ASD children was higher than that in TD children. For ASD children, the social skills score in ABAS-II was positively correlated with the relative frequency of SI gesture and eye-gaze-integrated gestures; the total score of ABAS-II was positively correlated with the relative frequency of total gestures and eye-gaze-integrated gestures. In conclusion, ASD children produce fewer gestures and have deficits in JA gestures. The deficiency of integrating eye gaze and gesture is the core deficit of ASD children’s gesture communication. Relatively, ASD children might be capable of integrating vocalization/verbalization into gestures. SI gestures and the ability to integrate gesture and eye gaze are related to social ability. The quantity of gestures and the ability to integrate gesture with eye gaze are related to adaptive behavior.

Clinical Trial Registration: www.ClinicalTrials.gov, identifier ChiCTR1800019679.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder with heterogeneous manifestations mainly characterized by impairments in social interaction (SI) and communication, as well as the presence of restrictive and repetitive behaviors [Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth Edition (DSM-V); American Psychiatric Association, 2013]. The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention reported that the rate of ASD has increased to 1 in 54 (Matthew et al., 2020). Deficits and delays in gestures act as early signs of impairment in SI and communication (Lebarton and Iverson, 2016).

Gestures in Typical Development Children

From very early in life, expressive behavior is multimodal, and early behavioral coordination is refined and strengthened for communication over time (Iverson, 2010). In the first months of life, typically developing (TD) children can communicate with others non-verbally through gestures, vocalizations, and facial expressions (Russell, 2007). It has been found that even during the prelinguistic stage, over half of the TD children can coordinate gesture and vocalization during communication (Wetherby et al., 1988). At 12 months, most TD children produce their first meaningful word (Blackwell and Baker, 2002). Before using word–word combinations, TD children start to use gesture–word combinations (Guidetti and Nicoladis, 2008; Iverson, 2010). Between 12 and 18 months, productive language and gesture are generally mutually exclusive (Capone and McGregor, 2004). At the multi-word stage, TD children use verbalization as their principal means of communication (Wetherby et al., 1988). However, from toddlers to preschoolers, combinations of gestures and verbalizations become more closely, and these cross-modal combinations can promote the development of language. At school age, gesture–speech mismatch occurs in TD children. Gesture–speech mismatch combination is a general cognitive phenomenon that reflects the transitional learning state for both science and math concepts (Capone and McGregor, 2004). In adults, communicative interactions are multimodal. They communicate with others through complex, fluid, and rapid coordination between speech, altering facial expression, changing eyebrows or head position, and gestures (Iverson, 2010).

Previous studies have found that gestures and early language development are closely linked (Bates and Dick, 2002). The development of gesture predates and predicts change in children’s language development (Iverson, 2010). For instance, Camaioni et al. (1991) found that communicative pointing at 12 months was significantly and positively correlated with vocabulary size at 20 months. And the number of different meaning gestures at 18 months predicted vocabulary at 42 months (Rowe and Goldin-Meadow, 2009). Additionally, the onset of gesture + word combinations could predict the emergence of word–word combinations (Iverson and Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Iverson et al., 2008; Iverson, 2010). Moreover, a study discovered that the number of gesture + speech combinations at 18 months predicted sentence complexity at 42 months (Rowe and Goldin-Meadow, 2009).

According to Bruner’s three earliest functions of communication, gestures can be divided into three categories: SI gestures, behavior regulation (BR) gestures, and joint attention (JA) gestures (Bruner, 1981). BR gestures are used to coordinate other people’s actions in order to make them do something or stop doing something; SI gestures are utilized to attract or keep others’ attention to oneself, with the purpose of initiating or maintaining the interaction with each other; JA gestures are used to attract others’ attention toward an event, an object, a person, or a topic, and just for showing or sharing (Bruner, 1981; Watson et al., 2013). A previous study reported that in TD children, BR gestures (at the mean age of 6.09 months) and SI gestures (at the mean age of 8.42 months) emerged before JA gestures (at the mean age of 9.33 months) (Crais et al., 2004). In general, gestures of these three communicative functions are seen in most TD children by 12 months of age (Watson et al., 2013). Wetherby et al. (1988) found that TD children displayed BR gestures and JA gestures most frequently in the second year of life.

Gestures in Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder

Previous studies explored the quantity of gestures in children with ASD of different ages and found that the main manifestation of ASD children is the reduction of gestures (Shumway and Wetherby, 2009; So et al., 2015; Lebarton and Iverson, 2016; Özçalışkan et al., 2016). Additionally, some studies discussed the communicative function and the integration ability of gestures in ASD children.

However, the conclusions of studies about the communitive function of gestures in ASD children were not entirely consistent. In early childhood, some studies have found that ASD children used fewer gestures (i.e., pointing, showing) to direct JA compared to TD children and children with language delay (LD) (Franchini et al., 2018). Additionally, Clifford et al. (2007) detected that ASD children used proto-declarative showing less than TD children and children with developmental disorders or LD at 12–24 months. Still, there was no difference in proto-declarative pointing, social gestures, and request gestures. However, another study found that infants with autism used fewer BR gestures, SI gestures, and JA gestures than TD children and children with other developmental disabilities at 15–18 months (Watson et al., 2013). In the pre-school stage, ASD children displayed fewer JA gestures than language-matched intellectual disability (ID) children or mental-age-matched ID children and showed fewer requesting gestures than mental-age-matched ID children; however, there was no difference in SI gestures (Mundy et al., 1990). These suggest that ASD children are less likely to use JA gestures, while the use of BR gestures and SI gestures is not determined.

