Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 11 March 2021
Sec. Emotion Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): Psychological Reactions to the Pandemic View all 66 articles

“What If We Get Sick?”: Spanish Adaptation and Validation of the Fear of Illness and Virus Evaluation Scale in a Non-clinical Sample Exposed to the COVID-19 Pandemic

  • 1Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 2Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Finis Terrae, Santiago, Chile
  • 3Instituto Milenio para la Investigación en Depresión y Personalidad, MIDAP, Santiago, Chile
  • 4Escuela de Psicología, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile
  • 5Facultad de Ciencias Sociales, Escuela de Psicología, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
  • 6Departamento de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental Oriente, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Chile, Santiago, Chile

Distinct sources of stress have emerged during the COVID-19 pandemic. Particularly, fear is expected to generate significant psychological burden on individuals and influence on either unsafe behavior that may hinder recovery efforts or virus-mitigating behaviors. However, little is known about the properties of measures to capture them in research and clinical settings. To resolve this gap, we evaluated the psychometric properties of a novel measure of fear of illness and viruses and tested its predictive value for future development of distress. We extracted a random sample of 450 Chilean adult participants from a large cross-sectional survey panel and invited to participate in this intensive longitudinal study for 35 days. Of these, 163 ended up enrolling in the study after the demanding nature of the measurement schedule was clearly explained to them. For this final sample, we calculated different Confirmatory Factor Analyses (CFA) to evaluate the preliminary proposed structure for the instrument. Complementarily, we conducted a content analysis of the items to qualitatively extract its latent structure, which was also subject to empirical test via CFA. Results indicated that the original structure did not fit the data well; however, the new proposed structure based on the content analysis did. Overall, the modified instrument showed good reliability through all subscales both by its internal consistency with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.814 to 0.913, and with test–retest correlations ranging from 0.715 to 0.804. Regarding its convergent validity, individuals who scored higher in fears tended to also score higher in depressive and posttraumatic stress symptoms at baseline. Furthermore, higher fears at baseline predicted a higher score in posttraumatic stress symptomatology 7 days later. These results provide evidence for the validity, reliability, and predictive performance of the scale. As the scale is free and multidimensional potentially not circumscribed to COVID-19, it might work as a step toward understanding the psychological impact of current and future pandemics, or further life-threatening health situations of similar characteristics. Limitations, practical implications, and future directions for research are discussed.

Introduction

17,334,539 is the number of people who, as of July 31th 2020, have been identified worldwide as positive cases of Coronavirus 2019 (COVID-19). This disease, declared 6 months ago as a public health emergency of international concern, is caused by the SARS-CoV-2 virus consisting of a new human pathogen with a high transmission capacity with animals (Rodríguez-Morales et al., 2020). Of the total infected, 674,038 people have died (World Health Organization, 2020). The first number (of cases) is equivalent to the total population of countries, such as the Netherlands or Syria, while the second number is higher than the total population of Luxembourg.

A disease is declared a pandemic when the transmission between people without immunity surpasses what was expected on a global scale (Morens et al., 2009). Furthermore, the virus has appeared in the age of technology. This has permitted us to witness the impressive pace at which the number of cases is rising and re-surging in waves across countries. This information can be obtained through official reports issued in real time by the World Health Organization through ProMEDmail or by “Coronavirus COVID-19 Global Cases/CSSE,” a specialized website of Johns Hopkins University (Bonilla-Aldana et al., 2020; Dong et al., 2020).

The coronavirus arrived in Latin America on the 26th of February when the first case of infection was confirmed in Brazil (Rodríguez-Morales et al., 2020). Since then, it has spread rapidly, encountering a 640,000,000-population region ill-prepared for this massive sanitary challenge and the social, economic, and psychological ramifications of the crisis (Biscayart et al., 2020). To reduce the viral transmission, each country in the South American region has scrambled to activate mitigation measures, including recommended confinement in some countries and mandatory lockdowns in others (Rodríguez-Morales et al., 2020). In practical terms, the strategy has been to isolate those who are infected, quarantine those who have potentially been exposed to the disease, and keep social contact to a minimum (Brooks et al., 2020), with extra precautions on high-risk populations such as the elderly and people with previous illnesses (Public Health England, 2020). Despite these strategies, by the time this paper is being written, three South American countries belong to the top 10 list with the highest number of confirmed cases (2,610,102 in Brazil; 400, 683 in Perú; and 353, 536 in Chile), the United States being the highest globally (4,496, 737) where 17% of the population is Hispanic or Latin American.

Chile, in particular, has been a highly affected country with 355.667 and 9.457 deaths. Moreover, we believe fear reports will be especially variable within the country considering the differential strategy that the Chilean government implemented compared to the rest of the continent. Most of Latin American countries implemented expansive quarantives and national lockdowns, while Chile started with partial lockdowns in specific districts, starting with the ones where COVID-19 was supposedly originated due to people returning from holidays outside the country.

Even though scholars and researchers of all disciplines have invested time and effort outlining tentative approaches to make predictions, it is still difficult to estimate the impact that this pandemic will have on our way of conceiving the world and our way of living. There are traditional epidemiological tools, but since it is a new virus, with unknown characteristics, it is more complicated to make predictions using the trajectory of previous diseases (The Lancet, 2019). Because of this, the balance between the benefit of such unpleasant but necessary coping strategies needs to be weighed against the present and future costs (Brooks et al., 2020).

Uncertainty, loneliness, vulnerability, economic insecurity, fear of infection, and facing death for ourselves or our loved ones are among the distinct sources of stress that have emerged in the pandemic’s setting and are expected to generate a significant burden on individuals (Lima et al., 2020; Montemurro, 2020; Moreno et al., 2020; Thakur and Jain, 2020). Gu et al. (2015) found that after the H1N1 epidemic, 45% of those surveyed felt fear for themselves or a loved one to become infected, and around 10% felt panic because of the contagion. Unconfirmed beliefs about the virus and perceived lethality increased emotional affectation, which underscores the value of providing adequate information from reliable sources (Gu et al., 2015).

In the setting of the current pandemic, though heterogeneous, preliminary evidence obtained mostly through online surveys reveals moderate-to-severe impact on the mental health of the general population, including an increase in symptoms of depression and posttraumatic stress disorders (Moreno et al., 2020; Tang et al., 2020). This impact seems to be higher in younger people (Tang et al., 2020) and at-risk populations, including individuals with preexisting mental health problems (Steenblock et al., 2020), who have or have been infected (Bo et al., 2020) or who are health-care workers (Chen Q. et al., 2020).

Understanding the scope and intensity that the COVID-19 pandemic has on mental health requires appropriate instrumentation to track directly related stressors. In addition to standard measurements of psychopathology (e.g., depression, anxiety, trauma), the unprecedented nature of this emergency likely requires the development of new instrumentation covering constructs directly related to the virus.

