- 1Warwick Business School, University of Warwick, Coventry, United Kingdom
- 2School of Engineering, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
- 3Department of Marketing, Faculty of Economics and Business, University of Groningen, Grongingen, Netherlands
- 4Department of Organization, Strategy, and Entrepreneurship, School of Business and Economics, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands
- 5Department of Marketing, Centre for Business, Organisations and Society, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom
Prolific research suggests identity associates with pro-environmental behaviours (PEBs) that are individual and/or group focused. Individual PEB is personally driven, self-reliant, and are conducted on one's own (e.g., home recycling). Group focused PEB is other people-reliant and completed as part of a group (e.g., attending meetings of an environmental organisation). A wide range of identities have been related to PEBs. For example, a recent systematic qualitative review revealed 99 different types of identities studied in a PEB context. Most studies were correlational, few had an experimental design. However, the relationships between all these identities and PEBs have so far not been tested quantitatively with meta-analytical techniques. As such, a clear overview of this field is currently lacking. Due to the diverse nature of the field, a priori hypotheses were not possible and relatively broad definitions of identity had to be used to encompass all types of identities and the diverse meanings of identity that have been included in PEB research. What prior theory did allow for was to assess the distinction between two main types of identity, namely how people label, describe, and recognise oneself individually (individual identity), or as part of a group (group identity). Our overall goal was thus to assess the current state of knowledge on identities and PEBs. In 104 studies using a meta-regression following the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses guidelines, our random-effects meta-analysis showed that the overall concept of identity associated with PEB with a medium Pearson's r (Aim 1). Furthermore, we found that individual identities associated more strongly with PEBs than group identities (Aim 2). The associations between individual and group identities were stronger when the identity and PEB were from the same category (e.g., when both were group-focused; Aim 3). Methodologically, the findings revealed that group identities and group PEBs were most strongly associated for self-reported rather than observed PEBs (Aim 4). Overall identity associated most strongly with group PEBs in the field rather than in the lab (Aim 5) and in student- rather than non-student samples (Aim 6). We discuss the theoretical and practical implications.
Introduction
Humans generate many environmental problems via behaviours which are not sustainable in the longer term (Allen et al., 2018). Consequently, there is an urgent need for researchers to understand how to encourage people to behave in a pro-environmental way. Pro-environmental behaviour (PEB) is defined as actions that minimise the negative impact on (such as preserving and preventing damage to), and/or promoting improvements to, the natural and the built world (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). People can carry out individual PEB which is largely personally driven. It is primarily an action that is self-reliant and driven by the self. In particular, it is a behaviour that is conducted on one's own, for example home recycling. Alternatively, people can carry out group PEB which is behaviour that is carried out as part of a group, for example, attending meetings of an environmental organisation.
There has been a recent surge of research on understanding how to encourage PEBs (Chernev and Blair, 2015; Gershoff and Frels, 2015; Brick et al., 2017; Walton and Jones, 2017; Brick and Lai, 2018). Specifically, a meta-analysis showed that research in this field was largely based on four dominant psychological theories (Klöckner, 2013, see also Van Den Broek and Walker, 2019), namely the theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1991), the norm-activation theory (Schwartz, 1992), the value-belief-norm theory (Stern, 2000), and habits (Verplanken and Aarts, 1999; Verplanken and Ryan, 2018). However, combining these four theories accounted for only 36% of variance explained in a variety of PEBs (Klöckner, 2013). We argue that the explanatory power of these models may be improved by including identity.
Generally, identity refers to how people see themselves (Pronin, 2008). While research has emerged to incorporate identity into the most popular theories used to explain PEBs, such as the theory of planned behaviour (Pierro et al., 2003; Fielding et al., 2008a; Fielding and Hornsey, 2016; see meta-analysis by Rise et al., 2010), identities seem to be important independent from these theories as well (Murtagh et al., 2012). Indeed, identity in PEB research has blossomed over the last three decades (Dagher and Itani, 2014; Graham-Rowe et al., 2015; Walton and Jones, 2017; Hamerman et al., 2018; Brieger, 2019). A recent systematic literature review showed that 99 different identities have been studied in a PEB context (Udall et al., 2020), such as pro-environmental identity, moral identity, and social identity. Some of these identities have been studied in experimental designs, while others have taken correlational designs. However, there is no meta-analysis to show the extent to which this multitude of identities studied in relation to PEB are actually associated with PEB. Therefore, the main aim of the present study is to consolidate the PEB research by empirically testing the relationships of these identities to PEB. More specifically, we will aim to provide a structure within the diffused research on identities and PEBs by ordering them in two theoretically meaningful ways: Making a distinction between individual and group identities, and, individual and group PEBs.
Identities can be categorised as either an individual or group identity which originates from the self-categorisation theory (Turner et al., 1987), a key psychological identity theory. An individual identity is defined as how people label, describe, and recognise oneself individually or personally (e.g., environmental identity). A group-identity is defined as how people label, describe, and recognise themselves as part of a group (e.g., environmental group identity). The literature on identity and PEB has not only identified 99 different types of identity but has also examined these identities in a variety of PEBs which can also be meaningfully distinguished between individual or group types (Udall et al., 2020). The distinction between individual and group PEBs as well as identities allow us to not only explore the importance of identity in relation to PEB in general, but to understand the extent to which these different identities per category associate with these PEBs per category. Hence, unifying the diverse conceptualisations of identity and PEB in the field can contribute to further consolidate the diverse field. Furthermore, as the field seems to have taken quite different methodological approaches to investigate the identity-PEB relationship, we also check how three key methodological choices may affect this association. More specifically, the present study explores how the measurement of PEBs (self-reported or observed), using different research designs (lab or field), and using student or non-student samples (Kormos and Gifford, 2014) moderate the identity and PEB associations.
Based on the prolific study of identity in PEB research, and the diffuse nature of these studies, the present study proposes six broad aims (rather than a priori hypotheses) to assess the literature quantitatively to consolidate this topic (Thomas et al., 2019). This approach enables us to provide a comprehensive quantitative summary of the evidence, which is currently lacking, yet which is important for the advancement of a field (which has been the case in, for example, predicting crime; Walters, 2020). Also, we can explore consistency, and therefore generalisability, of findings across different types of research on this topic which is yet to be completed (Thomas et al., 2019; Walters, 2020). Our meta-analysis will therefore be an exploration following the preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Moher et al., 2009). From this meta-analysis, we deduce future directions and practical implications about how to assess identities alongside other psychological variables in PEB research. We will introduce the relevance of the six aims in relation to state-of-the-art of the literature in the field of identity and PEB.
Aim 1: Associations Between Overall Identity and PEB
Identity theory (Stryker and Burke, 2000) and social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, 1979) are dominant psychological theories that explicitly deal with how identity relates to behaviour generally and to PEB specifically (Udall et al., 2020). Identity theory suggests that when an individual identity is in the foreground or “salient,” this identity will more likely associate with behaviour when the meaning of the behaviour corresponds to the meaning of the identity (Stryker and Burke, 2000)—and that either individual or group behaviours can be associated with it. Social identity theory assumes that if a person identifies with a specific group (salient group identity), they internalise the norms of the group and are therefore more likely to act in accordance with those norms (Tajfel and Turner, 1979). On the contrary, Udall et al.'s (2020) systematic literature review in identities and PEBs suggests that when an identity (individual or group) is in the foreground, the identity will associate with any PEB (either individual and/or group) as long as the meaning of the behaviour corresponds to the meaning of the identity. For example, people with a strong pro-environmental self-identity or pro-environmental group identity will carry out private nature conservation (individual PEB) and environmental activism (group PEB) because the meaning of the behaviours are PEB focused and correspond to the meaning of the identity's (also PEB focused). Therefore, the latter reasoning assumes that regardless of the type of identity and PEB (individual/group), they will associate with each other. However, it has never been explored.
Furthermore, the identity theories above imply that identity can associate with PEB in two ways. Firstly, identity associates with PEB because they incite schemas. Schemas are internally stored information about situations and expected behaviours, which are linked to that identity (Markus, 1977; Stryker and Burke, 2000). Secondly, identities incite norms (internally stored information about what is usual, typical, or standard in the given situation) toward the PEB in question (Cialdini et al., 1990). Understanding the extent to which schemas or norms explain the relationship between identity and PEB, we first need to gain consensus as to whether identity relates to PEB. There are currently many identity types measured in PEB research at the moment and related to many different types of PEBs, making it unclear which overall conclusions can be drawn. A meta-analysis enables us to reveal the inconsistencies within the literature about whether the prolific amount of identities overall associate (if at all) with PEB. Our meta-analysis will therefore not establish the causal mechanism, but it simply explores whether we can uncover a meaningful relationship to begin with. Therefore, Aim 1 focuses on how the overall concept of identity relates to PEBs in general.
Aim 2: Associations Between Individual and Group Identities and Overall PEB
Identity can be either focused on the individual or group. However, the studies in PEB research usually have not made a distinction between individual and group identities explicitly (Udall et al., 2020). In line with this, they have largely ignored the theoretical assumptions that this distinction implies (Murtagh et al., 2012). For example, many different identity terms have been used to refer to somewhat the same individual identity (Whitmarsh and O'Neill, 2010; Bhattacharjee et al., 2014), such as an “ecological self-identity” (Castro et al., 2009), and “environmental identity” (Brügger et al., 2011; Tam, 2013). Furthermore, the same diverse terms have been used for group identities, for example, social identity (Costa-Pinto et al., 2014, 2016), social identity importance: “member of the local community” (Murtagh et al., 2012), and “environmental movement identity” (Dunlap and McCright, 2008). Such confusions in conceptualisation and operationalisation makes the literature in this field disparate. Structuring the multitude of identities as individual or group identities will help to understand the findings of each study in relation to identity theory. To provide structure, it is therefore useful to categorise the multitude of studied identity types.
Furthermore, research shows that the strength of the associations between individual and group identities on PEBs vary. For example, individual identities associate with PEBs, like an environmental identity (Hinds and Sparks, 2008), car-authority identity (Schuitema et al., 2013), and ecological self-identity (Castro et al., 2009). However, some group identities do not associate with PEBs like a social identity (Costa-Pinto et al., 2014, 2016), consumer's identification with a socially responsible insurance company (Pérez, 2009), environmental movement identity (Dunlap and McCright, 2008), and rural group identification (Fielding et al., 2008b). Comparing the extent to which individual and groups identities associate with PEB has never been explored. Our meta-analysis will therefore distinguish the multitude of identities studied in the field of PEB into individual and group identities. This distinction will help to understand the findings of each study in relation to the assumptions that have been made in popular identity theories. Therefore, Aim 2 focuses on how individual and group identities relates to PEBs overall.
Aim 3: Associations Between Overall Identity and Individual and Group PEBs
Like identities, PEBs can also be categorised as either individual or group PEB, although studies usually have not made such distinction (Udall et al., 2020). The lack of categorising PEBs in identity research has resulted in confusion about the conceptualisations and measurement of PEBs. For example, many different individual PEBs have been used to refer to somewhat the same PEB (Whitmarsh and O'Neill, 2010; Matsuba et al., 2012), such as an “ecological behaviour” (Whitmarsh and O'Neill, 2010; Brügger et al., 2011) and “environmental behaviour” (Matsuba et al., 2012). While, the same diverse terms have been used for group PEBs, for example, “public environmental behaviour” (Matsuba et al., 2012) and “member of an environmental organisation” (Dunlap and McCright, 2008). Structuring the multitude of PEBs as individual or group PEBs will help to understand the findings of each study in relation to theories of identity. For example, identity theory (Stryker and Burke, 2000) suggests that when the shared meanings of the identity and PEB are most similar, the associations are likely to be strongest. People with a strong “self-identity toward private nature conservation” (an individual identity) will more likely carry out individual PEB, such as engaging in private nature conservation (Lokhorst et al., 2014). Conversely, people with a strong group identity such as “a member of an environmental group” will more likely carry out a group PEB such as engaging in environmental activism (Fielding et al., 2008a). Therefore, it is useful to categorise the multitude of studied PEBs in a similar way as identities. Our meta-analysis will distinguish between individual and group PEBs which enables us to explore whether there is a systematic pattern of results between the aforementioned established individual and group identities. This distinction will help to further explore the extent to which a similar two-way distinction in PEBs as used in identities might be helpful to provide order in the identity-PEB literature (rather than testing specific a priori hypotheses in relation to the alignment assumption as coined by identity theory). Therefore, Aim 3 focuses on how individual and group identities relate to individual and group PEBs.