Previous research on the integration of gesture and other communitive means in ASD children discovered that ASD children displayed deficits in integrating gestures and vocalization/verbalization compared to TD children or LD children (Parladé and Iverson, 2015; Choi et al., 2019). However, Shumway and Wetherby (2009) found no significant difference in the proportion of total acts coordinated with a vocalization, eye gaze, and gesture at the same time between ASD children, TD children, and children with developmental delays. And Heymann et al. (2018) found that ASD children were less likely to integrate JA behaviors (including gestures and eye gaze) and vocalization than TD children. Furthermore, a study analyzed the complex level of integrating different communicative forms (gesture, vocalization, and eye gaze) and found that the level of complexity in ASD children is significantly lower than TD children (Maljaars et al., 2011). To conclude, studies on the integration of vocalization/verbalization, eye gaze, and gesture in ASD children were contradictory. Notably, when exploring the integration of gestures and eye gaze, the above studies included other communicative means, such as smile, voice, and language. Therefore, gestures which only integrated with eye gaze may be missed. The current study coded and analyzed the integration of gesture and vocalization/verbalization, and the integration of gestures and eye gaze separately. It may help us understand the cross-modal coordination ability of gestures more accurately and comprehensively for ASD children.

Recent studies showed that gesture use might also play an essential role in the development of language for ASD children. Özçalışkan et al. (2016) found that the deictic gestures of 30-month-old children with ASD could predict the vocabulary 1 year later. Besides, the emergence of distal pointing was closely linked with the emergence of first words, and the onset of gesture + word combinations predates and predicts the onset of two-word combinations (Talbott et al., 2018). In adolescence, gesture use was positively associated with vocabulary for youths with ASD (Medeiros and Winsler, 2014).

Current Study

Previous studies have shown that the use of gestures was influenced by the variation of culture (Kita, 2009; Kwon et al., 2017). However, most of the current research works on ASD children’s gestures are based on Western-cultural conventions. This study was designed to explore the atypical gesture pattern of ASD children from the dimensions of quantity, communicative function, and integration ability based on the Chinese-cultural convention; and its relationship with social ability and adaptive behavior.

Materials and Methods

Participants

TD Group

Typically developing children were recruited through the internet or the Outpatient Department of Child Health Care at the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University in the corresponding period and met the following inclusion criteria: (1) Age 12–36 months. (2) The native language is Mandarin. (3) Parents of 12–18-month-old participants were asked to complete the Infant–Toddler Checklist (ITC; Wetherby and Prizant, 2002), and the results of the ITC must be “typical skills.” Parents of 18–36-month-old participants were asked to complete the Autism Behavior Checklist (ABC; Krug et al., 1978), and the total score of the ABC must be lower than 31. (4) All participants were confirmed as TD by two experienced developmental-behavioral pediatric specialists. The exclusion criteria are as follows: Developmental disorders, for example, ASD, ID, language disorder, etc.

ASD Group

All participants were recruited through the Child Developmental and Behavior Center of the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University from November 2018 to January 2020 and met the following inclusion criteria: (1) Age 24–48 months. (2) The native language is Mandarin. (3) All participants were diagnosed with ASD by two specialists in developmental-behavioral pediatrics using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2003) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; LeCouteur et al., 2003) following the DSM-V (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria. The exclusion criteria are as follows: (1) Other developmental disorders, such as ID, language disorder, etc. (2) Genetic conditions associated with autism, for example, Rett syndrome, Fragile X syndrome, and tuberous sclerosis.

All participants were Chinese people. This research was approved by the Institutional Review Board at the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University and informed consent was obtained from the parents of all participants.

There are 33 children in the ASD group and 24 children in the TD group. All ASD children have received behavior intervention about 20–28 hours per week. There was no significant difference between the ASD and TD groups regarding gender, mother’s educational attainment levels, and father’s educational attainment levels. The mean age of the ASD group was older. Compared to the TD group, the scores of social skills, conceptual skills, practical skills in ABAS-II, the total score of ABAS-II, and the total score of CCDI were significantly lower in the ASD group. The characteristics and inferential statistics of participants are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics and inferential statistics of children by group.

Tasks

Assessment of Diagnosis

In this study, the ADOS (Lord et al., 2003) and ADI-R (LeCouteur et al., 2003) were used for diagnosing ASD. We used the Chinese version of ADOS and ADI-R, which was revised by Professor Wu YuYu of Taiwan and authorized by Western Psychological Service.

Assessment of Communicative Gestures

A doctor who was not familiar with participants evaluated the participants’ gestural communications during a semi-structured play interaction. The content and sequence of play interaction were adapted from the ADOS. There were three main contents: blowing bubbles, blowing balloons, and snacking. Two social situations were set up in every content to encourage the children to express their demands or to show and share, while two kinds of communication opportunities. Child-initiated interaction and reactive interaction were also set up in every social situation. Only one parent was allowed to be present during the play interaction. The whole process of play interaction was videotaped for about 10 min by an assistant. The camera ensured that the child’s face and hands were recorded at the same time and the doctor’s face and hands. (The content and sequence of play interaction are shown in Supplementary Appendix 1).

