Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 January 2021
Sec. Cultural Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Social Convergence in Times of Spatial Distancing: The Role of Music During the COVID-19 Pandemic View all 45 articles

Uses and Perceptions of Music in Times of COVID-19: A Spanish Population Survey

  • 1Department of Education and Specific Didactics, University Jaume I, Castelló de La Plana, Spain
  • 2Department of Didactics of Musical, Plastic and Corporal Expression, University of the Basque Country, Bilbao, Spain
  • 3Department of Pedagogy, University Jaume I, Castelló de La Plana, Spain

Since March 14, 2020, Spanish citizens have been confined to their homes due to the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic. Participating in musical activities has been associated with reduced anxiety and increased subjective wellbeing. The aim of this study is to analyze how Spanish citizens used music during the lockdown period. We also study perceptions of the impact music has in everyday life, in particular examining the respondents’ insights into the effects of listening to music in situations of isolation. The study was conducted using the MUSIVID19 questionnaire administered to a total of 1868 Spanish citizens. The results indicate that during lockdown, respondents perceived an increase in the time they devoted to musical activities such as listening, singing, dancing or playing an instrument. The participants also reported using music to cope with the lockdown, finding that it helped them to relax, escape, raise their mood or keep them company. The findings suggest an improvement in their perception of the value of music in personal and social wellbeing during the lockdown. However, the study reveals significant differences in the use and perceptions of music according to respondents’ personal situations. Age and feelings of vulnerability may lead to more conservative uses of musical practice and to more moderate perceptions of the positive values of music.

Introduction

The new COVID-19 pandemic is having a profound effect on the lives of people around the world. Since December 2019, many countries have declared a state of emergency as a result of the health crisis and have imposed various confinement measures on their populations to prevent the spread of the disease. These measures were taken during the middle of March 2020 in most European countries, many of which remain under quarantine at the time of writing. Spain imposed a lockdown on March 16th that confined people to their homes, implemented social distancing measures and closed all businesses, with the exception of those essential to the country’s supply chains (Tobías, 2020). Two weeks later, on March 30th, a more restrictive lockdown was brought in, aimed at reducing mobility and non-essential industrial activity countrywide (Mitjà et al., 2020).

Europe has been heavily impacted by the virus, and this had repercussions not only on citizens’ physical health, but also on social and psychological aspects. Whereas confinement has been shown to have a positive effect in the spread of the disease, it has been reported to have a negative impact on people’s mental health (Liu, 2020; Wang C. et al., 2020; Wang G. et al., 2020; Xiang et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2020). Some of the effects analyzed in people under confinement included confusion, anger, fear, frustration, boredom or post-traumatic stress symptoms (Brooks et al., 2020). Furthermore, confinement causes an increase in negative emotions, such as anxiety, depression and indignation, and of sensitivity to social risks, while decreases are reported in positive emotions such as happiness and life satisfaction (Li S. et al., 2020).

COVID-19 has therefore affected psychological wellbeing. While we acknowledge the complexity of defining the term wellbeing (Ryff, 1989), we understand the construct as a combination of feeling good and functioning effectively in one’s individual and social life (Deci and Ryan, 2008; Huppert, 2009). In Spain, several studies analyzed the impact of COVID-19 on people’s psychological wellbeing during the initial stages of the pandemic. González-Sanguino et al. (2020) and Ozamiz-Etxebarria et al. (2020) conducted online surveys that revealed high levels of depression, anxiety and post-traumatic stress disorders in their samples. Morales-Vives et al. (2020) found variations in the psychological consequences of lockdown according to certain variables, such as gender, age and marital status, and also to personality traits such as subjective happiness, life satisfaction, or higher extraversion. In turn, Rodríguez-Rey et al. (2020) acknowledge the importance of people’s work situation in the psychological impact of the pandemic.

From listening to music to any active participation in music making, individually or in groups, music has been associated with psychological wellbeing, reduced anxiety, lower levels of depression, or coping among people with health conditions and, in general, increased subjective wellbeing (Daykin et al., 2018). Music goes beyond the aesthetic dimension and is part of individuals’ identities and the communicative, cultural, leisure, and psychological aspects of their lives (Fritz and Avsec, 2007; Hormigos-Ruiz, 2010; Cross, 2014; Van der Hoeven, 2018). Participating in any kind of musical experience, in general, and music making, more specifically, can have an impact on positive emotions (Lamont, 2012; Croom, 2015; Campayo–Muñoz and Cabedo–Mas, 2017). These effects can increase when music occurs in a social context (Wuttke-Linnemann et al., 2019). Music has been widely used in psychological and therapeutic interventions to enhance physical and mental health (Peters, 1987; Bunt and Pavlicevic, 2001). The intentional use of music to improve psychological wellbeing has been studied in many areas and with different groups of people, such as those with severe diseases (Chirico et al., 2020; Li Y. et al., 2020), physical impairments (Grau-Sánchez et al., 2020; Hart et al., 2020), various mental health problems (Brancatisano et al., 2020; Greene et al., 2020), relational problems (Dunn et al., 2019; Dvir et al., 2020; Mossler et al., 2020), social or integration difficulties (Crawford, 2017; Henderson et al., 2017; Rodríguez-Sánchez et al., 2018), and also in interventions with different population groups such as pregnant women (Corey et al., 2019; Belloeil et al., 2020), children (Christian Gold et al., 2004; Hallam, 2010) or older people (Coffman, 2002; Creech et al., 2013), among others. Furthermore, for many years (Van de Wall, 1924) music has been used as a tool to increase psychological wellbeing in people experiencing situations of isolation or confinement; this area of interest is currently paramount in psychological and music research (Edri and Bensimon, 2019; Hjørnevik and Waage, 2019; Gold et al., 2020).