The Importance of Measuring Fear Specifically Related to COVID-19

The construct of fear, as related to the COVID-19 pandemic, requires attention. Fear, as one of the basic emotions of the human being (Plutchik, 1980), plays a fundamental role in survival and adaptation to the environment, since it fulfills two important functions: first, the activation of physiological systems that prepare the organism for flight and defense, and secondly, avoiding exposure to potentially harmful stimuli through learning and cognitive assessment of danger or threat (Sosa and Capafóns, 2003; de Hoog et al., 2008). Although fear is functional and adaptive for humans, it ceases to be so when it occurs in the absence of a real danger or when it appears excessively, becoming a complex problem that is difficult to control (Martínez Pérez et al., 2009). Knowing the different fear levels might help us develop specific programs for groups of people according to certain characteristics or identify which of them require specific considerations because of a particular risk (Pakpour and Griffiths, 2020).

In the setting of natural disasters or traumatic events, the fear intensity seems to have a bearing on the development or exacerbation of mental health problems, appearing to be particularly relevant in the context of a virus as reported in previous pandemics (Maunder et al., 2003), as well as for COVID-19 (Moreno et al., 2020; Tang et al., 2020). In a sample of 2,485 participants, Tang et al. (2020) found that feeling extreme fear was one of the most significant predictors of both depression and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which may have higher and more lasting implications than the actual pandemic (Ornell et al., 2020).

The still unknown short- and long-term medical consequences of contracting COVID-19, with the invisible and rapid airborne dissemination mechanism, naturally comes with uncertainty and major changes in lifestyle that can further promote a sense of fear in the population (Huang and Zhao, 2020). In particular, fears of infection can promote stigmatization and discrimination against infected individuals or those thought to be infected, leading to increased depression, anxiety, and even suicide. The latter was anecdotally reported by Mamun and Griffiths (2020) in their account of the first suicide related to COVID-19 fear and more systematically by Dsouza et al. (2020) in India.

Also, above and beyond adverse mental health outcomes related to individual fears, “pandemic fear” may hinder recovery efforts, promote unsafe behaviors, and inhibit prosocial behaviors (e.g., hoarding, violence against health professionals, discrimination against potentially infected individuals, and stigma toward the certain societies) (Ahorsu et al., 2020; Lin, 2020). Most of these may be prevented since they seem to be consequences of misperception and misinformation among individuals, particularly when recommendations are constantly changing (Lin, 2020).

Interestingly, virus-mitigating behaviors, such as compliance with recommendations and enforced indications to prevent infection, have also been related to fear of COVID-19, specifically “functional fear.” This is thought to promote compliance with ordinances and thus decrease infection rates. In an international study, the fear of COVID-19 was the only significant predictor of several virus-mitigating behaviors, including hand washing and social distancing (Harper et al., 2020; Pakpour and Griffiths, 2020).

Taken together, all this evidence points to the importance of measuring fear as specifically related to COVID-19.

Scales Developed for Measuring Fear During the COVID-19 Pandemic

Some measures have been developed for this purpose since the beginning of the pandemic, including the Fear of COVID-19 Scale (FCV-19S Ahorsu et al., 2020) and the Fear of Illness and Virus Evaluation (FIVE, Ehrenreich-May, in preparation). The efforts for developing appropriate scales might help advance research on the specific contribution of fear to the development of mental health problems as well as to individual behaviors related to community-level recovery (e.g., less stigmatization, pro-social and virus-mitigating behaviors).

On one hand, the FCV-19S is a very short seven-item, unidimensional measure used to measure the fear level of COVID-19. This has the advantage of being easier to understand and truly capture the essence of the fear construct. We believe that this scale is appropriate when the goal is to measure severity of fear, since it has one dimension and robust psychometric properties. This scale seems to be very useful for massive-scale studies and for comparing intersubject levels of fear.

Compared to the FCV-19, the FIVE is a multidimensional measure that, even though it is longer, may have the potential to disaggregate different dimensions related to fear. We believe that it would be useful to develop and validate scales that could let us comprehend the different motives behind one to be fearful about COVID-19, particularly in such uncertain context. Distinct facets, as measured by the FIVE, may be differentially related to these outcomes allowing grouping specific information about fear and thus more nuanced associations with other psychological and behavioral outcomes. This may help us understand community-level responses and develop adapted and contingent strategies.

The Fear of Illness and Virus Evaluation

The FIVE (Ehrenreich-May, in preparation) was developed in this context, to measure specific fears related to the possibility of infection and the socioemotional distress caused by it. The original scale is freely available in the Supplementary Materials section. It may be freely used with permission from Jill Ehrenreich-May, Ph.D. (j.ehrenreich@miami.edu). As identified in the literature, fears of illness can be a moderator for the impact of the pandemic on mental health (i.e., more fears, more psychological vulnerability) (Moreno et al., 2020; Tang et al., 2020). The FIVE is an original self-report questionnaire for adults that measures fears of illness and viruses across four dimensions: (1) fears associated with infection and illness, (2) fears associated with social distancing, (3) behaviors associated with fear of illness and the virus, and (3) the functional impact of fears associated with illness and the virus. The first two dimensions are directly related to fears whereas the third one relates to behaviors due to the fears and the fourth dimension presents an overall measure of functional affectation. The questionnaire has 35 Likert-scale type items. It was originally developed in English, and to date, there are no published validations in Spanish and psychometric properties of the original measure (including internal consistency, factor structure, dimensionality analysis, test–retest reliability, and convergent validity) have not been published yet.

Even though there are other, specifically developed measures of COVID-19 fears available, the FIVE has two potential advantages. First, it has a theoretical multidimensional structure that may allow for the disaggregation of specific information about the construct and thus a more nuanced examination of the relationships between fear of illness and mental health or behavioral outcomes. Secondly, it is a more general measure of fear of illness and virus, which means it can be used in different situations where illness may be the source of fear for individuals. In other words, it has a larger generalization for its current and future use.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Data was collected between April and June 2020 and coincided with general social distancing recommendations and a mandatory quarantine due to the COVID-19 pandemic in several provinces of Chile. In the study, 450 participants were purposely sampled (Campbell et al., 2020) from a pool of 2,757 voluntary participants initially contacted through social networks who signed consents and provided basic socio-demographic information. This reduction of the sample was conducted in order to correct for typical biases of online convenience sampling, including a majority of female participants (88.6%) and a disproportionate number of students and younger participants (28.4%). Because of these biases, concerns have been raised about the use of convenience sampling and online surveys during the COVID-19 pandemic (Pierce et al., 2020). We attempted to correct for these biases by using quota sampling procedures, and the random sample of 450 participants was forced to maintain a 50% female–male ratio and a cap of 9% of students, which is in line with known population parameters in Chile. An additional reduction in the sample was observed when the demanding nature of the study protocol (i.e., daily prompts for 35 days and subsequent follow-ups at 2, 4, and 6 months) was clearly explained to the invited participants. At this stage, of the 450 invited participants, only 163 participants registered and downloaded the study app. Even though sample size was significantly smaller than expected, this was the result of a trade-off between a larger, unbalanced sample and a smaller, more committed sample, balanced for gender and proportion of students.