Aim 4-6: Measures, Research Setting, and Samples as Moderators
Research in identity and PEB has taken quite different methodological approaches to investigate the identity-PEB relationship. In particular, the way in which PEB has been measured (Aim 4), the type of research design (Aim 5), and the sample (Aim 6) are three key moderators that may affect the identity-PEB associations.
Identity-PEB research typically relies on two types of PEB measures, namely self-reported intention/behaviour (Williams et al., 2004, 2006; Fitzsimons et al., 2007), and objective/observed behaviour (Chandon et al., 2004; Williams et al., 2004; Conner et al., 2011). Past research suggests that these types of measures may moderate empirical relations (Nigbur et al., 2010; Bamberg et al., 2015; Reese and Kohlmann, 2015). For example, identity-PEB associations were significant for self-reported PEB and not significant for observed PEBs (Reese and Kohlmann, 2015), or, they were at least stronger for self-reported PEBs (Spence et al., 2009; Nigbur et al., 2010). However, other studies have shown that the empirical associations are sometimes weaker for self-reported rather than observed measures (Sprott et al., 1999).
Identity-PEB associations may be moderated by measurement type because self-reported measures seem to be more susceptible to flaws in our ability to reliably reflect on our behaviour. For example, participants may exaggerate (Kormos and Gifford, 2014) or over-estimate their PEB (Geller, 1981; Warriner et al., 1984; Barr, 2007) due to a self-serving bias (Tarrant and Cordell, 1997). These biases are likely to occur as self-reports rely on reflecting on our memory of past behaviour, which can be easily re-written (Loftus and Palmer, 1974). To establish the role of PEB measures, Aim 4 focuses on how PEB measurements (self-reported and observed) moderate identity-PEB associations. Exploring the impact of measures on the identity-PEB relationship will help further understand the mixed empirical identity-PEB relationships, rather than understand which measure increases/decreases the identity-PEB associations.
Identity-PEB research is mostly conducted in either the laboratory (Crimston et al., 2016) or the field (Trump et al., 2015). Empirical associations may be strongest in a laboratory setting due to demand characteristics (Wood et al., 2015). People may feel pressure to act pro-environmentally in line with their identity because of the presence of the experimenter or the authority of the setting. However, the extent to which the research setting moderates the strength of this relationship is still unknown. To establish the role of research setting, Aim 5 focuses on how research settings (laboratory and field) moderate identity-PEB associations. Exploring the impact of settings on the identity-PEB relationships will help further understand the associations, rather than understand which setting increases/decreases the identity-PEB associations.
Finally, in PEB research usually two types of research samples are used, namely, student or non-student samples. Reliance on student samples has been criticised in psychological research as they do not reflect a generalised sample of the population at large and provide more positive results as students are more impressionable (Henrich et al., 2010). However, the extent to which the sample moderates the strength of this relationship is still missing. Therefore, Aim 6 focuses on how samples (students and non-students) moderate identity-PEB associations. Exploring the impact of samples on the identity-PEB relationships will help to further understand the identity-PEB findings, rather than understanding which setting increases/decreases the identity-PEB associations.
Method
We used the PRISMA method (Moher et al., 2009), a widely used method, especially in the medical sciences (Drubbel et al., 2014; Holden et al., 2014). PRISMA offers a concise and replicable standard for conducting and reporting meta-analyses by advocating several reproducible steps (Higgins and Green, 2011), which we outline below.
Protocol
A peer-reviewed protocol is necessary prior to the meta-analysis. This protocol was pre-registered on the Open Science Framework1. There were three changes between the pre-registered and the eventual methods and analysis. Firstly, in the pre-registration, we referred to “sustainable consumer behaviour” (SCB), rather than “PEB.” Both terms were used as search terms. Therefore, this only alters the narrative of the report, rather than the method and analysis. Secondly, after undertaking the initial review phase, we found further moderators that became pertinent to the study of identity in PEB, which were not included in the pre-registration. These include behavioural measure (observed and self-reported), study design (lab or field), and sample type (student or non-student). Additionally, we removed behaviour visibility as a moderator, because after collating the data, we found that there was insufficient information available to determine the visibility of many of the behaviours reported in the literature.
Eligibility Criteria
Based on the main aims, in terms of populations, interventions, comparators, outcomes, and study designs (PICOS), we were interested in including all research that measured identity in relation to PEB, regardless of identity type (e.g., all individually focused identities: environmental identity, personal identity; all group focused identities: environmental group identity, social identity), outcome PEB type (i.e., self-reported intention, behaviour, observed PEB), study design type (i.e., correlational, quasi-experimental, experimental/interventional, within and/or between participants), comparator type (e.g., additional psychological variables assessed like habits—Verplanken and Ryan, 2018), or population type (e.g., student, non-student). These inclusion criteria were in line with our six aims. However, we used four additional criteria to answer these aims.
Criterion 1
The studies needed to examine identity and PEB empirically together. Studies needed to explicitly state that they examined identity and PEB or similar constructs that we interpreted as matching our definitions of identity and PEB, respectively. To clarify, eligible papers could capture any identity provided it was measured in relation to PEB. Furthermore, papers were eligible regardless of their definition of identity. When definitions were missing, and the term “identity” was not explicitly used, we checked if papers matched our definitions of identity before including them. For example, papers were included if they could be interpreted as matching our definition of the individual identity and/or group identity (Udall et al., 2020). In a similar way, in relation to PEB, papers were eligible if they explicitly stated that the behaviour was considered a PEB in line with our PEB definition (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002).
Criterion 2
Studies needed to use a design that allowed for the associations between identity and behaviour to be measured, compared, and obtained. Hence, quantitative studies were included and used in the analysis, such as correlational, experimental, or quasi-experimental data, but not qualitative studies.
Criterion 3
Studies needed to report statistics needed for the meta-analysis, such as means, standard deviations, odds ratios, correlation coefficients (r), Cohens d, partial eta squared, and other types of effect sizes enabling us to identify the strength of the identity-PEB associations. In line with previous research (Abrahamse and Steg, 2013), and due to the prevalence of correlational studies, we needed the effect size information that enabled us to convert any effect size to Pearson's r for consistency and comparability purposes (Pearson, 1895).
Criterion 4
We included primary studies that were published in peer-reviewed academic journals only. If data was missing from the published article, we contacted authors requesting this data, as well as any additional unpublished data they had (Rosenthal and DiMatteo, 2001; Field, 2005; Field and Wright, 2006; Abrahamse and Steg, 2013). We also contacted the authors to request additional information in relation to unreported statistics we needed. We attempted to gather the unpublished data pertaining to the published articles where possible. If these were not provided, we calculated the correlation coefficients, if sufficient data was available.
Information Sources
We used the following electronic databases: (1) PsycArticles using PsycNet, (2) Web of Science, (3) EBSCOhost Business Source Complete, and (4) Scopus. Additional sources were Google Scholar alert and hand searching (Higgins and Green, 2011). Sources were searched from inception to 4th March 2016.
Search Strategy
Using a modified checklist from the Cochrane Collaboration of Systematic Reviews (Higgins and Green, 2011), search terms and keywords were identified with search categories and filters (see Table 1). We chose search terms to maximise the identification of suitable articles and thus started with many search results. From that solid foundation we could then select the final, more focused, set of search results.
Study Selection Process
In five steps, we identified the total number of included studies: 86 articles, comprising 104 studies, ranging from 1992 to 2016. These steps are presented graphically in Figure 1.
1. The total number of records identified via electronic database searching was 6,039.
2. Additional records were identified via Google Scholar alert (n = 1) and hand searching (n = 1). Therefore, the total number of records were 6,041.
3. Duplicate records were identified (n = 163) and removed, leaving 5,878 records.
4. The title and abstracts of the remaining records (n = 5,878) were reviewed, based on the four selection criteria. Ninety percent of the records were independently assessed by two reviewers. The reviewers disagreed about the inclusion of 16% of the records, which often resulted from different interpretations about whether they met inclusion criteria, for example, whether the behaviour in question was a PEB, or whether the independent variable was indeed a measure of identity. Discussion resolved differences to reach a consensus, which occurred during all stages of the double-reviewing process. Based on this step, 5,451 records were excluded, leaving 427 records for full-text review.
5. Full-text articles (n = 427) were reviewed based on the four selection criteria. Fifty percent went through the above double-blind review process. The reviewers disagreed about the inclusion of 9% of the articles. After this step, 341 articles were excluded, leaving a total number of 86 articles including a total number of 104 studies for the meta-analysis.
In Step 4 of the study selection process, the main cause of disagreement was due to papers not clearly using the word identity, or PEB in the abstract. Although the definitions of PEB and identity were agreed upon at the start and did not change by the end of meta-analysis, the omissions and inconsistencies of the term identity or PEB meant Reviewer 1 was more likely to include papers when (synonyms for) identity and PEB were not included in the abstract (i.e., Eligibility Criterion 1) compared to Reviewer 2. Through discussions, the two reviewers agreed on the interpretations of these abstracts, and that to meet the Eligibility Criterion 1, identity and PEB needed to be explicit and not ambiguous for inclusion for the next Step 5.
In Step 5 of the study selection process, the main difference occurred because often the papers did not clearly define identity and used many different terms to refer to the same identity construct within the paper (Udall et al., 2020). Again, Reviewer 1 was more likely to include papers when (interpretations for) identity and PEB were not included in the abstract (i.e., Eligibility Criterion 1) compared to Reviewer 2. Again, through discussions, the two reviewers noticed the disagreements were again regarding the ambiguity of identity and PEB relating to Eligibility Criterion 1. The reviewers came to an agreement to exclude those papers that did not explicitly look at identity or PEB in line with Eligibility Criterion 1.
Data Extraction Process and Analytical Procedures
The extracted data included article information, study number, studied identity type, studied PEB measure (self-reported, observed, or both), sample size, research design (laboratory/field), sample type (student, non-student, or both), overall effect size (unweighted), weighted effect size, sampling variance, z-score, lower confidence interval (CI), and upper CI.
In line with previous research (Abrahamse and Steg, 2013), we converted all effect sizes, where necessary to Pearson's r for consistency and comparability purposes (Pearson, 1895). The studies sometimes included multiple PEBs or identities, analysed with multiple regression on the same participants. If we had included the coefficients for each identity-PEB relationship, we would have included the same participants multiple times which breaks the assumptions of a random effects meta-regression analysis. Therefore, when studies included multiple measures of identity or PEB, average correlation coefficients were calculated to avoid double counting of participants enabling us to be in line with the assumptions of a random effects meta-analysis.
