Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 December 2020
Sec. Movement Science

Physical Activity and Well-Being of High Ability Students and Community Samples During the COVID-19 Health Alert

  • 1Institute of Psychology and Special Education, Department of Applied Psychology, University Center for Health Sciences, University of Guadalajara, Guadalajara, Mexico
  • 2Faculty of Social Sciences, Universidad Europea de Canarias, La Orotava, Spain
  • 3Transdisciplinary Research Center in Psychology, Autonomous University of the State of Morelos, Cuernavaca, Mexico
  • 4Department of Clinical Psychology, Psychobiology and Methodology, Faculty of Psychology and Speech Therapy, University of La Laguna, Santa Cruz de Tenerife, Spain

The health alert caused by the COVID-19 pandemic and the lockdown have caused significant changes in people’s lives. Therefore, it has been essential to study the quality of life, especially in vulnerable populations, including children and adolescents. In this work, the psychological well-being, distribution of tasks and routines, as well as the physical activity done by children and adolescents from two samples: community and high abilities, have been analyzed. The methodology used was Mixed Method Research, through a survey conducted online through social networks. The informants were the parents of the children and adolescents, 209 in the community sample and 116 in the high ability sample. The instrument used was a questionnaire implemented through Google Forms, with open and closed questions, including the Kidscreen-27 scale to measure well-being. The assessment of the adequacy of the physical activity levels was analyzed through ALCESTE. The results showed the absence of differences between students from community samples and those with high capacities in well-being and physical activity. Parents residing in Spain observed less play time in the high ability sample, and more time spent on homework, whether or not they have a diagnosis of high ability. It is concluded that these results question the misconceptions held about high ability students in terms of poorer personal adjustment and better interest in physical activities.

Introduction

The health alert caused by the COVID-19 pandemic, which has forced people to remain in their homes, making it impossible to live normally, has had important effects at the psychological level, giving rise to emotions such as fear (Asmundson and Taylor, 2020), panic, anxiety or depression (Bao et al., 2020; Rajkumar, 2020). On the other hand, children seem to be more vulnerable when they experience traumatic events that disrupt their daily lives (Barlett et al., 2020), can react to the emotional states of adults (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention., 2020; Dalton et al., 2020). However, there are conflicting positions bout this issue: while Enmienda et al. (2020) consider that conditions of social isolation and confinement, together with mandatory online educational training may increase adverse feelings, Boonstra (2020) points out that anxiety is similar to that which may be exhibited by the general population.

A specific population to be analyzed in the health emergency caused by COVID-19 is high ability students, since following the theory that says they show high sensitivity, they may be more likely to present anxiety or depression (Karpinski et al., 2018; Amstrong et al., 2019). High capacities is not an easy construct to define, there is no definition shared by all experts (López Andrada et al., 2000; Colangelo and Davis, 2003; Pérez, 2006; Matthews and Yun Dai, 2014). Additionally, high capacity children and adolescents do not constitute an heterogenous group. Although the literature shows superior cognitive functioning than that observed in their less talented peers (Sastre-Riba, 2008; Sastre-Riba and Ortiz, 2018).

High intellectual capacity is the result of the expression of a high neurobiological potential of intellectual capacities, modulated by intra- and interpersonal variables along the evolutionary development (Olzewski-Kubilius et al., 2015). Intellectual capacity has been and still is one of the important variables in the definition of high capacity together with other variables (Fernández et al., 2017).

As far as the academic field is concerned, myths are especially related with the idea that high capacity means high performance. This idea is clearly mistaken, since it is a fact that the performance of high ability students is very often below what would be expected based on their abilities, giving rise to an important line of research under the label of underachievement (Gallagher, 1991; Hoover-Schultz, 2005; Dai et al., 2011; Steenbergen-Hu et al., 2020).

Stereotypes regarding lesser personal adjustment (Preckel et al., 2015; Baudson and Preckel, 2016; Matheis et al., 2017) have been shown to be false (Reis and Renzulli, 2004; Borges et al., 2011; Bacal, 2015; Valadez et al., 2015; Egeland, 2019; França-Freitas et al., 2019), or those relating to their satisfaction with life (Bergold et al., 2015).

Something similar occurs with psychological well-being, as studies show none or very small differences between gifted students and the control group (Zeidner and Shani-Zinovich, 2011; Olivier et al., 2016), or it depended on categories within the gifted group, where the creative gifted showed a lower degree of psychological well-being (Kroesbergen et al., 2016).

Household confinement has had other effects as well. Restricted mobility has reduced the possibility of exercise, which is a problem for the whole population, but it is particularly serious for children and adolescents, because of their need to move around (Hall et al., 2020; Hammami et al., 2020).

