Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 06 January 2021
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience

The Association Between Effectiveness of Tinnitus Intervention and Cognitive Function—A Systematic Review

  • 1Centre for Speech and Language Therapy and Hearing Sciences, Cardiff School of Sport and Health Sciences, Cardiff Metropolitan University, Cardiff, United Kingdom
  • 2Center for Rehabilitative Auditory Research, Guizhou Provincial People's Hospital, Guiyang, China
  • 3Department of Hearing and Speech Science, Xinhua College, Sun Yat-sen University, Guangzhou, China
  • 4Department of Applied Psychology, Cardiff School of Sport and Health Sciences, Cardiff Metropolitan University, Cardiff, United Kingdom

Tinnitus refers to the perception of sound in the absence of an external stimulus. This can be problematic and can lead to health problems in some sufferers, including effects on cognitive functions such as attention and memory. Although several studies have examined the effectiveness of tinnitus interventions, e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy and sound therapy, it is still unclear as to the overall quality and limitations of these studies and whether their results could be generalized. Clarification is also needed as to whether poor cognitive function will lead to a less favorable intervention outcome in tinnitus patients. The present systematic review was therefore designed to critically appraise and synthesize findings from randomized controlled trials (RCTs) of tinnitus intervention and its effects on cognition. The methodology followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA). Medline (PubMed), Embase, and PsycINFO were searched. Only RCTs that compared the effectiveness of a tinnitus intervention and a measure of cognitive function in adult participants with tinnitus were included. A total of 8 studies involving 610 participants tested using 11 cognitive function assessment tools (e.g., Stroop Color and Word Test and Visual Continuous Performance Task) and 5 tinnitus intervention outcome measurements (e.g., Tinnitus Handicap Inventory and Tinnitus Questionnaire) were included and analyzed. The outcomes of the review suggest that tinnitus intervention not only facilitates tinnitus management but also improves cognitive functions. It is likely that cognition and emotion play an important role in a patient's adjustment to tinnitus. Whether cognition can predict treatment outcomes is unclear due to insufficient evidence. Future research is needed using a standardized assessment protocol focusing on the effect of sound-based interventions on tinnitus severity and cognitive functions. Studies on whether cognitive function measurement can be used as a predictor for the effectiveness of tinnitus therapy are also needed.

Introduction

Subjective tinnitus refers to the perception of sound without the presence of an external stimulus (De Ridder et al., 2014). It is commonly experienced, and almost all adults have had some form of temporary tinnitus at some point, but often without significant annoyance or discomfort (Tyler et al., 2004). Tinnitus can however be highly problematic if symptoms persist and cause health problems, such as insomnia, hearing difficulties, and psychological problems (Tyler et al., 2004). Others with chronic and severe tinnitus can complain of anxiety, depression, and cognitive function-related issues, such as difficulty concentrating (Tyler and Baker, 1983; Andersson et al., 1999). Prevalence studies indicate that persistent tinnitus symptoms affect 10–17% of adults in the United States of America and Europe, with 2–7% of these seeking medical advice and treatment (Davis and El Refaie, 2000; Holmes and Padgham, 2009; Langguth et al., 2013).

In general, cognitive functions are the mental processes that enable us to receive, select, store, transform, develop, and recover information from external stimuli (Stevens and Bavelier, 2012; Clarke et al., 2018). They are associated with concentration, attention, and use of memory, which closely correlate with daily activities and life experience involving communication, perception, thoughts, and emotions (Andersson and McKenna, 2006). Good cognitive function is essential to enable people to recognize and understand the meaning of language and their environment.

Andersson and McKenna (2006) suggest that cognitive function plays an important role in the pathway from tinnitus perception to annoyance. In the study by Hallam et al. (2004), five cognitive measures were conducted under single and dual-task conditions to test sustained attention, reaction time, verbal fluency, and immediate and delayed memory. Patients with tinnitus had significantly slower responses than participants without tinnitus on the variable fore-period reaction time task under dual-task conditions. It indicated that that tinnitus sufferers had to pay more efforts to maintain attention on daily activities in comparison to people without disturbance caused by tinnitus.

Further evidence also suggested that patients with tinnitus had poor performance relevant to attention management, particularly in task-based behavior, revealing a slow response and reduced accuracy on cognitive performance tests for attention and working memory (Rossiter et al., 2006; Pierce et al., 2012; Jackson et al., 2014; Cardon et al., 2019). For example, the patients with tinnitus had significantly lower scores or longer response times on cognitive performance tests (i.e., Stroop Color and Word test assessing the ability to inhibit cognitive interference) in comparison to normal healthy control participants without tinnitus (Jackson et al., 2014; Cardon et al., 2019). Although an association between self-reported cognitive dysfunction and poor cognitive performance in patients with tinnitus has been suggested (McKenna et al., 1995; Hallam et al., 2004), Mohamad et al. (2016) argue that any correlation between self-reported failures in cognition and cognitive performance is unclear due to a lack of consistent findings.

Additionally, psychological issues such as depression and anxiety in tinnitus patients could potentially contribute to their poor cognitive performance (Tegg-Quinn et al., 2016), with a certain level of selection bias and other confounding factors likely to influence analysis of tinnitus and cognitive function (Mohamad et al., 2016). For example, using a self-reported assessment of the Cognitive Failure Questionnaire (CFQ), Hallam et al. (2004) revealed that more cognitive difficulties reported in the CFQ significantly correlated with a higher degree of anxiety in patients with tinnitus. It implies that some cognitive problems are influenced by the psychological issues associated with tinnitus. Therefore, several recent review studies suggest that further investigations on the impact of tinnitus on cognitive function are required (Mohamad et al., 2016; Tegg-Quinn et al., 2016; Trevis et al., 2018).

