Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychol., 28 January 2021
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19): The Impact and Role of Mass Media During the Pandemic View all 16 articles

Framing Messages to Deal With the COVID-19 Crisis: The Role of Loss/Gain Frames and Content

  • 1Psychology Department, Universidad de los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 2Laboratorio de Cognición, Universidad de los Andes, Bogotá, Colombia

The goal of this study was to test the role of message framing for effective communication of self-care behaviors in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic, contrasting health and economic-focused messages. We presented 319 participants with an unforced choice task where they had to select the message that they believed was more effective to increase intentions toward self-care behaviors, motivate self-care behaviors in others, increase perceived risk and enhance perceived message strength. Results showed that gain-frame health messages increased intention to adopt self-care behaviors and were judged to be stronger. Loss-framed health messages increased risk perception. When judging effectiveness for others, participants believed other people would be more sensitive to messages with an economic focus. These results can be used by governments to guide communication for the prevention of COVID-19 contagion in the media and social networks, where time and space for communicating information are limited.

Introduction

The pandemic caused by the novel coronavirus COVID-19 (World Health Organization [WHO], 2020a) during 2020, has resulted in hundreds of thousands of deaths around the world, and has consequently led governments to take extraordinary measures to face it. The development and commercialization of a vaccine for COVID-19 will be a long and expensive process. The cost of developing a vaccine for an infectious disease is estimated to be between 1.2 and 8.4 billion dollars (Gouglas et al., 2018), and the process to produce a licensed vaccine typically takes many years (Lurie et al., 2020). Therefore behavioral change and modification (i.e., hand washing, physical distance and staying home) is one of the main strategies to manage the pandemic (World Health Organization [WHO], 2020b). Governments have encouraged the population to adopt these behaviors through messages in mass media and social networks, hoping people would develop new habits and in doing so help reduce or postpone the number of contagions and the strain on health systems.

Behavioral economics has shown that using gain-loss frames to communicate information impacts decision making, risk perception, and behavioral intention (Kahneman, 2003). In the context of health communication, gain-framed messages emphasize the benefits or the positive outcomes that are accrued through adopting the behavior. On the other hand, loss-framed messages attempt to persuade by pointing at the negative consequences or costs incurred by not adopting the recommended behavior (Rothman and Salovey, 1997). Research in this field has found that, for example, gain-framed messages are more effective in motivating healthy eating behaviors (Roberto and Kawachi, 2014), while loss-framed messages are more effective to motivate breast self-examination (Williams et al., 2001) and to quit smoking (Nan et al., 2015).

Recently, several systematic and theoretical reviews have been conducted to propose how behavioral sciences could contribute to managing the COVID-19 outbreak (Lunn et al., 2020a; Van Bavel et al., 2020). However, the empirical evidence is scarce. Lunn et al. (2020b) used negative-framed messages to effectively motivate social distancing in Ireland. However, this study aimed to identify effects of communication strategies already implemented. So far there have been no studies to systematically identify the characteristics and structure that messages should have in order to effectively motivate population to change or adopt new behaviors such as frequent hand washing and physical distancing.

It is also plausible that cultural variation can play a role in the impact of these messages. We foresee two dimensions along which cultural differences could emerge. First, focusing on a particular content and/or frame might be more or less effective depending on particular countries and communities. Many citizens and national governments around the world have expressed concerns about the economic impact of public health measures implemented (McKee and Stuckler, 2020). Some even ponder whether the public health measures centered around stringent lockdowns could result in even worse consequences due to the psychological and economic consequences of unemployment, bankruptcies and social isolation (Singer and Plant, 2020; Tankersley, 2020). An open question is whether the public is also sensitive to these concerns and whether its effect would interact with a loss/gain frame. This question is especially relevant for societies where economies are more fragile and thus their citizens are more likely to be more responsive to economic concerns.

