- 1Research Institute for Neuroscience, Education and Didactics, Patrizio Paoletti Foundation, Assisi, Italy
- 2Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Neuroimaging Laboratory, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
- 3Human Neuroscience Department, Neuroimaging Laboratory, Sapienza University, Rome, Italy
- 4Department of Criminology, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
- 5The Leslie and Susan Gonda (Goldschmied) Multidisciplinary Brain Research Center, Bar-Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel
- 6Department of Neuroscience, Imaging, and Clinical Science, Behavioral Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND) Center, University of Chieti-Pescara G. d’Annunzio, Chieti, Italy
- 7Department of Educational Sciences, Roma Tre University, Rome, Italy
- 8Departmental Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, Università “Campus Bio-Medico di Roma,” Rome, Italy
Silence is an important aspect of various meditation practices, but little work has focused specifically on the underlying neurophysiology of silence-related meditative practice, and on how it relates to the self-reported experiences of practitioners. To expand current knowledge regarding the neurophenomenology of silence in meditation, we directly investigated first-person reports of silence-related experiences during the practice of Quadrato Motor Training (QMT) and their association with changes in fractional anisotropy (FA). Participants recorded their cognitive, emotional, and physical experiences upon beginning QMT and again after 6 weeks of QMT practice. These reports were evaluated qualitatively and quantitatively. Findings showed that change between the two time points in self-reported silence-related experiences was negatively correlated with change in attentional effort, and positively correlated with changes in the left uncinate fasciculus. These results expand current knowledge regarding the neuroanatomical correlates of silence-related experiences during meditation.
Introduction
Silence plays a role in most Eastern and Western meditative practices, such as Zen meditation and Vipassana (Lin et al., 2008; Shonin et al., 2013). In the history of Buddhist doctrines described in Ariya Pariyesanā Sutta, for example, the Buddha urges monks to maintain “Noble Silence” (Anālayo, 2017). This expression represents a meditative state called “dhyana,” an altered state of consciousness characterized by freedom from thoughts and mental representations, enabling a deep stillness (Feuerstein, 1996).
While many researchers have emphasized the importance of silence in meditation (Vago and Zeidan, 2016; Pfeifer et al., 2019, 2020), to date little work has focused on the self-reported experiences of practitioners and the underlying neurophysiological changes associated with silence-related experience of meditative practices (Hernández et al., 2018). For example, a 15 min Quiet Time training consisting of meditation or another quiet activity such as reading silently was found to significantly increase resilience and decrease anxiety compared to a control group, showing increased emotional regulation (Wendt et al., 2015). The students who spent more time meditating also had higher resilience scores, self-reported improved sleep, happiness, and self-confidence.
It has recently been suggested that through a voluntary act of silence, used as a paradigm similar to sensory deprivation one can intentionally move from the narrative self, to the minimal and eventually overcoming the self (Paoletti et al., 2020; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020). The state of overcoming of the self, which can further be compared with self-transcendence, absorption, and non-dual states (Josipovic, 2014; Vieten et al., 2018), is characterized by changes in the perception of time and space shifting gradually toward “timelessness” and “spacelessness” (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2013; Wittmann, 2015, 2020). Importantly, similar experiences following the Quadrato Motor Training (QMT) have been reported (Ben-Soussan et al., 2013, 2014b, 2019). For example, a recent cross-sectional study revealed that advanced-QMT practitioners showed longer and more accurate produced durations in a time production task, than did Aikido practitioners and a physically inactive control group (Ben-Soussan et al., 2019), suggesting that similarly to other forms of meditation (Wittmann, 2015), QMT has the capacity to dilate the subjective time experienced, possibly by inducing greater awareness of the present moment and of the body. In fact, QMT has been recently ascribed to mindfulness practices involving movement (Diamond and Ling, 2019), trading off speed with the opportunity for mindfulness and reflectivity (Ben-Soussan et al., 2014b; Diamond and Ling, 2017).
Mindful movements are characterized by a focus on movement in the present moment while excluding other thoughts and body movements (Kabat-Zinn, 2009; De Fano et al., 2019). Mindful movement practices involve key aspects of mindfulness such as preparation and execution of movement, regulation of attention, working memory, and decreased mind-wandering (Russell and Arcuri, 2015), mind-wandering being a construct opposing that of mindfulness (Mrazek et al., 2012). QMT involves each of these aspects: regulation of divided attention, working memory updating (e.g., noting one’s current location to know where to move to next), and prevention of mind-wandering via a need to be “in the here and now” due to constantly updating commands (Ben-Soussan et al., 2014b; De Fano et al., 2019). Mindful movement further engages “higher-order” inhibition and response selection that underlie attention and cognitive control that require moment-by-moment sensorimotor updating (Clark et al., 2015; Kimmel and Rogler, 2018). In line with this, QMT requires second-by-second mindful awareness for attending the upcoming next command (Ben-Soussan et al., 2014b; De Fano et al., 2019).