Physical activity is also important in the area of high ability, since one of the misconceptions that can be found is related to physical characteristics of high ability students, where it is common to consider them sickly, weak and not very likely to do physical activity and sports (Artiles and Jiménez, 2005; Guirado, 2015). The findings of a recent study on the difficulty of modifying beliefs showed that when the differences in the difficulty parameter are analyzed on a Likert-type scale using the item response theory in order to determine which statements are more easily modified, the idea that gifted pupils are weak and unhealthy seemed relatively easy to eliminate, but the myth that they are not very likely to be good sportsmen was among those that were more difficult to eliminate (Pérez et al., 2020).

However, the literature on physical activity among high ability students is quite limited, which makes it difficult to confirm or refute the idea that these students are not very interested in physical activity. There is relatively little research on how academically talented students occupy their time, as well as the place of sports and general physical activity in their free time. A study by Makel et al. (2015) comparing the distribution of time in and out of school for gifted students in the United States and India shows that time spent on extracurricular activities can vary depending on cultural and social variables, age, and family perceptions of the value of these activities [such as academics clubs, arts, service clubs, and sports activities (athletics)]. The authors cite previous research (Makel et al., 2011), which confirm the findings of Rinn and Wininger (2007) in which they found a high rate of participation of American adolescents in sports activities, which contradicts the idea that they spend little time on activities other than academics.

Some studies focus on pointing out the importance of sport and physical activity. Vidosavljević and Krulj-Drasković (2013) point out that it is a joint task between the family and the school to determine the ideal exercise to do. Lutostanski (2018) proposal is along with the same lines.

In terms of empirical studies, there is research that points to possible limitations of this student body. Thus, Lutostanski (2018) points out that, while most children with high ability can be good athletes, others can have asynchronous development, with physical activities not being easy for them, resulting in the idea that they may be clumsy or unprepared for tasks that require coordination.

In a similar line, Litster and Roberts (2011) found in their meta-analysis describing characteristics of gifted students that they scored better than their peers of lesser ability in perceived academic and behavioral competence and in overall self-concept, but their score was lower in perceived athletic competence and in appearance.

An important variable is whether or not they play sports, since gifted people who play sports have a better self-concept about their physical abilities than those who do not (Rinn and Wininger, 2007). The study by Hormazábal-Peralta et al. (2018) found that adolescents with high abilities reported high physical activity levels measured in hours per week. In contrast, females showed less weekly physical activity and a greater tendency to be obese.

The aim of this study is to compare the perceptions and opinions of parents or caregivers of a community sample of children and adolescents against another with high abilities on psychological well-being, physical activity and sedentary lifestyles developed during the health crisis produced by the COVID-19 pandemic. It is hypothesized, based on the studies reviewed, that high ability students will not present less psychological well-being, nor will they show lower activity patterns than the comparison group (community sample). Nor is it expected that there will be significant differences depending on the country of residence.

Materials and Methods

Methodology and Design

Mixed methodology was used (Mixed Methods Research, MMR; Johnson and Onwuegbuzie, 2004; Denscombe, 2008). The data was collected through a cross-sectional design (Smith et al., 2016), including survey methodology and qualitative methodology, through open questions that allow a qualitative analysis.

Participants

A total of 325 participants responded to the survey. The average age was 41.54 (SD = 7.36), with a range of 23 to 65 years. More mothers (279) than fathers (44) participated and only two of the informants were guardians of the minors, specifically family members in charge of the child. Table 1 presents the data regarding the educational level of the parents and caregivers of both samples (high abilities and community). The procedure for selecting the sample was convenience, contacting the high ability population through specific high-skill partnerships. The consideration of high ability students was made by the affirmative response of the parents to the question of whether their child had been diagnosed as high ability. The number of families with children of high ability was 116, compared to 209 in the community sample (control group).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Educational level of parents and caregivers in the two samples.

Table 2 shows the age and sex of the children by type of sample. They have been divided into three groups: up to 7 years old, from 8 to 11 years old and from 12 years old onward. This division is made because the Psychological Wellbeing test is only applied to children of 8 years old or more, dividing the following group from 12 years old, following Piaget’s classification (Piaget, 1968) which indicates the appearance of formal operations from this age onward.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive variables of age and sex in the two samples.

The average age of the community sample is 9.64 (SD = 4.06) and the average age of the high capacity sample is 9.92 (SD = 3.27). There are no significant differences in age (Welch’ test = −0.692; freedom degree (f.d.) = 282.633; p-value (p) = 0.489).

The relationship between the sex of the child and belonging or not to the high ability sample is significant (Phi = 0.220; p < 0.001). However, it is common for the number of males in the high ability sample to be greater than females (Jiménez, 2010). In Spanish state data1, the statistics show that while the percentage of males in school is 51% (4241,424 of the total of 8217,662), the male student body diagnosed with high ability is 65% (23,092 of the total student body diagnosed, 35,494).

The participants resided in Spain (55.4%), Mexico (36.6%), Panama (4.3%), Colombia (2.5%), Chile (0.9%), and Argentina (0.3%). At the time of data collection (May 2020), all these countries had mobility restrictions outside the home due to the pandemic. This sample of different nationalities has been used to check the cross-validity of the psychological well-being and physical activity of gifted students compared to their peers, given that this reality is not limited to a single country. For this reason, an international Spanish-speaking sample has been used.