Second, personal and injunctive norms vary greatly between societies. Injunctive norms refer to perceptions of what others approve or encourage and have been shown to be closely related to personal intentions (Ball et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2012). There is evidence that misperception or underestimation of these social norms has an impact on engaging in behaviors falsely believed to be common or accepted in a group or community. Research and interventions based on social norms has mostly been done on alcohol and substance abuse related behaviors (McAlaney et al., 2011), but we believe it offers an interesting tool in the context of COVID-19. By identifying and assessing the gap between what one believes would be a good message for oneself versus others in one’s context, not only makes it possible to map out the extent of the misperception but also to intervene to correct it (Dempsey et al., 2018).

The objective of the present study was thus to evaluate the impact of gain-loss frames and the content of the message (health/economy) on self-reported motivation to engage in self-care behaviors (i.e., hand washing, physical distance, and staying home), engage others in the same self-care behaviors, risk perception of contagion and perceived message strength. The results of this study will help policy makers to design more effective messages to mitigate the impact of COVID-19.

In line with this objective and previous research, we expected that gain-frames were more effective to motivate low-risk behaviors (i.e., hand washing) while loss-frames were more effective to motivate high-risk behaviors like staying at home, because for many this latter behavior jeopardizes employment. Thus, the effectiveness of the message depends both on the content (economic versus health) and the frame (low risk behaviors might be more effectively framed as gains while high cost behaviors could be better framed as losses). The preregistration, hypotheses, analysis plan, materials, raw data, and scripts for analysis are available online at the Open Science Framework1, in line with best practice in reproducible science.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A convenience sample of 319 subjects (69.9% female, 30.1% male), ranging from 18 to 60 years of age (M = 27.01, SD = 9.37) participated in the study. Participants were originally contacted through student’s university mailing lists from different faculties (e.g., social sciences, engineering, medicine, basic sciences, among others) and from one to final year, and through Facebook postings, where a short description of the study was included. Our sample comprised participants from the main cities in Colombia, all native Spanish speakers.

The sample was divided randomly into two groups: Frame message group (gain/loss) (n = 160) and Content message group (economy/health) (n = 159). Sample size was decided based on a priori power analysis for a crossed random effects design, assuming a power of d = 0.35 (f = 0.175) for a power of 0.95 to detect simple effects (and of 0.70 for a two way interaction) (see Supplementary Material). Table 1 summarizes the basic demographic characteristics of the sample.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the sample.

Stimuli

We created eight messages related to consequences of following, or not, the self-care recommendations issued by public health authorities, so that half of these messages were gain-framed and the other half were loss-framed. In turn half the messages portrayed health consequences of the measures and the other half economic consequences. Each message was written in a white font on a black background to make them easy to read in either a mobile cellphone or a computer screen.

Design and Procedure

The experiment used a 2 Frame (gain/loss) × 2 Content (health/economy) both-within-condition design (Westfall et al., 2014). Dependent variables were: (a) intentions of self-care behaviors, (b) perceived efficacy to motivate others to perform self-care behaviors, (c) perceived risk, and (d) perceived message strength (i.e., attention, importance, consequences expressions, and perceived effective to engage in self-care behaviors) (Nan et al., 2015). We decided to use this design because a fully factorial design would have likely led to effects of practice and fatigue (Bradley, 2009; Gantiva et al., 2019). The study was conducted between April 19th and 28th of 2020.

A Web-based experiment was conducted in Spanish on the Qualtrics platform. After digitally signing the consent form, participants were randomly assigned to one of the experimental conditions (Frame or Content). In both conditions participants had to choose one of the messages in an unforced choice task, for four pairs of messages. In the Frame condition, the two messages of each pair had the same content (either both on Health or both on Economics) while the frame (Gain/Loss) was systematically varied. On the Content condition, the two messages of each pair shared the same frame but varied in content (always Health vs. Economics). The display order of the stimuli pairs and the location (right or left) of each message were randomly determined.

After seeing each pair of messages, participants had to choose the message of their preference for each dependent variable using a Visual Analog Scale (VAS) (see Figure 1). At the end of the experiment, participants responded to a set of questions on several demographic characteristics. The median duration time of the task was 10 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of message pairs in the Frame condition.

