Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 24 February 2021
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Sensorimotor Foundations of Social Cognition View all 22 articles

Gratitude Affects Inter-Subjective Synchronicity for Cognitive Performance and Autonomic Responsiveness

  • 1International Research Center for Cognitive Applied Neuroscience (IrcCAN), Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
  • 2Research Unit in Affective and Social Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy

Recently, social neurosciences have been interested in the investigation of neurophysiological responses related to the experience of positive emotions, such as gratitude, during social interactions. Specifically, the aim of the present research was to investigate whether gratitude related to gift exchange could favor cooperative behavior and bond construction, by improving behavioral and autonomic responsivity. At this regard, the autonomic synchronization and behavioral performance of 16 friends coupled in dyads were recorded during a joint attentional task. Gift exchange could be occurred either at the beginning or in the middle of the task. For the recording of simultaneous autonomic activity [heart rate (HR) and skin conductance level (SCL)], a hyperscanning biofeedback paradigm was used. Intra-subjective analysis showed an increase in behavioral [accuracy (ACC)] and autonomic responses (HR and SCL) when the gift exchange took place at the beginning of the task rather than in the middle. Moreover, inter-subjective analysis revealed an increase in behavioral performance and greater autonomic synchronization of HR index. The present research, therefore, shows how gratitude and trust experienced following gift exchange can modify participants’ reactions by creating a shared cognition and the adoption of joint strategies.

Introduction

The act of giving or receiving something can be considered as a moment of interpersonal exchange that leads to the development of important links that strengthen social relations (Mick and Demoss, 1990; Mick and Faure, 1998). Specifically, positive emotions experienced during gift exchange, such as gratitude, turn out to be important in the construction of social ties, improving subjective well-being and prosocial behavior by strengthening interpersonal relationships (McCullough and Tsang, 2004; Penner et al., 2005; Algoe et al., 2008; Froh et al., 2008; Lambert et al., 2010). In particular, the moment of gift exchange, as an act of social interaction, involves the individuals by influencing and modeling their behaviors (Golland et al., 2015), thanks to those basic mechanisms that allow to perceive, imitate, and understand others’ feelings, actions, and intentions (Niedenthal, 2007; Balconi and Bortolotti, 2012, 2013; Balconi and Canavesio, 2014). As demonstrated by previous studies, indeed, when these mirroring mechanisms happen in interacting individuals, an implicit behavioral, neural, and psychophysiological activity attunement occurs (Richardson et al., 2007; Konvalinka et al., 2010, 2011; Dumas et al., 2011; Müller and Lindenberger, 2011; Hasson et al., 2012; Giuliano et al., 2015), leading neuroscientists to consider inter-agent actors as a single complex system (Balconi and Pagani, 2015; Chung et al., 2015; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017a). Specifically, the body synchronization that is experienced between the inter-agents may be due to the sharing of positive emotional experiences during gift exchange (Chauhan et al., 2008; Balconi and Canavesio, 2013). Emotions, indeed, improve behavioral, cognitive, and affective individuals’ synchronization representing the basis of prosocial behavior (Balconi et al., 2011; Balconi and Bortolotti, 2012; Vanutelli et al., 2016, 2017; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017a).

An innovative paradigm, called hyperscanning, has been proposed to explore the synchronization that occurs between the interacting individuals during joint actions (Vanutelli et al., 2016, 2017; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017b; Balconi et al., 2018). According to this recent paradigm, the focus is, therefore, on the recording of individuals’ neurophysiological activity during various interpersonal dynamics (Schilbach et al., 2010). As demonstrated by some studies, indeed, the neural and autonomic responses of two interacting individuals can show a strong synchronization during a significant interpersonal exchange (Levenson and Ruef, 1992). Specifically, simultaneous neural activity can be recorded through the use of different techniques, such as electroencephalography (EEG; Koike et al., 2015; Balconi and Vanutelli 2017a), and neuroimaging techniques, such as functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), which permit to observe hemodynamic activity changes in specific brain regions (Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017a). On the other hand, peripheral activity can be recorded through the use of biofeedback (Mirgorodsky et al., 2013). In particular, as consistently shown by previous work on autonomic attunement (Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017a; Vanutelli et al., 2017, 2018), the measurement of peripheral synchrony permits to evaluate the physiological synchronization (PS), defined as the covariation of autonomous measurements between dyads or interacting groups (Butler, 2011), by analyzing their changes throughout the task.