Table 1 shows the demographic and clinical characteristics of the final sample (N = 163). When available, reliable population benchmarks are presented to describe the extent to which the study sample differs from population parameters. It can be seen that in terms of age, gender, and work status distribution the study sample is similar to the population. However, it is also evident that the study sample differs significantly from population parameters related to psychosocial factors, including education and income. The comparison between the study sample and population estimates for clinical variables is also of interest. The depression benchmark indicates the prevalence of depressive symptomatology (i.e., root symptoms of anhedonia and low mood) in the general population. As expected, in a sample of adults exposed to the COVID-19 pandemic and related restrictions and alterations of daily life, depressive symptomatology is higher, even using a more stringent criterion (PHQ-9 > 10) (Margozzini and Passi, 2018). The PTSD benchmark is interesting because it comes from a population-representative study that used a short posttraumatic symptomatology screener (PTSS) just after the Chilean 2010 earthquake and tsunami. The percentage of individuals presenting with these symptoms after the earthquake and tsunami is similar to the estimate of individuals with PTSD in our study sample. Of note, in coastal areas most affected by the earthquake and the tsunami, the percentage of individuals endorsing post-traumatic symptoms rises to 13.1% (Abeldaño et al., 2014).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the participants.

Procedure

The 163 participants who were selected from the initial large sample were invited to participate in the intensive phase of the study, which lasted 35 days. All the measurements taken during the study were collected from the participants’ cell phones through a commercially available application (Ethica Data Services Inc, 2020), who provided us with a free license for this study. The participants received instructions to download the application, through which questionnaires and questions were sent to the participants’ smartphones (Android or IOS).

After registering for the study and installing the application, participants completed a more detailed socio-demographic characterization instrument, with a baseline consisting of the Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9), the International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ), and the FIVE. We re-sent these instruments for participants to complete every 5 (PHQ-9) or 7 days (ITQ and FIVE) during the 35 days of the intensive phase of the study. During the study, participants also completed daily measurements of positive and negative emotionality, satisfaction with romantic relationships, parenting roles, and social support, which were part of the broader study. There was also daily monitoring of hours of sleep, fluctuations in appetite, weight, substance use, daily hours of exercise, contact on social media through the internet, and internet use. Lastly, we collected passive information from their cellular device registered through pedometry, ambient light, GPS location, and screen time.

During the intensive phase of the study, we held two raffles with prizes to compensate participants.

The study protocol was authorized by the Committee of Ethics in Science of Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, prior to data collection.

Measures

Fear of Illness and Virus

We used the Spanish version of the Fear of Illness. This version is available in the Supplementary Materials section. We used the “Fear of Illness and Virus Evaluation” (FIVE) (Ehrenreich-May, in preparation), a scale designed during the current COVID-19 pandemic to evaluate fear and fear-associated behaviors. The adult version was originally designed in English and consists of 35 items on a 4-point Likert scale. Its first 19-item range from “I’m not afraid of this at all” to “I’m afraid of this all the time,” reflecting two dimensions: “Fears about Contamination and Illness” (items 1–9) and “Fears about Social Distancing” (items 10–19). The instrument also includes a third part consisting of a list of potential behaviors related to the previously mentioned fears (e.g., staying away from people and using hand sanitizer), ranging from “I have not done this in the last week” to “I did this all the time last week” (items 20–33). Finally, a fourth part has two questions about the impact of Illness and virus fears (experiencing strong emotions and getting in the way of enjoying life), ranging from “Not true for me at all” to “Definitely true” (items 34 and 35). To date, no psychometric properties have been reported for this instrument, while the author describes the organization of the items as “provisional subscales.”

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Complex Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Posttraumatic stress was evaluated using the ITQ (Cloitre et al., 2018), based on the International Classification of Diseases 11th Revision (ICD-11) criteria for PTSD and complex posttraumatic stress disorder (CPTSD). It consists of 18 items on a 5-point Likert scale from 0 “Not at all” to 4 “Extremely.” The participant needs to identify a particularly troublesome experience, its onset, and the time frame in which it occurred, to then answer all questions in regard to this experience. The first six items measure the criteria for PTSD, while items 7–9 measure functional impairment related to it. Items 10–15 measure the criteria for CPTSD, while items 16–18 measure their related functional impairment. From that 12 items, specific items are then used to identify PTSD and CPTSD, and the 6 remaining represent the general functional impairment. The scale has been used dimensionally and showed good levels of general internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.90; Ho et al., 2019). An available Chilean translation was used in the current study (Fresno et al., in preparation).

Depressive Symptomatology

Depressive symptomatology was measured using the nine-item “Patient Health Questionnaire” (PHQ-9; Kroenke et al., 2001), based on DSM-IV criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 1990). It consists of nine items on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 “Never” to 3 “Almost every day,” measuring frequency of criteria met for the last 2 weeks. A version validated in Chile was used for the present study (Tomas Baader et al., 2012), which showed good reliability (Cronbach’s α = 0.835).

Analytic Plan

We analyzed data using the statistical environment R (R Core Team, 2020) and Mplus 8 (Muthén and Muthén, 2017). We present descriptive statistics and a correlation matrix for the first measurement. Then, we present the computed set of CFA where we tested the underlying structure of the FIVE using the R library “lavaan” (Rosseel, 2012). We used a robust maximum likelihood (MLR) method of estimation to compensate for violations of the multivariate normality assumption (Kline, 2015). MLR corrects both standard errors and chi-square statistics for deviations from normality (Li, 2015). We assessed fit to the data using the more stringent criteria proposed by Hu and Bentler (1999; TLI > 0.95 CFI > 0.95, RMSEA < 0.06, and RMSR < 0.08).

We performed CFA to test six potential structures. First, we tested a one-factor structure where all items load in one general factor including items 20–35. Then, we computed a one “fear” factor excluding items 20–35. While this solution was not informed by the authors’ scoring instructions, we nonetheless examined it as a baseline comparison. We then tested a third factor using the four theoretical dimensions derived from the proposed provisional subscales described above (Ehrenreich-May, in preparation). Fourth, we computed a bi-factor model (Reise et al., 2010) in which a general factor captured the commonality between items, while four orthogonal factors capture residual variability not accounted for by the former.

Given the preliminary nature of the structures, we also made a content analysis of the items to propose latent dimensions based on the qualitative commonalities between the items and following an open coding technique based on the framework of grounded theory (Strauss and Corbin, 1998). We tested the proposed content-based structure with both its structure and a bi-factor approach.

After we calculated the CFAs and selected a final structure, we provide its internal consistency (Cronbach’s α, composite reliability or Omega, Average Variance Extracted, and item-total correlations), test–retest reliability (Pearson’s r and Intra-Class Correlations between time point 1 and the next), and convergent validity with depressive (PHQ-9), and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (ITQ). Finally, we assessed the instrument’s predictive validity by predicting depressive and posttraumatic symptomatology in a subsequent measurement wave, controlled by baseline levels with an OLS regression.