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the IRB of the University of los Andes (Approval #1169/2020), with written informed consent from all participants in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Results

We derived a score per message by calculating the difference between the indifference point in the scale (50) and the final position of the slider selected by each participant for each pair. For example, when comparing two health messages, one with a gain-frame (left side of Figure 1) and the other with a loss-frame (right side of Figure 1); a participant might have selected 90, choosing the target on the right. In this case, this means that the loss-framed health message got assigned a score of 40 (i.e., subtracting the indifference point from the score, in this example 90 minus 50) while the gain-framed health message got a score of zero. When participants chose the indifference point, both messages got a score of zero. Manipulation checks showed that participants indeed recognized gain/loss-framed messages as such (over 91% of participants for all messages). Results below include the whole sample, since excluding data based on the manipulation check did not result in any difference.

We derived a perceived message strength index by averaging the scores assigned to the messages across the questions on attention, importance, consequences and perceived effective to engage in self-care behaviors (Nan et al., 2015). A reliability analysis showed very good internal consistency for these items (Cronbach’s α = 0.85, McDonald’s ω = 0.87), as expected.

Analyses were conducted using the R statistical language (R Core Team, 2020). We fitted a series of linear mixed models with the lme4 library (Bates et al., 2015) and performed corrected pairwise comparisons with the Tukey method with the emmeans library (Lenth, 2020), as per preregistration. However, all models resulted in singular fits, suggesting overcomplex model structures (Barr et al., 2013). Therefore, we fitted the same nested models as generalized linear models, omitting the random effect term for participants. Overall results are summarized in Table 2 and Figure 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of models fitted for each dimension.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Interaction between frame and content for risk communication (Risk) and perceived strength (Strength) of the message. (B) Interaction between frame and content for judged impact of hand washing on others (Hands/others) and on oneself (Hands/self) and judged impact of isolation measures on others (Lockdown/others) and oneself (Lockdown/self).

The best model for each dimension, except perceived message strength, includes simple and interactive effects for the main experimental variables: Content Type, Frame, and Experimental Condition. For perceived message strength, there was also an effect of perceived socio-economic status, so that people with higher status tended to perceive messages as stronger. However, this coefficient is not significant. Including other variables, such as gender and age, did not improve the models fit (e.g., including them did not result in greater variance explained in our dependent variables).

There are main effects of both Content and Frame type for all dimensions examined except when judging how good the messages were at convincing others to wash their hands. In all models fitted, gain-frames and Health-themed messages were always considered better [bearing in mind that interpretation of the main effects can be problematic in the presence of interactions (Salkind, 2010)].

We will now focus on the two-way interaction between Content and Frame, the main point of the study. There is an overall effect of Content for all dimensions so that health messages were considered more effective, better at communicating risk, and better to persuade oneself and others, except when assessing the impact of health messages on others regarding the lockdown. Messages focused on health and with a gain-frame were generally perceived as stronger than both loss-framed health messages (MHealthGain = 22 vs. MHealthLoss = 17.4, z = −4.90, p < 0.001) and gain-framed economics messages (MEconomicsGain = 8.2, z = −12.80, p < 0.001), as can be seen in Figure 2A. On the other hand, loss-framed health messages were judged as better to communicate risk than any other type of message (all comparisons significant at p < 0.001).

The assessment of message impact on oneself and others is very similar for the two behaviors evaluated (hand washing and lockdown compliance). While in all cases the gain-framed health messages are considered more effective both for oneself and for others, the magnitude of the differences is smaller when judging the impact on others versus oneself. That is, people believed that others are more susceptible to economic-themed messages than they are. When considering the impact of messages on self-isolation measures (see Figure 2B), participants believe that other people are as receptive to gain-framed health messages as they would be to loss-framed economic messages (Figure 2B, lower right) while they considered themselves to be more influenced by gain-framed health messages and indifferent between loss and gain-framed economic messages (z = −1.30, p = 0.59). The pattern is similar for hand washing, but not as pronounced.

