Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 24 February 2021
Sec. Cultural Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Untangling Cultural Influences on Human Cognition: Integrating Evidence across Cultural Contexts and Methodological Approaches View all 15 articles

There and Back Again: A (Reversed) Vygotskian Perspective on Digital Socialization

  • 1School of Psychology, HSE University, Moscow, Russia
  • 2Institute for Social Sciences, RANEPA, Moscow, Russia

Introduction

Among the most important current advances of the contemporary material and non-material culture is the development of digital technologies as new tools or instruments of the human mind. Their reciprocal influence on the human mind is, in turn, becoming one of the front edges of research in both personality and cognitive psychology and neighboring disciplines, such as philosophy of mind, neurobiology, and cultural anthropology. The unprecedented speed of technological development, together with the merging of digital and wireless technologies with human daily routines across the planet, makes this research quite complicated. One of the first-priority tasks for psychology is to develop methodology which could become a foundation for the study of co-development of human mind and digital technologies, including both common-use devices and applications for data generation, storage, and processing.

Vygotsky's Instrumental Method and Mediation

Over half a century earlier, the founder of cultural-historical psychology Lev Vygotsky proposed a framework to understand cultural development of human higher mental (psychological) functions (Vygotsky, 1978; Cole, 1996). As one of the core concepts, he introduced a concept of cultural mediation as acquisition of new cognitive tools (such as concepts, or digits, or mnemonics) from adults within a so-called zone of proximal development. The latter refers to a gap between a child's unaided performance in a certain new domain and performance supported by an experienced adult. Mediation transforms the structure of human mental functions, so that psychological tools become the essential part of the entire cognitive system, and “limitlessly broadens the range of activities within which the new psychological functions may operate” (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 55).

In his paper entitled The instrumental method in psychology, Vygotsky wrote: “In human behavior, we can observe a number of artificial means aimed at mastering one's own psychological processes. These means can be conditionally called psychological tools or instruments… Psychological tools are artificial and intrinsically social, rather than natural and individual. They are aimed at controlling human behavior, no matter someone else's or one's own, just as technologies are aimed at controlling nature” (Vygotsky, 1982, vol. 1, p. 103, my translation). What is crucial, these tools emerge similarly in human evolution and in individual development and are “inherited” from social environment through the process called socialization. In this paper, I will demonstrate that the idea of the instrumentality of human mind might be fruitful for the understanding of our mind's transformation together with new technologies through digitally mediated socialization.

In Vygotsky's cultural-historical psychology, the acquisition of means allowing to control one's mental functions and behaviors as the core part of a child's socialization is described as internalization, or transformation of externalized social forms of these functions into internal. According to Vygotsky, higher psychological functions are first divided or shared between a child and an adult. What the child cannot do on her own, she can perform together with an adult who meanwhile introduces cultural tools which mediate cognition and performance (e.g., words for naming, indicating and categorizing; numbers for counting; mnemonic tools for remembering, etc.). Mental operations not yet available to a child on her own but available together with an adult define the child's zone of proximal development. Then, these culturally inherited tools of the mind (cf.: Bodrova and Leong, 2007) are internalized through the course of development to become tools of an individual mind.

Vygotskian Framework In The Modern Context

In our days, the Vygotskian framework is becoming more and more popular in the developmental cognitive science. This tendency dates back to the publication of Vygotsky's Mind and Society in English (Vygotsky, 1978), immediately followed by a resonant article by Toulmin Mozart in Psychology (Toulmin et al., 1978). Vygotsky's ideas about cognitive development in general and distributed cognition in particular found further elaboration, first of all, in the area of evolutionary cognitive science, for example, in the studies of human communication, joint attention, and shared intentionality (Tomasello and Carpenter, 2007), and in the so-called neuroarchaeology, a new interdisciplinary research area which aims at studying how human brain and mind change through the historical development of material tools and environment (e.g., Malafouris, 2013).