The PS index, therefore, can be used to define the intensity of the interaction between the inter-agent individuals (Hatfield et al., 1994). Moreover, the peripheral activity, considered as a response implemented by the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) to identify “fight-or-flight” responses, can provide information on individuals’ emotional state (Diamond and Otter-Henderson, 2007). It turned out to be correlated to some important social, emotional, and empathic processes (Levenson and Ruef, 1992; Adolphs, 2003) and it gives information related to individuals’ synchrony (Chaspari et al., 2015). For these reasons, autonomic synchronization was observed above all in cooperative behaviors (Vanutelli et al., 2016; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2017a) in significant relational ties, such as parent-child interaction (Barrett, 2006) and in the patient-therapist relationship (Marci et al., 2007). Specifically, information on individuals’ emotional activation (Diamond and Otter-Henderson, 2007; Boucsein, 2012) and the quality of social interactions (Guastello et al., 2006) were provided by electrodermal activity (EDA), or skin conductance level (SCL), that is, a representative index of skin conductivity changes. In addition to EDA, the cardiovascular activity also provides information on the individual’s emotional activation. In fact, it increases mainly while experiencing highly positive or negative emotions, such as happiness, joy, fear, sadness, and anger (Levenson, 1992; Sinha et al., 1992), and varies according to emotional closeness (Konvalinka et al., 2011).

These indices, therefore, can provide information related to the emotional synchronization between two individuals involved in social exchange, as revealed in previous studies that observed a higher autonomic activity [increased arousal, SCL, and heart rate (HR)] during social interactions, such as cooperative conditions (Balconi and Bortolotti, 2012). Furthermore, it was shown that the emotional attunement between inter-agents that is experienced during cooperative behaviors leads individuals to improve their behavioral performance through the releasing of dopamine in the prefrontal brain regions. Such release produces an improvement in cognitive performance on a wide range of tasks involving the use of attention and working memory (Gray, 2004; Isen, 2009; Nadler et al., 2010).

The aim of this study was, therefore, to explore the peripheral activity synchronization between two individuals, using a hyperscanning technique based on biofeedback, during the performance of a task that involved a gift exchange (consisting of a material or experiential gift) at the beginning or in the middle of the performance of a cognitive task consisting of three blocks in which was asked to recognize, among others, a target stimulus. Specifically, we hypothesized that the performance of joint action, involving the establishment of cooperative behavior, would lead to an increase in individuals’ peripheral synchronization and behavioral performance. In this regard, an attentive task was devised that required pairs of subjects to cooperate by synchronizing their responses. In particular, the main aim of the present work was to investigate whether the participants’ behavioral performance and peripheral activity improved following the gift exchange. Secondly, we aimed at verifying if the specific moment of gift exchange (at the beginning or in the middle of the task) had specific effects on individuals’ peripheral activity and behavioral responses. Thirdly, we hypothesized that the moment of gift exchange, involving the sharing of positive emotions, such as gratitude, could strengthen individuals’ behavioral performance and autonomic synchronization. Specifically, we expected this effect to be higher in the first condition in which the gift exchange took place at the beginning of the task. Finally, we expected to find different effects in the autonomic activity of the donor compared to that of the receiver before and after the gift exchange, in terms of a higher emotional engagement and a higher arousal for the donor than receiver (Flynn and Brockner, 2003; Duclos and Barasch, 2014).

Materials And Methods

Participants

Sixteen pairs of subjects (N = 32, M = 23.34; SD = 1.23) involved in a friendship relationship were recruited to carry out the experiment, using the following exclusion criteria: normal or correct-normal visual acuity and absence of neurological or psychiatric pathologies, verified by specific measurements. Two dyads were excluded due to the low quality of autonomic signal. The dyads took part in the study after signing the informed consent. The research was conducted according to the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the local ethics committee of the Department of Psychology of the Catholic University of Milan.

Procedure

To carry out the experiment, the subjects were seated in a dark room at a 60 cm from a computer.

Specifically, the subjects were asked to carry out a computerized task that involved a gift exchange at the beginning or in the middle of the task, which had to be donated by one of the subjects of the dyad (donor) to his partner (receiver). For seven couples, the gift exchange occurred before the beginning of the first part (after block 1); for the other seven pairs, it occurred at the end of the second block. Two different procedures were used: the first involved the performance of a basic condition (block 1), the gift exchange, and the execution of the other two task blocks (blocks 2 and 3), and the second provided for the unwinding of blocks 1 and 2, the gift exchange, and the carrying out of the block 3. Therefore, at the beginning or in the middle of the task (concerning the two procedural orders), they were asked to exchange a gift, which could be a material or experiential object (objects or tickets to visit a museum or a concert). Before the execution of blocks 2 and 3, after recording a basic condition of 120 s, the participants were asked to perform a familiarization activity with the activity (block 1; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure. Two different procedures were performed: order 1 composed by block 1 (a control condition), gift exchange, and blocks 2 and 3; and order 2 composed by block 1, block 2, gift exchange, and block 3. Blocks 1, 2, and 3 involve a cooperative task which consisted of a game of selective attention.