This pattern itself interacted with the experimental condition. This three-way interaction shows that the differences identified were stronger when the comparisons were between-content than when they were between-frames. That is, when people compared health versus economic messages, especially for gain-framed health messages, the differences tended to be larger than when the same message was paired with a loss-framed health message. This suggests an interesting joint evaluation effect, with potential real world repercussions (Hsee et al., 1999). We refrain from putting too much stock into this interpretation, since we did not make any predictions on this aspect of our design.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to evaluate the impact of message framing and content (health/economy) on several measures related to self-care behaviors (i.e., motivation to engage in self-care behaviors, engage others in the same self-care behaviors, risk perception of contagion and perceived message strength) in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. The results showed that gain-framed messages were more effective to generate motivation to engage in self-care behaviors and were perceived as stronger. On the other hand, loss-framed messages were more effective at increasing awareness of risks. We also found that health messages were overwhelmingly preferred for all the measures, even though there is a tendency to judge that others may be more susceptible to economic messages than oneself. Our results suggested that gain-framed health messages are more effective to motivate self-care behaviors, whereas loss-framed health messages are more effective to communicate the risk of contagion.

Contrary to our expectation, gain-framed messages were more effective to motivate both self-care behaviors (i.e., not only hand washing, but also staying home). This result may have occurred because self-care behaviors have been previously associated with the avoidance of contagion with COVID-19, and avoiding contagion is a form of gain. In short in length messages (like the ones used in this study), it is easier to execute the frame (gain or loss), with which the behavior has been previously associated. In a similar vein, since the spread of COVID-19 is already associated with negative consequences in public awareness, a loss-frame might be more effective to increase risk perception despite the short length of the message. Similar results were found previously in warning labels in the context of quit smoking (Goodall and Appiah, 2008; Nan et al., 2015).

In mass media and on social networks space is limited (e.g., the maximum length of a tweet is 280 characters); our study results thus suggest that messages designed in this kind of media to motivate self-care behaviors to avoid the spread of COVID-19 should use a gain-frame structure. Conversely, if the target is to improve risk perception, a loss-frame message will be more effective.

Health content messages had a greater impact on the main variables studied, however, participants tended to assess other people as more influenced by economic content than themselves. This result may indicate the beginning of a growing concern about the economic situation of the country, although reflected for now in the economic situation of other people (McKee and Stuckler, 2020; Singer and Plant, 2020; Tankersley, 2020). Because the participants of the present study belonged to a medium-high socioeconomic level, they have not yet experienced the negative economic consequences of the lockdown, but they are aware of the economic difficulties of other citizens through the media, social networks, and direct experience, especially in a country with a fragile economy. Notice that this study was conducted at a moment when the lockdown was the only widespread measure against the spread of the virus in the country and other measures that have proven successful had not been implemented (e.g. mask wearing), which needs to be factored in when judging the behavior of others (complying with the stay at home orders). This mismatch between preferences reported by participants and those attributed to others opens the way for potential interventions based on social norms feedback (Dempsey et al., 2018): since larger misperceptions tend to be associated with a greater likelihood of engaging in negative behaviors (e.g., in this context, not complying with self-isolation recommendations), message delivery based on adequately communicating true rates of observance of recommendations could be a promising strategy.

Governments are going to great lengths to communicate and persuade the general population of the best measures to prevent COVID-19 spread. Many of these messages are disseminated through mass media and social networks, where time and space are limited. The results of the present study suggest that gain-framed health messages are more effective to motivate people to engage in self-care behaviors, additionally, these increase the attention, perceived importance, consequences expressions, and perceived effectiveness (i.e., increase perceived message strength). Conversely, loss-framed health messages improve risk perception. This is especially important in some populations where risk perception is usually low (e.g., adolescents), and because recent studies have found that risk perception is significantly correlated with reported adoption of self-care behaviors to prevent COVID-19 contagion (Dryhurst et al., 2020; Lohiniva et al., 2020). These results may be used to develop more effective messages by policy makers.