There was hardly any incidence of having suffered from COVID-19 in the family, since a large majority (318, which represents 97.2%) reported no cases. Four families reported having a confirmed patient in the family unit, while in another four cases there was a suspicion that a family member had the disease.

Instruments

The data was collected with a questionnaire containing the following sections (see Table 3): (a) descriptive data of the participants; (b) characteristics of the family and their dwelling; (c) physical activity done by the minor and his/her assessment; (d) activity done during the day by the minor.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Questions included in the used questionnaire.

In addition, the Kidscreen-27 Parent Questionnaire (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2005) was included in the same questionnaire to measure the psychological well-being of the children. It consists of 27 items, which are answered on a Likert-type scale of five alternatives (from nothing to a lot), structured into five scales: physical activity, mood, family life, friends and school. The reliability of the instrument is 0.82. The construct validity of the instrument is satisfactory; the calculation of the factor analysis explains 56% of the variance and for the reliability of each of the five dimensions.

Procedure

The questionnaire was made in electronic format with the Google Forms program. It was sent by email and through social networks (WhatsApp, Facebook, Twitter) during the month of May 2020.

Regarding the ethical aspects, this research has been approved by the Commission of Ethics of Research and Animal Welfare of the University of La Laguna (CEIBA) with the Registration Number: CEIBA2020-0396. In the questionnaire the corresponding information for the participants was exposed, gathered in the Organic Law 3/2018, of December 5th, of Protection of Personal Data and guarantee of the digital rights (BOE n° 294 of December 6th), guaranteeing the anonymity and the confidentiality of the data. Finally, the use of the Kidscreen-27 Parent Questionnaire (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2005), in its Spanish version, whose use was authorized by the authors of the Spanish version of the instrument.

Data Analysis

The reliability of the Kidscreen Parent Questionnaire-27 test for the present sample was determined using Cronbach’s alpha. The identification of univariate outliers was made using the box plot diagram and the multivariate outliers calculating the Mahalanobis distance. To respond to the hypothesis of differences in well-being between the group of high communities and the community sample pupils, a MANOVA has been made, being the independent variables the type of sample (high capacities versus community sample) and the country of residence (Spain, Mexico and other American countries), the dependent ones the scales that compose the Kidscreen-27 Parent Questionnaire (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2005. To confirm the equality of the covariance matrices, the Box test was used, and the Equality of Error Variances using the Levene test. The test used for the verification of multivariate normality was Shapiro–Wilks. The hypothesis of the existing differences in both samples in terms of physical activity and sedentary life were established by contrasting ANOVA, being the independent variables the type of sample and the country of residence, and the effect sizes by ηp2. The testing of homoscedasticity was done with the Levene test. All analyses were performed with the SPPS program v.21. The chosen significance threshold is 0,05.

For the qualitative analysis, the phenomenological discourse analysis method was used, which identifies the meanings of language through lexical analysis using the ALCESTE (Lexical Analysis of Co-occurrences in Simple Text Statements) software (Reinert, 2001). This software uses statistical procedures to extract essential information from a text, in such a way that it will receive essential information from itself, quantifying its strongest lexical structures, grouping the co-occurrence. This co-occurrence is the association by proximity of various words (nouns, adjectives or verbs) using the Chi-square statistic, with the aim of differentiating the most significant lexical world. The unit of analysis is the Elementary Context Unit (ECU), which corresponds to the idea of a sentence or a set of between 8 and 20 words (De Alba, 2004). One of the advantages of this approach is that it avoids the subjectivity involved in the construction of categories by the researcher, since the computer program establishes the connections using statistical procedures (Bauer, 2003).

Results

Quantitative Analysis

Identification of any Univariate/Multivariate Outliers

To confirm the presence of univariate extreme cases, the box and whiskers diagram was used, without obtaining extreme cases. To contrast the existence of multivariate extreme cases, the Mahalanobis distance was calculated. A probability less than 0.001 was not obtained in any case.

Psychological Well-Being

The assumption of normal distribution was tested using the Shapiro–Wilks test. Although the distributions were not normal (Table 4), both ANOVA and MANOVA are robust to moderate violations of normality, provided that the violation is created by asymmetry and not by outliers (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007). In the same Table 4 are presented the results of Levene’s test.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Shapiro–Wilk normality test and Levene test.

In order to analyze if there were significant differences in the scales of psychological well-being in relation to the type of sample (having or not high ability) and to country of residence, a MANOVA was carried out. Since the Box test of equality of covariance matrices was significant (Box M = 100.402; F60, 2596.440 = 1.577; p = 0.003), the statistic used was Pillai’s trace. It did not yield significant results for any of the main effects (Sample: Pillai = 0.32; F5,217 = 1.785; p = 0.116; ηp2 = 0.032. Country: Pillai = 0.61; F10,536 = 1.691; p = 0.800; ηp2 = 0.031 or for the interaction (Pillai = 0.47; F10,536 = 1.691; p = 0.800; ηp2 = 0.031).