The data was analysed using the R metafor package (Viechtbauer, 2010). We used a random effects meta-regression model to account for both within—(sampling error) and between-study variance (Overton, 1998). We calculated the mean Pearson's r of all identities on PEB by calculating the mean r, weighted by the sample sizes of each study (Hunter and Schmidt, 2004). To assess if the weighted Pearson's r differed significantly, we used Fisher's (1925) r-to-z transformation. We will report on the variation between studies, the weighted Pearson's r of each study, weighted by the studies' sample size, and include the 95% CI for the Pearson's r estimate. We will also indicate if the Pearson's r are considered small (r ≤ −0.10/r ≥ 0.10), medium (r ≤ −0.30/r ≥ 0.30), or large, (r ≤ −0.50/r ≥ 0.50; Cohen, 1992), and the τ2, defined as the variance of the true Pearson's r (Borenstein et al., 2011).
To assess the overall presence or absence of heterogeneity among a set of studies we used the Q statistic, and to assess the degree of heterogeneity we used the I2 statistics and τ2 (Higgins and Green, 2011). A percentage below 40 indicates there are no heterogeneity issues in the meta-analysis (Higgins et al., 2003). Furthermore, we used three types of analysis to test for publication bias. Firstly, we checked for funnel plot asymmetry and assessed Egger's regression intercept analysis (Egger et al., 1997). This analysis plots each study's Pearson's r against its standard error. In the absence of publication bias, this distribution should be symmetrical around the mean Pearson's r (Borenstein, 2005). Secondly, we conducted a trim and fill analysis, which removes smaller studies which may be causing funnel plot asymmetry (Duval and Tweedie, 2000). The analysis iteratively re-computes the mean Pearson's r, until the Pearson's r is symmetrically distributed. An adjusted Pearson's r is then calculated, accounting for publication bias. Finally, we calculated Rosenthal's fail-safe N (Rosenthal, 1979). If the fail-safe N is larger than the number of studies used in the meta-analysis, it is a good sign that there is no publication bias, because one would need many studies to reduce the Pearson's r.
Finally, we conducted a meta-regression to assess if moderators alter the identity-behaviour associations. Specifically, a random-effects meta-analysis was conducted with identity, behaviour type (self-report or observed), and research design (laboratory/field), and sample type (student or non-student). Furthermore, the heterogeneity accounted for by the moderators was identified (R2).
Results
Over 99 different identities have been studied in a PEB context (Udall et al., 2020). We coded identity as individual (1) or group (2) focused according to how they best fit the definitions described in the introduction. If the identity could be described as individual and group, we coded it as both (0). We coded PEB types as individual (1) or group (2) according to how they fit the definitions in the introduction. Behaviours were categorised as both (0) if it could be described as both individual and group focused. We coded measurement type as PEB self-report (1), PEB observed (2), or both (0). We coded research setting as lab (1), field (2) or unknown (0). We coded sample types as student (1), non-student (2), or both (0). Furthermore, a summary of how many of the included studies were categorised for each moderator can be found in Table 2. In our analysis, we categorised behaviours as whether they are done on their own (e.g., taking our recycling), or are a behaviour that requires being part of group (e.g., attending an activists group meeting). While we did not assess inter-rater reliability, we coded the identities/behaviours through a reading of the measures used in the primary data, and discussion amongst collaborators in cases where there was ambiguity according to how they best fit the definitions of individual/group identities/behaviours. In most cases, it was clear from the primary data whether the behaviour could be performed alone or as part of a group.
Table 2. Summary of individual and group identities included for analysing type of behaviour, measure, research setting, and sample types.
In a series of nine different meta-regression models, the associations between the type of identity and PEB in question are given along with the moderating results—how the moderators contrast from the conditions labelled zero (0). All results reported are given as a Pearson's r. In each of the nine random effects meta-regressions, the moderator Pearson's r is interpreted as the difference between the moderated Pearson's r and the (unmoderated) main association for that model. Each reported moderator Pearson's r is the relative difference to the unmoderated (no moderation) Pearson's r for that model. Therefore, the Pearson's r for a moderated effect should be calculated by adding the unmoderated (no moderation) and moderated Pearson's r values.
Aim 1: Identity and PEB
We meta-analytically looked at the 99 different identities that were identified in a PEB context, for example, pro-environmental identity (Kaklamanou et al., 2015), moral identification (May et al., 2015), and place identity (Hernández et al., 2010). We were interested to capture all the identities, regardless of how they were defined and how they were studied (e.g., experimental, correlational) to identify whether, overall, the identities associated with PEB. A list of all the different identity-PEB associations are in Supplementary Table 1. Figure 2 shows the forest plot with the weighted Pearson's r for each study, the overall weighted Pearson's r, and accompanying statistics, under the random effects meta-regression model. Overall, identity associated with PEB as shown in Table 3 and the weighted average Pearson's r was medium (r = 0.340, p < 0.001). Our analyses indicated no evidence of publication bias (Table 4). To visually assess publication bias, see the funnel plot in Figure 3. The funnel plot shows the Pearson's r estimate of each study against the standard error of each study. Studies with a more reliable estimate will have lower standard error, and therefore should appear near the top-centre. As more rigorous studies to have a lower standard error, but also more conservative estimate of the Pearson's r, we should expect to see a symmetrical inverted V-shaped distribution. We found relatively many highly reliable studies represented by low standard errors, and there was symmetrical distribution of the Pearson's r estimates around the mean. This finding explains why the Egger's regression intercept analysis showed no indication for publication bias (Z = −0.531, p = 0.595). The trim and fill analysis showed that zero studies were trimmed. Finally, the Rosenthal's fail-safe N analysis revealed that 167,406 studies with Pearson's r of zero would be needed to render the Pearson's r non-significant. Therefore, we conclude that identities are positively and significantly associated with PEB.
Figure 2. Forest plot of Pearson's r regression coefficient, associated 95% confidence intervals (CI) for studies in a random-effects meta-analysis regression.
Figure 3. Funnel plot of study residuals against standard error for the model comparing all identity's on all PEBs without moderators.
Aim 2: Individual and Group Identities and PEB
To test the unique contribution of individual and group identities on PEB, we categorised the identities as either individual, group, or both. For individual identities (Table 2, n = 58), we found a medium weighted average Pearson's r (r = 0.373, p < 0.001). As shown in Table 4, Egger's regression intercept analysis (Z = 0.620, p = 0.535), the trim and fill analysis (zero studies were trimmed), and, the Rosenthal's fail safe N (67,294 studies with Pearson's r of zero would be needed to render the Pearson's r non-significant) showed no indication of a publication bias. For group identities (Table 2, n = 29), we found a small, but close to medium Pearson's r (r = 0.274; p < 0.001; Table 3). Table 4 shows that there was no publication bias: Egger's regression intercept analysis (Z = −1.793, p = 0.073), the trim and fill analysis (zero studies were trimmed), and the Rosenthal's fail-safe N was 8,523 studies.
Next we explored whether individual and group identities related differently to PEB. The moderation analyses (Table 3) showed that neither the Pearson's r estimate for individual identities on PEB (r = 0.058, p = 0.459), nor for group identities on PEB (r = −0.042, p = 0.635), were significantly different from the overall Pearson's r of identity on PEB. However, these results are on a (smaller) study level only (nstudy max = 104). Therefore, we complemented these moderation tests with a Fisher's r-to-z transformation to compare the two regressions reported above (i.e., on the larger sample level rather than the smaller study level). This showed that the two Pearson's r were significantly different (r1 = 0.373, n1 = 33,371, r2 = 0.274, n2 = 11,591; z = 12.95, p < 0.001). Therefore, individual identities associate with PEB more so than group identities.
Aim 3: Identity and Individual and Group PEBs
Firstly, we explored whether all 99 identities associated with individual PEB. We focused only on studies with individual PEB (Table 2, n = 72). As Table 3 indicates, we found the weighted average Pearson's r was medium (r = 0.340; p < 0.001). There was no publication bias (Table 4), as shown by Egger's regression intercept analysis (Z = 0.817, p = 0.414), the trim and fill analysis (a total of 21 studies were trimmed), and, the Rosenthal's fail safe N (67,294 studies with Pearson's r of zero would be needed to render the Pearson's r non-significant). Therefore, identities are associated with individual PEB.
Secondly, we explored if all identities associated with group PEB. We focused only on studies with group PEB (Table 2, n = 14). As Table 3 indicates an association with a medium weighted average Pearson's r (r = 0.328; p < 0.001). As Table 4 shows, Egger's regression intercept analysis revealed that there was no publication bias (Z = −1.723, p = 0.805). The trim and fill analysis (a total of 3 studies were trimmed), and the Rosenthal's fail-safe N was 84,738 studies. Therefore, identities are associated with group PEB.
To complement the analyses above, we explored whether all identities related differently to individual and group PEB. The moderation analyses showed that neither the Pearson's r estimate for identities on individual PEB (r = 0.007, p = 0.919), nor for identities on group PEB (r = 0.023, p = 0.821), was significantly different from the overall Pearson's r of identity on PEB. To supplement these moderation tests, we also performed Fisher's r-to-z transformation to compare the two regressions reported above. This showed that the two Pearson's r were not significantly different (r1 = 0.340, n1 = 36,038, r2 = 0.328, n2 = 4,522; z = 0.86, n.s.). Therefore, identities are not differently associated with individual or group PEB.
To explore the extent to which individual and group identities match individual and group PEB, we first looked at the observed Pearson's r in Table 3 when identity and PEB associations were matched. When identity and PEB associations were individually matched (individual identity-individual PEB), the weighted Pearson's r was r = 0.381; and, when they were group matched (group identity-group PEB) the weighted Pearson's r was r = 0.316. The individually matched r was 0.041 more than the average observed r. The group matched r was 0.024 less than the average observed r given the average observed r. Fisher's r-to-z transformation showed that the individual-matched association was significantly different from the average (r1 = 0.381, n1 = 26,041, r2 = 0.340, n2 = 49,860; z = 6.16, p < 0.001), yet the group-matched association was not (r1 = 0.316, n1 = 2,702, r2 = 0.340, n2 = 49,860; z = −1.36, n.s.).
Next, we looked at the observed Pearson's r in Table 3 when identity and PEB associations were mismatched. We found that individual identity and group PEB associations were r = 0.247; group identity and individual PEB associations were r = 0.249. The individual identity-group PEB r was 0.093 less than the average observed r. The group-identity-individual PEB r was 0.091 less than the average observed r. Fisher's r-to-z transformation showed that these mismatched associations significantly differed from the average, respectively (r1 = 0.247, n1 = 869, r2 = 0.340, n2 = 49,860; z = −2.97, p < 0.01) and (r1 = 0.249, n1 = 6,880, r2 = 0.340, n2 = 49,860; z = −7.75, p < 0.001).
Lastly, we also assessed if the matched category of identity-PEB estimates significantly differed from the mismatched identity-PEB associations. The Pearson's r estimate for the highest scoring matched association (i.e., individual identity and individual PEB) was higher than the estimate for the lowest scoring mismatched associations (i.e., individual identity and group PEB), while the difference between the lowest scoring matched associations and highest scoring mismatched associations were lower. Fisher's r-to-z transformation showed that these were significantly different, respectively (r1 = 0.381, n1 = 26,041, r2 = 0.247, n2 = 869; z = 4.31, p < 0.001) and (r1 = 0.316, n1 = 2,702, r2 = 0.249, n2 = 6,880; z = 3.21, p < 0.01). Based on these findings, we conclude categorising identity and PEB as individual or group is useful because individual identity better explains individual PEBs and group identities better explain group PEBs.
Aims 4-6: Measures, Research Setting, and Samples as Moderators
In the final part of our meta-analysis, we explored the extent to which the behavioural measurement (self-reported vs. observed), research setting (laboratory vs. field), and sample type (student vs. non-student) moderated the identity-PEB associations. The associations between all identity types and all PEB types were not moderated by the type of behavioural measure (self-report: r = −0.104, p = 0.625 vs. observed: r = −0.397, p = 0.153). However, a more detailed analysis of individual vs. group identity and PEB revealed that the association between group identity and group PEB was significantly moderated by self-reports (r = 0.849, p < 0.001). This finding indicates that the associations for group identity-group PEB were stronger for self-reported behaviours than observed behaviours. Thus, to some extent the type of behavioural measurement alters the associations between identity and PEB.