Specifically, the execution of the three blocks required the participants to cooperate during the development of a selective attention task modified by a previous computerized version (Balconi and Pagani, 2015; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2016; Vanutelli et al., 2016, 2017; Balconi et al., 2018, 2019a,b, 2020; Balconi and Fronda, 2020). The task required subjects to memorize a target stimulus (triangle or circle and green or blue) that they should have subsequently recognized among others by pressing the right or left key of the computer keyboard. Specifically, the stimulus appeared on the screen for 500 milliseconds (ms) with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 300 ms and an inter-trial interval (ITI) of 5,000 ms. The task required the subjects to recognize the target stimulus by synchronizing the speed and accuracy (ACC), understood as the percentage of the correct responses concerning the recognition of the target stimulus, of their responses with the support of some feedback indicating the individuals’ cooperation level represented by two arrows pointing upward, indicating a level of cooperation over 75%. Then, the subjects were asked to temporarily associate their reaction times (RTs) to increase their perception and psychological condition of cooperation and joint task. At the end of the task, both participants were given a questionnaire in order to investigate the perception of the partner (his/her friend) and the level of couple tuning during the performance of the first and second task blocks. The questionnaire consisted of the following items: “What was the perception of your workmate in the first phase of the game?,” “What was the perception of your workmate in the second phase of the game?,” “What was the perception of your collaboration and degree of gratitude in the first phase of the game?,” and “What was the perception of your collaboration and the degree of gratitude in the second phase of the game?.” Participants could respond to items by assigning a Likert-scale score from 1 (perception of non-synchronicity/non-cooperation) to 3 (perception of synchrony and cooperation).

Autonomic Measures Recording and Analysis

The autonomic activity was recorded using two Expert2000 portable Biofeedback systems with a MULTI radio module (Schuhfried GmbH, Mödling, Austria) that allow to measure the level and response of SCL in μS and HR in beats per minute (bpm). The SCL value was recorded with an EDA1 gold electrode using current measurement at a sampling frequency of 2 kiloHertz (kHz). The use of alternating voltage prevents polarization. The measurement resolution for the SCL calculation is 12 nanosecond (ns) with a sampling frequency of 20 Hertz (Hz). HR was measured by the infrared absorption principle with a sampling frequency of 500 Hz. The parameter range was 30–200 bpm. Furthermore, the mobility of the non-dominant hand was monitored with an accelerometer in meter/square second (m/s2) integrated into the sending unit to ensure that the recordings were not compromised by hand movements. Trials with motor artifacts have been removed from the analyses.

Data Analysis

Questionnaire Responses

For the answers to the questionnaire, a preliminary analysis was conducted. Specifically, two mixed model ANOVAs with Block (pre vs. post) and Role (donor vs. receiver) as a repeated factor, and Condition as between-subjects factor (Cond: order 1 vs. order 2) were applied to questionnaire scores.

ANOVA results showed a significant effect for Block (F[1,97] = 22.56; p < 0.0001; η2 = 0.90), with a greater perception of tuning after (M = 3.0; SD = 0.07) than before (M = 1.04; SD = 0.05) gift exchange. With regard to the perception of cooperation, instead, ANOVA showed a significant effect for Block (F[1,97] = 34; p < 0.0001; η2 = 0.94), with a greater perception of cooperation later (M = 2.99; DS = 0.06) compared to before (M = 1.34; SD = 0.03) gift exchange.

Behavioral and Autonomic Data Analyses

Three main orders of analyses were performed: (1) Repeated measures ANOVAs on the modulation of the dependent variables (ACC, RTs, SCL, and HR) for the subjects throughout the task were conducted (A – intra-subjective analysis); (2) inter-subjects correlational indices were performed (B – inter-subjective analysis) to compute the synchronization values within each couple for each autonomic measure; and (3) repeated measures ANOVAs on such indices to assess differences in synchrony strength across the experimental conditions. For all the ANOVA tests, the degrees of freedom were corrected using Greenhouse-Geisser epsilon where appropriate, with the level of significance set at 0.05. Also, post-hoc comparisons (contrast analyses) were applied to the data. Bonferroni test was applied for multiple comparisons. In addition, the normality of the data distribution was preliminary tested (kurtosis and asymmetry tests). The normality assumption of the distribution was supported by these preliminary tests.

(A) Intra-Subjective Analysis

Behavioral Results

Accuracy and RTs scores were obtained for each subject using E-prime software during the three blocks. Specifically, for the analysis of ACC, the percentage of correct answers on the total answers was considered, while RTs were calculated starting from the presentation of the stimulus.

Before the pre-gift training condition, after the 120-s reference record, the subjects were provided a familiarization task. Specifically, two mixed-model ANOVAs were applied to ACC and RT with Blocks (1baseline vs. 2 vs. 3) as a repeated factor and Condition (Cond: order 1 vs. order 2) and Role (Role: donor vs. receiver) as between-subjects factor.

For ACC, ANOVA revealed a significant effect for Cond (F[1,60] = 12.34; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.38), with better performance for order 1 than order 2; Block (F[2,97] = 9.77; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.32) and Cond × Block (F[2,97] = 12.98; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.37). In particular, as shown by post-hoc comparisons applied to interaction effects, order 1 revealed a higher ACC in block 2 (F[1,31] = 9.32; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.31) and in block 3 more than block 1 (F[1,31] = 8.87; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.29).