Physical Activity

For the activity analyses, the responses given by parents of all ages were taken into account, i.e., also including those younger than 8 years old. To check if there were extreme cases in the variables related to the physical activity levels, the box and mustache diagram was made. Based on that, 48 participants were selected as extreme cases and removed, and the activities of yoga and ballet were eliminated, due to the low frequency observed.

Families were asked whether their children’s activity during confinement was sufficient or insufficient. No relationship was shown between type of sample and satisfaction or dissatisfaction with the activity performed (Phi = −0.068, p = 0.260). Response frequencies are shown in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Perception of the adequate physical activity developed by their children.

As for the conditions of the dwelling, that is, whether they had an outside space for exercises, the results are presented in Table 6. The variables of sample type and outdoor space are not related (Phi = −0.111; p = 0.064).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Outdoor space in the home for exercise.

After removing extreme cases from the sample, the normality of the distribution was verified with the statistic of Shapiro–Wilk, observing absence of normality (Table 7), although the violation of normality does not imply a problem of great relevance (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2007).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Shapiro–Wilk normality test and Levene test.

In order to determine if parents perceive significant differences in their children’s levels of physical activity according to the sample (diagnosed or not with high capacities) and the country of residence, ANOVA was carried out for the different activities. Results are shown in Table 8.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. ANOVA of physical activity by sample and country of residence.

As it can be seen, there are only significant differences in gymnastic activity between countries. Since heteroscedasticity has been proven, the existence of significant differences was tested by the Brown-Forsythe test (B-F2,69.92 = 13.227; p > 0.001). The average time dedicated to gymnastics in Spain was 1.34 (SD = 1.85), 0.32 (SD = 1.12) in Mexico and 0.50 (SD = 1.75) in the rest of the American countries. Tukey’s test showed significant differences between Spain and Mexico (difference of 1.02, p > 0.001).

Regarding what activities they do during the day, we also calculated the normality of the scores (Shapiro–Wilks test) and the homogeneity (Levene’s test), which are shown in Table 9.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Shapiro–Wilk normality test and Levene test.

To check if parents observed differences in the daily activities done by their children according to the country of residence and the type of sample, ANOVA tests were carried out. Results are shown in Table 10.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. ANOVA of the differences in activities they do during the day.

Significant differences are observed in play, both for interaction and for the main effects. Tukey’s post-contrast shows no significant differences between countries, with significantly less time played by the high ability group (Mean = 0.851; DT = 1.62) in relation to the other sample (Mean = 0.912; DT = 1.70). The interaction (Figure 1) indicates that the American high ability students and the Spanish community sample play more.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Interaction of the variables country and sample in time devoted to play.

Another activity where significant differences are found is in doing homework, depending to the country of residence. Since heteroscedasticity has been proven, the Brown-Forsythe (B-F2, 69.92 = 36.46; p > 0.001). The highest average time is in Spain (Mean = 2.48; SD = 2.92), followed by the rest of the American countries (Mean = 0.57; SD = 1.53) and Mexico (Mean = 0.50; SD = 1.50). Tukey’s post-test shows significant differences in the time spent doing homeworking between Spain and Mexico (1.98; p > 0.001) and between Spain and the rest of the American countries (1.91; p > 0.001).

Qualitative Analysis

In order to determine the perception that parents have about whether or not their children’s physical activity is sufficient, an analysis has been carried out with ALCESTE (Reinert, 2001), separating the two samples. In the case of the parents of the community sample, six classes are obtained (Need for interaction with peers, Absence of physical activity, Space for physical activity, Going outside, Activity routines and Physical activity at home), which classify 74% of the textual units and implies a high relevance of the treatment. Figure 2 shows the dendrogram obtained in the analysis. Three links are observed between the classes: the one that links the first with the second (Need for interaction with peers, Absence of physical activity), which in turn connects with the others, organized in connection with 3 with 4 (Space for physical activity, Going outside), related to the place where the physical activity is done, and 5 with 6 (Activity routines and Physical activity at home), which have to do with how the physical activity is done.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Dendrogram of responses given by parents in the community sample.

Table 11 shows the details of each class: name, number of UCEs it comprises, percentage of variable it explains, most representative word and example sentences of each class.

TABLE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Information from the analysis to the question “Is the physical activity developed sufficiently or insufficiently?” Community sample.

In the case of the sample of parents with children of high abilities, the analysis extracts five classes (Practice of physical activity, Little physical activity, Changes in physical activity, Outdoor and Physical activity at home), which classifies 73% of the textual units, with a high relevance of the treatment. Class 1 (Practicing physical activity) links the union of class 2 (Little physical activity) with class 3 (Changes in physical activity), on the one hand, which are related to the type of physical activity developed, and with class 4 (Going outside) and class 5 (Physical activity at home), whose content refers to where the physical activity is performed. The dendrogram is shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Dendrogram of responses given by parents in the high ability sample.