The associations between all identity types and all PEB types were not significantly moderated by the laboratory setting (r = 0.163, p = 0.332) or field setting (r = 0.248, p = 0.140). However, the more detailed analysis showed that all identity types combined with group PEB were significantly moderated by the laboratory research setting (r = −0.806, p < 0.001) indicating that the associations were weaker in a lab than in a field setting. Also, the associations between individual identities and group PEB were significantly moderated by the laboratory research setting (r = −0.233, p < 0.05), indicating that the associations decreased more in a lab than in a field setting. Hence, to some extent the research setting alters the associations between identity and PEB.
The sample type (student or non-student) might alter the associations between identity and PEB. The associations between all identity types and all PEB types were not significantly moderated by student samples (r = 0.155, p = 0.209) or non-student samples (r = −0.060, p = 0.582). However, the identity-group PEB associations were larger in student samples (r = 0.583, p < 0.001). Yet, the group identity-overall PEB associations were smaller in student samples (r = −0.332, p < 0.05). Finally, the individual identity-group PEB associations were weaker in student samples (r = −0.167, p < 0.05). Thus, to some extent the sample type (student or non-student) altered the associations between identity and PEB.
Discussion
To follow the core structure of this article, the findings and implications for each of the six aims will be discussed in their respective order. Then we will address possible future research directions as well as practical implications, before ending with overall conclusions.
Aim 1: Overall Identity and Behaviour
Our random-effects meta-analysis showed that identity associates with PEB with a medium Pearson's r. These findings are in line with the two key theories of identity, identity theory (Stryker and Burke, 2000) and social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, 1979), as well as with the more recently proposed PEB-identity theory (Udall et al., 2020). These theories assume that identity is a key antecedent of (pro-environmental) behaviour. Hence, the four dominant psychological theories used in PEB research (Klöckner, 2013), namely theory of planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1991), norm-activation theory (Schwartz, 1992), value-belief-norm theory (Stern, 2000), and habits (Verplanken and Aarts, 1999) might be significantly improved by including the concept of identity. Our results showed that—overall—there is an association between identity and PEB. Yet, our research also shows that not all the identities need to be included as we found different associations for different types of identity and PEB.
Aim 2: Different Identities and Overall Behaviour
Based on self-categorisation theory (Turner et al., 1987) we categorised all these identities in two main ways, either as an individual identity or a group identity. Such categorisation is sometimes done in the primary articles but is often implicitly assumed. Our research reveals that regardless of what identity we use (individual or group), they relate to PEB. However, we showed that individual identities-overall PEB associations were stronger when compared to group identities-overall PEBs associations. This finding suggests it is useful to categorise identities as individual or group because the two groups have different association strengths.
We found that group identity-PEB associations were overall weaker than the individual identity-PEB associations. For example, one study (Fielding et al., 2008a) investigated the relationship between an individual identity (self-identity with environmental activism) and a group identity (social identity: group membership of an environmental group) with intention to engage in environmental activism. While researchers found that both were significantly associated with activism, the group identity had a smaller Pearson's r than the individual identity. A weaker association for the group identity might be because these identities need the presence of the group to strengthen the identity, thus association with PEB, while the individual identity does not need this cue. We found that in many studies the group was not physically present when these group identities were measured (e.g., Dunlap and McCright, 2008) or cued (made salient) experimentally (e.g., Costa-Pinto et al., 2014, 2016). Behaviour might occur because of group identity if the relevant group is physically present because this functions as a suitable cue (makes salient) the group identity. Perhaps merely thinking about the physical presence of a group (e.g., Costa-Pinto et al., 2014, 2016) may not function sufficiently as a cue (make salient) the group identity and thus not lead to the corresponding behaviours. Perhaps group identities are reliant on the group in line with social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, 1979). Therefore, we suggest that when research is focused on a particular in-group context, or, when the context is relying on group cues, it might be necessary for future researchers to explicitly focus on group rather than individual identities and account for this missing and weak group setting. In contrast, focus on the individual identities when the group settings are absent.
Aim 3: Identity and Different PEBs
As we distinguished between individual and group identities, we also categorised PEBs accordingly (Tajfel, 1982). Overall identities related to individual and group PEBs. However, individual identities associated more strongly with individual PEBs while group identities associated more strongly with group PEBs. These finding suggest that future research might benefit from distinguishing and matching these identities and PEBs as individual or group. This matching is in line with social identity theory (Tajfel, 1982; Stern, 2000) where the associations between identity and PEB are assumed to be particularly strong when the identity and PEB are matched and weakest when they are mismatched. However, future research is needed because scholars have not explicitly categorised and assessed identities and PEBs accordingly. At a minimum, we suggest being explicit about the choice of individual-/group-focused identity and/or individual-/group-focused PEB. Finally, and ideally measuring these combinations would shed the light on the matching proposition. For example, we suggest measuring an individual identity (e.g., self-identity with environmental activism) and a group identity (e.g., social identity: group membership of an environmental group) in relation to an individual PEB (e.g., PEB at home) and a group PEB (e.g., intention to engage in environmental activism with peers).
Aim 4-6: Measures, Research Designs, and Samples
Finally, our results indicate that the methods used in identity and PEB research moderate the identity-PEB associations. We focused on three moderators, specifically, the types of behaviour measures, research setting, and sample type (Wood et al., 2015). Firstly, the findings indicated that self-report vs. objective measures moderated identity and PEB associations. Specifically, we revealed group identities-group PEB associations were stronger for self-reports. A reason for this result could be that people wish to feel, experience, and act in ways that are consistent with the group identity (Tajfel and Turner, 1979). Consequently, group identities will guide self-reports of group behaviours, because it is easier to report than to execute the behaviour.
Secondly, our findings show that studies conducted within the lab, rather than the field, negatively moderated certain identity-PEB associations (i.e., all identities-group PEB; individual identities-group PEB). In field settings these associations were found to be larger, possibly because of more researcher degrees of freedom—The inherent flexibility regarding how studies are designed and analysed (Simmons et al., 2011). Also, as recently suggested (e.g., Udall et al., 2020), when in the field, factors which may be more likely to cue a person's identity, may be more salient, such as promotional messages like the “Veganuary” campaign in the United Kingdom that provides information on posters, television adverts, and in supermarkets to encourage a meat and dairy free (Vegan) January. In the field vs. the lab there is less control over extraneous variables that might act as situational cues that make salient the identities unintentionally. These cues change aspects of the environment that might affect the participant's identity and subsequent PEB. These extraneous and situational cues may be less prevalent in a lab because they can be more easily controlled for and minimised.
Furthermore, weaker associations (all identities-group PEB; individual identities-group PEB) in the lab contradicted with the popular “demand effects hypothesis” (Haney et al., 1973; Wood et al., 2015). This hypothesis assumes associations will be stronger in laboratory rather than field settings because of demand characteristics. Participants may feel pressured to act consistently with their perceived expectations of the researcher. However, as we revealed the opposite, social identity theory (Tajfel and Turner, 1979) might shed some light on these findings. Social identity theory assumes that people carry out behaviours because of their group's expectations, irrespective of whether the group is present. However, given our findings, it might be that perhaps the group pressure needs to be salient, at least to some extent, for the behaviour to occur. In labs, identity may relate to group PEB to a lesser extent due to lower group-based demand characteristics. The participants do not feel pressure to act consistently with their intentions or self-reports because the group presence is often missing. However, our research also revealed that group PEBs are less well-studied. Further research is thus needed to determine this boundary condition for group PEBs. We have the above theorising as a starting point for future research. We suggest focusing on measuring the same identity and group PEB behaviour in the lab and field using the same sample, to further understand these associations.
Finally, sample type moderated some identity-PEB (i.e., all identities-group PEB; Individual identity-group PEB; Group identity-group PEB) associations. Indeed, reliance on student samples has largely been criticised in psychological research with the argument that they do not reflect the population at large (Henrich et al., 2010). Critics point out that the greater the student composition of the sample, the larger the associations evidenced (Henrich et al., 2010; Wood et al., 2015). Students are assumed to be more susceptible to cognitive dissonance, show increased levels of self-monitoring, greater inclination toward attitude change, and, are more susceptible to group presence compared to others (Wood et al., 2015). In line with these critiques, our findings showed that student samples moderated all identity-group PEB, and group identities-all PEB, compared to non-student samples. Furthermore, non-student samples negatively moderated individual identity-group PEB associations. Future research might benefit from these insights, by carefully considering the sample used based on which identity-PEB association is examined. We argue that this approach is especially needed when focusing on group identities and group PEBs. For example, if the aim is to understand students, then this sample type is suitable in this context. However, if the aim is to understand the population in general, then future studies should seek to use a representative sample.
Future Directions
While our paper focused on identifying whether the different identities studied in a PEB context associated with PEB, we acknowledge that future research needs to focus on why these associations are expected. For example, identity may associate with PEB via norms (Tajfel and Turner, 1979). We lay the foundations for much more specific and detailed future work as our study revealed and summarised what identities were studied, what measures were used, and in what settings. With this “clean up” and overview, scholars can now further explore the mechanisms by which identity associates with PEB. For example, one mechanism might be schemas: internally stored information about situations and expected behaviours (Markus, 1977; Stryker and Burke, 2000). The second mechanism might be norms: a salient group identity activates a group norm, which encourages the PEB. Social norm research proposes that individuals have multiple social norms which must be salient or activated to affect PEB (Cialdini et al., 1990). However, our meta-analysis of the existing literature shows that these mechanisms that best explain identity-PEB associations need exploring. While different group identities may associate with PEB via norms, there are three reasons why this is yet to be known. Firstly, it is currently not known which social norms might activate a specific group identity. Secondly, it is not known whether the social norms associated with a particular group include the specific PEB being measured. Thirdly, even if the group identity included social norms and behaviour, it is not known if, when the person was asked, whether the social norm was present. We evoke these explanations, as they may explain why we observed a weaker relationship for group identities than for individual identities. However, future research is necessary to assess the group identities and PEB alongside variables like social norms. Furthermore, while we did not measure inter-rater reliability of our coding of individual/group identity/behaviours, this paper takes a first step to consolidate the topic in such a way. Future research would benefit from more explicitly categorising their measures along these lines to further confirm how these different categories of identity relate to different categories of behaviour.
Furthermore, given the current state of the literature, this research could not examine quasi- experimental (Matsuba et al., 2012) vs. experimental (Van der Werff et al., 2013, 2014a,b; Bhattacharjee et al., 2014; Costa-Pinto et al., 2014, 2016) vs. correlational designs (Murtagh et al., 2012). These designs were “lumped” together (Thomas et al., 2019), which is not uncommon in meta-analyses (Caldwell and Welton, 2016). While we agree it would be beneficial to “split” the data in this way, there are simply not enough (quasi-)experimental primary studies to explore this. For example, 11% of identity-PEB studies used experimental designs and only 14% used within-participant designs (Udall et al., 2020), as such aggregation was required (Caldwell and Welton, 2016). Based on there being scant within-participant experimental (or longitudinal) data in the field, we suggest that it is not possible to make any strong claims about causality of identity and PEB associations. Hence, our research shows this is a much needed area for future research. Also, assessing these associations independently of other possible extraneous variables and situational cues that might be confounding the specific identity-PEB associations is needed. We suggest more research is needed in controlled (laboratory and field) settings using (quasi-)experimental and longitudinal research designs. Specifically, measuring the same PEB as an intention, a self-reported PEB, an observed PEB, an individually focused PEB, and as a group focused PEB, alongside the identity types in question would help researchers to better understand these additional moderators on identity-PEB associations.