These reviews did not, however, incorporate studies on tinnitus interventions and currently no review has been conducted relating to the association between tinnitus interventions and cognitive function. Several interventions such as sound therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) have been implemented clinically to manage tinnitus-related symptoms, with effectiveness demonstrated in numerous studies (Hesser et al., 2011; Pienkowski, 2019). CBT is currently recognized as one of the most effective interventions for tinnitus management (Hesser et al., 2011; Fuller et al., 2020). A very recent systematic review shows that CBT has better outcomes in terms of reducing the detrimental impact of tinnitus on quality of life in comparison to audiological interventions such as sound therapy and tinnitus retraining therapy (Fuller et al., 2020).

The importance of cognitive function and emotion has been highlighted in a review by Hesser et al. (2011), who conclude that CBT focuses on changes to a patient's emotional and/or behavioral response, making it an effective tool to alleviate tinnitus-related distress. This also implies that cognitive function might be crucial in the management of tinnitus. Although several studies have investigated the association between tinnitus therapy and cognitive function, the evidence remains limited and controversial. For example, Kallogjeri et al. (2017) suggested that a computer-based brain fitness program was associated with self-reported changes in attention, memory, and perception of tinnitus, together with self-reported improvement in tinnitus perception, but no significant changes in the cognitive performance tasks of memory and sustained and selective attention between patients with tinnitus and controls. In contrast, Krick et al. (2017) showed alleviation in the severity of tinnitus and improvement in the cognitive performance tasks of memory and sustained and selective attention using music therapy. Such discrepancies are likely due to heterogeneity in study design, i.e., these studies used different types of outcome measurements, interventions, and cognitive performance tasks, and thus making a comparison of these studies challenging.

Furthermore, although Kröner-Herwig et al. (2006) attempted to investigate cognitive characteristics as a predictor of the effectiveness of tinnitus intervention, there was no significant correlation between cognitive performance and intervention outcomes in patients with tinnitus assessed using Tinnitus-Dysfunctional-Cognitions Questionnaire and Tinnitus Questionnaire, respectively (r = −0.21, p > 0.05). However, Conrad et al. (2015) found a negative correlation between a subscale of Tinnitus Cognitions Scale and Tinnitus Handicap Inventory post CBT (r = −0.36, p = 0.045). The inconsistent findings are likely due to differences in the study design, such as assessment tools for measuring cognition function and tinnitus-related questionnaires. Therefore, whether a poor performance in cognitive tasks would lead to a less favorable outcome in patients with tinnitus remains unclear. Because randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have an important advantage in terms of minimizing differences in characteristics of the groups that may influence the outcome, for the purpose of providing strong evidence for interpreting and critically evaluating clinical research data, the present review aimed to critically appraise and synthesize findings obtained from the RCT studies, which would address the efficacy of tinnitus intervention on tinnitus and cognitive function in adult patients with tinnitus, and thus facilitate to translate research data into clinical practice. In addition, whether the cognitive function can be used as a predictor for interventional outcomes was also examined.

Methods

The research questions were raised and formulated according to the PICOS principle, i.e., Patients (patients with tinnitus), Intervention (specific tinnitus management, e.g., sound therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy), Comparison (with and without interventions), Outcomes (measurements in tinnitus severity and cognitive performance), and Study Design (RCTs) (Siadaty et al., 2007). The methodology of this review follows the checklist of Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA).

Information Sources and Search Strategy

Studies were identified through the following electronic database: Medline (PubMed), Embase, and PsycINFO. The search was conducted in March 2020. In addition to searching databases, hand searching of reference lists from identified studies was conducted. The keywords used to search were first identified by reviews on tinnitus and cognitive function. Then, Medical Subject Headings (MeSH) were used to confirm the final search terms.

The following search terms and strategy were used to search the databases:

• #1 Search: (Tinnitus[MeSH Terms]) OR (Subjective Tinnitus) (Hits: 35,952)

• #2 Search: (((((Cognition[MeSH Terms]) OR (Cognitive Function)) OR (Function, Cognitive)) OR (Attention)) OR (Concentration)) OR (Memory) (Hits: 6,382,500)

• #3 Search: ((((((((tinnitus management) OR (tinnitus therapy)) OR (tinnitus intervention)) OR (tinnitus sound therapy)) OR (tinnitus music therapy)) OR (tinnitus masking)) OR (cognitive behavioral therapy)) OR (cognitive psychotherapy)) OR (cognition therapy) (Hits: 114,344)

• #4 Search: ((#1) AND (#2)) AND (#3) (Hits: 668)

• #5 Search: ((#1) AND (#2)) AND (#3) Filters: Randomized Controlled Trial (Hits: 98).

Inclusion Criteria

Randomized control trials studying the impact of tinnitus interventions on cognition or whether cognition is a predictor of intervention outcomes were considered. Only published, English, and peer-reviewed journal articles were considered to be included in this review. There was no restriction on the date of publication.

Participants

Studies including participants over 18 years of age with subjective tinnitus were considered. There was no restriction on tinnitus duration and severity.

Interventions

This review was limited to RCTs on tinnitus managements such as sound therapy and cognitive behavioral therapy, but studies on the outcomes of tinnitus intervention using medications or surgical operation alone were excluded.

Outcome Measurements

Self-reported measurements of cognitive function or behavioral measurements of cognitive function.

Study Selection

The title and abstract of studies retrieved from databases were first screened by the first author. Repeated records and studies that did not meet the criteria were excluded. A full-text screening against the inclusion criteria was then conducted on the remaining records. Discrepancies were resolved by FZ OR NP.