For both CFA and OLS regressions, missing values were handled by a listwise deletion method. For the regression analyses, sums were calculated for the rows with complete cases within a scale only.

Results

Almost all correlations between the 19 first items were significant and positive. On the other hand, no clear correlation pattern appeared between items 20 and 33 (behaviors related to fears) and items 1–19 (fears). There was also no clear pattern within the behaviors. This may be an indication of two distinct sets of instruments, with the first 19 composing a scale of fears and the next 20–33 composing a set of individual behaviors. Most of the fear items were also correlated with the “impact” items (33–34) and with depressive and posttraumatic stress symptomatology, which was not evident for all distinct behaviors. The correlation matrix together with mean and standard deviations of the described variables are in Supplementary Materials.

Initial Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the Original Theoretical Structure

As can be seen in the upper part of Table 2, none of the first latent structures derived from the provisional subscales fitted the data well. It seems that neither a general factor of fear, a composed factor structure including fears and behaviors, or a bifactor model accounted for the data’s latent structure. Because of this, the next step was to subject the instrument to content analysis.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. New four-factor solution loadings.

Content Analysis, Development, and Testing of a Newly Proposed Factor Structure

Based on the low fit indices for the originally proposed factor structure, we set out to explore alternative distributions of items based on content analysis. Because the FIVE is largely based on substantive theory and not yet empirically derived, this can be considered a necessary step in the development of a strong measure. Looking at the items of the FIVE, the first 19 items are phrased to measure fears related to contamination and illnesses (e.g., “I am afraid….”). On the other hand, items 20–33 describe some behaviors that can be a consequence of those fears. However, even in the presence of low fears, individuals may score very high on the behaviors listed in these items, as a result, for example, of general recommendations to engage in virus-mitigating behaviors (e.g., “I use purell/other sanitizer,” “I work or do my job on a computer,” “I wash my hands at times other than just using the bathroom or before eating.”). This is why, as per the developer’s instructions, these items should not be considered toward scoring a general fear factor and likely provide ancillary information about the extent to which exposure to an illness or virus alters people’s behaviors (Ehrenreich-May and Saez, personal communication). Finally, the content of items 34 and 35 cover a general distress dimension (e.g., “On average in the last week, being afraid of an illness or virus has caused me to experience strong emotions.”) and a disturbance of the quality of life dimension (e.g., “On average in the last week, being afraid of an illness or virus has gotten in the way of enjoying my life.”). These are likely related constructs but not measures of fear themselves and thus can be equally considered to provide ancillary information about the exposure of an individual to an illness or virus situation such as a pandemic.

Based on this general description, we focused our content analysis on the first 19 fear items, which is indeed the main latent construct purportedly measured by the FIVE. A member of the research team with experience in qualitative studies and psychometric theory read the items and looked for alternative thematic organizations, extracting general categories representing the grouping of items. This was then confirmed by an independent rater following principles of open coding outlined by the grounded theory framework (Strauss and Corbin, 1998). Table 3 provides a visual representation of the proposed theoretical structure based on the content analysis as compared to the original structure. Four new subscales were thus extracted from this content analysis. The first category was named fears of getting sick from an illness or virus. The second subscale was called fears that others may get sick from an illness or virus. The third subscale was called fears of concrete limitations due to an illness or virus. The fourth subscale was called fears of not being able to meet the basic needs of subsistence and work due to an illness or virus.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Provisional subscale and proposed subscale.

We tested this newly proposed structure using a CFA procedure. The bottom part of Table 4 shows the results for these analyses. When we computed the new four-factor solution, the model fit improved significantly. Even though the fit for a bi-factor solution increases in the newly proposed item structure, the four-factor solution yields a better fit. The FIVE seems to behave as a multidimensional scale, measuring different components of the construct “fear of illness and virus.” We should thus interpret these results primarily using scores from the four subscales and not using an aggregated sum of fear items. It is important to note that, even though we found an acceptable fit, it was still below the criteria proposed by Hu and Bentler (1999). Because of this, we tried a final factor solution including only the first two factors. This is an important step if we consider that these are items that actually reflect to what extent individuals are afraid of infection, either for themselves or others, which we believe is the main underlying construct of the scale. For the proposed solution, we found an excellent fit (Table 4; two Fear Factors). We present factor loadings for the four and two-factor solutions in Table 2.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Model fit indicators for examined factor structures (n = 159).

The original measure also included 14 items that measure different behaviors related to fear of illness and viruses. These items should not be used as indicators of fear of illness and viruses construct, as stated by the original authors (Ehrenreich-May and Saez, personal communication). We retained them as a supplement that may help assessors, both in clinical or in research settings, to quantify some behaviors related to illnesses and viruses. In the same way, the last two items provide an estimate on the overall level of impact of fear over two domains, emotionally and quality of life. These may be used as supplements, just like the behavior scale.

In short, we believe that only the first 19 items of the FIVE contribute to measuring a multidimensional construct, namely, fear of illness and virus. There is the long version with an acceptable fit consisting of four subscales, and the short one with an excellent fit focused on fears of self and others’ contagion. The remaining items constitute two supplemental sets of questions, namely, behaviors and impact ancillary information for the assessor, but not contributing to the measurement of the underlying construct.

Because of this, the next sections will provide information about the four subscales derived from our content analysis.

Reliability

We examined the reliability of the newly proposed structures for the FIVE using Cronbach’s alpha (α), Composite reliability or Omega (ω; Viladrich et al., 2017), and average variance extracted (AVE; Fornell and Larcker, 1981), as the ratio of variance captured by the construct v/s error variance. All measures were calculated based on the structural model and using the “semTools” package for R (Jorgensen et al., 2019). All subscales exhibited an adequate degree of reliability by means of α and ω, while most of them had a ratio of explained variance above or equal to 50%:

The subscale fears of getting sick from an illness or virus (FS) showed α = 0.875, ω = 0.879, and AVE = 0.645; the subscale fears that others may get sick from an illness or virus (FOS) showed α = 0.744, ω = 0.762, and AVE = 0.402; the subscale fears of concrete limitations due to an illness or virus (FL) showed α = 0.854, ω = 0.857, and AVE = 0.503; while the subscale fears of not being able to meet basic needs of subsistence and work due to an illness or virus (FBN) showed α = 0.789, ω = 0.798, and AVE = 0.499. Finally, the overall Average Variance Extracted was 0.503.

Almost all average variance extracted values were above the higher correlation with the other constructs squared, an indication of discriminant validity (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). One exception was FOS, which showed a lower AVE than its squared correlation with FS (r2 = 0.461). In this case, it is expected not to have a high degree of discriminant validity as both latent variables refer to fear of contagion, differentiated by its target (oneself or others). We finally calculated the item-total correlations for each factor using the “multilevel” package for R (Bliese, 2016). Item-total correlations for the FS factor ranged from 0.679 to 0.793; for the FOS factor, they ranged from 0.369 to 0.696. The FL factor showed item-total correlations that ranged from 0.536 to 0.721, while they ranged from 0.491 to 0.704 in the FBN factor. Specific values can be seen in parentheses on Table 3.