Our research also reveals that group identities in general were under-represented and were examined mainly in student samples. Yet we argue that, especially for understanding group PEB, the setting needs to represent reality as much as possible, as the setting itself might raise or lower certain group identities. Hence, to better understand the function of group identities and behaviours, we suggest that future research needs to test identities in these ways alongside factors that may be contributing to the increased identity and behaviour associations (Balakrishnan, 1999; Prince et al., 2008; Henrich et al., 2010; Wood et al., 2015). One approach could be by creating settings that “represent realities,” for example by considering “living labs research” whereby studies are conducted in a residential home research facility where people's behaviour and identity can be observed, non-invasively, and in real time (Schwartz et al., 2015).
Our research was the first to assess identity in the PEB research-field meta-analytically. However, we acknowledge the limitation of when this analysis was conducted. Since our meta-analysis was conducted, there has been new empirical and theoretical work in this field which we now discuss. A few notable examples include the development of an Ecological Identity Scale to measure an “ecological identity” (Walton and Jones, 2017). This identity measure captures both an individually focused and group focused identity. The individually and group focused ecological identity associated directly and indirectly with a wide range of individually focused PEBs (Walton and Jones, 2017). Furthermore, the value-identity-personal norm model has been developed where an individually focused identity (environmental identity) plays a central role for individually focused PEBs and observed individually focused PEB: Participation in smart energy systems (Van der Werff and Steg, 2016). The Social Identity Model of Pro-Environmental Action (SIMPEA; Fritsche et al., 2018) was also introduced which proposes the role of four social identity processes (Fritsche et al., 2018). Specifically, secondary data suggests that group identity, group norms and goals, and collective efficacy determine environmental appraisals, and both individual and group PEBs. These processes are driven by personal and collective emotions and motivations that arise from environmental appraisal and operate on both a deliberate and automatic processing level (Fritsche et al., 2018). Research also suggests interventions targeting environmental identity not only increase the adoption of a first PEB, but also increases the likelihood of adopting another PEB, referred to as positive spillover (Maki et al., 2019). Finally, research has begun to further promote an individually and group focused green identity, using the green identity labelling technique (Schwartz et al., 2020). The green identity labelling technique increased individual PEB purchases online, in a laboratory, and in two field experiments (Schwartz et al., 2020). With our meta-analytical findings, we provide an overview of the field before such recent developments, yet our findings are still relevant so researchers can learn from the past to inform the future. Hence, we hope that scholars working on these recently emerged lines of inquiry also take note of our findings and suggestions.
Furthermore, in our meta-analysis, we assessed study quality and the potential that studies have been selectively published or the “file-draw effect,” by assessing the distribution of the correlation coefficients and sample sizes. We found no risk of bias suggesting there was no large risk of this “file-draw effect.” However, since our meta-analysis, new approaches have also been developed that can be adapted and applied in our PEB research in future to deepen our understanding of the potential risk of bias of each study. Specifically, future research would benefit from assessing the risk of bias within a study using an example like the consensus-based standards for the selection of health measurement instruments (COSMIN) checklist (Mokkink et al., 2018). Furthermore, drawing on past research, study quality can be assessed using a revised tool for the quality assessment of diagnostic accuracy studies (Quadas)-2 (Whiting et al., 2011). Future research would benefit from creating checklists like these that are PEB research appropriate, namely, for correlational, non-intervention, and non-patient research. While we found no risk of bias for overall quality in our sample, future research would benefit from assessing the moderating role of specific individual study biases on the identity-PEB associations. Based on what our meta-analysis reveals and these notable developments in the field, we propose practical implications and policy recommendations.
Practical Implications
Our meta-analysis reveals which identities might best encourage PEB in future, namely those with a Pearson's r and confidence intervals which are above zero as they associate positively with PEB (Supplementary Table 1, Column: 16). Furthermore, in line with the idea that identities guide our thoughts, feelings, and subsequent behaviours (Tajfel and Turner, 1979; Burke, 1980), we found stronger correlations for identities that are conceptually most alike to the theme of PEB (in other words, which were PEB related). Therefore, we propose that identities can be categorised into three categories with respect to how they relate to PEB (Supplementary Table 1, Column 3), namely (1) pro-PEB related (i.e., the identity is actively (positively) related to the PEB definition), (2) anti-PEB related (i.e., the identity is actively appositionally (negatively) related to the PEB definition, and (3) neutral-PEB related (i.e., the identity is neither related nor unrelated, thus is neutral to the PEB definition). Specifically, pro-PEBs best associate with PEBs. Based on the Pearson's r and confidence intervals being above zero, focusing on pro-PEB identities and combining this with our dual perspective on individual and group identities, we identify the top 10 individual and group identities for practical interventions, policy makers, as well as for future scholars to target when encouraging PEB in future. In addition, we provide the exact definitions of the identities when these were present in the paper. When these explicit identity definitions were missing, we infer them based on our theoretical understanding of the identity in question.
Individual Identities
• Self-identity in private nature conservation (Lokhorst et al., 2014): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings, and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise the preservation of the land that they are responsible for personally.
• Self-identity with environmental activism (Fielding et al., 2008a, p. 30): “Identity (e.g., as an environmental activist) motivates action, and to not engage in role-appropriate behaviour (e.g., environmental activism) may create a state of internal tension due to conflict between identity and actions. In contrast, engaging in role-appropriate behaviour validates individuals' role, and therefore their self-identity.”
• Behaviour generic self-identity: Pro-environmental (Whitmarsh and O'Neill, 2010): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings, and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise engagement in any PEB.
• Behaviour specific self-identity: Carbon offsetting (Whitmarsh and O'Neill, 2010): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings, and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise a precise PEB—reducing carbon dioxide emissions/other greenhouse gases to compensate for emissions made elsewhere.
• Pro-environmental self-identity (Dermody et al., 2015, p. 1,478): “Refers to individuals possessing a sense of self that embraces pro-environmental actions.”
• Environmental identity (Hinds and Sparks, 2008, p. 110): “Meanings that one attributes to the self as they relate to the environment.”
• Fair Trade consumer identity (Andorfer and Liebe, 2013): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings, and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise using goods and services that have sustainable and equitable trade relationships with producers of developing countries.
• Internal ethics (Rex et al., 2015; Chatzidakis et al., 2016): “Refers to individuals' internalised ethical rules and their intrinsic ethical and moral considerations about what is right and wrong and relates to their self-identity” (Rex et al., 2015, p. 267).
• Self-identity as a recycler (White and Hyde, 2012): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise converting waste into reusable material.
• Green self-identity in environmental protection (Khare, 2015a, Khare, 2015b): An individual's observations of their own thoughts, feelings and behaviour that mean they personally prioritise the preservation of the planet.
Group Identities
• Social identity: Group membership of an environmental group (Fielding et al., 2008a): An individual's perception that they belong to a group which engages in PEB.
• Group identity with Transition Towns (Bamberg et al., 2015): An individual's perception that they affiliate with people in their municipality that provide PEB initiatives as follows—increase in self-sufficiency and support the reducing of the potential effects of peak oil, climate destruction, and economic instability.
• Group identification with (social) group of environmentalists (Dono et al., 2010): An individual's perception that they belong to a group which engages in PEB.
• Group identification with environmentalist (Dono et al., 2010): An individual's perception that they belong to a group which engages in PEB.
• Social identification with environmentally conscious consumer (Bartels and Hoogendam, 2011; Bartels and Onwezen, 2014): An individual's perception that they belong to a group of customers that use resources in a PEB aware way.
• Social identification with organic consumer (Bartels and Hoogendam, 2011; Bartels and Onwezen, 2014): An individual's perception that they belong to a group of customers that use resources that were grown or raised free of synthetic fertilisers and pesticides.
• Consumer social venture identification (Hall-Phillips et al., 2016, p. 485): “Defined as the psychological attachment a consumer feels/has to a social venture based on the perceived commonality between their self-concept and a social venture's perceived identity.”
• Identity similarity with typical recyclers (Mannetti et al., 2004): An individual who perceives themselves as having a high degree of likeness to the ideal group of people that convert waste into reusable material.
• Pro-environmental political identity: American (Sweetman and Whitmarsh, 2015): An individual's perception that they affiliate with a group of people from a nation (America) whereby that nation prioritises PEB in that country, government, and public affairs.
• Environmental movement identity: Active identity (Dunlap and McCright, 2008): An individual's perception that they belong to a group of people that deliberately engage in social and/or political PEBs.
What Identity Means for Policy
Based on our research, and interpretations, we propose that policy can utilise the above identities to encourage PEB in three example steps (Hanimann et al., 2015; Udall, 2020):
1. Identify the identity that relates best with the behaviour in question. For example, a cyclist identity did not reduce car use but did increase public transport use for work. Therefore, when interested in encouraging a behaviour, encouraging the appropriate identity is necessary. These levels of nuance are necessary for delivering appropriate messages (Schwartz et al., 2020), but have not always been attained in the existing literature nor practical or policy implications.
2. Only choose pro-PEB identities (Schwartz et al., 2020). For example, some identities (such as a cyclist) relates to PEB positively because the identity is concerned with minimising harm to the planet. However, a motorist identity relates negatively to PEB because it increases harm to the planet and is therefore less appropriate (e.g., Murtagh et al., 2012). As pro-PEB identities are conceptually most alike to PEB, these are most likely to increase PEBs in future.
3. Finally, importantly, decide if the selected identity is an individual and/or the group one and emphasise this clearly. Include messages that are personal and/or people focused to reflect individual and/or group identities, respectively. For example, to encourage a cyclist identity (which would lower CO2 compared to driving) you could construct messages based around the following:
a. Individual (me) focused cyclist identity—Please think about your impact on the planet when going to work—positively imagine yourself cycling.
b. Group (we) focused cyclist identity—Please think about our impact on the planet when we go to work—positively imagine yourself with others cycling, and your power as a group to do this (e.g., arrange bike parking spaces at work) as well as the fun while doing this together
Also, with diminishing time left to make meaningful reductions in greenhouse gas emissions, a broad coalition of distributed approaches is needed alongside behaviour change to mitigate climate change (World Bank, 2015). So far, the consensus in this paper has been on changing individual citizen's behaviour via identity. While we still believe this is imperative, we also acknowledge that systemic change, and policy changing approaches are needed (Buchanan, 1992; Lewis et al., 2020). We can only reach the target of limiting climate change to manageable degrees if all parts of society follow in the same direction; and that includes politicians, administrators, industry, citizens, and education (Szebeko and Tan, 2010; Pieters and Jansen, 2017). Getting all important actors in society to follow this direction is important, and hence research to achieve this is needed (Vandekerckhove et al., 2020).
Conclusion
Our meta-analysis shows overall identity associates with overall PEB. We found this association by analysing data from the available research on this topic. Before this article, research on the topic has been found to contain a wide range of identity and PEB types without providing a quantitative overview. Our article provided this overview and reveals the areas of identity-PEB research that warrant more attention. Specifically, we show some limitations to theory when understanding the role of identity in PEB research. Additionally, we showed that it is important to distinguish between types of identities and types of PEB to further understand their associations, and then hopefully strengthen them in individuals and groups across the world. Finally, we also show that this field would benefit from much more in-depth and explicit research design considerations, as we found that measurement type (self-reported or observed), setting (lab vs. field), and sample (student vs. non-student) moderated the identity-PEB associations. Thus, both practical interventions, and future research, would benefit from using designs that include representative samples, and suitable measures and settings, to draw robust conclusions. Finally, although our results suggest that the overall Pearson's r was 0.34, meaning that identity can explain 11.5% of the variation in PEB (proportion of variance explained can be calculated by squaring the r value, i.e., 0.342 = 0.1156 x 100 = 11.56%). While at a first glance this might seem to be a small percentage, considering the complexity of creating a more sustainable world, explaining 11.5% with this identity concept is quite impressive. Also, with millions (or potentially billions) of individuals, who engage (or not) in PEB over many years, even seemingly small changes can accumulate and have a large impact on the environment. Therefore, we maintain that future research would benefit from focusing on people's identities.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further enquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s. The dataset and analysis script can be found at: https://osf.io/q7kc4/.