Beyond the research experiences of the intentional uses of music for therapeutic purposes, the natural choice to make music in order to raise mood is in many cases an inherent human trait (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007; Storr, 2015). The study of music in everyday life (DeNora, 2000; North et al., 2004) helps us to understand why and how people engage with music, and how participating in musical activities forms part of people’s daily experiences and, therefore, their identities (Frith, 1996). Music may help to consolidate people’s sense of identity, which is built on aspects that include understanding what place music has in people’s lives (DeNora, 2000), how people relate to music (Green, 2011), what the personal and social uses of music are (Small, 1998; MacDonald et al., 2002) and how musical preferences are shaped (Rentfrow et al., 2011). The perceived use people make of music (North et al., 2000; Schäfer et al., 2013) suggests that it is made and consumed naturally to encourage relationships by enhancing the possibility of sharing and connecting with others (Kokotsaki and Hallam, 2007; Cabedo-Mas and Díaz-Gómez, 2013; Schäfer and Eerola, 2020), for cognitive or rational intentions of aesthetic enjoyment (Brattico et al., 2009) and also for emotional regulation (Campayo–Muñoz and Cabedo–Mas, 2017). And in all cases, the musical experience can have an impact on wellbeing.

The few studies that have analyzed the uses of music among the Spanish population have mainly focused on youth and adolescents (Oriola-Requena and Gustems-Carnicer, 2015), or university students (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009). Following initial research by Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham (2007) on three major uses of music, namely emotional (music for emotional regulation), cognitive (rational appreciation of music) and background (music for social events, work or interpersonal interaction), Chamorro-Premuzic et al. (2009) conducted a study applying the first test of the psychology of music use in Spain to analyze how personal traits influence these uses of music.

The aim of this study is to analyze the uses Spanish people make of music during this period of confinement, and also to explore their views on the impact of listening to and making music on their perceived wellbeing.

Materials and Methods

Context and Procedures

The study was largely descriptive in character, since we were interested in collecting data on various aspects, dimensions and components of the studied phenomenon (Hernández et al., 2010). The data were gathered in a Spanish nationwide survey. The research was designed to investigate a series of aspects in relation to the use of music by Spanish people during the period of confinement. The questionnaire – MUSIVID19 – was hosted on a Google Form application, and administered and shared on social media and through university institutional channels. MUSIVID19 was open for responses during the 2-week period between April 3 and 18, 2020.

Researchers from two universities in different regions of Spain designed the questionnaire. Prior to administering the survey, the questionnaire was reviewed by ten experts from the fields of educational research methods, pedagogy, psychology, music and music education. A pilot was conducted with a sample of 20 responses, which led to several changes to the questionnaire before it was finally sent out. MUSIVID19 is provided as Supplementary Material.

The sample consisted of 1868 participants across all the autonomous communities of Spain. Although we did not control the ways in which the questionnaire was disseminated, the fact that it was accessible across social media, certain web pages and snowballing influenced the randomness of the sample and, therefore, its internal representativeness in relation to different population strata. Although missing data are minimal, around a maximum of 5%, the N = 1868 is not constant throughout the study since not all the participants answered all the questions. Of the respondents, 69.3% were women and 30.7%, men. Respondents under the age of 18 were excluded from the sample. Ages ranged from 18 to 84 years old and showed a trend toward the Gaussian distribution (Figure 1). The degree of symmetry is good as demonstrated by the mean age, 42.5 years (CI at 0.95: 41.9–43.2; with standard deviation of ±13.96 years), which is very similar to the median of 42 years. Just over 5% of the participants were over 65 years old; only six people were 80 years old and above.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sample composition according to age (N = 1868).

A large majority of the participants, 88.1%, lived with other people, and a little less than half, 43.1%, with minors. Almost three quarters of the sample, 74.8%, had university education levels, compared to 3.6% of the participants with primary school level or lower. Over half of the participants, 51.9%, were teleworking from home during the period of confinement; a further 10.1% were in face-to-face work, and the rest were not occupationally active for various reasons.

Measures

In addition to items requesting demographic information, the online survey consisted of three sections: (1) musical identities in confinement; (2) uses and knowledge of COVID-19 musical initiatives and; (3) perceptions about the impact of music. The survey included 22 questions, most of them requiring responses on a 5-point Likert-type scale, and some having a single or multiple choice response format.

Musical Identities in Confinement

This section was designed to collect information about the frequency people claim to listen to and participate in music, and about their perceptions of their current use of musical activities in comparison to times of no confinement. The survey gathered information about possible curiosity to broaden musical preferences during confinement by asking whether participants listened to a greater diversity of musical styles or discovered new groups.

Uses and Knowledge of COVID-19 Musical Initiatives

This section aimed to collect information about participants’ knowledge and use of the numerous music-related social initiatives that have emerged during the confinement period. The survey asked participants about their level of knowledge of musical projects aimed at promoting personal and social wellbeing led by professional and amateur musicians during this period, such as streaming open concerts, music campaigns, open access platforms, free online music lessons, collaborative music creation initiatives, etc. Information about their uses of these initiatives was also collected to provide a picture of what kind of initiatives were more popular. Lastly, the Spanish people quickly established a ritual applause from their balconies every day at 8 pm to show their gratitude to medical staff. This became an opportunity for people to share musical initiatives with the neighborhood from their balconies. The survey collected information about musical initiatives occurring in their neighborhoods during the applause and the level of engagement participants had in these activities.

Perceptions About the Impact of Music

This last section gathered information on the perceptions participants had about the power of music to promote aspects related to dimensions of wellbeing such as the capacity to enhance relief, trust, reduce anxiety or loneliness, to raise mood, to cope with the situation and to feel connected to other people. The survey also collected data about the impact this situation had on participants’ perception of the value of music, its potential to enrich free time, its influence on wellbeing, and the social value of musicians and the role of music in education.

Data Analysis

Comparative descriptive and inferential statistics were applied to analyze the questionnaire. The chi-square test was used to determine the existence of significant differences between the categorical variables. In addition, the effect size calculation was performed as an indicator of the magnitude of these differences. To do this, the effect size through the R2 scale was estimated from the square of Cramer’s W Index. In the variables with a Likert response format, the contrast was carried out with the parametric method ANOVA. The effect size value was also calculated, as a means to assess the magnitude of the differences and to allow their comparison with those found in other procedures. This was achieved by expressing this effect size in the form of R2, as before, which is now obtained from Cohen’s “d” conversion equation.

In the results section we focus only on aspects of the analysis of the data from MUSIVID19 that yielded significant results. The analysis also led to interesting findings when gender, level of education or home situation were controlled for, although these aspects go beyond of the scope of this paper.