The detail of each class (name, number of UCEs it encompasses, percentage of variable it explains, most representative word and example sentences of each class) is presented in Table 12. Class 1, together with the link between 2 and 3, refers to the practice of physical activity, adapted to confinement, while classes 4 and 5 refer to the place where it is performed.

TABLE 12
www.frontiersin.org

Table 12. Information from the analysis to the question “Is the physical activity developed sufficiently or insufficiently?” High ability sample.

Discussion

The results found are in line with demystifying some issues in the high ability students. Firstly, with regard to physical well-being, the high-ability students do not show worse stability than the community samples in these conditions of uncertainty that have resulted from being kept at home due to the confinement caused by the health alert, thus confirming the results found in other studies and under pre-pandemic conditions (Zeidner and Shani-Zinovich, 2011; Olivier et al., 2016). This result also helps to dispel the myth that they establish worse relationships with their peers, and, in general, confirms that no specific problems in personal adjustment or in socio-emotional areas have to be expected in that sample (Borges et al., 2011; Valadez et al., 2015; Egeland, 2019).

With regard to physical activity, the samples are comparable in terms of parents’ perception of the appropriateness of their level of physical activity, since there is no relationship between type of sample and satisfaction with the activity they carried out. These results also go against the fact that high ability students do not enjoy physical activity and confirms the results of Rinn and Wininger (2007) or Hormazábal-Peralta et al. (2018).

The results obtained do not allow affirming that there is a differential perception between the parents of the different countries in the well-being of their children, and or in their level of physical activities during the lockdown due to COVID-19. A significant difference is only observed in gymnastics, which is performed more in America than in Spain.

Regarding the type of activity done during the day, significant differences are observed in two of them. The amount of time dedicated to playing shows a significant interaction both in the type of sample and in the country of residence. Parents perceive that high ability students dedicate more time to play in Spain and in the rest of the American countries, in relation to residents in Mexico.

Significant differences are also found in parents’ perception of time spent on homework, which is significantly higher in Spain. There is no difference in the type of sample. It would be convenient to carry out further studies which would allow us to analyze if, in general, schools in Spain give more homework or if it is an effect which appears only in our study.

On the other hand, in the qualitative analysis carried out with ALCESTE (Reinert, 2001), the perceptions of the families regarding the activity done by the children do not differ too much between the community samples and the high ability ones. In both cases, conditions have forced both a decrease in physical activity and modifications to adapt to the confinement and the community samples affirm miss their friends. The categories obtained in the analysis show this in both samples.

However, parents in the community sample feel that their children need to relate to others, which is not shown in the high-ability sample, which is in line with the findings of Makel et al. (2015) that stressed that the use of time in gifted children could be depending on social and family (as the perceptions of parents) variables and conditions. In line with the arguments of Vidosavljević and Krulj-Drasković (2013) and Lutostanski (2018), the family perceptions of the children’s need seems to be relevant in our results, where parents of the gifted do not seem feeling more concerned about insufficient social activities in the children compared to time the devoted to home schooling.

The limitations of this study coincide with its greatest strength: it has been carried out in an atypical situation, albeit one of a universal nature: the mobility limitations forced by the health alarm caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. On the one hand, these findings are limited to these conditions. But, on the other hand, it is important to know how children have been able to cope with this difficult reality. These special moments we have experienced allow us to analyze how they have handled the situation and fortunately, as a group, neither the community sample nor the high ability sample’s psychological well-being seems to have been affected in a significant way.

Another limitation found is that the reporting of physical activity by the children varies greatly, as can be seen from the great variability shown by the SD values, which means that these results must be taken with caution. In fact, family conditions, their context and the facilities they may or may not have in their home condition their mobility, but especially for the minors.

Finally, although the results obtained do not favor the myth of less physical activity in high-ability students, it is necessary to increase the number of studies, under normal conditions, in order to respond to this issue with greater precision.

What is essential is to continue investigating the real characteristics of high-abilities students in a rigorous way, since this is the appropriate way to know their strengths and weaknesses, so that the best lines of action can be clearly recommended, especially those related to educational intervention.

Conclusion

Despite the fact that both children and adolescents are a vulnerable population, these results show adequate psychological well-being. On the other hand, despite the limitations, they have engaged in physical activity during confinement, although there is a wide diversity in the number of hours per week they spend on such activity.

When comparing the two samples studied in the research, no significant difference between them appears. This result supports the adequate personal adjustment of these children and adolescents, it being another evidence against the myth of personal and social maladjustment of gifted people and high intellectual abilities.

On the other hand, there are no differences in the physical activity done by both groups of children. With regard to the distribution of time in their daily routine, Spanish parents seem to perceive that their children with high abilities spend less time playing than American parents, and, in both samples, parents residing in Spain also consider that they have done more homework during the day.