Author Contributions
AU conceived the project, conducted the method and analysis, and wrote the manuscript. JD, SD, and AS were the supervisors of this project and assisted writing the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This research was funded by the University of Bath.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.582421/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Abrahamse, W., and Steg, L. (2013). Social influence approaches to encourage resource conservation: a meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 1773–1785. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.07.029
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Org. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
1Alisat, S., and Riemer, M. (2015). The environmental action scale: development and psychometric evaluation. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 13–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.05.006
Allen, M. R., Dube, O. P., Solecki, W., Aragón–Durand, F., Cramer, W., Humphreys, S., et al. (2018). “Framing and context,” in Global Warming of 1.5°C. An Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change Special Report on the Impacts of Global Warming of 1.5°C Above Pre-Industrial Levels and Related Global Greenhouse Gas Emission Pathways, in the Context of Strengthening the Global Response to the Threat of Climate Change, Sustainable Development, and Efforts to Eradicate Poverty, eds V. Masson-Delmotte, P. Zhai, H. O. Pörtner., D. Roberts, J. Skea, P. R. Shukla, A. Pirani, W. Moufouma-Okia, C. Péan, R. Pidcock, S. Connors, J. B. R. Matthews, Y. Chen, X. Zhou, M. I. Gomis, E. Lonnoy, T. Maycock, M. Tignor, and T. Waterfield (In Press). Available online at: https://www.ipcc.ch/site/assets/uploads/sites/2/2018/11/SR15_Chapter1_Low_Res.pdf
2Andorfer, V. A., and Liebe, U. (2013). Consumer behaviour in moral markets. On the relevance of identity, justice beliefs, social norms, status, and trust in ethical consumption. Eur. Sociol. Rev. 29, 1251–1265. doi: 10.1093/esr/jct014
Balakrishnan, J. D. (1999). Decision processes in discrimination: fundamental misrepresentations of signal detection theory. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.25:1189. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.5.1189
3Bamberg, S., Rees, J., and Seebauer, S. (2015). Collective climate action: determinants of participation intention in community-based pro-environmental initiatives. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 155–165. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.06.006
4Barbarossa, C., Beckmann, S. C., De Pelsmacker, P., Moons, I., and Gwozdz, W. (2015). A self-identity based model of electric car adoption intention: a cross-cultural comparative study. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.04.001
Barr, S. (2007). Factors influencing environmental attitudes and behaviours: a UK case study of household waste management. Environ. Behav. 39, 435–473. doi: 10.1177/0013916505283421
8Barth, M., Jugert, P., and Fritsche, I. (2016). Still under detected–social norms and collective efficacy predict the acceptance of electric vehicles in Germany. Transport. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 37, 64–77. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2015.11.011
5Bartels, J., and Hoogendam, K. (2011). The role of social identity and attitudes toward sustainability brands in buying behaviours for organic products. J. Brand Manage. 18, 697–708. doi: 10.1057/bm.2011.3
6Bartels, J., and Onwezen, M. C. (2014). Consumers' willingness to buy products with environmental and ethical claims: the roles of social representations and social identity. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 38, 82–89. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12067
7Bartels, J., Reinders, M. J., and Van Haaster-De Winter, M. (2015). Perceived sustainability initiatives: retail managers' intrinsic and extrinsic motives. Br. Food J. 117, 1720–1736. doi: 10.1108/BFJ-11-2014-0362
9Bhattacharjee, A., Berger, J., and Menon, G. (2014). When identity marketing backfires: consumer agency in identity expression. J. Consum. Res. 41, 294–309. doi: 10.1086/676125
10Bissonnette, M. M., and Contento, I. R. (2001). Adolescents' perspectives and food choice behaviours in terms of the environmental impacts of food production practices: application of a psychosocial model. J. Nutr. Educ. 33, 72–82. doi: 10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60170-X
11Bobek, D. D., Hageman, A. M., and Radtke, R. R. (2015). The influence of roles and organisational fit on accounting professionals' perceptions of their firms' ethical environment. J. Bus. Ethics 126, 125–141. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1996-4
Borenstein, M. (2005). “Software for publication bias,” in Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments, eds Rothstein, H. R., Sutton, A. J., and Borenstein, M., (Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.), 193–220.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2011). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
Brick, C., and Lai, C. K. (2018). Explicit (but not implicit) environmentalist identity predicts pro-environmental behavior and policy preferences. J. Environ. Psychol. 58, 8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.07.003
Brick, C., Sherman, D. K., and Kim, H. S. (2017). “Green to be seen” and “brown to keep down”: visibility moderates the effect of identity on pro-environmental behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 51, 226–238. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.04.004
Brieger, S. A. (2019). Social identity and environmental concern: the importance of contextual effects. Environ. Behav. 51, 828–855. doi: 10.1177/0013916518756988
12Brügger, A., Kaiser, F. G., and Roczen, N. (2011). One for all? Connectedness to nature, inclusion of nature, environmental identity, and implicit association with nature. Eur. Psychol. 16:324. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000032
Buchanan, R. (1992). Wicked problems in design thinking. Design Issues 8, 5–21. doi: 10.2307/1511637
Burke, P. J. (1980). The self: measurement implications from a symbolic interactionist perspective. Soc. Psychol. Q. 43, 18–29. doi: 10.2307/3033745
Caldwell, D. M., and Welton, N. J. (2016). Approaches for synthesising complex mental health interventions in meta-analysis. Evid. Based Ment. Health 19, 16–21. doi: 10.1136/eb-2015-102275
13Castro, P., Garrido, M., Reis, E., and Menezes, J. (2009). Ambivalence and conservation behaviour: An exploratory study on the recycling of metal cans. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 24–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.11.003
Chandon, P., Morwitz, V. G., and Reinartz, W. J. (2004). The short and long-term effects of measuring intent to repurchase. J. Consum. Res. 31, 566–572. doi: 10.1086/425091
14Chatzidakis, A., Kastanakis, M., and Stathopoulou, A. (2016). Socio-cognitive determinants of consumers' support for the fair-trade movement. J. Bus. Ethics 133, 95–109. doi: 10.1007/s10551-013-1900-2
Chernev, A., and Blair, S. (2015). Doing well by doing good: the benevolent halo of corporate social responsibility. J. Consum. Res. 41, 1412–1425. doi: 10.1086/680089
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., and Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015
Conner, M., Godin, G., Norman, P., and Sheeran, P. (2011). Using the question-behaviour effect to promote disease prevention behaviours: Two randomized controlled trials. Health Psychol. 30, 300–309. doi: 10.1037/a0023036
15Cook, A. J., Kerr, G. N., and Moore, K. (2002). Attitudes and intentions towards purchasing GM food. J. Econ. Psychol. 23, 557–572. doi: 10.1016/S0167-4870(02)00117-4
16Costa-Pinto, D., Herter, M. M., Rossi, P., and Borges, A. (2014). Going green for self or for others? Gender and identity salience effects on sustainable consumption. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 38, 540–549. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12114
17Costa-Pinto, D., Nique, W., Herter, M., and Borges, A. (2016). Green consumers and their identities: How identities change the motivation for green consumption. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 40, 742–753. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12282
18Crimston, D., Bain, P. G., Hornsey, M. J., and Bastian, B. (2016). Moral expansiveness: examining variability in the extension of the moral world. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 111, 636. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000086
Dagher, G. K., and Itani, O. (2014). Factors influencing green purchasing behaviour: empirical evidence from the Lebanese consumers. J. Consum. Behav. 13, 188–195. doi: 10.1002/cb.1482
19Davis, J. L., Le, B., and Coy, A. E. (2011). Building a model of commitment to the natural environment to predict ecological behaviour and willingness to sacrifice. J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 257–265. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.01.004
20Dermody, J., Hanmer-Lloyd, S., Koenig-Lewis, N., and Zhao, A. L. (2015). Advancing sustainable consumption in the UK and China: the mediating effect of pro-environmental self-identity. J. Market. Manage. 31, 1472–1502. doi: 10.1080/0267257X.2015.1061039
21Dono, J., Webb, J., and Richardson, B. (2010). The relationship between environmental activism, pro-environmental behaviour and social identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 178–186. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.006
22Dresner, M., Handelman, C., Braun, S., and Rollwagen-Bollens, G. (2015). Environmental identity, pro-environmental behaviours, and civic engagement of volunteer stewards in Portland area parks. Environ. Educ. Res., 21, 991–1010. doi: 10.1080/13504622.2014.964188
Drubbel, I., Numans, M. E., Kranenburg, G., Bleijenberg, N., De Wit, N. J., and Schuurmans, M. J. (2014). Screening for frailty in primary care: a systematic review of the psychometric properties of the frailty index in community-dwelling older people. Biomed. Central Geriatr. 14:27. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-27
23Dunlap, R. E., and McCright, A. M. (2008). Social movement identity: validating a measure of identification with the environmental movement*. Soc. Sci. Q. 89, 1045–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-6237.2008.00573.x
Duval, S., and Tweedie, R. (2000). Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 56, 455–463. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., and Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Br. Med. J. 315, 629–634. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Field, A. P. (2005). Is the meta-analysis of correlation coefficients accurate when population correlations vary? Psychol. Methods 10, 444–467. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.444
Field, A. P., and Wright, D. B. (2006). A bluffer's guide to effect sizes. Psy Pag Q. 58, 9–23. Available online at: http://www.discoveringstatistics.com/docs/psypageffectsize.pdf#page=9
Fielding, K. S., and Hornsey, M. J. (2016). A social identity analysis of climate change and environmental attitudes and behaviours: insights and opportunities. Front. Psychol. 7:121. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00121
24Fielding, K. S., McDonald, R., and Louis, W. R. (2008a). Theory of planned behaviour, identity and intentions to engage in environmental activism. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 318–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.03.003
25Fielding, K. S., Terry, D. J., Masser, B. M., and Hogg, M. A. (2008b). Integrating social identity theory and the theory of planned behaviour to explain decisions to engage in sustainable agricultural practices. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 23–48. doi: 10.1348/014466607X206792
Fitzsimons, G. J., Nunes, J. C., and Williams, P. (2007). License to sin: the liberating role of reporting expectations. J. Consum. Res. 34, 22–31. doi: 10.1086/513043
26Forsyth, D. R., Vugt, M., Schlein, G., and Story, P. A. (2015). Identity and sustainability: localized sense of community increases environmental engagement. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 15, 233–252. doi: 10.1111/asap.12076
Fritsche, I., Barth, M., Jugert, P., Masson, T., and Reese, G. (2018). A social identity model of pro-environmental action (SIMPEA). Psychol. Rev. 125, 245–269. doi: 10.1037/rev0000090
Geller, E. S. (1981). Evaluating energy conservation programs: is verbal report enough? J. Consum. Res. 8, 331–335. doi: 10.1086/208872
Gershoff, A. D., and Frels, J. K. (2015). What makes it green? The role of centrality of green attributes in evaluations of the greenness of products. J. Market. 79, 97–110. doi: 10.1509/jm.13.0303
27Graça, J., Calheiros, M. M., and Oliveira, A. (2015). Attached to meat? (Un) Willingness and intentions to adopt a more plant-based diet. Appetite 95, 113–125. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.06.024
28Graham-Rowe, E., Jessop, D. C., and Sparks, P. (2015). Predicting household food waste reduction using an extended theory of planned behaviour. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 101, 194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.resconrec.2015.05.020
29Hall-Phillips, A., Park, J., Chung, T. L., Anaza, N. A., and Rathod, S. R. (2016). I (heart) social ventures: identification and social media engagement. J. Bus. Res. 69, 484–491. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.05.005
Hamerman, E. J., Rudell, F., and Martins, C. M. (2018). Factors that predict taking restaurant leftovers: strategies for reducing food waste. J. Consum. Behav. 17, 94–104. doi: 10.1002/cb.170020
Haney, C. W., Banks, C., and Zimbardo, P. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a simulated prison. Int. J. Criminol. Penol. 1, 69–97.