QMT (for a recent review see De Fano et al., 2019) is a movement meditation in which both individual experience and brain functioning have been examined. During QMT, practitioners step in different directions on a square delineated on the floor. The method requires smoothly executed, goal-directed behavior in response to predetermined verbal instructions separated by silent interstimulus intervals (ISIs), which are known to increase the duration of attention (Leckart et al., 1970). These quiet interstimulus intervals can be considered moments of external silence permitting also inner silence in which the participant awaits the upcoming command. In fact, it has previously been claimed that silence allows for better management of stimuli, representing a state of preparation in and toward emptiness (Teschner, 1981; Davies and Turner, 2002; Stratton, 2015; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020) which in the current context is manifested by the upcoming command.
In line with this, previous work suggests that QMT can induce a state of waiting (Ben-Soussan et al., 2014b), which might decrease mind-wandering and narrative-focused thought (Farb et al., 2007), thereby eliciting the experience of inner silence and increase a state of mindfulness. A month of QMT by both meditation practitioners and non-practitioners groups, in contrast to a single session of QMT was further found to report altered states of consciousness and attentional effort experiences. Crucially, these experiences were reported by the groups that practiced the QMT for 4 weeks, but not in the group that practiced it for only 1 day (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017).
In a recent longitudinal study, Piervincenzi et al. (2017) further reported that six weeks of daily QMT practice led to a bilateral increase of fractional anisotropy (FA) in tracts related to sensorimotor and cognitive functions (including the corticospinal tracts, anterior thalamic radiations, and uncinate fasciculi), reflecting better white matter integrity. However, the relationship between the brain changes and self-reported experiences associated with QMT remains currently unknown, particularly with respect to silence-related components.
Importantly, there is growing evidence that increased mindfulness, may act as a marker for improved emotion regulation skills (Creswell et al., 2007) and positive reappraisal (Garland et al., 2009, 2011). These two cognitive coping strategies are thought to be mechanisms by which mindfulness regulates emotion and stress (Vago and David, 2012), functions which are becoming increasingly important. In this context, it is important to note that the uncinate fasciculus is a tract of fibers connecting limbic structures to the prefrontal cortex. The uncinate fasciculus is associated with many functions ranging from learning visual associations to episodic memory, language and social emotional processing (Thomas et al., 2012, 2015; Von Der Heide et al., 2013). It is critical for both emotion regulation, transformation and reappraisal, and may thus underlie the improvements observed in the study of the effects of different types of meditation (Vago and David, 2012; d’Arbeloff et al., 2018).
These results related to the relationship between reappraisal, mindfulness and neurophysiological changes are further in line with different models of consciousness, such as the Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART) model (Vago and David, 2012) and the Sphere Model of Consciousness (SMC, Paoletti, 2002; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2019, 2020; Paoletti et al., 2020). According to the SMC, reappraisal and voluntary movement toward a state of contentless consciousness, also defined as momentary silent consciousness (Baars, 2013; Josipovic, 2019) may have a decisive role in the experience (Paoletti et al., 2020; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020), in which focusing one’s attention on silence can be used as a paradigm for facilitating better internal communication through an internal environment intentionally created (Paoletti and Selvaggio, 2013; Paoletti et al., 2017; De Fano et al., 2019). Consequently, in this study, silence was operationalized as self-reported absence of thoughts and a subjective experience of mental quietness as suggested by various authors (Rudrauf et al., 2003; Hinterberger et al., 2014; Josipovic, 2014, 2019; Winter et al., 2019; see Table 1), thus describing a state in which participants spontaneously experienced and reported a sort of contentless consciousness.
Table 1. Definition of the macro-categories and sub-units produced by qualitative content analysis of diary entries on Quadrato Motor Training (QMT).
While the described neuroplastic changes are intriguing and can potentially underlie the benefits ascribed to the practice of mindfulness meditation, it has often been neglected to actually test the relationship between neuroplastic processes and changes in lived experience (Varela et al., 2016), especially in the context of silence. To expand current knowledge regarding the neurophenomenology of silence in meditation, we used a new approach to investigate first-person reports of silence-related experiences during QMT in relation to the changes in white matter as measured by FA. Together with the subjective experiences of silence during QMT practice, we expected to find an association between changes in silence-related self-report categories and FA changes.
Examining the relationship between silence and neuroanatomical changes can aid in unraveling the neuronal mechanisms underlying potential changes in the lived experience reported by subjects, as well as training induced-silence, and cognitive and emotional changes reported by the participants (Ben-Soussan et al., 2013, 2017). As silence, which is both required and produced in the QMT, may also be the means by which attention and mindfulness could be increased (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017; Paoletti et al., 2020; Paoletti and Ben-Soussan, 2020), and since these changes have previously been linked to, among others, FA changes in the uncinate fasciculus (Luders et al., 2011; Tang et al., 2012; Hölzel et al., 2016; Piervincenzi et al., 2017), we hypothesized that changes in self-reported silence-related experiences would be correlated with changes in the uncinate fasciculus structural connectivity. More specifically, we expected to find a positive correlation between change in these two variables, reflecting a silence-related enhanced connectivity in brain structures previously reported to be related to emotional and cognitive regulation associated with mindful experience (Creswell et al., 2007; Garland et al., 2009, 2011). In this context, it is important to note that the uncinate fasciculus is a bidirectional, long-range white matter tract that connects lateral orbitofrontal cortex and Brodmann area 10 with the anterior temporal lobes (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2007; Wheeler et al., 2017). Brodmann area 10 is thought to enable maintenance of previously running tasks in a pending state for subsequent retrieval and execution upon completion of an ongoing task (Koechlin and Hyafil, 2007) – an ability that is constantly required during QMT (Ben-Soussan et al., 2014a,b; De Fano et al., 2019). In addition, it is further related to emotional regulation (Steffens et al., 2011), which was previously found to improve following QMT (for a recent review see De Fano et al., 2019).