How parents have perceived the adequacy of their children’s physical activity has been analyzed from a qualitative perspective, finding that in both groups (community and high ability samples) their assessments are very similar, except for families in the community sample indicating that they miss their friends.

Regarding the practical implications of this research, it should be noted that it contributes to demystifying high-ability students (Borges et al., 2011; Pérez et al., 2017). The results obtained support the position that it is necessary analyze in a scientific way the prejudices and myths that exist with respect to the group of high capacities. All this will help to have an accurate knowledge of this student and devoid of myths.

In conclusion, this study shows the importance of analyzing the differences between high ability students and community samples, in order to establish the aspects that really differentiate between groups, which is especially important when establishing educational responses according to their needs.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Commission of Ethics of Research and Animal Welfare of the University of La Laguna (CEIBA2020-0396). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

ER-N, DC-S, GL-A, MV, TA, and ÁB have participated in theoretical review. ÁB, ER-N, TA, DC-S, GL-A, MV, and JF have participated in research design and instrument. ÁB and TA has participated in the data analysis. ÁB, ER-N, DC-S, GL-A, MV, and JF have participated in discussion. ÁB, ER-N, DC-S, GL-A, MV, and JF have participated in the study planning, writing, and revision of the article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the families who have participated in this research.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://estadisticas.mecd.gob.es/EducaDynPx/educabase/index.htm?type=pcaxis&path=/Educacion/Alumnado/Apoyo/Curso18-19/AltasCapacidades&file=pcaxis&l=s0

References

Amstrong, L., Desson, S., St. John, E., and Watt, E. (2019). The D.R.E.A.M. program: developing resilience through emotions, attitudes, & meaning (gifted edition) – a second wave positive psychology approach. Couns. Psychol. Q. 32, 307–332. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2018.1559798

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Artiles, C., and Jiménez, J. E. (2005). Introducción a la Atención Educativa del Alumnado con Altas Capacidades Intelectuales. Spain: Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria.

Google Scholar

Asmundson, G. J. G., and Taylor, S. (2020). Coronaphobia: Fear and the 2019-nCoV outbreak. J. Anxiety Dis. 70:102196. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2020.102196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bacal, E. (2015). The relationship between placement and social skills in gifted students. [doctoral dissertation] Arizona: Arizona State University.

Google Scholar

Bao, Y., Sun, Y., Meng, S., Shi, J., and Lu, L. (2020). 2019-nCoV epidemic: address mental health care to empower society. Lancet 2, e37–e38. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(20)30309-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barlett, J. D., Griffin, J., and Thomson, D. (2020). Resources for supporting children’s emotional well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Available online at https://www.childtrends.org/publications/resources-for-supporting-childrens-emotional-well-being-during-the-covid-19-pandemic (accessed March 19, 2020).

Google Scholar

Baudson, T. G., and Preckel, F. (2016). Teachers’ conceptions of gifted and average-ability students on achievement-relevant dimensions. Gift. Child. Q. 60, 212–225. doi: 10.1177/0016986216647115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, M. W. (2003). Análisis de textos asistidos con programas computacionales. Subj. Y Proc. Cogn. 3, 101–111.

Google Scholar

Bergold, S., Wirthwein, L., Rost, D. H., and Steinmayr, R. (2015). Are gifted adolescents more satisfied with their lives than their non-gifted peers? Front. Psychol. 6:1623. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, K. (2020). COVID-19 and Anxiety in Gifted Children. National Association for Gifted Children. Available online at https://www.nagc.org/blog/covid-19-and-anxiety-gifted-children (accessed March 23, 2020).

Google Scholar

Borges, A., Hernández-Jorge, C., and Rodríguez-Naveiras, E. (2011). Evidencias contra el mito de la inadaptación de las personas con altas capacidades intelectuales. Psicothema 23, 362–367.

Google Scholar

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2020). Enfermedad del coronavirus (COVID-19). Georgia, EU: CDC. Retrieved from https://espanol.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/daily-life-coping/children.html (accessed September 17, 2020).

Google Scholar

Colangelo, N., and Davis, G. A. (2003). Handbook of Gifted Education, 3rd Edn. Boston, MA: Ally and Boston.

Google Scholar

Dai, D. Y., Swanson, J. A., and Cheng, H. (2011). State of research on giftedness and gifted education: A survey of empirical studies published during 1998—2010 (April). Gift. Child. Q. 55, 126–138. doi: 10.1177/0016986210397831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dalton, L., Rapa, E., and Stein, A. (2020). Protecting the psychological health of children through effective communication about COVID-19. Lancet Child. Adolesc. Health. 4, 346–347.

Google Scholar

De Alba, M. (2004). El método de ALCESTE y su utilización al estudio de las representaciones sociales del espacio urbano: el caso de la Ciudad de México. Text. Sur Les Rep. 13, 2–1.