Moreover, block 3 differed from block 2 (F[1,31] = 8.50; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.29) with higher ACC. In contrast, order 2 showed higher ACC only in block 3 more than block 1 (F[1,31] = 9.08; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.30) and block 2 (F[1,31] = 9.16; p < 0.001; η2 = 0.32; Figure 2A).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Increase in accuracy for order 1 in blocks 2 and 3 more than block 1 and in block 3 more than block 2. For order 2, instead, the figure shows an increase of accuracy in block 3 more than blocks 1 and 2. (B) Increase of inter-subjective accuracy (r values) for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3. Moreover, for order 1, the figure shows an increase of inter-subjective accuracy (r values) in block 3 more than block 1. In addition, higher synchronicity was observed in order 1 for block 3 more than block 2.

No significant effects were found for RTs.

Autonomic Measures

About SCL, Cond × Block interaction effect was significant (F[2,97] = 7.94, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.29). Specifically, as revealed by post-hoc comparisons, there was an increase of SCL for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3 (respectively, F[1,31] = 9.56, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.33; F[1,31] = 8.99, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.29). In addition, in order 1, block 2 and block 3 differed from block 1 with increased SCL (F[1,31] = 8.55, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.28), whereas the differences in order 2 were found only between block 1 and block 3 (respectively, F[1,31] = 8.90, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.29; F[1,31] = 9.56, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.32; Figure 3A).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Increase in individuals’ skin conductance level (SCL) for order 1 in block 2 and block 3 more than order 2. Moreover, the figure shows for order 1 an increase of SCL in blocks 2 and 3 more than block 1; while, for order 2, an increase of SCL in block 3 more than block 1. (B) For heart rate (HR), the figure shows an increase of HR for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3. Specifically, for order 1, the figure shows an increase of HR in blocks 2 and 3 more than block 1.

About HR, Cond × Block interaction effect was significant (F[2,97] = 9.33, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.35). Specifically, as revealed by post-hoc comparisons, there was an increase of HR for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3 (respectively, F[1,31] = 12.09, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.36; F[1,31] = 9.09, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.31). In addition, in order 1, block 2 and block 3 differed from block 1 with increased HR (respectively, F[1,31] = 10.06, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.37; F[1,31] = 9.50, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.34; Figure 3B).

(B) Inter-Subjective Analyses

Calculation of Correlational Indices

The synchronization indices were calculated by correlational coefficients (Pearson coefficients) applied to the data for each behavioral (ACC and RTs) and autonomic index (SCL and HR; Hernandez et al., 2014).

According to these indices, the subsequent step of analysis was finalized to test the statistical significance of independent factor Blocks (1baseline vs. 2 vs. 3) and Condition (Cond: order 1 vs. order 2) on these correlational indices for each couple by using repeated measures ANOVAs.

Behavioral Measures

For ACC, ANOVA showed a significant Cond × Block interaction effect (F[2,97] = 8.89, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.29). Specifically, pairwise post-hoc comparisons revealed a significant higher synchronization (higher r values) for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3 (respectively, F[1,13] = 9.11, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.32; F[1,13] = 8.44, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.30). In addition higher synchronicity was observed in order 1 for block 3 more than block 1 (F[1,13] = 6.34, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.26) and block 2 (F[1,13] = 6.89, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.27; Figure 2B).

No significant effect was found for RTs.

Autonomic Measures

For what concerns SCL coefficient data, no significant differences in synchrony were found for the main or interaction effects.

In contrast, significant main effect for Condition × Block was found for HR (F[1,15] = 8.87, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.29), which showed increased HR synchrony for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 (F[1,13] = 6.34, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.26) and block 3 (F[1,13] = 6.34, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.26). In addition in order 1, increased synchrony was found more in block 2 (F[1,13] = 6.34, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.26) and block 3 (F[1,13] = 7.09, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.27) than block 1, whereas in order 2, increased synchronicity was found only for block 3 compared to block 1 (F[1,13] = 6.34, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.26; Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Increased HR subjects’ synchrony (r values) for order 1 more than order 2 in block 2 and block 3. Moreover, for order 1, the figure shows an increase in HR synchrony in blocks 2 and 3 more than block 1 and in order 2, an increase of HR subjects’ synchrony was found block 3 more than block 1.

Discussion

The present research has analyzed a joint action dynamic through the application of a multimethodological paradigm requiring the recording of participants’ autonomic responses and behavioral performance during a cooperative activity involved a beginning or middle gift exchange. In addition to previous studies (Balconi et al., 2019a,b, 2020; Balconi and Fronda, 2020) aimed at detecting central indices (EEG or hemodynamic activity), which allow obtaining different information on different cognitive processes (Herrmann et al., 2003, 2008), the measurement of autonomic activity can be considered as a potential biological marker of emotions (Tupak et al., 2014), which allows to better define the interaction between peripheral and central systems (Furmark et al., 1997; Lang et al., 2000). Data were analyzed both at the individual (intra-subject) and at the dyadic level (inter-subject) to calculate PS and before and after gift exchange. Indeed, we hypothesized that sharing a gift could be accompanied by a positive emotional engagement that could influence both the behavioral and the physiological responses.