Risk of Bias Assessment

To evaluate the methodological quality of included studies, version 2 of the Cochrane Risk-Of-Bias tool for randomized trials was used to assess the adequacy of randomization, concealment of allocation processes, blinding of participants and trial personnel, the extent of missing outcomes, and selective outcome reports. In addition, RevMan 5.3 software was used to plot a figure showing the risk of bias.

Data Extraction

Descriptive data extraction was conducted by the first and second authors (TL and ZC) with use of the agreed data collection form. Data extracted included the type of study design, participant age and gender, description of tinnitus (severity and duration), and outcome measurements (types of cognitive evaluation). In addition, bias assessment was undertaken to show the overall quality of the included studies. Any disagreements were arbitrated by one of the other authors (FZ and NP) after discussion.

Results

Study Selection Procedure and Outcome

Ninety-eight studies were identified, 62 of which were identified from the PubMed (Medline), and 36 were identified through Embase and PsycINFO. A further two were found by manual references searching in the included studies because the terms used in the search strategy were not included in the title or abstract or the keyword list of these two studies (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Kröner-Herwig et al., 2006). After adjusting for repeat records, 89 remained. Of these, 78 studies were excluded after screening the title and abstract, as they did not meet the inclusion criteria because these studies used medical interventions for tinnitus management (e.g., medications or transcranial magnetic stimulation therapy). Following the full-text screening, 3 out of 11 studies were excluded because these studies measured neural activities associated with cognitive function in responding to tinnitus intervention. Finally, eight randomized controlled studies were included in this review. A flow diagram of the selection processes is shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A flow chart illustrating the study selection process.

Characteristics of Included Studies

As shown in Table 1, all eight included studies were designed as randomized controlled trials; however only one was conducted using a double-blind design (Krings et al., 2015). A total of 335 male and 275 female participants were included in the eight studies. All participants suffered chronic tinnitus defined as minimum 6 months in duration. The study by Krick et al. (2017) included participants with recent-onset tinnitus (i.e., persistent tinnitus <6 months) as a control group. No significant differences in the baseline characteristics of participants were suggested in all studies except the study by Hesser et al. (2009). This study showed the control group was significantly younger than the study group (control group vs. study group: 40.6 vs. 52.2, p = 0.016).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics and important information of studies included in this review.

Five different self-reported tinnitus assessment scales were used to measure tinnitus severity. They were the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (THI) (Hesser et al., 2009; Conrad et al., 2015; Kallogjeri et al., 2017), Tinnitus Questionnaire (TQ) (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Kröner-Herwig et al., 2006; Krick et al., 2017), Tinnitus Reaction Questionnaire (Hesser et al., 2009); Tinnitus Handicap Questionnaire (THQ) (Hoare et al., 2014), and Tinnitus Functional Index (TFI) (Krings et al., 2015). It is noteworthy that Kröner-Herwig et al. (2006) reported the changes in the TQ scores postintervention instead of the score at baseline and postintervention. Even though different measures of tinnitus severity were used, the majority of participants was identified as having moderate tinnitus severity.

A number of different interventions were used in these studies, i.e., cognitive behavioral therapy (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Kröner-Herwig et al., 2006; Conrad et al., 2015), brain fitness program (Krings et al., 2015; Kallogjeri et al., 2017), sound therapy (Hesser et al., 2009; Krick et al., 2017), and frequency discrimination training (Hoare et al., 2014). Of these, the brain fitness program was designed specifically for tinnitus sufferers to facilitate neuroplasticity for preservation and improvement in cognitive ability.

In the 8 studies, 11 different evaluations of cognitive functions were used. Three studies used self-reporting questionnaires as outcome measures (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Kröner-Herwig et al., 2006; Conrad et al., 2015). The other five studies used behavioral measurements with two using combinations of different tests to evaluate different aspects of cognitive functions (Krings et al., 2015; Kallogjeri et al., 2017), and three studies using three different behavioral tests to evaluate cognitive functions (Hesser et al., 2009; Hoare et al., 2014; Krick et al., 2017).

Risk of Bias Within Studies

Figure 2 shows the methodological quality of the eight studies. A small proportion and similar dropout rate occurred among the intervention and control groups in almost all studies, and consequently, bias due to attrition was low in all studies. Additionally, outcomes stated to be measured were all reported across studies, leading to a low risk of reporting bias. However, caution is required when interpreting the results of this review, as there is a possibility that the results might be influenced by publication bias. Although all studies were designed as randomized control trials, most studies did not provide details as to how the randomization and allocation concealment was conducted. As a result, a moderate risk of bias in randomized allocation was found in most studies. Of these included studies, Hoare et al. (2014) and Krings et al. (2015) provided sufficient information to avoid the risk of bias in most domains and provided a high-quality methodology. Although most studies did not report on the blinding of participants and outcome assessors, it is understandable that blinding might not be possible as the use of a placebo in the control group could be difficult in these types of studies, especially in those using no intervention group as comparison.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Risk of bias assessment of the included studies.