The present study has several limitations. First, only self-report measures were evaluated, we do not have behavioral measures (e.g., behavior frequency) that allow us to corroborate the results. However, although several models identified that intention is not sufficient to adopt a new behavior, intention is a necessary step to adopt it (Prochaska, 2008; Schwarzer, 2008). Second, our results are not necessarily applicable to countries, regions or communities with different socioeconomic or cultural conditions. It is possible that high-income or very low-income countries or regions may have different responses to message framing and content. These results are then more likely to generalize to middle-income countries, in particular middle-class urban settings. Lastly, in a rapidly changing situation, our results offer only a snapshot of a set of concerns that evolve as the pandemic changes.

Data Availability Statement

The preregistration, hypotheses, analysis plan, materials, raw data, and scripts for analysis are available online at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/mxa3q/).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of the Universidad de los Andes (approval #1169/2020). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CG and WJ-L are responsible for the original design of the experiments, stimuli design, data analysis, and writing of the manuscript. JU-R contributed to materials design, conducted the experiments, and contributed substantially to data analysis and drafting of the manuscript. All authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Elisa Arond for comments on the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Materials are available at the Open Science Framework repository: https://osf.io/mxa3q/

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://osf.io/mxa3q/

References

Ball, K., Jeffery, R. W., Abbott, G., McNaughton, S. A., and Crawford, D. (2010). Is healthy behavior contagious: associations of social norms with physical activity and healthy eating. Intern. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Activ. 7:86. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-86

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., and Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. J. Mem. Lang. 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Statist. Softw. 67:105291. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M. (2009). Natural selective attention: orienting and emotion. Psychophysiology 46, 1–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00702.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dempsey, R. C., McAlaney, J., and Bewick, B. M. (2018). A critical appraisal of the social norms approach as an interventional strategy for health-related behavior and attitude change. Front. Psychol. 9:2180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dryhurst, S., Schneider, C. R., Kerr, J., Freeman, A. L. J., Recchia, G., van der Bles, A. M., et al. (2020). Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world. J. Risk Res. 23, 994–1006. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2020.1758193

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gantiva, C., Casas, M., Ballén, Y., Sotaquirá, M., and Romo-González, T. (2019). Modulation of physiological response to affective stimuli: a tool for psychological processes assessment. Univers. Psychol. 18, 1–12. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy18-1.mrfe

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodall, C., and Appiah, O. (2008). Adolescents’ perceptions of canadian cigarette package warning labels: investigating the effects of message framing. Health Commun. 23, 117–127. doi: 10.1080/10410230801967825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouglas, D., Le, T. T., Henderson, K., Kaloudis, A., Danielsen, T., Hammersland, N. C., et al. (2018). Estimating the cost of vaccine development against epidemic infectious diseases: a cost minimisation study. Lancet Glob. Health 6, e1386–e1396. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30346-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsee, C. K., Loewenstein, G. F., Blount, S., and Bazerman, M. H. (1999). Preference reversals between joint and separate evaluations of options: a review and theoretical analysis. Psychol. Bull. 125, 576–590. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.125.5.576

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am. Psychol. 58, 697–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenth, R. (2020). Emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means aka Least-Squares Means. R package Version 1.4.5. Available online at: https://cran.r-project.org/package=emmeans (accessed April 2020).

Google Scholar

Lohiniva, A.-L., Sane, J., Sibenberg, K., Puumalainen, T., and Salminen, M. (2020). Understanding coronavirus disease (COVID-19) risk perceptions among the public to enhance risk communication efforts: a practical approach for outbreaks, Finland, February 2020. Eurosurveillance 25:317.

Google Scholar

Lunn, P. D., Belton, C. A., Lavin, C., McGowan, F. P., Timmons, S., and Robertson, D. A. (2020a). Using Behavioral Science to help fight the Coronavirus. J. Behav. Public Administ. 3, 1–15. doi: 10.30636/jbpa.31.147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lunn, P. D., Timmons, S., Barjaková, M., Belton, C. A., Julienne, H., and Lavin, C. (2020b). Motivating Social Distancing During the Covid-19 Pandemic: An Online Experiment. Available online at: https://www.esri.ie/pubs/WP658.pdf (accessed May 25, 2020).