Adaptive Behavior Assessment System Version II (ABAS-II; Oakland and Harrison, 2008)

The infant version of the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System Version II (ABAS-II) was used to assess the adaptive behavior of children aged 0–6 years. It is divided into a parent questionnaire and a teacher questionnaire. The adaptive behavior of children is evaluated from three levels. The first level is the overall adaptive function; the second level contains three composite areas of adaptive function: conceptual skills, social skills, and practical skills; the third level includes 10 concrete skill areas: communication, pre-school function, self-management, leisure, SI, community adaptability, family life, health and safety, self-care, and motor skills. Parents of participants completed the ABAS-II, which was revised by Professor Li YuQiu of Zhuhai Campus of Beijing Normal University and authorized by the American company, PEARSON. The social skills score and the total score (original score) were used to evaluate the social ability and adaptive behavior of participants. The higher the score, the better the social ability and adaptive behavior.

Chinese Communication Development Inventory (CCDI; Tardif et al., 2008)

Chinese Communication Development Inventory (CCDI) is the Chinese version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories (MCDI; Fenson et al., 2007), which is filled out by parents. CCDI is used to assess the early language development of children aged 8–30 months who speak Chinese (Mandarin or Cantonese). CCDI can also be used to assess older children with developmental disorders. There are two forms in CCDI: the infant form (Words and Gestures) and the toddler form (Words and Sentences). We used the toddler form of the Mandarin CCDI, which is divided into two sections: productive vocabulary and sentence complexity. The total score (raw score) of these two sections was used to evaluate the productive language of participants. The higher the score, the better the productive language. The highest total score of the toddler form of Mandarin CCDI is 903 (Tardif et al., 2008). In this study, there were 23 ASD children over 30 months old; and their mean CCDI total score is 236.13, with a minimum of 0 and a maximum of 795. There were six TD children over 30 months old; their mean CCDI total score is 778.17, with a minimum of 739 and a maximum of 841. In other words, no participants who were older than 30 months had a total score of CCDI above the 50th percentile score of 30 months old (boy: 844, girl: 850; Tardif et al., 2008), and this allowed us to use the CCDI to evaluate the productive language of all participants.

Gestures Coding

All behaviors of children in videos were coded using NVivo 12 (Windows) Pro software according to the following definitions.

Gestures

First of all, according to the checklist of coding gestures (Supplementary Appendix 2), we marked all target gestures of children. Second, we determined whether those gestures were used to communicate with another person (e.g., through the use of eye contact, vocalization, postural shift, repetition, or other interactive behaviors; Shumway and Wetherby, 2009; Parladé and Iverson, 2015; Özçalışkan et al., 2016). We excluded hand movements that were not used for communication. For example, we excluded imitation gestures (Braddock et al., 2015), hand movements that involved direct manipulation of an object, and hand movements that were part of a ritualized game (it should be noted that we did not exclude the showing gesture with communicative function; So et al., 2015).

The Communicative Function of Gestures

According to communicative function, gestures were coded using three categories (Bruner, 1981; Watson et al., 2013): (1) BR gestures are used to regulate another person’s behavior to get another person to doing something or stop doing something. (2) SI gestures are used to attract or maintain another person’s attention to oneself to initiate or maintain interaction. (3) JA gestures are used to draw another person’s attention to an object, event, person, or topic which only for sharing.

Integration Ability of Gestures

Temporal co-occurrence is defined as the duration of different communicative behavioral overlaps at any time point. Vocalization: children’s voices, such as vowel sound, laugh, cry, and squeal. Verbalization: single and multi-word spoken utterances. Vocalization/verbalization that was purely imitative (i.e., words repeated immediately after being spoken by another person) or not directed to another person were excluded (Shumway and Wetherby, 2009; Parladé and Iverson, 2015). With regard to any temporal co-occurrence between gesture and vocalization/verbalization, gestures were coded using these categories: vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures and gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration.

Eye gaze is defined as the visual attention children paid directly to another person’s eye region (Shumway and Wetherby, 2009; Parladé and Iverson, 2015). The eye region is defined as follows: In the horizontal direction, from the leftmost corner of the left eye to the rightmost corner of the right eye, and in the vertical direction, the area between the lower side of the eyebrow and the middle of the nose (He et al., 2019). The procedures of coding eye gaze: (1) Code the visual range of the child: We defined the visual range as within ± 20° of the child’s forward gazing direction. (2) Code the position in the relationship between child’s visual range and doctor’s eye region: We defined eye gaze behavior as that the visual range of the child can intersect with the eye region of the doctor (Figure 1). We defined the ±20° range based on previous research. Humans can pay visual attention to things inside the ±20° range around the facing direction, despite the direction the head faced. In contrast, they may choose to move their head when they pay visual attention to things outside of that range (Hachisu et al., 2018). Regarding whether there was any temporal co-occurrence between gesture and eye gaze, gestures were coded using these categories: eye-gaze-integrated gestures and gestures without eye-gaze integration.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Eye Gaze: the visual range of the child that can intersect with the eye region of the doctor.