Nowadays, the world and in particular technologies change faster than one could imagine, providing new forms of support for our memory, spatial navigation, attentional orienting, visual search, emphasizing an urgent need to investigate how human mind develops and changes in a culture which does not remain unchangeable (Cole and Packer, 2016), and in which digital technologies play a central role. Its current development could be best described as creating new artifact ecologies (Bødker and Klokmose, 2011), or “environments where multiple heterogenous technologies co-exist and are interlinked as a unified system” (Vasiliou et al., 2015, p. 55). What's remarkable about human artifact ecologies now is digitalization of social interactions, which Vygotsky considered a necessary condition of socialization. Or, to take a somewhat different perspective, from the very birth a child is embedded in a sociotechnical system, allowing to master “a cultural tool kit,” which nowadays includes new digital tools together with older ones, such as literacy or numeracy (Pea and Cole, 2019).

Mediation, Internalization, and Digital Technologies

I argue that the current boost of digital technologies causes two remarkable reversals in the course of cognitive development as outlined in Vygotsky's “cultural-historical psychology.”

First, according to Vygotsky, for centuries, the trend in both individual cognitive development and the cognitive evolution as a whole was from external to internal tools of the mind, e.g., from real knots to mental notes, or from chops on the wood to mental calculations. From the cultural-historical psychology viewpoint, socialization is internalization (see also: Pea and Cole, 2019). Today, human higher psychological functions are becoming mostly externalized again due to the use of new digital tools, such as reminders, web search instead of memory search, highlighted keywords which guide our visual attention, etc. In other words, the humanity moves back from internalization to what I would call “new externalization,” with digital tools becoming an integral part of our cognitive system. In philosophy, this phenomenon has been described as Extended Cognition (Clark and Chalmers, 1998). From the psychological viewpoint, this all means reconstruction of the system of higher psychological functions through the digitally mediated activity (cf. Kaptelinin and Nardi, 2009). What is unique about digital technologies is that “extension” goes far beyond the tool (device or application) itself and is unavailable without it. In other words, such tools provide extended access rather than just support, or scaffolding. As a consequence, the borders between one's cognitive system and a technical device become blurred, with no clear understanding where, for instance, one's memory ends and a distributed world-wide web memory begins. This, in turn, influences how we remember and recall when our memory is not externally scaffolded (Ward, 2013). Such changes have also been reflected upon within the Embodied Cognition framework, as a result of “off-load of cognitive work onto the environment”(Wilson, 2002, p. 626). It is one of the reasons why this framework might be fruitfully integrated with cultural-historical approach (Zhang et al., 2018).

A similar reorganization of cognition by cultural practices has been demonstrated in multiple domains of human cognition and organization of movement, for a multitude of cultural practices, starting from counting and reading. These changes, in turn, lead to the reorganization of functional systems in the brain, which can be revealed not only by functional neuroimaging (for counting, see, e.g., Hanakawa et al., 2003), but even in the volumetric changes of specific brain structures and tracts. Surprisingly, Vygotsky discussed these neural changes driven by cultural practices over eight decades ago, when he introduced his principle of signification in human behavior: “… a man introduces artificial stimuli, signifies behavior, and by means of the signs creates new connections in the brain from outside. Admitting this, we presume a new regulatory principle of behavior, a new understanding of determination of human reactions, namely a principle of signification. A man creates externally new connections in the brain, controls his brain and thus controls his body” (Vygotsky, 1982, vol. 3, p. 91, my translation). Now, these structural changes and the human brain plasticity are becoming a major point of interest in a so-called cultural neuroscience (e.g., Kim and Sasaki, 2014) across a wide variety of cultural practices, such as music (Gärtner et al., 2013; etc.), chess (Hänggi et al., 2014), and many others. They have been mostly studied in adult learners, but it is more than plausible, especially in the light of association between the brain structural changes and the age of the training onset (for music, see Vaquero et al., 2016), that they start from birth, as soon as the child finds herself in the social environment forcing to acquire a variety of cultural practices.

Second, the zone of proximal development is also being transformed by digital devices, because children now mostly master these devices on their own, without joint activities with adults, and the adults don't even need to share their skills and experience. The situation has obviously changed during the last three decades when the zone of proximal development in computer-mediated education was extensively discussed (Crook, 1991). Moreover, sometimes children are much ahead of their parents and teachers in their use of tablets, mobile apps, etc.