Google Scholar

Lurie, N., Saville, M., Hatchett, R., and Halton, J. (2020). Developing Covid-19 vaccines at pandemic speed. New Eng. J. Med. 2020:NEJM2005630. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp2005630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAlaney, J., Bewick, B., and Hughes, C. (2011). The international development of the “Social Norms” approach to drug education and prevention. Drugs Educ. Prevent. Policy 18, 81–89. doi: 10.3109/09687631003610977

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKee, M., and Stuckler, D. (2020). If the world fails to protect the economy, COVID-19 will damage health not just now but also in the future. Nat. Med. 26, 640–642. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-0863-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nan, X., Zhao, X., Yang, B., and Iles, I. (2015). Effectiveness of cigarette warning labels: examining the impact of graphics, message framing, and temporal framing. Health Commun. 30, 81–89. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.841531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O. (2008). Decision making in the transtheoretical model of behavior change. Med. Dec. Mak. 28, 845–849. doi: 10.1177/0272989X08327068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Google Scholar

Roberto, C. A., and Kawachi, I. (2014). Use of psychology and behavioral economics to promote healthy eating. Am. J. Prevent. Med. 47, 832–837. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothman, A. J., and Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: the role of message framing. Psychol. Bull. 121, 3–19. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salkind, N. (2010). Encyclopedia of Research Design, Vol. 1. Thousand Oak, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Schwarzer, R. (2008). Modeling health behavior change: how to predict and modify the adoption and maintenance of health behaviors. Appl. Psychol. 57, 1–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00325.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, P., and Plant, M. (2020). When Will the Pandemic Cure Be Worse Than the Disease?. Project Syndicate. Available online at: https://www.project-syndicate.org/commentary/when-will-lockdowns-be-worse-than-covid19-by-peter-singer-and-michael-plant-2020-04 (accessed May 25, 2020).

Google Scholar

Smith, J. R., Louis, W. R., Terry, D. J., Greenaway, K. H., Clarke, M. R., and Cheng, X. (2012). Congruent or conflicted? The impact of injunctive and descriptive norms on environmental intentions. J. Environ. Psychol. 32, 353–361. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tankersley, J. (2020). Job or Health? Restarting the Economy Threatens to Worsen Economic Inequality. New York, NY: The New York Times.

Google Scholar

Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., et al. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 4, 460–471. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westfall, J., Kenny, D. A., and Judd, C. M. (2014). Statistical power and optimal design in experiments in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli. J. Exper. Psychol. Gen. 143, 2020–2045. doi: 10.1037/xge0000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, T., Clarke, V., and Borland, R. (2001). Effects of message framing on breast-cancer-related beliefs and behaviors: the role of mediating factors. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 31, 925–950. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2001.tb02656.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2020a). Naming the Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) and the Virus that Causes it. Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2020b). Rolling Updates on Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19). Geneva: WHO.

Google Scholar

Keywords: framing effects, health communication, COVID-19, risk perception, behavioral science

Citation: Gantiva C, Jiménez-Leal W and Urriago-Rayo J (2021) Framing Messages to Deal With the COVID-19 Crisis: The Role of Loss/Gain Frames and Content. Front. Psychol. 12:568212. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.568212

Received: 31 May 2020; Accepted: 05 January 2021;
Published: 28 January 2021.

Edited by:

Francisco Esteves, Mid Sweden University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Victoria Team, Monash University, Australia
Cesar A. Acevedo-Triana, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States

Copyright © 2021 Gantiva, Jiménez-Leal and Urriago-Rayo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carlos Gantiva, c.gantiva@uniandes.edu.co; cgantiva@gmail.com; William Jiménez-Leal, w.jimenezleal@uniandes.edu.co

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.