Reliability

All videos were randomly assigned to two research assistants who were blind to group allocation. Two research assistants received coding training before coding the video separately. Approximately 20% of the participant videos were randomly selected and were double coded to calculate interrater agreement. The reliability of gestures coding between the two research assistants was estimated using an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) using an absolute agreement definition. The ICCs for the quantity of each gestures category (single measures) were as follows: total gestures (ICC = 0.940, p = 0.000), BR gestures (ICC = 0.949, p = 0.000), SI gestures (ICC = 0.852, p = 0.000), JA gestures (ICC = 0.842, p = 0.000), gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration (ICC = 0.993, p = 0.001), vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures (ICC = 0.936, p = 0.000), gestures without eye-gaze integration (ICC = 0.853, p = 0.000), and eye-gaze-integrated gestures (ICC = 0.989, p = 0.000).

Research Index

We utilized the relative frequency and the proportion of gestures in each category as research indexes. The conversion method was as follows: (a) the relative frequency of gestures: dividing the quantity of gestures in each category by the duration of videos in seconds separately and then multiplying by 600 to get the rate per 10 min of gestures in each category and (b) proportion of gestures: dividing the quantity of gestures in each category by the quantity of total gestures separately to get the proportion of gestures in each category.

Data Analysis

The analysis software used was SPSS Statistics version 23 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States), and the alpha was set at 0.05. Before conducting the data analysis, we detected the normal distribution of all data using normality tests. The total score of CCDI and some indices of gesture (the relative frequencies and the proportions of SI gestures and JA gestures, the relative frequency of eye-gaze-integrated gestures) were non-normally distributed, and the other variables were normally distributed. Therefore, Chi-square tests were conducted to explore the differences in gender and parents’ educational attainment levels between the TD and ASD groups. T-tests were used to analyze the differences in age and the ABAS-II between the two groups. Non-parametric statistics (Mann–Whitney tests) were utilized to explore the differences in the CCDI between the two groups. After the logarithm transformations of the gestures indexes with non-normal distribution, analysis of variance (ANOVA) and analysis of covariance (ANCOVA, corrected for the total score of CCDI) were used to explore the differences in gestures between ASD and TD groups. Pearson correlation analysis was utilized to test the correlation between gestures, social ability, and adaptive behavior. Furthermore, partial correlation analysis was used to control the total score of the CCDI to study the associations between gestures, social ability, and adaptive behavior.

Results

Considering that there were differences in the total score of CCDI between the ASD group and TD group, we use both ANOVA and ANCOVA (corrected for the total score of CCDI) when exploring the differences in gestures between groups.

The Quantity of Gestures

Whether we use ANOVA [F1(1,56) = 43.2801, p1 < 0.001] or ANCOVA [corrected for the total score of CCDI, F2(1,55) = 26.841, p2 < 0.001], we found that the relative frequency of total gestures in ASD group was lower than that in TD group (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive and inferential statistics for gestures of children by group.

The Communicative Function of Gestures

Whether we use ANOVA or ANCOVA, we found that the relative frequency of BR gestures [F1(1,56) = 33.346, p1 < 0.001; F2(1,55) = 20.136, p2 < 0.001], the relative frequency of SI gestures [F1(1,56) = 9.453, p1 = 0.003; F2(1,55) = 4.451, p = 0.040], the relative frequency [F1(1,56) = 17.111, p1 < 0.001; F2(1,55) = 10.083, p2 = 0.002], and the proportion [F1(1,56) = 8.416, p1 = 0.005; F2(1,55) = 4.913, p2 = 0.031] of JA gestures in the ASD group were significantly lower than those in the TD group, while there was no significant difference in the proportion of BR gestures and SI gestures among groups (p > 0.05) (Table 2 and Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean relative frequency and mean proportion of BR gestures, SI gestures, and JA gestures produced by TD children and ASD children. Error bars represent standard error. Note. BR, behavior regulation; SI, social interaction; JA, joint attention. *0.01 < p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

The Integration Ability of Gestures

We found that the relative frequency of gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration [F1(1,56) = 31.710, p1 < 0.001] and the relative frequency of vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures [F1(1,56) = 6.022, p1 = 0.017] in ASD group were significantly lower than that in TD group when we used ANOVA. While we utilized ANCOVA to adjust for the total score of CCDI, we found that only the relative frequency of gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration was significantly lower [F2(1,55) = 22.644, p2 < 0.001]. Moreover, when we used ANOVA or ANCOVA, we found no significant difference among ASD and TD groups in the proportion of vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures and gestures without vocalization/verbalization (p > 0.05).