To make this point more straightforward, let's consider the standard sequence of a certain cultural practice or mental tool acquisition during socialization, as outlined by Vygotsky, who has distinguished four steps in this process. First, an adult applies a practice to a child, e.g., saying “We're back home, wash your hands” or holding a child's visual attention on a way through a labyrinth in an illustrated magazine using the child's index finger. Second, a child applies it back to the adult. It's easy to imagine a child saying to her mom: “Mommy, we're back home, wash your hands!” Third, a child applies the practice to herself in a loud speech: “We're back home, I go wash my hands!” Finally, the practice becomes fully mastered by a child, or internalized, and no external mediation is needed any more: the child just goes to the bathroom to wash her hands after coming back home. What's clearly seen in this example is a common vector of the child's development: from a shared externalized function to the private internalized one. Just the same can be observed, for example, for mediated remembering or counting.

With the introduction of digital devices, the developmental trajectory of digital natives becomes less predictable for the previous generation, diverging from the above-mentioned inter-individual to intra-individual trend described by Vygotsky. For ages, all children were being born into a “wholly and completely socially mediated” reality, in Vygotsky's words. Now, it's also a digitally mediated reality which begins shaping a child's life quite early. According to the recent USA statistical data, by the age of 1, almost all children from low-income families were already exposed to their parents' mobile phones. By the age of 2, they used the devices without the adult involvement. By the age of 4, about 75% of children had their own mobile devices (Kabali et al., 2015).

Of course, digital devices and applications do incorporate certain social and/or cultural practices, as well as any other cultural artifact, such as a spoon, forcing a baby to use it in a certain culturally determined way, transforming her natural movements. However, with the new digital technologies, the adult is no longer necessary as an instructor, and the child's attention, memory, cognition, and activity are being shaped and organized by the interaction with the device itself.

May I emphasize that, whereas some of our cultural practices are just behavioral, the other are linked to material objects, which guide their use by new members of a certain culture. To describe this sort of guidance, Malafouris (2013) in his Material Engagement Theory of cultural evolution introduces a term material (enactive) sign, clearly opposing his understanding to Vygotsky's who contrasts material tools and signs: “We should not expect to find many similarities with tools in those means of adaptation we call signs” (Vygotsky, 1978, p. 53). For Malafouris, who criticizes the above-mentioned principle of signification and the representational nature of the first human artifacts, material sign is “a constitutive part of what it expresses” (Malafouris, 2013, p. 116), which “can be engaged in real space and time” (Malafouris, 2013, p. 117). However, this makes Malafouris' approach more complementary than opposing to Vygotsky's approach, together with their common understanding of human as an “artificial” being, ever developing through both material and social engagement (Theiner and Drain, 2017).

Extending Malafouris' ideas, I would hypothesize that the new gadgets become material signs of a sort for the developing generation, embodying new digital affordances, just as choppers, primitive material tools, embodied specific affordances and prompted the further actions on them at the earlier stages of human evolution. On the one hand, this matches well the proposal of radical embodied cognitive neuroscience “to think of cognitive function in the brain as context-sensitive” (Kiverstein and Miller, 2015), or driven by available affordances. On the other hand, this means that the very concept of the zone of proximal development requires reconsideration, so that it could incorporate not only human-human (child-adult), but also human-computer interaction per se. The more general perspective at the zone of proximal development assumes that it becomes bidirectional. A child might well act in the adult's “zone of proximal development,” teaching the adult to use a certain digital device or a service, with a previous step (an adult instructing a child) missing in the sequence. This transformation echoes Mead's definition of “prefigurative [cultures] in which adults learn also from their children” (Mead, 1970, p. 31).

Conclusions

New digital technologies apparently challenge the cultural-historical approach toward cognitive development. However, the constructivist nature of this approach and the concept of cultural mediation might provide new insights on extended cognition and its evolution.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

The work was supported by the HSE Program of Fundamental Studies (2020).

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Professor Alexander Asmolov for fruitful discussions of the topic, and both reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions which have substantially broadened my view of the field.

References

Bødker, S., and Klokmose, C. N. (2011). The human-artifact model – an activity theoretical approach to artifact ecologies. Hum. Comput. Interact. 26, 315–371. doi: 10.1080/07370024.2011.626709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodrova, E., and Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the Mind: The Vygotskian Approach to Early Childhood Education, 2nd Edn. Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice Hall.