Because the sample size differs across the included studies (the participant numbers varied from 34 to 128), GPower 3.1 was used for conducting power analysis by extracting the sample size, mean differences between the groups, standard deviation, and alpha level from the included studies. Of these studies, five studies provided sufficient information for power analysis (i.e., Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Hesser et al., 2009; Hoare et al., 2014; Conrad et al., 2015; Krick et al., 2017). The study by Krick had the greatest power of over 80%, and Conrad's study provide a medium power of around 55%. However, the three remaining studies (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Hesser et al., 2009; Hoare et al., 2014) could be considered underpowered as they only had the power of 10, 30, and 15%, respectively. For example, a couple of studies demonstrated significant differences in both self-reported assessments of tinnitus and cognitive performance tests between the tinnitus patient group and the control group. Krick et al. (2017) included 113 participants (statistical power > 80%), whereas Hesser et al. (2009) had a small sample (18 participants in the study group vs. 17 controls, 35 participants in total, estimated statistical power = 10%) that the age differed significantly between patient group and control group. Therefore, their significance found by Hesser et al. (2009) should be interpreted with caution due to the possible age influence on the measurements. In contrast, the power analysis was unable to be conducted for the other three studies (i.e., Kröner-Herwig et al., 2006; Krings et al., 2015; Kallogjeri et al., 2017) because they did not provide essential information to calculate the mean differences between groups for power analysis. Therefore, it would be important for these studies to provide sufficient information on the power calculation, which would enable to improve the research efficiency and reliability of the research findings.

Impact of Tinnitus Interventions on Cognitive Functions

Hiller and Haerkötter (2005) and Conrad et al. (2015) both reported a positive impact of intervention on tinnitus perception and cognitive function. As shown in Table 1, their results using CBT showed significant improvements in tinnitus-related distress and cognitive dysfunction in comparison to control groups. In addition, the effect was long-term determined from the follow-up period. It should be noted that only self-reporting outcome measures were used in both studies, with no evidence gained from subjective behavior performance tests.

The brain fitness program and the program in combination with the antianxiety drug of D-cycloserine (DCS) used by Krings et al. (2015) and Kallogjeri et al. (2017), respectively, improved the performance in behavioral cognitive tests, but no significant difference was found between the tinnitus and control groups. In addition, the improvement in memory and attention assessed using a self-reporting questionnaire was found in the tinnitus group in comparison to the controls. However, it should be noted that the results of the study by Kallogjeri et al. (2017) might be affected by the nonblinding of participants because this study used nonintervention as the control group. The intervention effects may be characterized as a possible response bias because of different perspectives between intervention and nonintervention groups.

In the study by Hesser et al. (2009), the effect of background sounds on tinnitus intrusiveness, and cognitive function was compared. The group with control of background sounds chose a sound and loudness level they preferred, while participants in the second group had no control of background sounds and had to listen to a sound, which was determined for them in type and loudness over the trials. The results showed that participants in the group with controlled sound showed a significantly worse self-rated tinnitus interference over the trials, together with poorer performance in the Digit-Symbol Substitution Test than those receiving no control of background noise. This seems an unexpected result because a positive effect on reducing tinnitus perception was found when a masking noise at the same loudness was used (Vernon and Meikle, 1981; Aytac et al., 2017). The result was interpreted as indicating that controlled masking sounds may have a positive effect on the perception of tinnitus interference initially. However, the participants may increasingly experience that controlled masking sounds eventually lead to increased tinnitus interference (Hesser et al., 2009). An early study also found that excessive use of sound to mask tinnitus was associated with increased tinnitus severity and emotional distress in a sample of individuals with tinnitus (Budd and Pugh, 1996).

On the other hand, Jones and Macken (1993) suggested that complex auditory stimuli had a negative impact on cognitive processing when they investigated the effect of irrelevant tones on working memory. Because tinnitus often varies in pitch and loudness (Andersson et al., 2002), it would be interesting to know the association between tinnitus interference and cognitive processing function assessed using the Digit-Symbol Substitution Test. However, the study by Hesser et al. (2009) did not examine whether increased tinnitus interference was correlated with poorer cognitive performance.

Krick et al. (2017) investigated the effect of tinnitus duration and Heidelberg Neuro-Music therapy on visual attention in a total of 113 participants. This therapeutic approach is a manualized short-term music intervention, consisting of four components, and they are counseling, resonance training, neuroauditive cortex training, and tinnitus reconditioning (Argstatter et al., 2012). A negative correlation was found between tinnitus duration and visual continuous performance task error rates. This suggests that participants with chronic tinnitus make fewer mistakes in visual continuous performance tasks and suggests that Heidelberg Neuro-Music therapy helps shift attention from the tinnitus to cognitive tasks in patients with chronic tinnitus when compared to those patients with recent-onset tinnitus (Krick et al., 2017).

Hoare et al. (2014) evaluated the tinnitus severity and cognitive function of sustained attention following frequency discrimination training (FDT). The results suggested that FDT did not have a positive impact on tinnitus severity measured using THQ in the traditional FDT group and the computer-gameplay-based FDT group. In addition, no significant changes in The Test of Everyday Attention were found in any groups in this study. The negative results are likely due to only one cognitive function measure (i.e., Test of Everyday Attention) being used to assess sustained attention. The Test of Everyday Attention might not sensitively identify the changes in cognitive performance of attention as a result of FDT intervention. A review by Shipstead et al. (2012) suggests that changes in cognitive performance should be assessed using various tasks. This would improve the accuracy of detecting any changes in cognitive performance.