Regardless of utilizing ANOVA or ANCOVA, we found that the relative frequency of eye-gaze-integrated gesture [F1(1,56) = 41.840, p1 < 0.001; F2(1,55) = 26.581, p2 < 0.001] and the proportion of eye-gaze-integrated gesture [F1(1,56) = 28.864, p1 < 0.001; F2(1,55) = 19.726, p2 < 0.001] in the ASD group were significantly lower than that in TD group. And ASD group showed a higher proportion in gestures without eye-gaze integration than the TD group [F1(1,56) = 28.864, p1 < 0.001; F2(1,55) = 19.726, p2 < 0.001]. Besides, we found no significant difference among ASD and TD groups in the relative frequency of gestures without eye-gaze integration (p > 0.05) (Table 2 and Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean relative frequency and mean proportion of gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration, vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures, gestures without eye-gaze integration, and eye-gaze-integrated gestures produced by TD children and ASD children. Error bars represent standard error. Note. GWVVI, gestures without vocalization/verbalization integration; VVIG, vocalization/verbalization-integrated gestures; GWEGI, gestures without eye-gaze integration; EGIG, eye-gaze-integrated gestures. *0.01 < p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

The Relationship Between Gestures and ABAS-II

We found some statistically different gestural indexes between ASD and TD groups by utilizing ANCOVA. Those indexes might reflect the deficiency of ASD children’s gestural communications most. Therefore, for reducing the number of calculated correlations, we only analyzed the correlation between ABAS-II and those gestural indexes. Besides, considering that the “proportion” indexes (i.e., the proportion of gestures without eye-gaze integration and the proportion of eye-gaze-integrated gestures) are merely complementary to each other, we only choose the proportion of eye-gaze-integrated gestures.

In ASD group, social skills score in ABAS-II was positively correlated with the relative frequency of SI gestures (r = 0.368, p = 0.035) and eye-gaze-integrated gestures (r = 0.375, p = 0.032); the total score of ABAS-II was positively correlated with the relative frequency of total gestures (r = 0.401, p = 0.021) and eye-gaze-integrated gestures (r = 0.411, p = 0.017). In TD group, the scores of ABAS-II were not significantly correlated with gestures (p > 0.05 for all) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation between gestures and ABAS-II.

When we controlled the total score of CCDI, the scores of ABAS-II were not significantly correlated with gestures in ASD group (p > 0.05 for all). In TD group, the social skills score in ABAS-II was positively correlated with the relative frequency of JA gestures (r = 0.439, p = 0.036) when controlling the total score of CCDI (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Partial correlation between gestures and ABAS-II.

Discussion

The present study aimed to explore the differences in gestures between ASD children and TD children in different productive language levels. As expected, we found an atypical gestures pattern of ASD children from the dimensions of quantity, communicative function, and integration ability.

The Quantity of Gestures

We found that ASD children had lower scores of CCDI compared to TD children. And no matter that we corrected for the total score of CCDI or not, we found that ASD children displayed fewer gestures than TD children. ASD children have impairments in SI and social communication, and their communication deficits are not limited to spoken language but also gesture (Iverson et al., 2017). In other words, in the early development of life, ASD children produce fewer gestures than TD children regardless of their productive language. Similarly, Mastrogiuseppe et al. (2015) found that the quantity of gestures produced by ASD children (chronological age range 30–66 months) was significantly lower than in TD children.

The Communicative Function of Gestures

Before and after controlling for the total score of CCDI, we found that ASD children used less BR, SI, and JA gestures than TD children, and the proportion of JA gestures in ASD children was significantly lower. The differences in the relative frequencies of BR, SI, and JA gestures between ASD and TD groups might be due to the overall differences in gesture productions. And the possible explanation for the lower proportion of JA gestures is that JA gestures are related to more complex triadic interactions. For example, ASD children might need to coordinate attention between themselves, the doctor, and objects/location/event at the same time when using JA gestures (e.g., pointing to the bubble to let the doctor notice the bubble’s location). In TD children, dyadic interaction with another person forms in the first 3 months of life, and dyadic interaction with object forms in the first 6 months of life. At 9–12 months, TD children begin to coordinate the two types of dyadic interactions to form triadic interactions (Bard, 2016). These complexities of triadic interactions might explain the deficits of ASD children in the use of JA gestures. Other studies have reported reduced triadic gestures in ASD children. For example, Watson et al. (2013) coded retrospective home videotapes and found that ASD children use fewer JA gestures at 9–12 and 15–19 months. In a prospective study of infant siblings at high-risk and low-risk ASD infants, Franchini et al. (2018) reported that initiations of JA were impaired from 12 months in ASD children, especially in the use of gestures (i.e., showing and pointing). Significantly, there was no difference in the proportion of SI gestures between ASD and TD children. One interpretation of this finding is that the limited semi-structured play situations might not have effectively triggered SI gestures from the children. Therefore, we may have underestimated the use of SI gestures in TD children.

The Integration Ability of Gestures

Before adjusting for the total score of CCDI, we found that ASD children were less likely to integrate gesture and vocalization/verbalization than TD children. However, after correcting for the total score of CCDI, we found that the ability of ASD children to integrate gesture and vocalization/verbalization was no different from TD children. Recently, Murillo et al. (2020) found that there is no difference in the proportion of gesture + vocalization combinations between ASD children and language-matched TD children. Previous studies have shown that gestures will be combined with speech temporally and semantically when children enter the two-word stage of language development (Sowden et al., 2008). This suggests that the development of spoken language and gestures is concurrent. From the results of this study, we believe the reduction in the integration of gesture and vocalization/verbalization may merely be a potential sign of LD. ASD children might be capable of integrating vocalization/verbalization with gestures.