Google Scholar

Denscombe, M. (2008). A Research Paradigm for the Mixed Methods Approach. J. Mix. Met. Res. 2, 270–283. doi: 10.1177/1558689808316807

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egeland, J. (2019). Emotion Perception in Members of Norwegian Mensa. Front. Psychol 10:27. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enmienda, E., Koehler, J., Parker, M., Joerg, M., and Nilles, K. (2020). Supporting Your Gifted Child During COVID-19. Natl. Assoc. Gift. Child 2020, 1–4.

Google Scholar

Fernández, E., García, T., Arias-Gundín, O., Vázquez, A., and Rodríguez, C. (2017). Identifying Gifted Children: Congruence among Different IQ Measures. Front. Psychol. 8:1239. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

França-Freitas, M., Del Prette, A., and Del Prette, Z. (2019). Bem estar subjetivo: um estudo comparativo entre crianças dotadas e talentosas e crianças não dotadas. Bol. Acad. Paulista De Psicol. 39, 183–191.

Google Scholar

Gallagher, J. J. (1991). Personal patterns of underachievement. J. Educ. Gift. 14, 221–233.

Google Scholar

Guirado, A. (2015). ¿Qué Sabemos de las Altas Capacidades? Preguntas, Respuestas y Propuestas para la Escuela y la Familia. Spain: Graó.

Google Scholar

Hall, G., Laddu, D. R., Phillips, S. A., Lavie, C. J., and Arena, R. (2020). A tale of two pandemics: How will COVID-19 and global trends in physical inactivity and sedentary behavior affect one another? Prog. in Cardiovasc. Dis. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.005 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hammami, A., Harrabi, B., Mohr, M., and Krustrup, P. (2020). Physical activity and coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19): specific recommendations for home-based physical training. Sport Lei. 2020, 1–6. doi: 10.1080/23750472.2020.1757494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoover-Schultz, B. (2005). Gifted underachievement: Oxymoron or educational enigma? Gift. Child. Today 28, 46–49.

Google Scholar

Hormazábal-Peralta, A., Espinoza, J., Cáceres, P., and Lizana, P. A. (2018). Adolescentes con alta capacidad intelectual: Diferencias en su composición corporal y actividad física por sexo. Nutr. Hosp. 35, 38–43.

Google Scholar

Jiménez, C. (2010). Diagnóstico y educación de los más capaces. Madrid: Pearson Educación.

Google Scholar

Johnson, R. B., and Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2004). Mixed methods research: A research paradigm whose time hascome. Educa. Res. 33, 14–26. doi: 10.3102/0013189X033007014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karpinski, R. I., Kolb, A. M. K., Tetreault, N. A., and Borowski, T. B. (2018). High intelligence: A risk factor for psychological and physiological overexcitabilities. Intell 66, 8–23.

Google Scholar

Kroesbergen, E. H., van Hooijdonk, M., Van Viersen, S., Middel-Lalleman, M. M., and Reijnders, J. J. (2016). The psychological well-being of early identified gifted children. Gift. Child. Q. 60, 16–30. doi: 10.1177/0016986215609113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litster, K., and Roberts, J. (2011). The self−concepts and perceived competencies of gifted and non−gifted students: a meta−analysis. J. Res. Spéc. Educ. Needs 11, 130–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-3802.2010.01166.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López Andrada, B., Beltrán, M. T., López, B., and Chicharro, D. (2000). CIDE: Alumnos Precoces, Superdotados y de Altas Capacidades. Madrid: Centro de investigación y documentación educativa (CIDE).

Google Scholar

Lutostanski, S. (2018). Sports that work for gifted children. Parent. High. Potential 7, 9–15.

Google Scholar

Makel, M. C., Li, Y., Putallaz, M., and Wai, J. (2011). High-ability students’ time spent outside the classroom. J. Adv. Acad. 22, 720–749. doi: 10.1177/1932202X11424880

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makel, M. C., Wai, J., Putallaz, M., and Malore, P. (2015). The Academic Gap: An International Comparison of the Time. Gift. Child. Q. 59:8746. doi: 10.1177/0016986215578746

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matheis, S., Kronborg, L., Schmitt, M., and Preckel, F. (2017). Threat or challenge? Teacher beliefs about gifted students and their relationship to teacher motivation. Gift. Talent. Int. 32, 134–160. doi: 10.1080/15332276.2018.1537685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, D. J., and Yun Dai, D. (2014). Gifted education: changing conceptions, emphases and practice. Int. Stud. Sociol. Educ. 24, 335–353. doi: 10.1080/09620214.2014.979578

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olivier, P. R., Guzmán, J. I. N., Jiménez, I. M., Sinoga, M. M. L., and Sedeño, M. A. G. (2016). Bienestar psicológico en personas con alta capacidad intelectual. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 9, 72–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ejeps.2015.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olzewski-Kubilius, Subontnik, R. F., and Worrell, F. C. (2015). Repensando las altas capacidades: una aproximación evolutiva. Rev. De Educ. 368, 40–56. doi: 10.4438/1988-592X-RE-2015-368-297

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, J., Aperribai, L., Cortabarría, L., and Borges, A. (2020). Examining the Most and Least Changeable Elements of the Social Representation of Giftedness. Sustainability 12:5361. doi: 10.3390/su12135361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pérez, J., Borges, A., and Rodríguez-Naveiras, E. (2017). Conocimientos y Mitos sobre Altas Capacidades. Talincrea 4, 40–51.