The analyses allowed to highlight some main results. (1) An improvement of both behavioral and autonomic responsivity has emerged in order 1, when gift donation came earlier, compared to a delayed exchange. A similar pattern emerged also at the inter-personal level, with increased physiological synchronization after the early gift. (2) The modulation of such effects by block variable was observed. In detail, both at the individual and the joint level, the advantage of order 1 was maximized over the blocks in favor of the last block. (3) Both cardiovascular (HR) and electrodermal (SCL) indices proved to be effective in detecting participants’ individual and joint responses. However, HR was more sensitive to such dynamics, especially at the interpersonal level. Finally, (4) although condition and blocks were able to modulate participants’ responses during the task, the specific role adopted (donor or receiver) was not influential.

Beginning with the first point (1), an earlier gift exchange was associated with increased accuracy and higher autonomic responses in detecting the attentional targets both at the individual level and the interpersonal level. Such result can be interpreted by referring to the construct of gratitude. In fact, giving and receiving a gift within an interpersonal interaction is associated with a positive feedback loop which leads to emotional sharing (Aknin et al., 2011). Previous research, indeed, already underlined the influence of gratitude and positive emotions in strengthening cooperative behavior (Rumble et al., 2010). Such greater tuning was also explicitly attested by the participants through the self-report questionnaire. According to previous research, these emotional states can be the consequence of sharing a pleasant experience, which increases the feeling of being part of a whole and the sense of interpersonal cohesion (Balconi and Pagani, 2015; Chung et al., 2015; Vanutelli et al., 2017). Moreover, such result is of particular importance since this kind of interpersonal attuning makes the implementation of prosocial behaviors more likely (Ruby and Decety, 2004; Spinella, 2005; Balconi et al., 2011; Balconi and Bortolotti, 2012) and represents a social glue thanks to a reciprocity mechanism (Balconi and Lucchiari, 2006; Balconi and Pozzoli, 2007).

However, to better understand our results, it is also important to consider trust mechanisms. Indeed, although both order 1 and order 2 involve gift exchange, order 1 could have facilitated bond construction, emotional sharing, and cooperation, since it immediately allowed participants to increase their trust based on initial gift exchange. Previous research underlined how the experience of trust is highly influenced by mood and emotions (Jones and George, 1998). Accordingly, we believe that an interaction based on trust since its beginning could be more efficient in engaging participants in a cooperative activity. Such result finds support in previous research that showed a relation between bond construction, cooperation, and interpersonal coordination, even in studies with unrelated participants (Chung et al., 2015; Balconi and Vanutelli, 2016; Vanutelli et al., 2017; Balconi et al., 2018). For example, research on autonomic synchrony showed that the covariation between couples’ physiological indices can reveal insights about the quality of their interaction representing a key marker of social engagement (Vanutelli et al., 2017).

This hypothesis is also corroborated by the second result (2). Indeed, the advantage of an earlier gift, shown by significant effects founded across the three blocks is potentially relevant to suppose a trend with an increased synchronization during the task and over the time. Even if order 2 displays an increasing synchronization over blocks, order 1 has a more definite distribution of both behavioral and autonomic responses, which can be appreciated especially between blocks 2 and 3. Indeed, in order 1, the gift is donated after the first block. Thus, there is sufficient time for the participants to develop dyadic strategies that become refined and improved in the last block. Contrarily, in order 2, the gift is donated between blocks 2 and 3, in a way that the effect of positive emotions cannot be translated into consolidated joint strategies both at behavioral and autonomic levels, as shown for order 1.

For what concerns this last point, it is important to underline the functional role of the autonomic responses (3). At the individual level, both SCL and HR proved to be sensitive to gift exchange modulation. However, referring to PS, a systematic synchronization emerged only for HR correlational indices. Interestingly, although SCL can be interpreted in relation to emotional arousal (Sohn et al., 2004), an increase in HR indices has been reported when experiencing highly positive emotional states (Ekman et al., 1983; Levenson, 1992; Sinha et al., 1992). Also, the covariation of such indices within an interpersonal relationship can be an index of emotional closeness (Konvalinka et al., 2010) and trust. Moreover, the degree of HR synchrony could predict participants’ expectations about the moves of their partners (Mitkidis et al., 2015). Since SCL is generally interpreted as an emotional-arousal related measure (Picard et al., 2016), the simple increasing of arousal is not enough to support a synchronous response by the members of the dyad.

Finally, the role played by the two members of the dyad (donor or receiver) did not have any significant effects in modulating participants’ responses. Thus, we could assume that both the act of giving and receiving a gift can similarly contribute to creating stronger cooperative ties, underlining how the implementation of prosocial behaviors can represent a social reward, even without a material return (Vanutelli et al., 2016). However, the absence of significant differences based on the role may be due to the familiarity and previous friendship between donor and receiver.