Conversely, no matter that we corrected for the total score of CCDI or not, ASD children were worse at integrating gesture and eye gaze than TD children. Likewise, Murillo et al. (2020) suggested that ASD children did not integrate gaze with gestures as TD children did, regardless of their productive vocabulary. This indicates that eye gaze is closely related to the functional use (i.e., integration ability) of gestures. Previous studies have found that individuals with ASD have already experienced difficulties in social orientation in their infancy. Compared to TD individuals, individuals with ASD demonstrate decreased attention to socially relevant stimuli. In particular, they have a deficiency in processing the facial information of other people, as well as in establishing and maintaining eye contact (Guillon et al., 2014). We believe that the deficiency in the ability to integrate gestures with eye gaze seen in ASD children might be the core feature of their social impairment on the level of gestures.

The Relationship Between Gestures and Social Ability

For ASD children, the better social ability, the more SI gestures, and the better ability to integrate gesture and eye gaze. SI gestures are used to attract or maintain another person’s attention to oneself to initiate or maintain interaction (Watson et al., 2013). Thus, we decided that in terms of gestures, communication by SI gestures manifests better social ability in ASD children. Besides, previous studies have found that ASD children had impairment in facial perception; they reduced fixation on faces and eye region (Klin et al., 2002; Rice et al., 2012). The ability of facial perception of ASD children was related to their social ability (Klin et al., 2002; Mcpartland et al., 2011; Parish-Morris et al., 2013). Combined with the above results, we believe that the ability to integrate gesture and eye gaze in ASD children might be able to reflect their social ability.

The Relationship Between Gestures and Adaptive Behavior

Previous studies have shown that ASD children had deficits in adaptive behavior (Mouga et al., 2015; Bradshaw et al., 2018). The improvement of adaptive behavior is one of the crucial outcomes of ASD intervention (Zachor and Ben-Itzchak, 2017). However, only a few studies preliminary discussed the relationship between adaptive behavior and gestures. For example, Kjellmer et al. (2012) found that for ASD children, non-verbal communications seem to be related to adaptive behavior, and Stamper et al. (2010) found that ASD children’s deficits in gestural communication are related to adaptive behavior. Importantly, this study explored the relationship between gestures and adaptive behavior from the aspects of gestures’ quantity, gestures’ communicative function, and gestures’ integration ability. The results showed that in ASD children, the number of gestures and the ability to integrate gesture and eye gaze positively correlate with adaptive behavior. It is probably because when ASD children produce more gestures, they communicate with others more. Furthermore, when communicating by gestures, eye-gaze integration may make communicative behavior more natural and smooth so that ASD children can better adapt to social life.

However, when controlling the productive language, the correlations between gesture, adaptive behavior, and social ability disappeared in ASD children. It may indicate that the relationships between gesture, adaptive behavior, and social ability are influenced by productive language. Future studies should explore the role productive language plays on relationships between gesture, social ability, and adaptive behavior.

In the TD group, we found no significant relationship between gestures, social ability, and adaptive behavior. However, when we controlled productive language, the social ability was positively correlated with JA gestures. That is probably because the development of gestures in TD children is more closely related to language development (Goldin-Meadow and Alibali, 2013).

Limitation

By reviewing research, we can find that TD children’s gestures and language develop rapidly in the second and third years after birth. Consequently, we enrolled 12–36 months TD children. And gestures and early language development are closely linked. It is necessary to consider the impact of language when exploring the difference in gesture patterns between ASD children and TD children. In some previous research on gestures of ASD children, the chronological age of TD children ranged from 12 to 36 months. After matching, they enrolled ASD children who are 1–2 years older than TD children (Mastrogiuseppe et al., 2015; Özçalışkan et al., 2016, 2017). According to these research works, we enrolled 24–48 months ASD children to make the productive language between ASD children and TD children more comparable. In future work, we should use relevant assessments to match productive language development between the ASD and control groups. The semi-structured play situation may elicit communication strategies that are not operated by ASD children in naturalistic situations. In the future, we can investigate the pattern of ASD children’s gestures in natural situations by coding family videos. Moreover, there are only TD children in the control group of this study. Children with other developmental disorders need to be included in future studies to ensure that the results are more specific. Last, in the associations between gestures and ABAS-II, the significance level around 0.01 < P < 0.05, which might be a consequence of the Type 1 error chance. In future studies, we should increase the number of participants and set up the significance level of p < 0.01.

Conclusion

We discovered the atypical gesture patterns of ASD children: (1) ASD children produce fewer gestures and have deficits in triadic interaction gestures (i.e., JA gestures). (2) The deficiency of integrating eye gaze and gesture is the core deficit of ASD children’s gesture communication. Relatively, children with ASD might be capable of integrating vocalization/verbalization into gestures. Furthermore, we found that SI gestures and the ability to integrate gestures and eye gaze are related to the social ability. The quantity of gestures and the ability to integrate gestures with eye gaze are related to adaptive behavior.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Third Affiliated Hospital of Sun Yat-sen University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

QY, XZ, and HD contributed to conception and design of the study. QY and SC organized the database. QY, LL, and HZ performed the statistical analysis. QY wrote the first draft of the manuscript. QY, SL, YX, and HD wrote the sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, and read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Science and Technology Program of Guangzhou, China, Key Area Research and Development Program (202007030011).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.604542/full#supplementary-material

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bard, K. A. (2016). Dyadic interactions, attachment and the presence of triadic interactions in chimpanzees and humans. Infant Behav. Dev. 48, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., and Dick, F. (2002). Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Dev. Psychobiol. 40, 293–310.