Hanimann, R., Vinterbäck, J., and Mark-Herbert, C. (2015). Consumer behavior in renewable electricity: can branding in accordance with identity signaling increase demand for renewable electricity and strengthen supplier brands? Energy Policy 78, 11–21. doi: 10.1016/j.enpol.2014.12.010
30and Halpenny, E. A. (2010). Pro-environmental behaviours and park visitors: the effect of place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 409–421. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.04.006
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behav. Brain Sci. 33, 61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
31Hernández, B., Martín, A. M., Ruiz, C., and del Carmen Hidalgo, M. (2010). The role of place identity and place attachment in breaking environmental protection laws. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.009
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br. Med. J. 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
Higgins, J. P. T., and Green, S., (eds.). (2011). Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]. The Cochrane Collaboration, 2011. Available online at: www.cochrane-handbook.org
32Hinds, J., and Sparks, P. (2008). Engaging with the natural environment: the role of affective connection and identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 109–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.11.001
Holden, M. A., Haywood, K. L., Potia, T. A., Gee, M., and McLean, S. (2014). Recommendations for exercise adherence measures in musculoskeletal settings: a systematic review and consensus meeting (protocol). Syst. Rev. 3:10. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-3-10
Hunter, J. E., and Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of Meta-Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings (Second Edition). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
33and Hwang, J. (2016). Organic food as self-presentation: the role of psychological motivation in older consumers' purchase intention of organic food. J. Retail. Consum. Serv. 28, 281–287. doi: 10.1016/j.jretconser.2015.01.007
34Juneman and Rufaedah A. (2013). Influence of five types of ecological attachments on general pro-environmental behaviour. Proced. Soc. Behav. Sci. 85, 535–543. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.382
35Kaklamanou, D., Jones, C. R., Webb, T. L., and Walker, S. R. (2015). Using public transport can make up for flying abroad on holiday: compensatory green beliefs and environmentally significant behaviour. Environ. Behav. 47, 184–204. doi: 10.1177/0013916513488784
36and Khare, A. (2015a). Antecedents to green buying behaviour: a study on consumers in an emerging economy. Market. Intell. Plann. 33, 309–329. doi: 10.1108/MIP-05-2014-0083
37and Khare, A. (2015b). Influence of green self-identity, past environmental behaviour and income on Indian consumers' environmentally friendly behaviour. J. Glob. Scholars Market. Sci. 25, 379–395. doi: 10.1080/21639159.2015.1073423
38Kiesling, F. M., and Manning, C. M. (2010). How green is your thumb? Environmental gardening identity and ecological gardening practices. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 315–327. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.02.004
39Klineberg, S. L., McKeever, M., and Rothenbach, B. (1998). Demographic predictors of environmental concern: it does make a difference how it's measured: research on the environment. Soc. Sci. Q. 79, 734–753
Klöckner, C. A. (2013). A comprehensive model of the psychology of environmental behaviour—A meta-analysis. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 1028–1038. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.05.014
Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behaviour? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401
Kormos, C., and Gifford, R. (2014). The validity of self-report measures of proenvironmental behaviour: a meta-analytic review. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 359–371. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.09.003
40and Lee, K. (2009). Gender differences in Hong Kong adolescent consumers' green purchasing behaviour. J. Consum. Market. 26, 87–96. doi: 10.1108/07363760910940456
41Lee, C. K., Levy, D. S., and Yap, C. S. F. (2015). How does the theory of consumption values contribute to place identity and sustainable consumption? Int. J. Consum. Stud. 39, 597–607. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12231
42Leung, A. K. Y., Koh, K., and Tam, K-P. (2015). Being environmentally responsible: Cosmopolitan 1133 orientation predicts pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environ.l Psycho. 43, 1134 79–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.05.011
Lewis, J. M., McGann, M., and Blomkamp, E. (2020). When design meets power: design thinking, public sector innovation and the politics of policymaking. Policy Polit. 48, 111–130. doi: 10.1332/030557319X15579230420081
43Loebnitz, N., and Bröring, S. (2015). Consumer acceptance of new food technologies for different product categories: the relative importance of experience versus credence attributes. J. Int. Consum. Market. 27, 307–317. doi: 10.1080/08961530.2015.1022923
44Loebnitz, N., Schuitema, G., and Grunert, K. G. (2015). Who buys oddly shaped food and why? Impacts of food shape abnormality and organic labeling on purchase intentions. Psychol. Market. 32, 408–421. doi: 10.1002/mar.20788
Loftus, E. F., and Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: an example of the interaction between language and memory. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 13, 585–589. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80011-3
45Lois, D., Moriano, J. A., and Rondinella, G. (2015). Cycle commuting intention: a model based on theory of planned behaviour and social identity. Transport. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 32, 101–113. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2015.05.003
46Lokhorst, A. M., Hoon, C., Le Rutte, R., and De Snoo, G. (2014). There is an I in nature: the crucial role of the self in nature conservation. Land Use Policy 39, 121–126. doi: 10.1016/j.landusepol.2014.03.005
47Lu, L., Rahman, I., and Chi, C. G. Q. (2016). Can knowledge and product identity shift sensory perceptions and patronage intentions? The case of genetically modified wines. Int. J. Hosp. Manage. 53, 152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.ijhm.2015.10.010
Maki, A., Carrico, A. R., Raimi, K. T., Truelove, H. B., Araujo, B., and Yeung, K. L. (2019). Meta-analysis of pro-environmental behaviour spillover. Nat. Sustain. 2, 307–315. doi: 10.1038/s41893-019-0263-9
48Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., and Livi, S. (2004). Recycling: planned and self-expressive behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 227–236. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2004.01.002
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 35, 63–78. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.35.2.63
49Matsuba, M. K., Pratt, M. W., Norris, J. E., Mohle, E., Alisat, S., and McAdams, D. P. (2012). Environmentalism as a context for expressing identity and generativity: patterns among activists and uninvolved youth and midlife adults. J. Pers. 80, 1091–1115. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2012.00765.x
50May, D. R., Chang, Y. K., and Shao, R. (2015). Does ethical membership matter? Moral identification and its organisational implications. J. Appl. Psychol. 100, 681–694. doi: 10.1037/a0038344
51McCright, A. M., and Dunlap, R. E. (2015). Comparing two measures of social movement identity: the environmental movement as an example. Soc. Sci. Q. 96, 400–416. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.12148
52Memery, J., Angell, R., Megicks, P., and Lindgreen, A. (2015). Unpicking motives to purchase locally-produced food: analysis of direct and moderation effects. Eur. J. Market. 49, 1207–1233. doi: 10.1108/EJM-02-2014-0075
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analyses: the Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analyses Statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
Mokkink, L. B., De Vet, H. C., Prinsen, C. A., Patrick, D. L., Alonso, J., Bouter, L. M., et al. (2018). Consensus-based standards for the selection of health measurement instruments (COSMIN) risk of bias checklist for systematic reviews of patient-reported outcome measures. Qual. Life Res. 27, 1171–1179. doi: 10.1007/s11136-017-1765-4
53Murtagh, N., Gatersleben, B., and Uzzell, D. (2012). Multiple identities and travel mode choice for regular journeys. Transport. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 15, 514–524. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2012.05.002
54Nigbur, D., Lyons, E., and Uzzell, D. (2010). Attitudes, norms, identity and environmental behaviour: using an expanded theory of planned behaviour to predict participation in a kerbside recycling programme. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 49, 259–284. doi: 10.1348/014466609X449395
55Nilsson, A., Andersson, K., and Bergstad, C. J. (2015). Energy behaviours at the office: an intervention study on the use of equipment. Appl. Energy 146, 434–441. doi: 10.1016/j.apenergy.2015.02.045
56Oh, J. C., and Yoon, S. J. (2014). Theory-based approach to factors affecting ethical consumption. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 38, 278–288. doi: 10.1111/ijcs.12092
Overton, R. C. (1998). A comparison of fixed-effects and mixed (random-effects) models for meta-analysis tests of moderator variable effects. Psychol. Methods 3, 354–379. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.3.3.354
Parris, C. L., Hegtvedt, K. A., and Johnson, C. (2020). Assessments of environmental injustice among black Americans. Soc. Curr. 8:2329496520950808. doi: 10.1177/2329496520950808
Pearson, K. (1895). Notes on regression and inheritance in the case of two parents. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. 58, 240–242.
57and Pérez, R. C. (2009). Effects of perceived identity based on corporate social responsibility: the role of consumer identification with the company. Corporate Reput. Rev. 12, 177–191. doi: 10.1057/crr.2009.12
Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., and Livi, S. (2003). Self-identity and the theory of planned behavior in the prediction of health behavior and leisure activity. Self Ident. 2, 47–60. doi: 10.1080/15298860309024
Pieters, M., and Jansen, S. (2017). The 7 Principles of Complete Co-creation. Amsterdam: BIS Publishers.