Test–Retest Reliability

Taking advantage of the longitudinal design of the study in which we evaluated the FIVE, we could examine the stability of the scores over time for each subscale. By design, the participants completed the FIVE every 7 days. We then calculated Pearson correlations between subscale scores of the first two applications, 7 days apart. We also calculated the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC), given that both measures are widely used to test for stability and reliability. As there are many available models for ICCs, we opted for a 2-way mixed model with only one reading per occasion and based on absolute agreement, also called ICC3,1 (Trevethan, 2017), as it is considered an appropriate measure for repeated measurements. ICCs were calculated with the “icc3.inter.fn” function from the “irrICC” package for R (Gwet, 2019). Table 5 shows such correlations with ICC3,1’s in parentheses. All Pearson’s correlations were all statistically significant and strong, which is an indicator of adequate stability, together with equivalent scores of reliability by means of ICC.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Test–retest Pearson correlations and ICC3,1 for absolute agreement between first and second assessment (7 days apart) for subscales and for total scale.

Convergent Validity

We examined the convergent validity of the FIVE with respect to depressive symptoms and PTSD. During the COVID-19 pandemic, it is reasonable to think that levels of fear of contamination and disease, fears of others becoming sick, fears of the limitations due to confinement, and fears of not meeting basic needs should correlate with indicators of traumatic reactivity and depression. This has been found in studies even during the COVID-19 pandemic such as the one carried out by Moreno et al. (2020) and by Tang et al. (2020).

As can be seen in Table 6, all subscales were significantly and positively correlated with both depressive symptomatology (PHQ-9) and posttraumatic stress (ITQ) at time point 1. This indicates that individuals who reported higher levels of fear also tended to report higher posttraumatic stress and depressive symptomatology.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Concurrent validity correlation table.

To further explore the predictive validity of the FIVE, we fitted a separate OLS regression for each subscale predicting depressive symptomatology (PHQ-9) 5 days after baseline, and posttraumatic stress symptomatology (ITQ) 7 days after baseline. All equations were controlled for initial PHQ-9 and ITQ values. Because there were eight equations, a Bonferroni correction was applied by dividing our critical alpha value of 0.05 by 8 (0.006) while interpreting the results to correct for type 1 error probability (Haynes, 2013). We present details of the main parameters in Table 7.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Regression results predicting future posttraumatic stress and depressive symptomatology.

None of the four subscales significantly predicted depressive symptoms later in time. However, when future posttraumatic stress symptoms were predicted and controlled by baseline values, fears of getting sick [b = 0.428, t(130) = 3.273, p = 0.001], fear that others get sick [b = 0.371, t(128) = 2.889, p = 0.005], fears related to limitations due to a virus or illness [b = 0.335, t(131) = 3.566, p = 0.001], and fears related to fears about not meeting the basic needs for subsistence or work [b = 0.392, t(131) = 3.234, p = 0.002] at time one exerted a significant effect, even when correcting for multiple comparisons. Altogether, these results indicate that people who are more scared of contagion and lockdown consequences tend to be more depressed and show more posttraumatic stress symptoms concurrently. These fears, however, only predict subsequent posttraumatic stress.

Discussion

The study described in this article was part of a longitudinal study conducted for understanding the effects that quarantine and isolation may have on mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic. When deciding which variables to include, we repeatedly found fear and fear-related constructs as one of the most reported emotions in studies about the effects of quarantine and isolation because of a virus (Gu et al., 2015; Brooks et al., 2020; Lima et al., 2020; Montemurro, 2020; Moreno et al., 2020; Tang et al., 2020; Thakur and Jain, 2020). Measuring fear seemed to be a key component for understanding the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental health as well as on behaviors that can contribute to either propagating or mitigating the infection (Ahorsu et al., 2020; Harper et al., 2020).

When facing the challenge of finding an instrument that could capture this construct profoundly enough to understand people’s emotional response to such a massive and pervasive phenomenon, we found that most of the existing instruments were either unidimensional or restricted to be used in the COVID-19 pandemic. In this context, we found the FIVE, which is both multidimensional and, even though developed during the COVID-19 pandemic, extendable to other pandemics or further life threatening situations.

The FIVE was originally proposed by Ehrenreich (2020), with a theoretical distribution of items measuring two facets of a fear factor, namely, fears about contamination and illness and fears about social distancing, not being exclusively applicable to the current COVID-19 pandemic. The authors included two additional scales, one for behaviors related to fear of illness and virus and one for the impact of fear of illness and virus. This structure was found to provide a bad fit to the data in a sample of 159 adult individuals during the COVID-19 pandemic in Chile. Content analysis revealed that items covering fear (e.g., “I am afraid…”) could be more aptly distributed in four groups covering facets of the fear of illness and virus construct.

As proposed by this psychometric study, our adaptation of the FIVE consists of a 19-item multidimensional scale that measures four components of the underlying fear of illness and virus construct. These components work as subscales of the instrument, namely, fears of getting sick, fear that others will get sick, fears about the limitations due to an illness or a virus, and fears about not meeting the basic needs for subsistence or work. We found an acceptable fit for the model including all four subscales, however, subthreshold based on criteria proposed by Hu and Bentler (1999). We found an excellent fit to the data on a smaller scale, including only the first two factors. All subscales showed equivalent subsequent psychometric properties, including good internal consistency, test–retest reliability, convergent validity, and predictive power. Based on this, we recommend the reader to use the four factors when a broader coverage of constructs is preferred, while using a short version composed of the first two subscales when construct validity is preferred, with the cost of restricting the measurement to fears of contagion to oneself or others.

Our adaptation also includes two supplemental scales that provide ancillary information about behaviors potentially associated with the fears, and to the extent to which fears may have led individuals to experience strong emotions and to decrease their quality of life. These supplemental scales do not represent the multidimensional fear of illness and virus construct but may be useful to understand other areas of affectation during a pandemic or exposure to illness. However, it is not clear if the behaviors listed in the first supplemental scale (i.e., behaviors related to fear of illness and virus) are in fact related to fears or can be a result of adaptive behaviors prescribed by health institutions or governments (e.g., “handwashing other than after using the bathroom or before eating”). The relationship between fears of illness and viruses, with behaviors, needs to be further examined. We thus recommend using the first 19 items (short format from 1 to 9, and long format from 1 to 19) to measure fears related to viruses and the consequences of lockdowns, items 20–33 if specific behaviors are of interest, and items 34 and 35 if affective consequences of the aforementioned fears are also a focus of interest.

We acknowledge that shorter measures have been developed to measure fear during the Covid-19 pandemic. However, the FIVE may have three distinct advantages.