Results

Descriptive Statistics

In the first section of the questionnaire, musical identities in confinement, and specifically from responses to item 1 (During this period of confinement, how often do you listen to music?), we observed that the vast majority of participants, 83.6% (N = 1561), reported listening to music every day during this period. In contrast, only 1.8% (N = 34) did so less than once per week, along with another 4.6% (N = 85) who said they listened to music 1 or 2 days per week. Among those who responded that they listen to music every day (item 2: If you selected “Every day,” approximately how many hours a day?), 45.6% (N = 712) selected between 1 and 2 h a day, followed by 31.3% (N = 489) who listen to music between 3 and 5 h a day. The two extreme categories, with the most and the least listening time, accounted for 11 and 12%, respectively. Regarding item 3 (During this period of confinement, have you listened to more diverse styles of music?), 73.6% reported listening to the same music as before confinement; the rest of the sample (26.4%) had discovered new musical styles. In relation to whether they had discovered new musical groups (item 4: Have you discovered new musical groups that you like during this period?), regardless of their previous answer, more than half of the participants (53.1%) affirmed that they had discovered new musical groups that they enjoyed listening to during this period.

In the same section dealing with perceptions about the uses of music, we reviewed the items related to different activities before and after confinement. The results suggest that almost all the respondents listened to as much music or more than before confinement (item 5: Consider that you listen to music…). Specifically, 49.7% say they listened to more music; only 3.7% mentioned that they listened to less. In turn, 8.7% reported singing less than before (item 6: Consider that you sing…). The rest sang as much (the majority, 65.0%) or even more than before confinement (26.3%). Regarding dancing (item 7: Consider that you dance…), 31.4% of participants danced more during confinement than before. While a little more than half (54.2%) danced as much as before, the rest (14.2%) danced less than previously. In reference to playing an instrument (item 8: Consider that you play an instrument…), more than half of the sample, 56.3%, said their activity had not changed since confinement began, while 30.1% of the participants spent more time performing music.

In the second section of the survey the questions relate to uses and knowledge of COVID-19 musical initiatives. Item 9 (Do you know of any initiatives or projects that musicians have undertaken to share or contribute to wellbeing during this period of confinement?) asks about social music-based projects or initiatives that contribute to wellbeing during the confinement period. Responses showed that 31.1% of the sample enthusiastically followed such initiatives, and another 24.2% were aware of them and had also created or participated in some of them. Only 6.6% had no knowledge of these projects. Regarding music performed in their neighborhood (item 10: In your neighborhood, are people making music on their balconies?), more than half of the sample (53.5%) reported that their neighbors were making music on the balconies.

The last section is related to perceptions about the impact of music. Regarding respondents’ perceptions of the value of music during confinement, slightly more than half of the participants (53.1%) stated that music greatly helped them to relax (item 11: Do you think music helps you relax?), and 26.8% considered it helped quite a lot; only 1% said that music did not help them to relax at all. Similar results emerged in responses to the following item, in which 56.0% of the respondents stated that music definitely gave them a way of escaping (item 12: Do you think of music as a form of escape?); and 25.2% answered that it helps them quite a lot. Along the same lines, just 1% responded negatively. Regarding the potential of music to affect mood (item 13: Do you think music cheers you up?), 62.9% reported that music greatly helped to cheer them up, and another 21.9% felt it helped them to positively change their mood. Only 0.2% answered that it did not help them at all. We found similar results in the following question (item 14: Does music keep you company?), to which 58.1% of the sample responded with the maximum score, and another 24.7% answered that music kept them company quite a lot. Regarding capacity to deal with the situation (item 15: Does music help you cope better with confinement?), more than half of the group (51.5%) indicated that music was a great help in coping with confinement, and 27.0% answered that it helped them a lot. Moreover, 77.4% of the sample reported that music boosted their confidence or sense of positivity (item 16: Does listening to music boost your confidence and positivity?), scoring 4 or 5 points in the question. Finally, slightly less than half of the participants (46.9%) stated that sharing music with other people made them feel much more connected (item 17: Does listening to or making music with other people make you feel more connected to them?) with these people, whereas 27.3% said it made them feel quite a lot more connected.

Continuing in the line of enhanced perceptions of music in confinement, (item 18: Do you think confinement has improved your perception of the value of music?), the most frequent responses were values 5 (very much) and 3 (neutral) (30.8 and 28.1%, respectively). For this reason, the average (3.48) suggests that confinement has led to a medium to high improvement in the perception of the value of music. Regarding the value of the work musicians do (item 19: Do you think confinement has improved your perception of the value of musicians’ work?), values 5 (33.8%), 3 and 4 were the most frequent responses; we can therefore conclude that the average (3.59) indicates a medium to high improvement in the perception of the work of musicians. As for the role of music in education (item 20: Do you think confinement has improved your perception of the role of music in education?), the distribution of the responses is similar to the previous ones, with 3 and 5 being the most frequently repeated scores. Therefore, again an average value of 3.64 indicates a medium to high perception. As regards the role of music in enriching free time (item 21: Do you think confinement has improved your perception that music enriches your free time?), a slight increase in value 5 responses (39.0%) raises the average to 3.77; it can therefore be claimed that perception of the possibilities of music to enrich free time has improved notably. Finally, regarding the influence on personal wellbeing (item 22: Do you think confinement has improved your perception of the influence music has on your personal wellbeing?), the mean value (3.78) indicates a notable improvement in this perception.

Table 1 summarizes the main results of the descriptive analysis organized by the three sections of the questionnaire.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive analysis in percentages (%).

Inferential Statistics

As this research is contextualized in the COVID-19 crisis, which has had a profound impact on the economy as well as on physical and mental health, we carried out an inferential study considering respondents’ current employment situation. This analysis showed that current employment circumstances have considerable implications for the subject studied. Employment situation is defined in five categories: two representing people who were in active employment: telework and face-to-face work; and three representing those who are not currently working: unemployed, furloughed, and retired people. People who were on holiday at the time only represented 2% of the sample and generated very low frequency values; they were therefore removed from the sample. Likewise, responses marked “none of the above” (11.6% of the participants) were eliminated as it was an undefined category. Taking this situation into account, and considering that 4.1% of the sample did not answer this question, the maximum N valid for this inferential analysis block is 1537, 82.3% of the total number of participants.