Cognitive Functions as a Predictor of Intervention Outcomes

Among all the included studies, only two studies addressed whether cognitive dysfunctions would predict poorer intervention outcomes. Conrad et al. (2015) examined cognitive dysfunction as a possible predictor of the effectiveness of CBT on tinnitus-related distress. No association was found between cognitive dysfunction and therapy outcome when using the complete sample. There was, however, after separating the sample into ICBT and GCBT groups, a negative correlation between catastrophic tinnitus-related thoughts and outcome for ICBT, but not for GCBT, i.e., a higher score of subscales at baseline predicted poorer therapy outcomes in emotional tinnitus-related distress in ICBT. In the follow-up assessments, no correlations were observed, suggesting that the negative impact of catastrophic thoughts did not have any long-term effect on therapy outcomes. However, inconsistent results were found in the study by Kröner-Herwig et al. (2006). Although they hypothesized that a higher level of catastrophic tinnitus-related cognition results in a poorer intervention outcome, their results did not suggest any negative association between dysfunctional cognitions and therapy outcomes. The discrepancy between these studies may be caused by the use of different cognitive assessments (i.e., Tinnitus Cognitions Scale and Tinnitus-Dysfunctional-Cognitions Questionnaire in Conrad's study and Kröner-Herwig's study, respectively) and different outcome measures of tinnitus severity (i.e., TQ in Kröner-Herwig's study vs. THI in Conrad' study). In addition, a weak significant correlation (p = 0.045) between self-reported questionnaire of cognition and intervention outcomes found in Conrad et al. (2015) may be due to increase in the sample size, because Conrad et al. (2015) had a larger sample size than Kröner-Herwig et al. (2006) (128 vs. 58 participants).

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to investigate the association between the effectiveness of tinnitus interventions and cognitive functions in patients with tinnitus by critically analyzing relevant randomized control trials. Most of the included trials utilized behavior therapies as tinnitus interventions with a focus on cognitive training. The results suggest that interventions used in these trials not only facilitate tinnitus management but also improve cognitive functions measured using either self-reported questionnaires or behavior performance tests in patients with tinnitus.

CBT, a psychological treatment that focuses on cognition and emotion, provides a better understanding of the relationships between our feelings, thinking, behaviors, and environment, and the ways in which these can become problematic. Currently, there is substantial evidence on the positive benefits of CBT on various mental health conditions and chronic psychological disorders as well as patients with chronic tinnitus (Hesser et al., 2011; Hans and Hiller, 2013). As a result, the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) on tinnitus assessment and management recommends CBT as an effective tool to reduce the tinnitus-related distress, and thus alleviate the severity of tinnitus.

In this review, cognitive behavioral therapy and brain fitness program were used in four included studies, primarily focusing on alteration of cognition and emotion (Hiller and Haerkötter, 2005; Conrad et al., 2015; Krings et al., 2015; Kallogjeri et al., 2017). Their results showed positive outcomes in terms of improvement in tinnitus severity and cognitive function, which indicates that improvement in cognitive ability and alteration in how tinnitus and its consequences are perceived leads to positive attitudes toward tinnitus and reduction in tinnitus severity. Interventions that focus on cognition and emotion were also used in studies of other disciplines of chronic pain, and study results suggested that improved cognitive ability and positive emotions have an effect on symptoms alleviation (George et al., 2010; Keefe and Somers, 2010). This suggests that psychological factors, e.g., cognition, emotion, and behavior play an important role for patients with a chronic disease to cope with chronic symptoms, such as tinnitus.

A previous study by Andersson and McKenna (2006) suggested that cognitive deficits, cognitive bias, and beliefs and attitudes toward tinnitus are three important stages on the course from tinnitus perception to annoyance. Of these stages, cognitive deficits in terms of control of attention lead to disruption of information processing when tinnitus competes with other activities. Evidence suggested that patients with tinnitus performed worse in the cognitive tests of attention compared with healthy control without tinnitus (Jackson et al., 2014; Cardon et al., 2019). A cognitive bias in patients with tinnitus can be defined as a systematic error, particularly in memory and attention, when they are processing and interpreting information that occurs around them and thus affects judgement and decision-making. Evidence suggested that patients with tinnitus paid selective attention to information related to tinnitus and tend to make decisions based on a biased recourse (Andersson et al., 2000, 2003). Beliefs and attitudes toward tinnitus are also important in terms of moderating the adverse effect of tinnitus. The study by Conrad et al. (2015) showed that negative thoughts of tinnitus were negatively associated with the effect of ICBT on tinnitus distress.

According to the model proposed by these authors, it is not sufficient for tinnitus to attract attention for it to be annoying, and even though tinnitus is correlated with psychopathology, it does not always lead to distress. Tinnitus is likely to become a serious problem when it interferes with the ability of the patient to think, when his or her general attitude in life is either anxious or depressive and when tinnitus is seen as the cause of the problems (e.g., insomnia and concentration problems). A meta-analysis by Hesser et al. (2011) concluded that CBT has a positive impact on annoyance and distress associated with tinnitus. Therefore, with an understanding of underlying issues, together with a focus on retraining thoughts and alteration of behaviors, it is possible that therapeutic interventions on cognitive functions in terms of making changes to how people feel can provide a viable and effective approach in ameliorating tinnitus.

It is noteworthy that sound-based interventions demonstrate positive results in both tinnitus and cognitions in the study by Krick et al. (2017). This suggests that better performance in cognitive function is the result of less attention being placed on the tinnitus and indicates an association between the benefits of sound-based approaches and better cognitive function in patients with tinnitus. However, more studies using standard intervention and outcome measures need to be undertaken to reach a solid conclusion of the association between the effectiveness of sound-based interventions and cognitive function in patients with tinnitus. As discussed earlier, although the study by Conrad et al. (2015) found a negative correlation between catastrophic tinnitus-related thoughts (a form of cognitive dysfunction) and therapeutic outcomes on emotional changes, it remains unclear whether cognitive functions would be a predictor of outcomes when using tinnitus interventions.

However, negative correlations between cognition and treatment outcomes were reported in other disciplines, such as chronic pain. The cognitive dysfunction is consistently correlated with reported pain severity and poor treatment outcomes in patients with rheumatic diseases (Edwards et al., 2011). The authors suggested that negative attitudes and beliefs related to pain can be used as a predictor for non-improvement after surgery or other treatments. A similar conclusion has been drawn by Quartana et al. (2009), who conclude that catastrophizing thoughts toward the chronic pain can lead to more difficulties in suppressing pain-related thoughts and associate with a number of important outcomes in pain treatment.