First, multidimensionality is one of the greatest advantages of this scale. This feature can further contribute to understanding the role of different facets of the construct of fear as related to exposure to an illness or a virus. Fear of illness and viruses has been related to both prosocial, virus-mitigating behaviors as well as harmful and virus-propagation behaviors. Different facets, as measured by the FIVE, may be differentially related to these outcomes. For example, fear of becoming sick may be a driver of discrimination and prejudice (Ren et al., 2020) and can inhibit treatment-seeking behaviors when needed (Lazzerini et al., 2020). However, fear of COVID-19 can also be a driver for virus-mitigating behaviors (Harper et al., 2020). A study using the Fear of COVID-19 scale supported a two-factor structure that differentiated emotional fear reactions from symptomatic expressions of fear such as heart palpitations or sleep problems. The second factor was highly correlated with anxiety symptoms, which supports the applicability of exploring different facets of fear (Tzur Bitan et al., 2020). Thus, the multidimensional structure of the FIVE may allow grouping specific information about fear and thus more nuanced associations with other psychological and behavioral outcomes.

Secondly, we believe this may work as a broader measure of fear of illness and virus, so that it may potentially be used to study other contexts in which exposure to illness and virus may be related to mental and behavioral health as well as to societal issues. Despite the content of the FIVE as it actually is restricted to the COVID-19 scenario, we believe it may be adapted so that its use could be generalized for future scenarios when fear is related to similar sources. We believe that developing empirically informed assessment instruments for measuring emotions such as the FIVE may not only help us develop and adapt massive psychosocial strategies but also let us improve communication strategies for heightening adherence to recommendations by inviting people to behave in such a way that could collectively help mitigate the damage by preventing contagion and protect those who present the highest risk of dying or end with long-lasting or permanent sequelae. We found that the FIVE was also related to depressive and posttraumatic stress symptomatology when assessed concurrently, showing that relatively higher scores in fear appear together with the aforementioned symptoms. Also, higher values of the FIVE at baseline predicted posttraumatic stress symptomatology later in time, highlighting its practical value. This may have concrete implications if those fears are subject to change based on the diffusion of public policies and communication strategies, as they may also serve as a protective strategy for the stress-related consequences of a pandemic. Evoking fear has been used as an adherence strategy in response to public health emergencies, such as vaccination promotion or behaviors that mitigate contagion (see Taylor, 2019). However, fear (e.g., particularly extreme fear) may lead to decreased preventive behaviors and increased psychological distress particularly in high-risk populations such as individuals with mental health conditions, mainly through irrational thinking (Chang et al., 2020). Thus, instruments like the FIVE may be useful not only for the evaluation of the psychological impact of fears—as it has been shown in its association with complex symptomatology and behaviors—but also for designing and implementing public health policies either general or group-specific. One clear discussion of how measuring fear can help communities decrease mental health consequences can be seen on Lin et al. (2020). The authors found that when individuals received negative COVID-19 information from social media, fear was magnified with increased levels of psychological distress in both adults and children (e.g., the fear of COVID-19 was found to be a mediator in the relation between problematic social media use and distress/insomnia) (Chen I.-H. et al., 2020; Lin et al., 2020). A clear example on how to distinguish between at-risk populations from others that are not at risk can be seen in research from Lin et al. (2020) who identified that individuals with problematic use of social media were particularly exposed to these risks, while Chen I.-H. et al. (2020) identified that fear of COVID-19 did not seem to be serious in children.

Thirdly, the FIVE is a freely available measure that can be easily adapted globally. The FIVE scale and correction templates of both English and Spanish versions—and probably additional languages in the future—were developed and intended to be kept free, brief, and accessible.

These results should be critically evaluated given their limitations. First, our study used a convenience sampling strategy, with relatively small sample size, making the results not generalizable to the population, especially for underrepresented samples without access to a reliable internet connection. However, according to known population parameters in Chile, we attempted to correct the accompanying biases by using quota sampling procedures. Second, the time window of the study was short, meaning that only predictions of short-term consequences of fears were granted, and thus our results may not be extended to longer periods of time. Third, our relatively small sample size did not allow for the test of measurement invariance (Kline, 2016) or a cross-validation procedure. However, we think that the proposal of a new factor structure with a good fit to the data based on a content analysis of the items, together with the prospective predictive power of the FIVE with regard to post-traumatic stress symptomatology, is an encouraging result that requires further investigation by using larger sample sizes and longer time windows, together with a probabilistic design if a representation of the population is sought for.

Despite its limitations, our study provided the translation and evaluation of the psychometric properties of an instrument capable of measuring fears of getting sick and the consequences of social isolation and quarantine during a pandemic in a region badly affected by it. It is estimated that the COVID-19 pandemic is not an isolated event (e.g., Frutos et al., 2020) but a catastrophic event with a high likelihood of reappearing in the coming years after its resolution. We have also learned from previous pandemics that their consequences are usually prevalent and long-lasting in nature (Maunder et al., 2003; Taylor, 2019; Ornell et al., 2020), highlighting the need to measure them. Future studies are needed to confirm these findings, expand them to other mental health outcomes, and explain through which mechanisms they operate. These efforts may grant us more preparation for the mental health problems that will appear as a consequence of the pandemic and the needed public health policies, together with more preparation for future similar situations.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Committee of Ethics in Science of the Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MC contributed to the conception of the study, and to the conception, drafting, and editing process of the manuscript, and approved the final version. CH and AB contributed to the conception and design of the study, the collection and interpretation of data, and approved the final version. CN and NL contributed to the acquisition of the data, drafting of the manuscript, and approved the final version. YQ and AD contributed to the drafting process of the manuscript and approved the final version. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the ANID Millennium Science Initiative/Millennium Institute for Research on Depression and Personality-MIDAP ICS13_005.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer GM declared a shared affiliation with several of the authors, MC, YQ, to the handling editor at the time of review.

Acknowledgments

The study acknowledges the support of the company Ethica Data for providing a free license to conduct COVID-19 research using their platform and application during the pandemic. We acknowledge the valuable work of Helena Cirne, Edmund Kim, Victoria Comunale, and Paul Sargent. We are particularly thankful to Jill Ehrenreich-May, developer of the FIVE, and Estefany Saez for their generosity and support.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590283/full#supplementary-material

References

Abeldaño, R. A., Fernández, A. F., Estario, J. C., Enders, J. E., and López de Neira, M. J. (2014). Screening de trastornos de estrés postraumático en población afectada por el terremoto chileno de 2010. Cad. Saúde Pública 30, 2377–2386. doi: 10.1590/0102-311x00141313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahorsu, D. K., Lin, C. Y., Imani, V., Saffari, M., Griffiths, M. D., and Pakpour, A. H. (2020). The fear of COVID-19 scale: development and initial validation. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 27, 1–9. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00270-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (1990). DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder, 4th Edn. Philadelphia, PA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Apablaza, M., and Vega, F. (2018). Contexto Demográfico Global y Chileno, Situación de La Población Adulto Mayor. Santiago: Generación Silenciosa.