With the factor thus set out, in contrasting the first and second sections of general variables (Tables 2, 3), the results for almost all of them were statistically significant. Table 2 shows information about the first and second sections, except the comparison of the uses of music now and before confinement, for the sake of clarity. The results indicate statistical significance in all the variables, although with small effect sizes, except for the non-significance of neighbors making music on their balconies. As regards the rest, from the least to the most statistical relevance, the factor of musical diversity indicates that retirees, followed by those still active in face-to-face work, tended to listen to the same music as before. Respondents who most listened to music every day were those in the teleworking group, followed by furloughed workers. The furloughed group was the largest in terms of those who had discovered new musical groups they liked, whereas retired participants tended to listen to music they were familiar with and had not explored new styles of music. Regarding the respondents’ knowledge of music projects designed to help enhance wellbeing, the data indicates that retirees were either unaware of them or had only heard about them, whereas teleworking participants had the most knowledge about these initiatives and had also participated in some of them.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Comparative inferential analysis: Musical identities during confinement and uses and knowledge of COVID-19 musical initiatives.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparative inferential analysis: Uses of music during and before lockdown.

In the results of the comparison between uses of music during and before confinement (Table 3) only one variable, singing, was not statistically significant. However, significant results with small effect sizes were observed for some musical practices, such as listening to music, playing an instrument, or dancing.

The perceptions of the importance of music in lockdown (Table 4) varied according to the respondents’ employment situation. We found high statistical significance in all variables, some with moderate (e.g., greater connection with others) and notable (e.g., helps you cope with the situation) effect sizes. These results therefore provide solid evidence of the effect of the employment factor. Our analysis of the mean values revealed that these differences follow the same pattern as the previous items: the retirees have the lowest perception of the value of music as compared to the other groups.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Comparative inferential analysis: Perceptions of the impact of music (I).

Finally, the intersection with information from the second group of questions on the perceptions of the value of music revealed similar results (Table 5). The retirees scored lower average values. However, they were not statistically significant in all variables: neither the value of musicians’ work nor the role of music in education had significant values. The following variables were significant: (1) the value of music, (2) music enriches free time, and (3) the influence of music on personal wellbeing.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Comparative inferential analysis: Perceptions of the impact of music (II).

Discussion

COVID-19 has greatly impacted musical activities and, in many cases, has driven a decline of certain forms of musical consumption, first because the majority of concerts and cultural events have been cancelled during the pandemic (Agarwal and Sunitha, 2020), second due to the decrease of music in public spaces (Botstein, 2019), and third as a result of the decline in digital consumption of music (Sim et al., 2020), in many cases linked to people’s mobility and transit time or the time spent on retail and recreation, in parks, transit stations, and in workplaces.

Following methodological approaches to studying the use of music in people’s everyday lives (Craft et al., 1993; DeNora, 2000; North et al., 2000, 2004; Schäfer et al., 2013) the results of this study draw on testimonies of the perceived uses people make of music, and also the degree of impact they believe these activities have on their everyday lives. The data analyzed in this study reports subjective perceptions of the time people claim to devote to musical activities, such as listening, performing a musical instrument, singing or dancing.

According to the study’s findings, the Spanish population has turned to music during the present lockdown. The values in our results on uses of music, in the section analyzing musical identities in lockdown, reveal that a large percentage of the sample reported spending more time to music listening and making and show a keen interest in participating in cultural activities, specifically those of a musical nature.

Compared with previous research on the uses of music, Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found that people reported listening to music most regularly while traveling, followed by alone at home. Music consumption in these cases correlates strongly with background use of music (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2012) and in most cases it occurs outside the home. These results are consistent with the aforementioned study of Sim et al. (2020), who analyze the decrease in digital consumption of music during the pandemic. However, when the consumption of music occurs in public environments or as background music (Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham, 2007) people are less engaged with the music and have less intentional control over what they are listening to. In these cases, therefore, the usefulness of music for fulfilling logical mood management strategies may be not so obvious (North et al., 2004). This in turn affects people’s perceptions of their conscious use of music. Indeed, a previous analysis of the uses of music in everyday life (North et al., 2004) indicates that intentional exposure to music occurs mainly in the home. Our results coincide with this conclusion and indicate that as the amount of time spent at home increases, the perception of the intensity of the use of music also rises. At the same time, it is interesting to note that during confinement, while still remaining faithful to their musical preferences, a large number of people investigated new groups and styles of music. Discovering new music generates pleasure and is a stimulating choice. Variety, surprise and the discovery of the unknown are therefore essential factors in the enjoyment and appreciation of music (Hagendoorn, 2003). Devoting time and effort to explore new art has positive benefits (Tepper and Hargittai, 2009), which becomes more evident in difficult situations such as confinement.

During socially challenging circumstances, there is a practically unanimous consensus that music can be beneficial. Musical initiatives during the period of confinement, by both professional and amateur musicians, have fundamentally aspired to share music and raise the spirits of neighbors and society in general. These initiatives reflect the widespread perception among citizens that making music for other people contributes to their personal wellbeing (Daykin et al., 2018). In this way, the new musical initiatives to emerge in this period are essentially social in character, and go beyond aesthetic enjoyment. One heartening result to come out of this study, according to the analysis of the uses and knowledge of COVID-19 musical initiatives, is that practically all the survey respondents (93.4%) were aware of the musical initiatives for social purposes undertaken during confinement, and more than half (55.3%) followed or actively participated in them. Also, more than half of the surveyed population (53.2%) live in neighborhoods where residents have shared music on their balconies, and a quarter of them were actively involved in some of these proposals. These findings show that sharing music and musical practices is seen as an act of altruism and solidarity (Laurence and Urbain, 1999; Fukui and Toyoshima, 2014).