Various outcome measures of cognitive functions have been used in categorizing either self-reported measurement or behavioral cognitive tests. Evidence shows a lack of correlation between self-reported measures and performance in cognitive function tests. For example, a study by Hallam et al. (2004) examined the correlation between cognitive performance and score in a self-reported CFQ. The results showed that CFQ was associated with performance on the dual-task version delayed serial digit recall (a working memory test), but the correlation coefficients were low (between 0.21 and 0.33). Among the included studies within the present review, only two used both self-reported measurement and behavioral tests to measure cognitive functions. The study by Krings et al. (2015) revealed improvement in tinnitus, behavioral, and self-reported cognitive functions, while Kallogjeri et al. (2017) only reported improvement in tinnitus and self-reported cognitive functions. Therefore, the reliability of self-reported measurement or behavioral cognitive tests needs to be further examined.

Furthermore, although confounding factors, e.g., age, degree of hearing loss, depression, and anxiety, have been considered and controlled in most of the individual studies, the average age of participants ranged between 45 and 55 years old. A psychological study testing memory capacity among people aged from 50 to 79 suggested that the precision of the evaluations of individuals' own abilities is dramatically altered by age-related changes in self-regard and lifestyle (Rabbitt and Abson, 1991). Moreover, a cross-sectional and longitudinal study by Wielgos et al. (1999) also showed a significant negative correlation between the cognitive performance test, digit symbol task, and age. Therefore, it would be useful to consider a wider age range in future studies on the association between the effectiveness of tinnitus interventions and cognitive functions in patients with tinnitus.

Evidence indicates that tinnitus is commonly associated with hearing loss (Baguley et al., 2013; Basso et al., 2020). Because several previous studies suggest the associations between hearing loss and impairment of cognitive function (e.g., memory, executive function, and attention) (Lin, 2011; Saunders et al., 2018), it is therefore important to consider the negative influence of hearing loss on cognitive performance when investigating the impact of tinnitus on the cognitive function in individuals with tinnitus. Waechter et al. (2020) found that working memory was not influenced by tinnitus but rather by hearing status in patients with tinnitus. Therefore, the factor of hearing loss should be considered when the studies compare cognitive performance in individuals with tinnitus and without tinnitus.

One of the selected studies recruited patients with chronic and recent-onset tinnitus where there was no difference in tinnitus severity but poorer performance in cognitive tasks in patient with chronic tinnitus after intervention (Krick et al., 2017). This is consistent with the findings by Trevis et al. (2016), which investigated a variety of components that affect attention switching, including cognitive control, inhibition, and working memory in patients with tinnitus. Their results suggested that major impairment in cognitive control and inhibition as well as lower emotional well-being can be caused by long-lasting tinnitus. The difficulties in attention switching caused by the long-term chronic tinnitus may be associated with the structural change between attention-related and self-reference brain networks (Lanting et al., 2016; Leaver et al., 2016). Therefore, there is a need for more evidence on the influence of tinnitus duration on the association between the effectiveness of tinnitus interventions and cognitive functions in patients with tinnitus.

The present review does need to be considered in light of several limitations. Although the quality of each included study appears appropriate, due to some heterogeneity in tinnitus interventions and behavioral cognitive measurements, it is difficult to conduct a meta-analysis and consequently did not provide a quantitative review of the effect of tinnitus intervention on both tinnitus severity and cognitive performance. Therefore, a more homogeneous set of interventions and outcome measurements is needed in order to be conclusive as to the association between tinnitus interventions and cognitive functions in patients with tinnitus.

Conclusion

Favorable results in both tinnitus severity and cognitive functions of patients with tinnitus were reported in most of the included studies in this review. However, whether cognition can predict treatment outcomes is unclear due to insufficient evidence. The significant heterogeneity of interventions and outcome measures in the included studies makes it challenging to compare the studies' outcomes. Therefore, the association between tinnitus interventions and cognitive functions in a patient with tinnitus should be interpreted with caution. In addition, self-reported measures and behavioral assessments in cognition should both be employed in future trials. The influence of age and tinnitus duration should be considered as well. Future research with a more homogeneous methodology focused on the effect of sound-based interventions on both tinnitus and cognitive functions and whether cognition is a predictor of treatment outcome is needed.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary materials, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

TL, NP, and FZ conceived and designed the review. TL and ZC developed search strategies and conducted database search and article selection. All authors contributed to the writing.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank all three reviewers and the editor for their valuable time and insightful comments and suggestions. We would also like to acknowledge Dr. Christopher Wigham for his proofreading.