Conclusion

To conclude, the present study showed how gratitude and trust elicited within a gift donation protocol can modify the individuals’ behavioral and autonomic responses. The moment of gift exchange can actually influence the creation of a positive emotional feedback loop and the perception of trust. When the gift is donated early, participants have the possibility to build a safe interpersonal space to develop a shared cognition, which provide an increase of performance, autonomic responses, and synchrony. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that this pattern proved to follow an exponential path, which reaches its maximum strength in the last experimental block, and by the specific autonomic index involved.

We believe that our paradigm is innovative and that it could be shared among scholars and applied to different real-life contexts where trust can be improved to provide a better emotional experience, such as the organizational, the educational, and the clinical framework. Some further developments may be proposed: The integration of neurophysiological and neuroimaging measurements would allow obtaining information on the neural individuals’ synchronization and the integration of psychometric measures would allow evaluating some personalities traits; the comparison between dyad of subjects with and without previous friendship to better explore the effect of familiarity in distinguishing the role effect. Finally, the role of gender in cooperation and trust could be explored.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by local Ethics Committee of the Department of Psychology of the Catholic University of Milan. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MB contributed to the conception and design of the study and wrote the first draft and each section of the manuscript. MB and GF contributed to manuscript final writing and revision, and read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adolphs, R. (2003). Cognitive neuroscience: cognitive neuroscience of human social behavior. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 165–178. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aknin, L. B., Sandstrom, G. M., Dunn, E. W., and Norton, M. I. (2011). It’s the recipient that counts: spending money on strong social ties leads to greater happiness than spending on weak social ties. PLoS One 6:e17018. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Algoe, S. B., Haidt, J., and Gable, S. L. (2008). Beyond reciprocity: gratitude and relationships in everyday life. Emotion 8, 425–429. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Bortolotti, A. (2012). Detection of the facial expression of emotion and self-report measures in empathic situations are influenced by sensorimotor circuit inhibition by low-frequency rTMS. Brain Stimul. 5, 330–336. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Bortolotti, A. (2013). Emotional face recognition, empathic trait (BEES), and cortical contribution in response to positive and negative cues. The effect of rTMS on dorsal medial prefrontal cortex. Cogn. Neurodyn. 7, 13–21. doi: 10.1007/s11571-012-9210-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Bortolotti, A., and Gonzaga, L. (2011). Emotional face recognition, EMG response, and medial prefrontal activity in empathic behavior. Neurosci. Res. 71, 251–259. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2011.07.1833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Canavesio, Y. (2013). Emotional contagion and trait empathy in prosocial behavior in young people: the contribution of autonomic (facial feedback) and Balanced Emotional Empathy Scale (BEES) measures. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 35, 41–48. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.742492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Canavesio, Y. (2014). High-frequency rTMS on DLPFC increases prosocial attitude in case of decision to support people. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 82–93. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.861361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Fronda, G. (2020). The “gift effect” on functional brain connectivity. Inter-brain synchronization when prosocial behavior is in action. Sci. Rep. 10:5394. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-62421-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Fronda, G., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2019a). A gift for gratitude and cooperative behavior: brain and cognitive effects. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 14, 1317–1327. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Fronda, G., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2019b). Donate or receive? Social hyperscanning application with fNIRS. Curr. Psychol. 38, 991–1002. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00247-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Fronda, G., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2020). When gratitude and cooperation between friends affect inter-brain connectivity for EEG. BMC Neurosci. 21:14. doi: 10.1186/s12868-020-00563-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., Gatti, L., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2018). Cooperate or not cooperate EEG, autonomic, and behavioral correlates of ineffective joint strategies. Brain Behav. 8:e00902. doi: 10.1002/brb3.902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Lucchiari, C. (2006). EEG correlates (event-related desynchronization) of emotional face elaboration: a temporal analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 392, 118–123. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Pagani, S. (2015). Social hierarchies and emotions: cortical prefrontal activity, facial feedback (EMG), and cognitive performance in a dynamic interaction. Soc. Neurosci. 9, 82–93. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.977403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Pozzoli, U. (2007). Event-related oscillations (EROs) and event-related potentials (ERPs) comparison in facial expression recognition. J. Neuropsychol. 1, 283–294. doi: 10.1348/174866407X184789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2016). Competition in the brain. The contribution of EEG and fNIRS modulation and personality effects in social ranking. Front. Psychol. 7:1587. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2017a). Cooperation and competition with hyperscanning methods: review and future application to emotion domain. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 11:86. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2017.00086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balconi, M., and Vanutelli, M. E. (2017b). Interbrains cooperation: hyperscanning and self-perception in joint actions. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 39, 607–620. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1253666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2006). Are emotions natural kinds? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 1, 28–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boucsein, W. (2012). Electrodermal activity. 2nd Edn. New York: Springer Science + Business Media.

Google Scholar

Butler, J. (2011). Bodies that matter: On the discursive limits of sex. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Chaspari, T., Baucom, B., Timmons, A. C., Tsiartas, A., Del Piero, L. B., Baucom, K. J. W., et al. (2015). “Quantifying EDA synchrony through joint sparse representation: a case-study of couples’ interactions” in Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Acoustics, Speech and Signal Processing (ICASSP); April 19–24, 2015; 62, 960–971.