The data presented in this article show that Spanish society has a strikingly high perception of the value of music. The third section of the survey, perceptions on the impact of music, reveal that the perceived positive benefits of participating in artistic and musical activities for health, care and wellbeing, previously reported by health professionals (Wilson et al., 2016), is widely shared by society in general. Indeed, less than 1% of the sample perceived no benefit from such participation. The perception of the value of engaging in participatory musical activities has been studied in a range of settings (Kokotsaki and Hallam, 2007), and the literature reports benefits that coincide with this research: social improvements, such as a sense of belonging (Schäfer et al., 2013; Schäfer and Eerola, 2020) or altruism (Fukui and Toyoshima, 2014), and personal improvements, such as trust (Anshel and Kipper, 1988), positivity and mood regulation (Schäfer et al., 2013). Our results seem to suggest that the positive attributes of music were perceived as very high during lockdown. In addition, they show a rise in the value society now places on the social role of music, musicians and music education.

Although the conclusions drawn from the data are, in general, positive, this study also finds that people’s current employment situation has a significant direct influence on the uses and perception of the value of music. The groups that most reported listening to more music than before were the furloughed or teleworkers, while the unemployed and face-to-face workers claimed to listen to the same amount of music as before. This finding concurs with North et al. (2004) idea that most music is consumed at home, so those whose situation forced them to spend more time at home were consuming more music, while people whose day-to-day working practices remained the same did not change their cultural consumption behaviors to the same extent.

Although the pandemic is having a considerable impact for everyone in Spain, COVID-19 does not affect all citizens in the same way, as some people are more vulnerable to its effects on health, work activity, or individual or household economy. This differentiation has had a direct impact on the uses of music.

As a consequence of the health crisis, a large number of people are not in active work; many have been temporarily furloughed, and their return to work is dependent on economic recovery after the crisis (National Statistics Institute of Spain, 2020). It is precisely this group who, during confinement, have spent more of their time on musical activities such as listening, dancing or playing an instrument. These are also the people who have devoted most time to discovering new musical styles and groups, and are benefiting psychologically from exploring and finding new music (Hagendoorn, 2003; Tepper and Hargittai, 2009).

The oldest group in the sample, the retirees, differed in both their uses of music and their perceptions of its value. The elderly have been severely impacted by the pandemic to the point that those over the age of 65 account for 90.7% of deaths from COVID-19 in Spain (Spanish Ministry of Health, 2020). This situation inevitably causes a great deal of stress and feeling of vulnerability among the population. As a result, older people are generally dealing with stricter levels of isolation and, therefore, greater loneliness. As a protective measure against contagion, they are forced to stay at home and have little or no interaction with other people. Several studies have analyzed the age factor in aspects related to music, such as musical preferences (LeBlanc et al., 1996; Bonneville-Roussy et al., 2013), individual differences in music consumption (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2012), the perception of musical mode and rhythm (Halpern et al., 1998) or emotional judgments of music (Vieillard et al., 2012). Although previous studies have explored perceptions of the value of music among vulnerable people (O’Callaghan et al., 2014), its influence on the most vulnerable age group to the disease had not, to date, been examined.

In this case, our data reveal that older people in vulnerable situations spend time listening to music. Compared with studies that analyze how frequently older people listen to music in other countries (Laukka, 2007), Spanish people reported more frequent everyday music listening during the pandemic, although their use is less frequent than other age groups. They are also the group that has least increased the time they devoted to musical practices during lockdown. In a previous study, Chamorro-Premuzic et al. (2012) also identified age as a factor affecting significant effects of all music uses on music consumption. Likewise, the time they devote to musical practices is spent listening to and performing music that is already part of their cultural background and musical preferences, rather than discovering and getting to know other forms or musical styles. Finally, older people had a more positive view of the impact of music in personal wellbeing compared to previous studies (Laukka, 2007), particularly in aspects such as the power of music to improve mood, reduce feelings of loneliness, or encourage feelings of similarity and connection with others. However, although they scored positively, this group presents the least favorable data in the perception of the potential of music to contribute to psychological and social wellbeing. Chamorro-Premuzic et al. (2012) analyzed age as a factor influencing the emotional uses of music. Their findings supported those of a previous study by Chamorro-Premuzic et al. (2009), which found negative correlations between age and the emotional use of music in a Spanish study. In relation to the use of music for emotional regulation, our study presents new analysis of the perception of the value of music for psychological wellbeing, providing evidence of a relationship between these perceptions and conditions such as age or feelings of vulnerability. Our survey data showed the greatest divergence among the group of retired people and, consistent with the aforementioned studies (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009, 2012), older Spanish people scored lower in all the dimensions analyzing the perception of the value of music for wellbeing and positive emotions. In addition, our study goes a step further by showing that this group reported the lowest scores on the overall impact of the value of music to positively affect people’s lives during the pandemic.

In conclusion, the study reveals that music has played an important role in people’s lives during the lockdown and that aspects such as having more time at home, willingness to help others, and the feeling of facing a collective social problem, increase the use of musical practices and raise perceptions about the value of music in psychological and social wellbeing. However, age and, more specifically, the feeling of isolation or sense of vulnerability, can lead to more conservative use of musical practices and moderate perceptions of the positive value of music.

This research paints a broad picture of the uses and perceptions of music during lockdown, and invites reflections on how a major change in people’s everyday lives can influence their relationship with musical phenomena. The findings suggest several directions for future research.

First, although the number of participants reliably represents the target population, the sample is not internally representative of the Spanish population in strata such as gender or level of studies and could, therefore, introduce bias in analyzing patterns of uses and influence of music according to factors such as gender, level of studies or sociodemographic groups. Furthermore, the absence of a control group represents a methodological limitation in understanding actual use of music before and during the pandemic. As there are no previous studies analyzing the same aspects of music making and the perception of the value of music in psychological wellbeing among the Spanish population, it is difficult to observe causal directions of the pandemic in any dimensions of the findings. We believe that conducting similar research after the pandemic will offer new interesting information to map the actual effect the lockdown had on the use of music in everyday life. Lastly, we did not obtain data about the influence music has in children’s lives during lockdown. Now we are aware of the psychological effects the pandemic is having on children’s lives (Jefsen et al., 2020; Phelps and Sperry, 2020; Valenzuela et al., 2020), it is important to conduct further research in this area.