References

Andersson, G., Lyttkens, L., and Larsen, H. C. (1999). Distinguishing levels of tinnitus distress. Clin. Otolaryngol. Allied Sci. 24, 404–410. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.1999.00278.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Eriksson, J., Lundh, L. G., and Lyttkens, L. (2000). Tinnitus and cognitive interference: a Stroop paradigm study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 43, 1168–1173. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4305.1168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Ingerholt, C., and Jansson, M. (2003). Autobiographical memory in patients with tinnitus. Psychol. Health 18, 667–675. doi: 10.1080/0887044031000112100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., Khakpoor, A., and Lyttkens, L. (2002). Masking of tinnitus and mental activity. Clin. Otolaryngol. Allied Sci. 27, 270–274. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.2002.00581.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andersson, G., and McKenna, L. (2006). The role of cognition in tinnitus. Acta Otolaryngol. 126, 39–43. doi: 10.1080/03655230600895226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Argstatter, H., Grapp, M., Hutter, E., Plinkert, P., and Bolay, H. V. (2012). Long-term effects of the “Heidelberg Model of Music Therapy” in patients with chronic tinnitus. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Med. 5, 273–288.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Aytac, I., Baysal, E., Gulsen, S., Tumuklu, K., Durucu, C., Mumbuc, L. S., et al. (2017). Masking treatment and its effect on tinnitus parameters. Int. Tinnitus J. 21:83–89. doi: 10.5935/0946-5448.20170017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baguley, D., McFerran, D., and Hall, D. (2013). Tinnitus. Lancet. 382, 1600–1607. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)60142-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Basso, L., Boecking, B., Brueggemann, P., Pedersen, N. L., Canlon, B., Cederroth, C. R., et al. (2020). Gender-specific risk factors and comorbidities of bothersome tinnitus. Front. Neurosci. 14:706. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Budd, R. J., and Pugh, R. (1996). Tinnitus coping style and its relationship to tinnitus severity and emotional distress. J. Psychosom. Res. 41, 327–335. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(96)00171-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardon, E., Jacquemin, L., Mertens, G., Van de Heyning, P., Vanderveken, O. M., Topsakal, V., et al. (2019). Cognitive performance in chronic tinnitus patients: a cross-sectional study using the RBANS-H. Otol. Neurotol. 40, e876–e882. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, N. A., Akeroyd, M. A., Henshaw, H., and Hoare, D. J. (2018). Association between subjective tinnitus and cognitive performance: protocol for systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open 8, e023700. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conrad, I., Kleinstäuber, M., Jasper, K., Hiller, W., Andersson, G., and Weise, C. (2015). The changeability and predictive value of dysfunctional cognitions in cognitive behaviour therapy for chronic tinnitus. Int. J. Behav. Med. 22, 239–250. doi: 10.1007/s12529-014-9425-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, A., and El Rafaie, A. (2000) Epidemiology of tinnitus. In: Tinnitus Handbook, Vol 1 ed. Tyler R. (San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group), 1–23.

Google Scholar

De Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., Weisz, N., Londero, A., Schlee, W., Elgoyhen, A. B., et al. (2014). An integrative model of auditory phantom perception: tinnitus as a unified percept of interacting separable subnetworks. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44, 16–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, R. R., Cahalan, C., Mensing, G., Smith, M., and Haythornthwaite, J. A. (2011). Pain, catastrophizing, and depression in the rheumatic diseases. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 7, 216–224. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2011.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuller, T., Cima, R., Langguth, B., Mazurek, B., Vlaeyen, J. W., and Hoare, D. J. (2020). Cognitive behavioural therapy for tinnitus. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 1:CD012614. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD012614.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

George, S. Z., Wittmer, V. T., Fillingim, R. B., and Robinson, M. E. (2010). Comparison of graded exercise and graded exposure clinical outcomes for patients with chronic low back pain. J. Orthop. Sports Phys. Therapy 40, 694–704. doi: 10.2519/jospt.2010.3396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallam, R. S., McKenna, L., and Shurlock, L. (2004). Tinnitus impairs cognitive efficiency. Int. J. Audiol. 43, 218–226. doi: 10.1080/14992020400050030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hans, E., and Hiller, W. (2013). A meta-analysis of nonrandomized effectiveness studies on outpatient cognitive behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 954–964. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesser, H., Pereswetoff-Morath, C. E., and Andersson, G. (2009). Consequences of controlling background sounds: the effect of experiential avoidance on tinnitus interference. Rehabil. Psychol. 54, 381–389. doi: 10.1037/a0017565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesser, H., Weise, C., Westin, V. Z., and Andersson, G. (2011). A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials of cognitive-behavioural therapy for tinnitus distress. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 31, 545–553. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hiller, W., and Haerkötter, C. (2005). Does sound stimulation have additive effects on cognitive-behavioural treatment of chronic tinnitus?. Behav. Res. Ther. 43, 595–612. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoare, D. J., Van Labeke, N., McCormack, A., Sereda, M., Smith, S., Al Taher, H., et al. (2014). Gameplay as a source of intrinsic motivation in a randomized controlled trial of auditory training for tinnitus. PLoS One 9:e0107430. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, S., and Padgham, N. D. (2009). More than ringing in the ears: a review of tinnitus and its psychosocial impact. J. Clin. Nurs. 18, 2927–2937. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2702.2009.02909.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. G., Coyne, I. J., and Clough, P. J. (2014). A preliminary investigation of potential cognitive performance decrements in non-help-seeking tinnitus sufferers. Int. J. Audiol. 53, 88–93. doi: 10.3109/14992027.2013.846481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. M., and Macken, W. J. (1993). Irrelevant tones produce an irrelevant speech effect: implications for phonological coding in working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 369–381. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.19.2.369

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kallogjeri, D., Piccirillo, J. F., Spitznagel, E., Hale, S., Nicklaus, J. E., Hardin, F. M., et al. (2017). Cognitive training for adults with bothersome tinnitus: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 143, 443–451. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2016.3779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keefe, F. J., and Somers, T. J. (2010). Psychological approaches to understanding and treating arthritis pain. Nat. Rev. Rheumatol. 6, 210–216. doi: 10.1038/nrrheum.2010.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krick, C. M., Argstatter, H., Grapp, M., Plinkert, P. K., and Reith, W. (2017). Heidelberg neuro-music therapy enhances task-negative activity in tinnitus patients. Front. Neurosci. 11:384. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krings, J. G., Wineland, A., Kallogjeri, D., Rodebaugh, T. L., Nicklaus, J., Lenze, E. J., et al. (2015). A novel treatment for tinnitus and tinnitus-related cognitive difficulties using computer-based cognitive training and D-Cycloserine. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 141, 18–26. doi: 10.1001/jamaoto.2014.2669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kröner-Herwig, B., Zachriat, C., and Weigand, D. (2006). Do patient characteristics predict outcome in the outpatient treatment of chronic tinnitus?. GMS Psycho Soc. Med. 3, Doc07. Available online at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2736505/pdf/PSM-03-07.pdf