Google Scholar

Blackwell, P. B., and Baker, B. M. (2002). Estimating communication competence of infants and toddlers. J. Pediatr. Health Care 16, 29–35. doi: 10.1067/mph.2002.115137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braddock, B. A., Pickett, C., Ezzelgot, J., Sheth, S., Korte-Stroff, E., Loncke, F., et al. (2015). Potential communicative acts in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev. Neurorehabil. 18, 260–271. doi: 10.3109/17518423.2013.799243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, J., Gillespie, S., Klaiman, C., Klin, A., and Saulnier, C. (2018). Early emergence of discrepancy in adaptive behavior and cognitive skills in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 23, 1485–1496. doi: 10.1177/1362361318815662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruner, J. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Lang. Commun. 1, 155–178. doi: 10.1016/0271-5309(81)90010-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E., and Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Lang. 11, 345–358. doi: 10.1177/014272379101103303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capone, N. C., and McGregor, K. K. (2004). Gesture development: a review for clinical and research practices. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 173–186. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/015)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, B., Shah, P., Rowe, M. L., Nelson, C. A., and Tager-Flusberg, H. (2019). Gesture development, caregiver responsiveness, and language and diagnostic outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 2556–2572. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03980-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clifford, S., Young, R., and Williamson, P. (2007). Assessing the early characteristics of autistic disorder using video analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 37, 301–313. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0160-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crais, E., Douglas, D. D., and Campbell, C. C. (2004). The intersection of the development of gestures and intentionality. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 678–694. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/052)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., and Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, 2nd Edn. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Google Scholar

Franchini, M., Hamodat, T., Armstrong, V. L., Sacrey, L. R., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., et al. (2018). Infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: frequency, quality, and variety of joint attention behaviors. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 47, 907–920. doi: 10.1007/s10802-018-0471-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin-Meadow, S., and Alibali, M. W. (2013). Gesture’s role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 257–283. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guidetti, M., and Nicoladis, E. (2008). Introduction to special issue: gestures and communicative development. Frist Lang. 28, 107–115. doi: 10.1177/0142723708088914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guillon, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduel, S., and Roge, B. (2014). Visual social attention in autism spectrum disorder: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 279–297. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hachisu, T., Pan, Y., Matsuda, S., Bourreau, B., and Suzuki, K. (2018). FaceLooks: a smart headband for signaling face-to-face behavior. Sensors 18, 2066. doi: 10.3390/s18072066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., Su, Q., Wang, L., He, W., Tan, C., Zhang, H., et al. (2019). The characteristics of intelligence profile and eye gaze in facial emotion recognition in mild and moderate preschoolers with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychiatry 10:402. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heymann, P., Northrup, J. B., West, K. L., Parladé, M. V., Leezenbaum, N. B., and Iverson, J. M. (2018). Coordination is key: Joint attention and vocalisation in infant siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 00, 1–14. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M. (2010). Multimodality in infancy: vocal-motor and speech-gesture coordinations in typical and atypical development. Enfance 3, 257–274. doi: 10.4074/S0013754510003046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Volterra, V., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning to talk in a gesture-rich world: early communication in Italian vs. American children. First Lang. 28, 164–181. doi: 10.1177/0142723707087736

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol. Sci. 16, 367–371. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., Northrup, J. B., Leezenbaum, N. B., Parladé, M. V., Koterba, E. A., and West, K. L. (2017). Early gesture and vocabulary development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 48, 55–71. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3297-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kita, S. (2009). Cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: a review. Lang. Cogn. Process. 24, 145–167. doi: 10.1080/01690960802586188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjellmer, L., Hedval, L. A., Fernell, E., Gillberg, C., and Norrelgen, F. (2012). Language and communication skills in pre-school children with autism spectrum disorders: contribution of cognition, severity of autism symptoms, and adaptive functioning to the variability. Res. Dev. Disabil. 33, 172–180. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., and Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 59, 809. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krug, D. A., Arick, J. R., and Almond, P. J. (1978). “Autism screening instrument for educational planning: background and development,” in Autism: Diagnosis, Instruction, Management and Research, ed. J. Gillam (Austin, TX: University of Texas Press).