Prince, S. A., Adamo, K. B., Hamel, M. E., Hardt, J., Gorber, S. C., and Tremblay, M. (2008). A comparison of direct versus self-report measures for assessing physical activity in adults: a systematic review. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 5:56. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-5-56
Pronin, E. (2008). How we see ourselves and how we see others. Science 320, 1177–1180. doi: 10.1126/science.1154199
58Ramkissoon, H., and Mavondo, F. T. (2015). The satisfaction–place attachment relationship: potential mediators and moderators. J. Bus. Res. 68, 2593–2602. doi: 10.1016/j.tourman.2012.09.003
59Rees, J. H., and Bamberg, S. (2014). Climate protection needs societal change: determinants of intention to participate in collective climate action. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 466–473. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2032
61Reese, G., and Kohlmann, F. (2015). Feeling global, acting ethically: global identification and Fairtrade consumption. J. Soc. Psychol. 155, 98–106. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2014.992850
60Reese, G., Berthold, A., and Steffens, M. C. (2016). As high as it gets: ingroup projection processes in the superordinate group humans. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 50, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2015.11.003
Reese, G., Hamann, K. R., Heidbreder, L. M., Loy, L., Menzel, C., Neubert, S., and Wullenkord, M. C. (2020). SARS-Cov-2 and environmental protection: a collective psychology agenda for environmental psychology research. J. Environ. Psychol. 70:101444. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101444
62Rex, J., Lobo, A., and Leckie, C. (2015). Evaluating the drivers of sustainable behavioural intentions: an application and extension of the theory of planned behaviour. J. Nonprofit Public Sect. Market. 27, 263–284. doi: 10.1080/10495142.2015.1053342
Rise, J., Sheeran, P., and Hukkelberg, S. (2010). The Role of self-identity in the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analysis. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 40, 1085–1105. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00611.x
Rosenthal, R. (1979). The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results. Psychol. Bull. 86, 638–641. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.638
Rosenthal, R., and DiMatteo, M. R. (2001). Meta-analysis: recent developments in quantitative methods and literature reviews. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 59–82. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.59
63Schuitema, G., Anable, J., Skippon, S., and Kinnear, N. (2013). The role of instrumental, hedonic, and symbolic attributes in the intention to adopt electric vehicles. Transport. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 48, 39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.tra.2012.10.004
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). “Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, ed Zanna, M., (Orlando, FL: Academic Press), 1–65. doi: 10.1016/s0065-2601(08)60281-6
Schwartz, T., Stevens, G., Jakobi, T., Denef, S., Ramirez, L., Wulf, V., and Randall, D. (2015). What people do with consumption feedback: a long-term living lab study of a home energy management system. Interact. Comput. 27, 551–576. doi: 10.1093/iwc/iwu009
Schwartz, D., Loewenstein, G., and Agüero-Gaete, L. (2020). Encouraging pro-environmental behaviour through green identity labelling. Nat. Sustainab. 3, 746–752. doi: 10.1038/s41893-020-0543-4
66Steinheider, B., and Hodapp, V. (1999). Environmental worry: a concept to explain differences in environmentally conscious behaviour? Z. Hyg. Umweltmedizin 202, 273–289. doi: 10.1016/S0934-8859(99)80030-3
67Stets, J. E., and Biga, C. F. (2003). Bringing identity theory into environmental sociology. Sociol. Theor. 21, 398–423. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-9558.2003.00196.x
69Swim, J. K., Zawadzki, S. J., Cundiff, J. L., and Lord, B. (2014). Environmental identity and community support for the preservation of open space. Hum. Ecol. Rev. 20, 133–155. doi: 10.22459/her.20.02.2014.07
64Shaw, D., Shiu, E., and Clarke, I. (2000). The contribution of ethical obligation and self-identity to the theory of planned behaviour: an exploration of ethical consumers. J. Market. Manage. 16, 879–894. doi: 10.1362/026725700784683672
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., and Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1359–1366. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417632
65Sparks, P., and Shepherd, R. (1992). Self-Identity and the theory of planned behaviour: Assessing the role of identification with “green consumerism”. Soc. Psychol. Q. 55, 388–399. doi: 10.2307/2786955
Spence, J. C., Burgess, J., Rodgers, W., and Murray, T. (2009). Effect of pretesting on intentions and behaviour: a pedometer and walking intervention. Psychol. Health 24, 777–789. doi: 10.1080/08870440801989938
Sprott, D. E., Spangenberg, E. R., and Perkins, A. W. (1999). Two more self-prophecy experiments. Advances in Consumer Research, 26, 621–626.
Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behaviour. J. Soc. Issues 56, 407–424. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00175
Stryker, S., and Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present, and future of an identity theory. Soc. Psychol. Q. 63, 284–297. doi: 10.2307/2695840
68Sweetman, J., and Whitmarsh, L. E. (2015). Climate justice: high-status ingroup social models increase pro-environmental action through making actions seem more moral. Top. Cogn. Sci. 8, 196–221. doi: 10.1111/tops.12178
Szebeko, D., and Tan, L. (2010). Co-designing for society. Acad. Manage. J. 3, 580–590. doi: 10.4066/AMJ.2010.378
Tajfel, H., and Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. Soc. Psychol. Intergroup Relat. 33:74.
70and Tam, K.-P. (2013). Concepts and measures related to connection to nature: similarities and differences. J. Environ. Psychol. 34, 64–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.01.004
Tarrant, M. A., and Cordell, H. K. (1997). The effect of respondent characteristics on general environmental attitude-behavior correspondence. Environ. Behav. 29, 618–637. doi: 10.1177/0013916597295002
71Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., and White, K. M. (1999). The theory of planned behaviour: self-identity, social identity, and group norms. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 225–244. doi: 10.1348/014466699164149
Thomas, J., Kneale, D., McKenzie, J. E., Brennan, S. E., and Bhaumik, S. (2019). “Chapter 2: Determining the scope of the review and the questions it will address,” in Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions version 6.1 (updated September 2020), eds Higgins, J. P. T., Thomas, J., Chandler, J., Cumpston, M., Li, T., Page, M. J., and Welch, V. A., (Chichester: Cochrane), 13–32. doi: 10.1002/9781119536604.ch2
72Tonge, J., Ryan, M. M., Moore, S. A., and Beckley, L. E. (2015). The effect of place attachment on pro-environment behavioural intentions of visitors to coastal natural area tourist destinations. J. Travel Res. 54, 730–743. doi: 10.1177/0047287514533010
73Truong, N., Nguyen, D., and Hartley, N. (2015). Consumer social identity: cool and single or caring and attached. J. Strateg. Market. 23, 33–48. doi: 10.1080/0965254X.2014.914066
74Trump, R. K., Connell, P. M., and Finkelstein, S. R. (2015). Dissociation from beloved unhealthy brands decreases preference for and consumption of vegetables. Appetite 92, 192–199. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.025
Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., and Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorisation Theory. Basel: Blackwell.
Udall, A. M. (2020). How I See Me: The Power Of Identities For Encouraging Sustainable Actions. Bath Business and Society [Web log post]. Available online at: http://blogs.bath.ac.uk/business-and-society/2020/05/01/how-i-see-me-the-power-of-identities-for-encouraging-sustainable-actions/
Udall, A. M., De Groot, J. I. M., De Jong, S. B., and Shankar, A. (2020). How do I see myself? A systematic review of identities in pro-environmental behaviour research. J. Consum. Behav. 19, 108–141. doi: 10.1002/cb.1798
Van Den Broek, K. L., and Walker, I. (2019). Exploring the perceptions of drivers of energy behaviour. Energy Policy 129, 1297–1305. doi: 10.1016/j.enpol.2019.03.033
Van der Werff, E., and Steg, L. (2016). The psychology of participation and interest in smart energy systems: comparing the value-belief-norm theory and the value-identity-personal norm model. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 22, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.erss.2016.08.022
76Van der Werff, E., Steg, L., and Keizer, K. (2013). It is a moral issue: the relationship between environmental self-identity, obligation-based intrinsic motivation and pro-environmental behaviour. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 1258–1265. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2013.07.018
77Van der Werff, E., Steg, L., and Keizer, K. (2014a). Follow the signal: when past pro-environmental actions signal who, you are. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 273–282. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.07.004
78Van der Werff, E., Steg, L., and Keizer, K. (2014b). I am what I am, by looking past the present the influence of biospheric values and past behaviour on environmental self-identity. Environ. Behav. 46, 626–657. doi: 10.1177/0013916512475209
Vandekerckhove, P., de Mul, M., Bramer, W. M., and de Bont, A. A. (2020). Generative participatory design methodology to develop electronic health interventions: systematic literature review. J. Med. Intern. Res. 22:e13780. doi: 10.2196/13780
75Van Dam, Y. K., and Fischer, A. R. (2015). Buying green without being seen. Environ. Behav. 47, 328–356. doi: 10.1177/0013916513509481
Verplanken, B., and Aarts, H. (1999). Habit, attitude, and planned behaviour: is habit an empty construct or an interesting case of goal-directed automaticity? Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 10, 101–134. doi: 10.1080/14792779943000035
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. 36, 1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
Walters, G. D. (2020). Crime and social cognition: a meta-analytic review of the developmental roots of adult criminal thinking. J. Exp. Criminol. 1–25. doi: 10.1007/s11292-020-09435-w
Walton, T. N., and Jones, R. E. (2017). Ecological identity: the development and assessment of a measurement scale. Environ. Behav. 50, 657–689. doi: 10.1177/0013916517710310
Warriner, G. K., McDougall, G. H., and Claxton, J. D. (1984). Any data or none at all? Living with inaccuracies in self-reports of residential energy consumption. Environ. Behav. 16, 503–526. doi: 10.1177/0013916584164005
79Watson, L., Johnson, C., Hegtvedt, K. A., and Parris, C. L. (2015). Living green: examining sustainable dorms and identities. Int. J. Sustain. High. Educ. 16, 310–326. doi: 10.1108/IJSHE-09-2013-0118
81White, K. M., Smith, J. R., Terry, D. J., Greenslade, J. H., and McKimmie, B. M. (2009). Social influence in the theory of planned behaviour: the role of descriptive, injunctive, and in-group norms. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 48, 135–158.
80White, K. M., and Hyde, M. K. (2012). The role of self-perceptions in the prediction of household recycling behaviour in Australia. Environ. Behav. 44, 785–799. doi: 10.1177/0013916511408069
Whiting, P. F., Rutjes, A. W. S., Westwood, M. E., Mallett, S., Deeks, J. J., Reitsma, J. B., et al. (2011). Quality assessment of diagnostic accuracy studies (Quadas)-2: a revised tool for the quality assessment of diagnostic accuracy studies. Ann. Intern. Med. 155, 529–536. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-155-8-201110180-00009
82Whitmarsh, L., and O'Neill, S. (2010). Green identity, green living? The role of pro-environmental self-identity in determining consistency across diverse pro-environmental behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 305–314. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.003
Williams, P., Block, L. G., and Fitzsimons, G. J. (2006). Simply asking questions about health behaviours increases both healthy and unhealthy behaviour. Soc. Influence 1, 117–127. doi: 10.1080/15534510600630850
Williams, P., Fitzsimons, G. J., and Block, L. G. (2004). When consumers do not recognise “benign” intention questions as persuasive attempts. J. Consum. Res. 31, 540–550. doi: 10.1086/425088
Wood, C., Conner, M., Miles, E., Sandberg, T., Taylor, N., Godin, G., and Sheeran, P. (2015). The impact of asking intention or self-prediction questions on subsequent behaviour: a meta-analysis. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 20, 245–268. doi: 10.1177/1088868315592334
World Bank (2015). World Development Report 2015: Mind, Society, and Behaviour. Washington, DC. doi: 10.1596/978-1-4648-0342-0
83Yang, D., Lu, Y., Zhu, W., and Su, C. (2015). Going green: how different advertising appeals impact green consumption behaviour. J. Bus. Res. 68, 2663–2675. doi: 10.1016/j.jbusres.2015.04.004
84Yazdanpanah, M., and Forouzani, M. (2015). Application of the theory of planned behaviour to predict Iranian students' intention to purchase organic food. J. Clean. Prod. 107, 342–352. doi: 10.1016/j.jclepro.2015.02.071
85Zeugner-Roth, K. P., Zabkar, V., and Diamantopoulos, A. (2015). Consumer ethnocentrism, national identity, and consumer cosmopolitanism as drivers of consumer behaviour: a social identity theory perspective. J. Int. Market. 23, 25–54. doi: 10.1509/jim.14.0038
86Zhang, Y., Van Dijk, T., Tang, J., and Berg, A. E. (2015). Green space attachment and health: a comparative study in two urban neighborhoods. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 12, 14342–1436. doi: 10.3390/ijerph121114342
NThe articles assigned a number before the citation are articles included in the meta-analysis.
Keywords: identities, pro-environmental behaviour, group identity, methods—estimation, individual identification, climate change, sustainability, meta-analysis
Citation: Udall AM, de Groot JIM, De Jong SB and Shankar A (2021) How I See Me—A Meta-Analysis Investigating the Association Between Identities and Pro-environmental Behaviour. Front. Psychol. 12:582421. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.582421
Received: 11 July 2020; Accepted: 15 February 2021;
Published: 16 March 2021.
Edited by:
Bernardo Hernández, University of La Laguna, SpainReviewed by:
Laura S. Loy, University of Koblenz Landau, GermanyMagnus Bergquist, University of Gothenburg, Sweden
Ana M. Martín, University of La Laguna, Spain
Copyright © 2021 Udall, de Groot, De Jong and Shankar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Alina Mia Udall, YWxpbmFtaWF1ZGFsbCYjeDAwMDQwO2JhdGguZWR1