Google Scholar

Bertranou, F. M. (2007). Economía Informal, Trabajadores Independientes y Cobertura de La Seguridad Social En Argentina, Chile y Uruguay. Santiago: Oficina Internacional del Trabajo.

Google Scholar

Biscayart, C., Angeleri, P., Lloveras, S., Chaves, T. D. S. S., Schlagenhauf, P., and Rodríguez-Morales, A. J. (2020). The next big threat to global health? 2019 Novel coronavirus (2019-NCoV): what advice can we give to travellers? – Interim recommendations January 2020, from the Latin-american society for travel medicine (SLAMVI). Travel Med. Infect Dis. 33:101567. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2020.101567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bliese, P. (2016). multilevel: Multilevel Functions. R package Version 2.6.

Google Scholar

Bo, H. X., Yang, Y., Chen, J., Zhang, M., Zhang, D. Y., Li, Y., et al. (2020). Posttraumatic stress symptoms and attitude toward crisis mental health services among clinically stable patients with COVID-19 in China. Psychol. Med. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000999 Online ahead of print

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bonilla-Aldana, D. K., Holguin-Rivera, Y., Cortes-Bonilla, I., Cardona-Trujillo, M. C., García-Barco, A., Bedoya-Arias, H. A., et al. (2020). Coronavirus infections reported by ProMED, February 2000–January 2020. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 35:101575. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2020.101575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, S., Greenwood, M., Prior, S., Shearer, T., Walkem, K., Young, S., et al. (2020). Purposive sampling: complex or simple? research case examples. J. Res. Nurs. 25, 652–661.

Google Scholar

Chang, K. C., Strong, C., Pakpour, A. H., Griffiths, M. D., and Lin, C. Y. (2020). Factors related to preventive COVID-19 infection behaviors among people with mental illness. J. Formos. Med. Assoc. 119, 1772–1780. doi: 10.1016/j.jfma.2020.07.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, I.-H., Chen, C.-Y., Pakpour, A. H., Griffiths, M. D., and Lin, C.-Y. (2020). Internet-related behaviors and psychological distress among schoolchildren during COVID-19 school suspension. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 159, 1099–1102. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Liang, M., Li, Y., Guo, J., Fei, D., Wang, L., et al. (2020). Mental health care for medical staff in China during the COVID-19 outbreak. Lancet Psychiatry 7, e15–e16. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30078-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloitre, M., Shevlin, M., Brewin, C. R., Bisson, J. I., Roberts, N. P., Maercker, A., et al. (2018). The international trauma questionnaire: development of a self-report measure of ICD-11 PTSD and complex PTSD. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 138, 536–546. doi: 10.1111/acps.12956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Hoog, N., Stroebe, W., and de Wit, J. B. (2008). The processing of fear-arousing communications: how biased processing leads to persuasion. Soc. Influ. 3, 84–113. doi: 10.1080/15534510802185836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diaz Silva, L. (2011). Impactos Post Terremoto En Las Dimensiones Psicosocial y de Participación, Según Sexo. Santiago: Ministerio de Desarrollo Social.

Google Scholar

Dong, E., Du, H., and Gardner, L. (2020). An interactive web-based dashboard to track COVID-19 in real time. Lancet Infect. Dis. 20, 533–534. doi: 10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30120-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dsouza, D. D., Quadros, S., Hyderabadwala, Z. J., and Mamun, M. A. (2020). Aggregated COVID-19 suicide incidences in india: fear of COVID-19 infection is the prominent causative factor. Psychiatry Res. 290:113145. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ethica Data Services Inc (2020). Ethica Data. Ontario: Ethica Data Services Inc.

Google Scholar

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18:39. doi: 10.2307/3151312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frutos, R., Lopez Roig, M., Serra-Cobo, J., and Devaux, C. A. (2020). COVID-19: the conjunction of events leading to the coronavirus pandemic and lessons to learn for future threats. Front. Med. 7:223–238. doi: 10.3389/fmed.2020.00223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, J., Zhong, Y., Hao, Y., Zhou, D., Tsui, H., Hao, C., et al. (2015). Preventive behaviors and mental distress in response to H1N1 among university students in guangzhou. China. Asia Pac. J. Public Health 27, N1867–N1879. doi: 10.1177/1010539512443699

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gwet, K. (2019). irrICC: Intraclass Correlations for Quantifying Inter-Rater Reliability. R Package Version 1.0.

Google Scholar

Harper, C. A., Satchell, L. P., Fido, D., and Latzman, R. D. (2020). Functional fear predicts public health compliance in the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. doi: 10.1007/s11469-020-00281-5 Online ahead of print

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Haynes, W. (2013). “Bonferroni correction,” in Encyclopedia of Systems Biology, eds W. Dubitzky, O. Wolkenhauer, K. H. Cho, and H. Yokota (New York, NY: Springer), doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-9863-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, G. W. K., Karatzias, T., Cloitre, M., Chan, A. C. Y., Bressington, D., Chien, W. T., et al. (2019). Translation and validation of the Chinese ICD-11 International Trauma Questionnaire (ITQ) for the Assessment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and complex PTSD (CPTSD). Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 10:1608718. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2019.1608718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Modeling 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y., and Zhao, N. (2020). Generalized anxiety disorder, depressive symptoms and sleep quality during COVID-19 Outbreak in China: a web-based cross-sectional survey. Psychiatry Res. 288:112954. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas [INE] (2018). Síntesis de Resultados CENSO 2017. Santiago: Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas.

Google Scholar

Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas [INE] (2020). Boletín Estadístico: Empleo Trimestral. Santiago: Instituto Nacional de Estadísticas.

Google Scholar

Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., and Rosseel, Y. (2019). semTools: Useful Tools for Structural Equation Modeling. R Package Version 0.5-2.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. (2015). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling. New York, NY: Guilford publications.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. (2016). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 4th Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., and Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 16, 606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazzerini, M., Barbi, E., Apicella, A., Marchetti, F., Cardinale, F., and Trobia, G. (2020). Delayed access or provision of care in italy resulting from fear of COVID-19. Lancet Child Adolesc. Health 4, e10–e11. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(20)30108-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C.-H. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data: comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted least squares. Behav. Res. Methods 48, 936–949. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0619-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lima, C. K. T., Carvalho, P. M. M., Lima, I. A. A. S., Nunes, J. V. A. O., Saraiva, J. S., de Souza, R. I., et al. (2020). The emotional impact of coronavirus 2019-NCoV (New coronavirus disease). Psychiatry Res. 287:112915. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. Y. (2020). Social reaction toward the 2019 novel coronavirus (COVID-19). Soc. Health Behav. 3:1. doi: 10.4103/shb.shb_11_20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, C. Y., Broström, A., Griffiths, M. D., and Pakpour, A. H. (2020). Investigating mediated effects of fear of COVID-19 and COVID-19 misunderstanding in the association between problematic social media use, psychological distress, and insomnia. Int. Interv. 21:100345. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2020.100345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mamun, M. A., and Griffiths, M. D. (2020). First COVID-19 suicide case in bangladesh due to fear of COVID-19 and xenophobia: possible suicide prevention strategies. Asian J. Psychiatr. 51:102073. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Margozzini, P., and Passi, A. (2018). Encuesta nacional de salud, ENS 2016-2017: un aporte a la planificación sanitaria y políticas públicas en chile. ARS MEDICA Rev. Cienc. Méd. 43, 30–34. doi: 10.11565/arsmed.v43i1.1354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martínez Pérez, M., Retana Franco, B. E., and Sánchez Aragón, R. (2009). Identificación de las estrategias de regulación emocional del miedo en adultos. Psicol. Iberoam. 17, 49–59.