Google Scholar

Pérez, L. (2006). Alumnos Con Capacidad Superior. Experiencias de intervención educativa. Madrid: Síntesis.

Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1968). Los estadios del desarrollo intelectual del niño y del adolescente. La Habana, Cuba: Editorial Revolucionaria.

Google Scholar

Preckel, F., Baudson, T. G., Krolak-Schwerdt, S., and Glock, S. (2015). Gifted and maladjusted? Implicit attitudes and automatic associations related to gifted children. Am. Educ. Res. J. 52, 1160–1184. doi: 10.3102/0002831215596413

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajkumar, R. P. (2020). COVID-19 and mental health: A review of the existing literature. Asian, J. Psychiatr. 52:102066. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2020.102066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ravens-Sieberer, U., Gosch, A., Rajmil, L., Erhart, M., Bruil, J., Dür, W., et al. (2005). KIDSCREEN-52 medida de la calidad de vida de los niños y adolescents. Revis. Exp. Farmacoeconomía y Resultde Investig. 5, 353–364.

Google Scholar

Reinert, M. (2001). Alceste, une méthode statistique et sémiotique d’analyse de discours; application aux “Rêveries du promeneur solitaire. Rev. Française psychiatr. Psychol. Méd 49, 32–36.

Google Scholar

Reis, S. M., and Renzulli, J. S. (2004). Current research on the social and emotional development of gifted and talented students: Good news and future possibilities. Psychol. Sch. 41, 119–130. doi: 10.1002/pits.10144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinn, A. N., and Wininger, S. R. (2007). Sports participation among academically gifted adolescents: Relationship to the multidimensional self-concept. J. Educ. Gift. 31, 35–56. doi: 10.4219/jeg-2007-510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sastre-Riba, S. (2008). Niños con altas capacidades y su funcionamiento cognitivo diferencial. Rev. Neurol. 46, 11–16. doi: 10.33588/rn.46S01.2008008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sastre-Riba, S., and Ortiz, T. (2018). Neurofuncionalidad ejecutiva: estudio comparativo en las altas capacidades. Rev. Neurol. 66, 51–56. doi: 10.33588/rn.66S01.2018026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, T. M., Cannata, M., and Haynes, K. T. (2016). Reconciling data from different sources: Practical realities of using mixed methods to identify effective high school practices. Teach. Coll. Rec. 118, 1–34.

Google Scholar

Steenbergen-Hu, S., Olszewski-Kubilius, P., and Calvert, E. (2020). The effectiveness of current interventions to reverse the underachievement of gifted students: Findings of a Meta-Analysis and Systematic Review. Gift. Child. Q. 64, 132–165. doi: 10.1177/0016986220908601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Experimental designs using ANOVA. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Brooks/Cole.

Google Scholar

Valadez, M. D., Valdés, A., Wendlandt, T., Reyes, A., Zambrano, R., and Navarro, J. (2015). Differences in achievement motivation and academic and social self-concept in gifted students of higher education. J. Curric. Teach. 14, 83–90. doi: 10.5430/jct.v4n1p83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidosavljević, S., and Krulj-Drasković, J. (2013). Effect between family and schools in detecting gifted children in sport. Act. Phys. Educ. Spor. 3, 224–228.

Google Scholar

Zeidner, M., and Shani-Zinovich, I. (2011). Do academically gifted and nongifted students differ on the Big-Five and adaptive status? Some recent data and conclusions. Personal. Indiv. Differ. 51, 566–570.

Google Scholar

Keywords: psychological well-being, physical activity, Mixed Method Research, COVID-19 pandemic, high ability students

Citation: Valadez MD, Rodríguez-Naveiras E, Castellanos-Simons D, López-Aymes G, Aguirre T, Flores JF and Borges Á (2020) Physical Activity and Well-Being of High Ability Students and Community Samples During the COVID-19 Health Alert. Front. Psychol. 11:606167. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606167

Received: 15 September 2020; Accepted: 13 November 2020;
Published: 21 December 2020.

Edited by:

Lucia Monacis, University of Foggia, Italy

Reviewed by:

Alessandro Rovetta, Mensana srls, Italy
Rocco Servidio, University of Calabria, Italy
Sahadat Hossain, Jahangirnagar University, Bangladesh

Copyright © 2020 Valadez, Rodríguez-Naveiras, Castellanos-Simons, López-Aymes, Aguirre, Flores and Borges. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elena Rodríguez-Naveiras, elena.rodriguez@universidadeuropea.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.