Google Scholar

Chauhan, B., Mathias, C. J., and Critchley, H. D. (2008). Autonomic contributions to empathy: evidence from patients with primary autonomic failure. Auton. Neurosci. Basic Clin. 140, 96–100. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2008.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, D., Yun, K., and Jeong, J. (2015). Decoding covert motivations of free riding and cooperation from multi-feature pattern analysis of EEG signals. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1210–1218. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, L. M., and Otter-Henderson, K. D. (2007). “Physiological measures” in Handbook of research methods in personality psychology. eds. R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, and R. F. Krueger (New York: The Guilford Press), 370–388.

Google Scholar

Duclos, R., and Barasch, A. (2014). Prosocial behavior in intergroup relations: how donor self-construal and recipient group-membership shape generosity. J. Consum. Res. 41, 93–108. doi: 10.1086/674976

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dumas, G., Lachat, F., Martinerie, J., Nadel, J., and George, N. (2011). From social behavior to brain synchronization: Review and perspectives in hyperscanning. IRBM 32, 48–53. doi: 10.1016/j.irbm.2011.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., and Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 221, 1208–1210. doi: 10.1126/science.6612338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flynn, F. J., and Brockner, J. (2003). It’s different to give than to receive: predictors of givers’ and receivers’ reactions to favor exchange. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 1034–1045. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.6.1034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froh, J. J., Sefick, W. J., and Emmons, R. A. (2008). Counting blessings in early adolescents: an experimental study of gratitude and subjective well-being. J. Sch. Psychol. 46, 213–233. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2007.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furmark, T., Fischer, H., Wik, G., Larsson, M., and Fredrikson, M. (1997). The amygdala and individual differences in human fear conditioning. Neuroreport 8, 3957–3960. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199712220-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliano, R. J., Skowron, E. A., and Berkman, E. T. (2015). Growth models of dyadic synchrony and mother-child vagal tone in the context of parenting at-risk. Biol. Psychol. 105, 29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Golland, Y., Arzouan, Y., and Levit-Binnun, N. (2015). The mere co-presence: synchronization of autonomic signals and emotional responses across co-present individuals not engaged in direct interaction. PLoS One 10:e0125804. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, J. R. (2004). Integration of emotion and cognitive control. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 13, 46–48. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00272.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guastello, S. J., Pincus, D., and Gunderson, P. R. (2006). Electrodermal arousal between participants in a conversation: nonlinear dynamics and linkage effects. Nonlinear Dynamics Psychol. Life Sci. 10, 365–399.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Ghazanfar, A. A., Galantucci, B., Garrod, S., and Keysers, C. (2012). Brain-to-brain coupling: a mechanism for creating and sharing a social world. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., and Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion: Cambridge studies in emotion and social interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Hernandez, J., Riobo, I., Rozga, A., Abowd, G. D., and Picard, R. W. (2014). “Using electrodermal activity to recognize ease of engagement in children during social interactions” in Proceedings of the 2014 ACM International Joint Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous Computing ; September 13–17, 2014; 307–317.

Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Ehlis, A. C., and Fallgatter, A. J. (2003). Prefrontal activation through task requirements of emotional induction measured with NIRS. Biol. Psychol. 64, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(03)00095-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, M. J., Huter, T., Plichta, M. M., Ehlis, A. C., Alpers, G. W., Mühlberger, A., et al. (2008). Enhancement of activity of the primary visual cortex during processing of emotional stimuli as measured with event-related functional near-infrared spectroscopy and event-related potentials. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 28–35. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20368

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isen, A. M. (2009). “A role for neuropsychology in understanding the facilitating influence of positive affect on social behavior and cognitive processes” in The Oxford handbook of positive psychology. 2nd Edn. eds. C. R. Snyder and S. Lopez (New York: Oxford University Press), 503–518.

Google Scholar

Jones, G. R., and George, J. M. (1998). The experience and evolution of trust: implications for cooperation and teamwork. Acad. Manag. Rev. 23, 531–546. doi: 10.5465/amr.1998.926625

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koike, T., Tanabe, H. C., and Sadato, N. (2015). Hyperscanning neuroimaging technique to reveal the “two-in-one” system in social interactions. Neurosci. Res. 90, 25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konvalinka, I., Vuust, P., Roepstorff, A., and Frith, C. D. (2010). Follow you, follow me: continuous mutual prediction and adaptation in joint tapping. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 108, 8514–8519. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.497843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Konvalinka, I., Xygalatas, D., Bulbulia, J., Schjodt, U., Jegindo, E. -M., Wallot, S., et al. (2011). Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 63, 2220–2230. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016955108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lambert, N. M., Clark, M. S., Durtschi, J., Fincham, F. D., and Graham, S. M. (2010). Benefits of expressing gratitude: expressing gratitude to a partner changes one’s view of the relationship. Psychol. Sci. 21, 574–580. doi: 10.1177/0956797610364003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Davis, M., and Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. J. Affect. Disord. 61, 137–159. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00343-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, R. W. (1992). Autonomic nervous system differences among emotions. Psychol. Sci. 3, 23–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1992.tb00251.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, R. W., and Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy: a physiological substrate. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 234–246. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marci, C. D., Ham, J., Moran, E., and Orr, S. P. (2007). Physiologic correlates of perceived therapist empathy and social-emotional process during psychotherapy. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 195, 103–111. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000253731.71025.fc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCullough, M. E., and Tsang, J. A. (2004). “Parent of the virtues? The prosocial contours of gratitude” in Series in affective science. The psychology of gratitude. eds. R. A. Emmons and M. E. McCullough (New York: Oxford University Press), 123–141.