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Langguth, B., Kreuzer, P. M., Kleinjung, T., and De Ridder, D. (2013). Tinnitus: causes and clinical management. Lancet Neurol. 12, 920–930. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70160-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanting, C., WozAniak, A., van Dijk, P., and Langers, D. R. (2016). “Tinnitus-and task-related differences in resting-state networks,” in Physiology, Psychoacoustics and Cognition in Normal and Impaired Hearing, P. van Dijk, D. Başkent, E. Gaudrain, E. de Kleine, A. Wagner, and C. Lanting (Cham: Springer), 175–187.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Leaver, A. M., Turesky, T. K., Seydell-Greenwald, A., Morgan, S., Kim, H. J., and Rauschecker, J. P. (2016). Intrinsic network activity in tinnitus investigated using functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2717–2735. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, F. R. (2011). Hearing loss and cognition among older adults in the United States. J. Gerontol. A Biomed. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 1131–1136. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKenna, L., Hallam, R. S., and Shurlock, L. (1995). “Cognitive functioning in tinnitus patients,” in Proceedings of the Fifth International Tinnitus Seminar (Portland: American Tinnitus Association), 589–595.

Mohamad, N., Hoare, D. J., and Hall, D. A. (2016). The consequences of tinnitus and tinnitus severity on cognition: a review of the behavioural evidence. Hear. Res. 332, 199–209. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pienkowski, M. (2019). Rationale and efficacy of sound therapies for tinnitus and hyperacusis. Neuroscience 407, 120–134. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, K. J., Kallogjeri, D., Piccirillo, J. F., Garcia, K. S., Nicklaus, J. E., and Burton, H. (2012). Effects of severe bothersome tinnitus on cognitive function measured with standardized tests. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 34, 126–134. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.623120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quartana, P. J., Campbell, C. M., and Edwards, R. R. (2009). Pain catastrophizing: a critical review. Expert Rev. Neurother. 9, 745–758. doi: 10.1586/ern.09.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabbitt, P., and Abson, V. (1991). Do older people know how good they are?. Br. J. Psychol. 82, 137–151. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02389.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossiter, S., Stevens, C., and Walker, G. (2006). Tinnitus and its effect on working memory and attention. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49:150–160. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/012)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, G. H., Odgear, I., Cosgrove, A., and Frederick, M. T. (2018). Impact of hearing loss and amplification on performance on a cognitive screening test. J. Am. Acad. Audiol. 29, 648–655. doi: 10.3766/jaaa.17044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective?. Psychol. Bull. 138, 628–654. doi: 10.1037/a0027473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siadaty, M. S., Shu, J., and Knaus, W. A. (2007). Relemed: sentence-level search engine with relevance score for the MEDLINE database of biomedical articles. BMC Med. Inform. Decis. Makers 7, 1. doi: 10.1186/1472-6947-7-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C., and Bavelier, D. (2012). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: a cognitive neuroscience perspective. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2, S30–S48. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tegg-Quinn, S., Bennett, R. J., Eikelboom, R. H., and Baguley, D. M. (2016). The impact of tinnitus upon cognition in adults: a systematic review. Int. J. Audiol. 55, 533–540. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2016.1185168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevis, K. J., McLachlan, N. M., and Wilson, S. J. (2016). Cognitive mechanisms in chronic tinnitus: psychological markers of a failure to switch attention. Front. Psychol. 7, 1262. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevis, K. J., McLachlan, N. M., and Wilson, S. J. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of psychological functioning in chronic tinnitus. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 60, 62–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, R. S., and Baker, L. J. (1983). Difficulties experienced by tinnitus sufferers. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 48, 150–154. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4802.150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, R. S., Noble, W., Preece, J. P., Dunn, C. C., and Witt, S. A. (2004). “Psychological treatments for tinnitus,” in Tinnitus: Theory and Management, ed J. Snow (Hamilton: BC Decker), 314–323.

Google Scholar

Vernon, J. A., and Meikle, M. B. (1981). Tinnitus masking: unresolved problems. Ciba Found. Symp. 85, 239–256. doi: 10.1002/9780470720677.ch14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waechter, S., Wilson, W. J., and Brännström, J. K. (2020). The impact of tinnitus on working memory capacity. Int. J. Audiol. 1–8. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1822550. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wielgos, M., Walter, R., and Cunningham, C. (1999). Age-related slowing on the digit symbol task: longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses. Exp. Aging Res. 25, 109–120. doi: 10.1080/036107399244048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: tinnitus, cognition, tinnitus sound therapy, intervention, cognitive behavioral therapy

Citation: Lan T, Cao Z, Zhao F and Perham N (2021) The Association Between Effectiveness of Tinnitus Intervention and Cognitive Function—A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 11:553449. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.553449

Received: 18 April 2020; Accepted: 27 November 2020;
Published: 06 January 2021.

Edited by:

Mary Rudner, Linköping University, Sweden

Reviewed by:

Paul Smith, University of Otago, New Zealand
Andreia Aparecida Azevedo, Federal University of São Paulo, Brazil
K. Jonas Brännström, Lund University, Sweden

Copyright © 2021 Lan, Cao, Zhao and Perham. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fei Zhao, fzhao@cardiffmet.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.