Second, we agree with North et al. (2004) that the respondents’ interpretations of their uses and perceptions of music may lead to inaccurate responses. We are aware that, in certain cases, the idea people have about the intention of and attention to their participation in music is not always unequivocally linked to the amount of time they actually devote to music listening or making. We consider that the importance of analyzing people’s perception of their use of music relies on understanding the role of music in everyday life in terms of how it shapes and reflects an individual’s identity; in considering music in everyday life as a way of organizing one’s internal and social world. And this, as the sociologist Frith (1996) argues, comes from the “imagined self.” However, we believe that future research could usefully enrich this analysis by taking a complementary experimental approach.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All the authors equally collaborated in designing the research, collecting and analyzing the data, and wrote collaboratively in writing the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities (grant number EDU2017-86311-P), by Jaume I University of Castellón, Spain, (grant number UJI-B2019-16), and by KideOn Research Group of the Basque Government, Ref.: IT1342-19 (A category).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606180/full#supplementary-material

References

Agarwal, V., and Sunitha, B. K. (2020). COVID – 19: current pandemic and its societal impact. Int. J. Adv. Sci. Technol. 29, 432–439.

Google Scholar

Anshel, A., and Kipper, D. A. (1988). The influence of group singing on trust and cooperation. J. Music Ther. 25, 145–155. doi: 10.1093/jmt/25.3.145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belloeil, V., Tessier Cazeneuve, C., Leclercq, A., Mercier, M. B., Legendre, G., and Corroenne, R. (2020). Impact of music therapy before first-trimester instrumental termination of pregnancy: a randomised controlled trial. BJOG Int. J. Obstetr. Gynaecol. 127, 738–745. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.16102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonneville-Roussy, A., Rentfrow, P. J., Xu, M. K., and Potter, J. (2013). Music through the ages: trends in musical engagement and preferences from adolescence through middle adulthood. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105:703. doi: 10.1037/a0033770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botstein, L. (2019). The future of music in America: the challenge of the COVID-19 pandemic. Music. Q. 22, 351–360. doi: 10.1093/musqtl/gdaa007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brancatisano, O., Baird, A., and Thompson, W. F. (2020). Why is music therapeutic for neurological disorders? The therapeutic music capacities model. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 600–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brattico, E., Brattico, P., and Jacobsen, T. (2009). The origins of the aesthetic enjoyment of music. A review of the literature. Music. Sci. 13, 15–39. doi: 10.1177/1029864909013002031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenburg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30460-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bunt, L., and Pavlicevic, M. (2001). “Music and emotion: perspectives from music therapy,” in Music and Emotion: Theory and Research, eds P. N. Juslin, and J. A. Sloboda, (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 181–201.

Google Scholar

Cabedo-Mas, A., and Díaz-Gómez, M. (2013). Positive musical experiences in education: music as a social praxis. Music Educ. Res. 15, 455–470. doi: 10.1080/14613808.2013.763780

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campayo–Muñoz, E. Á, and Cabedo–Mas, A. (2017). The role of emotional skills in music education. Br. J. Music Educ. 34, 243–258. doi: 10.1017/s0265051717000067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., and Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and music: can traits explain how people use music in everyday life? Br. J. Psychol. 98, 175–185. doi: 10.1348/000712606x111177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Gomà-i-Freixanet, M., Furnham, A., and Muro, A. (2009). Personality, self-estimated intelligence, and uses of music: a Spanish replication and extension using structural equation modeling. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 3, 149–155. doi: 10.1037/a0015342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Swami, V., and Cermakova, B. (2012). Individual differences in music consumption are predicted by uses of music and age rather than emotional intelligence, neuroticism, extraversion or openness. Psychol. Music 40, 285–300. doi: 10.1177/0305735610381591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., Maiorano, P., Indovina, P., Milanese, C., Giordano, G. G., Alivernini, F., et al. (2020). Virtual reality and music therapy as distraction interventions to alleviate anxiety and improve mood states in breast cancer patients during chemotherapy. J. Cell. Physiol. 235, 5353–5362. doi: 10.1002/jcp.29422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coffman, D. D. (2002). Music and quality of life in older adults. Psychomusicology 18:76. doi: 10.1037/h0094050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corey, K., Fallek, R., and Benattar, M. (2019). Bedside music therapy for women during antepartum and postpartum hospitalization. MCN. Am. J. Mater. Child Nurs. 44, 277–283. doi: 10.1097/nmc.0000000000000557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craft, S. D., Cavicci, D., and Keil, C. (1993). My Music: Explorations of Music in Daily Life. Middleton, CT: Wesleyan University Press.

Google Scholar

Crawford, R. (2017). Creating unity through celebrating diversity: a case study that explores the impact of music education on refugee background students. Int. J. Music Educ. 35, 343–356. doi: 10.1177/0255761416659511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creech, A., Hallam, S., McQueen, H., and Varvarigou, M. (2013). The power of music in the lives of older adults. Res. Stud. Music Educ. 35, 87–102. doi: 10.1177/1321103x13478862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Croom, A. M. (2015). Music practice and participation for psychological well-being: a review of how music influences positive emotion, engagement, relationships, meaning, and accomplishment. Music. Sci. 19, 44–64. doi: 10.1177/1029864914561709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, I. (2014). Music and communication in music psychology. Psychol. Music 42, 809–819. doi: 10.1177/0305735614543968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daykin, N., Mansfield, L., Meads, C., Julier, G., Tomlinson, A., Payne, A., et al. (2018). What works for wellbeing? A systematic review of wellbeing outcomes for music and singing in adults. Perspect. Public Health 138, 39–46. doi: 10.1177/1757913917740391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2008). Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J. Happin. Stud. 9, 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeNora, T. (2000). Music in Everyday Life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Dunn, H., Coombes, E., Maclean, E., Mottram, H., and Nugent, J. (2019). Music Therapy and Autism Across the Lifespan: A Spectrum of Approaches. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Google Scholar

Dvir, T., Lotan, N., Viderman, R., and Elefant, C. (2020). The body communicates: movement synchrony during music therapy with children diagnosed with ASD. Arts Psychother. 69:101658. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2020.101658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edri, O., and Bensimon, M. (2019). The role of music among prisoners and prison staff: a qualitative research study. Eur. J. Criminol. 16, 633–651. doi: 10.1177/1477370818775295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, S. (1996). “Music and identity,” in Questions of Cultural Identity, eds S. Hall, and P. Du Gay, (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 108–128. doi: 10.4135/9781446221907.n7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fritz, B. S., and Avsec, A. (2007). The experience of flow and subjective well-being of music students. Horizons Psychol. 16, 5–17.