Google Scholar

Mick, D. G., and Demoss, M. (1990). Self-gifts: phenomenological Insights from four contexts. J. Consum. Res. 17, 322–332. doi: 10.1086/208560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mick, D. G., and Faure, C. (1998). Consumer self-gifts in achievement contexts: the role of outcomes, attributions, emotions, and deservingness. Int. J. Res. Mark. 15, 293–307. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8116(98)00006-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mirgorodsky, V. I., Dementienko, V. V., Gerasimov, V. V., Peshin, S. V., Dorokhov, V. B., and Shakhnarovich, V. M. (2013). Correlations between individual features of electrodermal response during viewing of the same videos. Int. J. Neurosci. Behav. Sci. 1, 5–12. doi: 10.13189/ijnbs.2013.010102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., Roepstorff, A., and Wallot, S. (2015). Building trust: heart rate synchrony and arousal during joint action increased by public goods game. Physiol. Behav. 1, 101–106. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, V., and Lindenberger, U. (2011). Cardiac and respiratory patterns synchronize between persons during choir singing. PLoS One 6:e24893. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadler, R. T., Rabi, R., and Minda, J. P. (2010). Better mood and better performance: learning rule-described categories is enhanced by positive mood. Psychol. Sci. 21, 1770–1776. doi: 10.1177/0956797610387441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1136930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., and Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: multilevel perspectives. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 56, 365–392. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Picard, R. W., Fedor, S., and Ayzenberg, Y. (2016). Multiple arousal theory and daily-life electrodermal activity asymmetry. Emot. Rev. 8, 62–75. doi: 10.1177/1754073914565517

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., and Kirkham, N. Z. (2007). The art of conversation is coordination: common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialogue: Research article. Psychol. Sci. 18, 407–413. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01914.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, P., and Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 988–999. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumble, A. C., van Lange, P. A. M., and Parks, C. D. (2010). The benefits of empathy: when empathy may sustain cooperation in social dilemmas. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 856–866. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.659

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schilbach, L., Wilms, M., Eickhoff, S. B., Romanzetti, S., Tepest, R., Bente, G., et al. (2010). Minds made for sharing: Initiating joint attention recruits reward-related neurocircuitry. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2702–2715. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinha, R., Lovallo, W. R., and Parsons, O. A. (1992). Cardiovascular differentiation of emotions. Psychosom. Med. 54, 422–435. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199207000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sohn, J. H., Sokhadze, E., and Watanuki, S. (2004). Electrodermal and cardiovascular manifestations of emotions in children. J. Physiol. Anthropol. Appl. Hum. Sci. 20, 55–64. doi: 10.2114/jpa.20.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinella, M. S. (2005). Prefrontal substrates of empathy: psychometric evidence in a community sample. Biol. Psychol. 70, 175–181. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tupak, S. V., Dresler, T., Guhn, A., Ehlis, A. C., Fallgatter, A. J., Pauli, P., et al. (2014). Implicit emotion regulation in the presence of threat: neural and autonomic correlates. NeuroImage 85, 372–379. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanutelli, M. E., Gatti, L., Angioletti, L., and Balconi, M. (2017). Affective synchrony and autonomic coupling during cooperation: a hyperscanning study. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017:3104564. doi: 10.1155/2017/3104564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanutelli, M. E., Gatti, L., Angioletti, L., and Balconi, M. (2018). May the best joint-actions win: physiological linkage during competition. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 43, 227–237. doi: 10.1007/s10484-018-9402-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanutelli, M. E., Nandrino, J. -L., and Balconi, M. (2016). The boundaries of cooperation: sharing and coupling from ethology to neuroscience. Neuropsychol. Trends 19, 83–104. doi: 10.7358/neur-2016-019-vanu

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: gratitude, heart rate, skin conductance level, biofeedback, hyperscanning

Citation: Balconi M and Fronda G (2021) Gratitude Affects Inter-Subjective Synchronicity for Cognitive Performance and Autonomic Responsiveness. Front. Psychol. 12:574983. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.574983

Received: 30 June 2020; Accepted: 01 February 2021;
Published: 24 February 2021.

Edited by:

Peter König, University of Osnabrück, Germany

Reviewed by:

Dan Zhang, Tsinghua University, China
Judith M. Burkart, University of Zurich, Switzerland

Copyright © 2021 Balconi and Fronda. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Fronda, giulia.fronda@unicatt.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.