Google Scholar

Clark, A., and Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis 58, 7–19. doi: 10.1093/analys/58.1.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. (1996). Cultural Psychology: A Once and Future Discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cole, M., and Packer, M. (2016). “A bio-cultural-historical approach to the study of development,” in Advances in Culture and Psychology: Vol. 6. Handbook of Advances in Culture and Psychology, eds M. J. Gelfand, C. Y. Chiu, and Y. Y. Hong (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 1–75. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190458850.003.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crook, C. (1991). Computers in the zone of proximal development: implications for evaluation. Comput. Educ. 17, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/0360-1315(91)90075-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gärtner, H., Minnerop, M., Piperhoff, P., Schleicher, A., Zilles, K., Altenmuller, E., et al. (2013). Brain morphometry shows effects of long-term musical practice in middle-aged keyboard players. Front. Psychol. 4:636. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanakawa, T., Honda, M., Okada, T., Fukuyama, H., and Shibasaki, H. (2003). Neural correlates underlying mental calculation in abacus experts: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroimage 19, 296–307. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00050-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hänggi, J., Brutsch, K., Siegel, A. M., and Jancke, L. (2014). The architecture of the chess player's brain. Neuropsychologia 62, 152–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kabali, H. K., Irigoyen, M. M., Nunez-Davis, R., Budacki, J. G., Mohanty, S. H., Leister, K. P., et al. (2015). Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics 136, 1044–1050. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaptelinin, V., and Nardi, B.A. (2009). Acting With Technology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Kim, H. S., and Sasaki, J. Y. (2014). Cultural neuroscience: biology of the mind in cultural context. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 64, 487–514. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiverstein, J., and Miller, M. (2015). The embodied brain: towards a radical embodied cognitive neuroscience. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:237. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malafouris, L. (2013). How Things Shape the Mind: A Theory of Material Engagement. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mead, M. (1970). Culture and Commitment: A Study of the Generation Gap. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press.

Google Scholar

Pea, R., and Cole, M. (2019). The living hand of the past: the role of technology in development. Hum. Dev. 62, 14–39. doi: 10.1159/000496073

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theiner, G., and Drain, C. (2017). What's the matter with cognition? A ‘Vygotskian' perspective on material engagement theory. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 16, 837–862. doi: 10.1007/s11097-016-9482-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., and Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10, 121–125. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toulmin, S. (1978), The Mozart of psychology. N. Y. Rev. Books 25, 51–57. doi: 10.2307/2214330.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaquero, L., Hartmann, K., Ripollés, P., Rojo, N, Sierpowska, J., François, C., et al. (2016). Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of musical training onset. Neuroimage 126, 106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasiliou, C., Ioannou, A., and Zaphiris, P. (2015). “An artifact ecology in a nutshell: a distributed cognition perspective for collaboration and coordination,” in Human-Computer Interaction – INTERACT 2015. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, Vol. 9297, eds J. Abascal, S. Barbosa, M. Fetter, T. Gross, P. Palanque, and M. Winckler (Cham: Springer), 55–72. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-22668-2_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and Society. The Development of Higher Psychological Processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Collected Works (in 6 Volumes). Moscow: Pedagogika.

Google Scholar

Ward, A. F. (2013). Supernormal: how the internet is changing our memories and our minds. Psychol. Inq. 24, 341–348. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2013.850148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/BF03196322

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, X., Wang, H., and Guo, D. (2018). Embodied cognition from the perspective of Vygotsky's socio-cultural theory. Philos. Study 8, 362–367. doi: 10.17265/2159-5313/2018.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cultural-historical psychology, digital technologies, mediation, zone of proximal development, cognitive development

Citation: Falikman M (2021) There and Back Again: A (Reversed) Vygotskian Perspective on Digital Socialization. Front. Psychol. 12:501233. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.501233

Received: 28 September 2019; Accepted: 03 February 2021;
Published: 24 February 2021.

Edited by:

Eirini Mavritsaki, Birmingham City University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Sabela Fondevila, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
Panagiotis Rentzelas, Birmingham City University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2021 Falikman. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria Falikman, bWZhbGlrbWFuJiN4MDAwNDA7aHNlLnJ1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.