Google Scholar

Fukui, H., and Toyoshima, K. (2014). Chill-inducing music enhances altruism in humans. Front. Psychol. 5:1215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, C., Due, F. B., Thieu, E. K., Hjørnevik, K., Tuastad, L., and Assmus, J. (2020). Long-term effects of short-term music therapy for prison inmates: six-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Int. J. Offender Ther. Comp. Criminol. doi: 10.1177/0306624X20909216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gold, C., Voracek, M., and Wigram, T. (2004). Effects of music therapy for children and adolescents with psychopathology: a meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 45, 1054–1063. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Sanguino, C., Ausín, B., Castellanos, M., Saiz, J., López-Gómez, A., Ugidos, C., et al. (2020). Mental health consequences of the coronavirus 2020 pandemic (COVID-19) in Spain. A longitudinal study. Front. Psychiatry 11:1256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.565474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grau-Sánchez, J., Münte, T. F., Altenmüller, E., Duarte, E., and Rodríguez-Fornells, A. (2020). Potential benefits of music playing in stroke upper limb motor rehabilitation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 112, 585–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.02.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Green, L. (2011). Learning, Teaching, and Musical Identity: Voices Across Cultures. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Google Scholar

Greene, R. D., Cook, A., Nowaskie, D., and Wang, S. (2020). Neurological changes and depression: 2020 update. Clin. Geriatr. Med. 36, 297–313. doi: 10.1016/j.cger.2019.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagendoorn, I. (2003). The dancing brain. Cerebrum 5, 19–34.

Google Scholar

Hallam, S. (2010). The power of music: its impact on the intellectual, social and personal development of children and young people. Int. J. Music Educ. 28, 269–289. doi: 10.1177/0255761410370658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halpern, A. R., Bartlett, J. C., and Dowling, W. J. (1998). Perception of mode, rhythm, and contour in unfamiliar melodies: effects of age and experience. Music Percept. 15, 335–355. doi: 10.2307/40300862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, M. K., Stewardson, E., Jamil, A. K., Tecson, K. M., and Millard, M. W. (2020). Usefulness of harmonica playing to improve outcomes in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Baylor Univ. Med. Center Proc. 33, 178–182. doi: 10.1080/08998280.2019.1704135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, S., Cain, M., Istvandity, L., and Lakhani, A. (2017). The role of music participation in positive health and wellbeing outcomes for migrant populations: a systematic review. Psychol. Music 45, 459–478. doi: 10.1177/0305735616665910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, R., Fernández, C., and Baptista, P. (2010). Metodología de la Investigación. México: McGraw Hill.

Google Scholar

Hjørnevik, K., and Waage, L. (2019). The prison as a therapeutic music scene: exploring musical identities in music therapy and everyday life in a prison setting. Punishm. Soc. 21, 454–472. doi: 10.1177/1462474518794187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hormigos-Ruiz, J. (2010). Music distribution in the consumer society: the creation of cultural identities through sound. Rev. Comun. 17, 91–98. doi: 10.3916/c34-2010-02-09

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huppert, F. A. (2009). Psychological well-being: evidence regarding its causes and consequences. Appl. Psychol. 1, 137–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2009.01008.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jefsen, O. H., Rohde, C., Nørremark, B., and Østergaard, S. D. (2020). Editorial Perspective: COVID-19 pandemic-related psychopathology in children and adolescents with mental illness. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. Available online at: https://acamh.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/jcpp.13292

Google Scholar

Kokotsaki, D., and Hallam, S. (2007). Higher education music students’ perceptions of the benefits of participative music making. Music Educ. Res. 9, 93–109. doi: 10.1080/14613800601127577

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, A. (2012). Emotion, engagement and meaning in strong experiences of music performance. Psychol. Music 40, 574–594. doi: 10.1177/0305735612448510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laukka, P. (2007). Uses of music and psychological well-being among the elderly. J. Happiness Stud. 8, 215–241. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9024-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laurence, F., and Urbain, O. (1999). Music and Solidarity: Questions of Universality, Consciousness, and Connection. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Google Scholar

LeBlanc, A., Sims, W. L., Siivola, C., and Obert, M. (1996). Music style preferences of different age listeners. J. Res. Music Educ. 44, 49–59. doi: 10.2307/3345413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S., Wang, Y., Xue, J., Zhao, N., and Zhu, T. (2020). The impact of covid-19 epidemic declaration on psychological consequences: a study on active weibo users. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:2032. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17062032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Xing, X., Shi, X., Yan, P., Chen, Y., Li, M., et al. (2020). The effectiveness of music therapy for patients with cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Adv. Nurs. 76, 1111–1123.

Google Scholar

Liu, K. (2020). How I faced my coronavirus anxiety. Science 367:1398. doi: 10.1126/science.367.6484.1398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, R. A. R., Hargreaves, D. J., and Miell, D. (2002). Musical Identities. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Mitjà, O., Arenas, À, Rodó, X., Tobias, A., Brew, J., and Benlloch, J. M. (2020). Experts’ request to the Spanish government: move Spain towards complete lockdown. Lancet 395, 1193–1194. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30753-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales-Vives, F., Dueñas, J. M., Vigil-Colet, A., and Camarero-Figuerola, M. (2020). Psychological variables related to adaptation to the COVID-19 lockdown in Spain. Front. Psychol. 11:2438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.565634

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mossler, K., Schmid, W., Assmus, J., Fusar-Poli, L., and Gold, C. (2020). Attunement in music therapy for young children with autism: revisiting qualities of relationship as mechanisms of change. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 50, 3921–3934. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04448-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Statistics Institute of Spain (2020). Statistical Information for the Analysis of the Impact of the COVID-19 Crisis. Available online at: https://www.ine.es/covid/covid_inicio_en.htm (accessed May 8, 2020).