Google Scholar

Kwon, A. Y., Vallotton, C. D., Kiegelmann, M., and Wilhelm, K. H. (2017). Cultural diversification of communicative gestures through early childhood: a comparison of children in English-, German-, and Chinese- speaking families. Infant Behav. Dev. 50, 328–339. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebarton, E. S., and Iverson, J. M. (2016). Gesture development in toddlers with an older sibling with autism. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 51, 18–30. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeCouteur, A., Lord, C., and Rutter, M. (2003). The Autism Diagnostic Interview: Revised. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Lord, C., Rutter, M., Dilavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., and Bishop, S. (2003). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule Manual. Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Maljaars, J., Noens, I., Jansen, R., Scholte, E., and Berckelaer-Onnes, I. V. (2011). Intentional communication in nonverbal and verbal low-functioning children with autism. J. Commun. Disord. 44, 601–614. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastrogiuseppe, M., Capirci, O., Cuva, S., and Venuti, P. (2015). Gestural communication in children with autism spectrum disorders during mother–child interaction. Autism 19, 469–481. doi: 10.1177/1362361314528390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthew, J. M., Kelly, A. S., Jon, B., Anita, W., Mary, P., Monica, D., et al. (2020). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. MMWR Surveill. Summ. 69, 1–12. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mcpartland, J. C., Webb, S. J., Keehn, B., and Dawson, G. (2011). Patterns of visual attention to faces and objects in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 41, 148–157. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1033-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Medeiros, K., and Winsler, A. (2014). Parent–child gesture use during problem solving in autistic spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 44, 1946–1958. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2069-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouga, S., Almeida, J., Café, C., Duque, F., and Oliveira, G. (2015). Adaptive profiles in autism and other neurodevelopmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 1001–1012. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2256-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundy, P., Sigman, M., and Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 20, 115–128. doi: 10.1007/BF02206861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murillo, E., Camacho, L., and Montero, I. (2020). Multimodal communication in children with autism spectrum disorder and different linguistic development. J. Autism Dev. Disord. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04637-7 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Oakland, T., and Harrison, P. L. (2008). Adaptive Behavior Assessment System-II: Clinical Use and Interpretation. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., and Dimitrova, N. (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism 20, 754–763. doi: 10.1177/1362361315605921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Özçalışkan, Ş, Adamson, L. B., Dimitrova, N., and Baumann, S. (2017). Early gesture provides a helping hand to spoken vocabulary development for children with autism, down syndrome, and typical development. J. Cogn. Dev. 18, 325–337. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1329735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parish-Morris, J., Chevallier, C., Tonge, N., Letzen, J., Pandey, J., and Schultz, R. T. (2013). Visual attention to dynamic faces and objects is linked to face processing skills: a combined study of children with autism and controls. Front. Psychol. 4:185. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parladé, M. V., and Iverson, J. M. (2015). The development of coordinated communication in infants at heightened risk for autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 45, 2218–2234. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2391-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rice, K., Moriuchi, J. M., Jones, W., and Klin, A. (2012). Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorders: visual scanning of dynamic social scenes in school-aged children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 51, 238–248. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, M. L., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Dev. Sci. 12, 182–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, R. L. (2007). Social communication impairments: pragmatics. Pediatr. Clin. North Am. 54, 483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2007.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shumway, S., and Wetherby, A. M. (2009). Communicative acts of children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 1139–1156. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0280)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

So, W.-C., Wong, M. K., Lui, M., and Yip, V. (2015). The development of co-speech gesture and its semantic integration with speech in 6- to 12-year-old children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism 19, 956–968. doi: 10.1177/1362361314556783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sowden, H., Perkins, M., and Clegg, J. (2008). The co−development of speech and gesture in children with autism. Clin. Ling. Phonet. 22, 804–813. doi: 10.1080/02699200801919745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stamper, K., Bernier, R., and Gerdts, J. A. (2010). “The relationship between gesture use and adaptive functioning in autism,” in Proceedings of the International Meeting for Autism Research 2010, Chicago, United States of America, (Kansas City, MO: International Society for Autism Research).

Google Scholar

Talbott, M. R., Young, G. S., Munson, J., Estes, A., Vismara, L. A., and Rogers, S. J. (2018). The developmental sequence and relations between gesture and spoken language in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Child Dev. 91, 743–753. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tardif, T., Fletcher, P., Zhi-xiang, Z., and Wei-lan, L. (2008). Chinese Communication Development Inventory Manual. Beijing: Peking University Medical Press.

Google Scholar

Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., Baranek, G. T., Dykstra, J. R., and Wilson, K. P. (2013). Communicative gesture use in infants with and without autism: a retrospective home video study. Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 22, 25–39. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0145)

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherby, A. M., Cain, D. H., Yonclas, D. G., and Walker, V. G. (1988). Analysis of intentional communication of normal children from the prelinguistic to the multi-word stage. J. Speech Hear. Res. 31, 240–252. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3102.240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherby, A. M., and Prizant, B. (2002). Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental Profile (1st Normed ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

Google Scholar

Zachor, D. A., and Ben-Itzchak, E. (2017). Variables affecting outcome of early intervention in autism spectrum disorder. J. Pediatr. Neurol. 15, 129–133. doi: 10.1055/s-0037-1601444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism spectrum disorder, gesture, language, adaptive behavior, social ability

Citation: Ye Q, Liu L, Lv S, Cheng S, Zhu H, Xu Y, Zou X and Deng H (2021) The Gestures in 2–4-Year-Old Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front. Psychol. 12:604542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.604542

Received: 09 September 2020; Accepted: 08 January 2021;
Published: 28 January 2021.

Edited by:

Els Ortibus, KU Leuven, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Bart Boets, KU Leuven, Belgium
Eva Murillo, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Spain

Copyright © 2021 Ye, Liu, Lv, Cheng, Zhu, Xu, Zou and Deng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: HongZhu Deng, ZGVuZ2h6aEBtYWlsLnN5c3UuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.