Google Scholar

Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Vincent, L., Bennett, J., Peladeau, N., Leszcz, M., et al. (2003). The immediate psychological and occupational impact of the 2003 SARS outbreak in a teaching hospital. CMAJ 168, 1245–1251.

Google Scholar

Montemurro, N. (2020). The emotional impact of COVID-19: from medical staff to common people. Brain Behav. Immun. 87, 23–24. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, C., Wykes, T., Galderisi, S., Nordentoft, M., Crossley, N., Jones, N., et al. (2020). How mental health care should change as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 813–824. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30307-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morens, D. M., Folkers, G. K., and Fauci, A. S. (2009). What is a pandemic?”. J. Infect. Dis. 200, 1018–1021. doi: 10.1086/644537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2017). Mplus User’s Guide, 8th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Google Scholar

Ornell, F., Schuch, J. B., Sordi, A. O., and Kessler, F. H. P. (2020). ‘Pandemic fear’ and COVID-19: mental health burden and strategies. Braz. J. Psychiatry 42, 232–235. doi: 10.1590/1516-4446-2020-0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pakpour, A. H., and Griffiths, M. D. (2020). The fear of COVID-19 and its role in preventive behaviors. J. Concurrent Disord. 2, 58–63.

Google Scholar

Pierce, M., McManus, S., Jessop, C., John, A., Hotopf, M., Ford, T., et al. (2020). Says who? The significance of sampling in mental health surveys during COVID-19. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 567–568. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30237-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience: Theories of Emotion. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Public Health England (2020). Coronavirus (COVID-19) - What You Need to Know. England: Public Health England.

Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Reise, S. P., Moore, T. M., and Haviland, M. G. (2010). Bifactor models and rotations: exploring the extent to which multidimensional data yield univocal scale scores. J. Pers. Assess. 92, 544–559. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2010.496477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ren, S. Y., Gao, R. D., and Chen, Y. L. (2020). Fear can be more harmful than the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 in controlling the corona virus disease 2019 epidemic. World J. Clin. Cases 8, 652–657. doi: 10.12998/wjcc.v8.i4.652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Morales, A. J., MacGregor, K., Kanagarajah, S., Patel, D., and Schlagenhauf, P. (2020). Going global – travel and the 2019 novel coronavirus. Travel Med. Infect. Dis. 33:101578. doi: 10.1016/j.tmaid.2020.101578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an r package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sosa, C., and Capafóns, J. (2003). “Guía de tratamientos psicológicos eficaces para las fobias específicas,” in Guía de tratamientos Psicológicos Eficaces I: Adultos, Coords. M. En, et al. (Madrid: Pirámide), 235–245.

Google Scholar

Steenblock, C., Todorov, V., Kanczkowski, W., Eisenhofer, G., Schedl, A., Wong, M. L., et al. (2020). Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) and the neuroendocrine stress axis. Mol. Psychiatry 25, 1611–1617. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0758-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, A., and Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of Qualitative Research – Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory. Management Learning, 2nd Edn, Vol. 31. London: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Tang, W., Hu, T., Hu, B., Jin, C., Wang, G., Xie, C., et al. (2020). Prevalence and correlates of PTSD and depressive symptoms one month after the outbreak of the COVID-19 epidemic in a sample of home-quarantined chinese university students. J. Affect. Disord. 274, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, S. (2019). The Psychology of Pandemics, Preparing for the Next Global Outbreak of Infectious Disease. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Google Scholar

Thakur, V., and Jain, A. (2020). COVID 2019-suicides: a global psychological pandemic. Brain Behav. Immun. 88, 952–953. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Lancet (2019). The unfolding migrant crisis in Latin America. Lancet 394:1966. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)32934-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomas Baader, M., José Luis Molina, F., Silvia Venezian, B., Carmen Rojas, C., Renata Farías, S., Fierro-Freixenet, C., et al. (2012). Validación y utilidad de la encuesta PHQ-9 (Patient health questionnaire) en el diagnóstico de depresión en pacientes usuarios de atención primaria en chile. Rev. Chil. Neuropsiquiatr. 50, 10–22. doi: 10.4067/S0717-92272012000100002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevethan, R. (2017). Intraclass correlation coefficients: clearing the air, extending some cautions, and making some requests. Health Serv. Outcomes Res. Methodol. 17, 127–143. doi: 10.1007/s10742-016-0156-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzur Bitan, D., Grossman-Giron, A., Bloch, Y., Mayer, Y., Shiffman, N., and Mendlovic, S. (2020). Fear of COVID-19 scale: psychometric characteristics, reliability and validity in the israeli population. Psychiatry Res. 289:113100. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viladrich, C., Angulo-Brunet, A., and Doval, E. (2017). Un viaje alrededor de alfa y omega para estimar la fiabilidad de consistencia interna. An. Psicol. 33:755. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.268401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2020). Coronavirus Disease 2020 (COVID-19) Situation Report–129. Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

Zubizarreta, J. R., Cerdá, M., and Rosenbaum, P. R. (2013). Effect of the 2010 chilean earthquake on posttraumatic stress: reducing sensitivity to unmeasured bias through study design. Epidemiology 24, 79–87. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e318277367e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, FIVE, fear, Spanish adaptation and validation, pandemic, psychological impact, virus, SARS-CoV-2

Citation: Cottin M, Hernández C, Núñez C, Labbé N, Quevedo Y, Davanzo A and Behn A (2021) “What If We Get Sick?”: Spanish Adaptation and Validation of the Fear of Illness and Virus Evaluation Scale in a Non-clinical Sample Exposed to the COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychol. 12:590283. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.590283

Received: 31 July 2020; Accepted: 25 January 2021;
Published: 11 March 2021.

Edited by:

Joanna Sokolowska, University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Poland

Reviewed by:

Amir H. Pakpour, Qazvin University of Medical Sciences, Iran
Gonzalo Migue, University of Chile, Chile

Copyright © 2021 Cottin, Hernández, Núñez, Labbé, Quevedo, Davanzo and Behn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alex Behn, albehn@uc.cl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.