Google Scholar

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and Hargreaves, J. J. (2004). Uses of music in everyday life. Music Percept. 22, 41–77. doi: 10.1525/mp.2004.22.1.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., and O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance of music to adolescents. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 70, 255–272. doi: 10.1348/000709900158083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Callaghan, C. C., McDermott, F., Michael, N., Daveson, B. A., Hudson, P. L., and Zalcberg, J. R. (2014). “A quiet still voice that just touches”: music’s relevance for adults living with life-threatening cancer diagnoses. Support. Care Cancer 22, 1037–1047. doi: 10.1007/s00520-013-2059-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oriola-Requena, S., and Gustems-Carnicer, J. (2015). Música y adolescencia: usos, funciones y consideraciones educativas. Univ. Tarracon. Rev. Cièn. l’Educ. 1, 27–45. doi: 10.17345/ute.2015.2.660

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozamiz-Etxebarria, N., Idoiaga-Mondragon, N., Dosil-Santamaría, M., and Picaza-Gorrotxategi, M. (2020). Psychological symptoms during the two stages of lockdown in response to the COVID-19 outbreak: an investigation in a sample of citizens in Northern Spain. Front. Psychol. 11:1491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01491

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, J. S. (1987). Music Therapy: An Introduction. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas Publisher.

Google Scholar

Phelps, C., and Sperry, L. L. (2020). Children and the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychol. Trauma 12, S73–S75.

Google Scholar

Rentfrow, P. J., Goldberg, L. R., and Levitin, D. J. (2011). The structure of musical preferences: a five-factor model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 100, 1139–1157. doi: 10.1037/a0022406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rentfrow, P. J., and Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi’s of everyday life: the structure and personality correlates of music preferences. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1236–1256. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Rey, R., Garrido-Hernansaiz, H., and Collado, S. (2020). Psychological impact and associated factors during the initial stage of the coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic among the general population in Spain. Front. Psychol. 11:1540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Sánchez, A., Odena, O., and Cabedo–Mas, A. (2018). Using life histories with sound postcards to investigate a music programme for social reconstruction in Colombia. Br. J. Music Educ. 35, 177–190. doi: 10.1017/s0265051717000298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1069–1081. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schäfer, K., and Eerola, T. (2020). How listening to music and engagement with other media provide a sense of belonging: an exploratory study of social surrogacy. Psychol. Music 48, 232–251. doi: 10.1177/0305735618795036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schäfer, T., Sedlmeier, P., Städtler, C., and Huron, D. (2013). The psychological functions of music listening. Front. Psychol. 4:511. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00511

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sim, J., Cho, D., Hwang, Y., and Telang, R. (2020). Virus shook the streaming star: estimating the COVID-19 impact on music consumption. SSRN Electron. J. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.3649085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Small, C. (1998). Musicking. The Meanings of Performing and Listening. Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.

Google Scholar

Spanish Ministry of Health (2020). COVID-19 Reports. Available online at: https://www.mscbs.gob.es/profesionales/saludPublica/ccayes/alertasActual/nCov-China/situacionActual.htm (accessed May 8, 2020).

Google Scholar

Storr, A. (2015). Music and the Mind. London: Harper Collins.

Google Scholar

Tepper, S. J., and Hargittai, E. (2009). Pathways to music exploration in a digital age. Poetics 37, 227–249. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2009.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobías, A. (2020). Evaluation of the lockdowns for the SARS-CoV-2 epidemic in Italy and Spain after one month follow up. Sci. Total Environ. 725, 1–3.

Google Scholar

Valenzuela, J., Crosby, L. E., and Harrison, R. R. (2020). Commentary: reflections on the COVID-19 pandemic and health disparities in pediatric psychology. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 45, 839–841. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsaa063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Wall, W. (1924). The Utilization of Music in Prisons and Mental Hospitals: Its Application in the Treatment and Care of the Morally and Mentally Afflicted, 1924. Fort Collins, CO: Colorado State University.

Google Scholar

Van der Hoeven, A. (2018). Narratives of popular music heritage and cultural identity: the affordances and constraints of popular music memories. Eur. J. Cult. Stud. 21, 207–222. doi: 10.1177/1367549415609328

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vieillard, S., Didierjean, A., and Maquestiaux, F. (2012). Changes in the perception and the psychological structure of musical emotions with advancing age. Exp. Aging Res. 38, 422–441. doi: 10.1080/0361073x.2012.699371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C., Pan, R., Wan, X., Tan, Y., Xu, L., Ho, C. S., et al. (2020). Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G., Zhang, Y., Zhao, J., Zhang, J., and Jiang, F. (2020). Mitigate the effects of home confinement on children during the COVID-19 outbreak. Lancet 395, 945–947. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30547-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, C., Bungay, H., Munn-Giddings, C., and Boyce, M. (2016). Healthcare professionals’ perceptions of the value and impact of the arts in healthcare settings: a critical review of the literature. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 56, 90–101. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2015.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wuttke-Linnemann, A., Nater, U. M., Ehlert, U., and Ditzen, B. (2019). Sex-specific effects of music listening on couples’ stress in everyday life. Sci. Rep. 9:4880.

Google Scholar

Xiang, Y. T., Yang, Y., Li, W., Zhang, L., Zhang, Q., Cheung, T., et al. (2020). Timely mental health care for the 2019 novel coronavirus outbreak is urgently needed. Lancet Psychiatry 7, 228–229. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30046-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Li, W., Zhang, Q., Zhang, L., Cheung, T., and Xiang, Y. T. (2020). Mental health services for older adults in China during the COVID-19 outbreak. Lancet Psychiatry 7:e19. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30079-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: COVID-19, music, wellbeing, perceptions, Spain

Citation: Cabedo-Mas A, Arriaga-Sanz C and Moliner-Miravet L (2021) Uses and Perceptions of Music in Times of COVID-19: A Spanish Population Survey. Front. Psychol. 11:606180. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606180

Received: 14 September 2020; Accepted: 16 December 2020;
Published: 12 January 2021.

Edited by:

Niels Chr. Hansen, Aarhus Institute of Advanced Studies, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Raymond Alfredo Rossi MacDonald, The University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom
Thomas Schäfer, Medical School Berlin, Germany
Katarzyna Grebosz-Haring, University of Salzburg, Austria

Copyright © 2021 Cabedo-Mas, Arriaga-Sanz and Moliner-Miravet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alberto Cabedo-Mas, cabedoa@uji.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.