In addition, we examined relationships between spontaneous silence-related and other categories of self-reported experience during QMT. Silence shares characteristics with other states elicited by deliberative mindful practice, such as flow (Ben-Soussan et al., 2013) and mindfulness (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017), both of which are associated with reduced attentional effort (Harmat et al., 2015). For this reason, we further wanted to investigate possible interactions between experience of silence and other states or cognitive functions associated with QMT practice. In accordance with previous studies, we expected changes in silence-related categories to be positively correlated with change in mindfulness categories, and to be negatively correlated with change in attentional effort categories.
Methods
Participants and Design
We recruited 50 healthy volunteers in accordance with the inclusion and exclusion criteria reported in Piervincenzi et al. (2017). The researchers explained the study aims and procedures to participants and verified that they understood. All participants signed written informed consent forms in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The ethical committee of the Università Campus Bio-Medico di Roma, Rome, Italy, approved the experimental phase I study entitled “Effect of Quadrato Motor Training on the brain of healthy volunteers” (MOTO-BRAIN, 09/14 PAR ComEt CBM). The present study was part of a larger project investigating longitudinal effects of QMT using various brain imaging techniques (results published in Lasaponara et al., 2017; Piervincenzi et al., 2017). Participants underwent magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and electroencephalography (EEG) at two time points: the day of recruitment (t0) and following 6 weeks of daily QMT practice (t1). Participants were asked to keep a personal diary to document information about their practice and habits during the period of exercise. After excluding participants who did not complete the questionnaires at both time points, the final sample included 22 healthy right-handed participants (12 women, mean age 35.6 years, SD 5.4).
Quadrato Motor Training (QMT)
During QMT, participants stand at one corner of a 0.5m × 0.5 m square and move to different corners in response to recorded verbal instructions provided at variable intervals indicating the next corner to which they should move. In order to correctly perform QMT there are different rules such as start each step with the leg inside the square (i.e., the right leg when on the left side of the square and vice versa), wait for the instruction without anticipating the movement and keep their gaze on an empty wall without looking at the platform or the position of the feet. So, participants must carefully control the quality of each movement, inhibit automatic motor responses before the instruction and divide their attention toward the external instructions, the proper task execution and the internal state of the body, sustaining their attention for the whole practice duration. For additional details, see Ben-Soussan et al. (2013).
First-Person Reports
Participants kept a diary in which they were required to document their responses to three questions, regarding their (1) cognitive, (2) emotional, and (3) physical experiences during QMT. Data from the diaries were coded into an Excel worksheet and analyzed using qualitative content analysis (Mayring, 2004). In the first step, we constructed a grid to define the content categories. We began this process with a grid developed and validated in previous work (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017) and proceeded describing macro-categories and sub-units that will be considered to evaluate the content of first person reports. Along with these already used categories, we added an additional category related to silence and reduced mind-wandering (collectively called “Silencing”), which represents the main focus of the current study. We included Reduced Mind-wandering in the Silencing macro-category, as the reduction in intrusive, irrelevant, and unintentional thoughts represents an important step toward quiet restful presence (Vago and Zeidan, 2016). An exhaustive list of macro-categories and sub-units with their definitions is provided in Table 1.
In this study, response provided to each of the three questions served as the meaningful unit of analysis. Our corpus thus comprised 132 units of analysis, which could be coded with one or more indicators depending on the content (examples of coding for each sub-unit are provided in Table 2). This codification method was chosen because it enables occurrence and co-occurrence analyses (see Qualitative Analysis section below). Three researchers coded the answers using Atlas.ti software (Atlas.ti Scientific Software Development GmbH, Berlin, Germany). Inter-coder reliability was calculated using Krippendorff’s alpha, an index based on disagreements rather than agreements in the coding phase. According to Krippendorff (2018), the simplest inter-coder agreement coefficient is:
The acceptability threshold for this index is >0.667, whereas >0.800 is considered good (Krippendorff, 2018). We calculated Krippendorff’s alpha separately for each macro-category used in the study, resulting in an overall alpha of 0.758 (Silencing: 0.906, Attentional Effort: 0.728, Mindfulness: 0.797, Altered State of Consciousness: 0.388). Given the low reliability of the Altered State of Consciousness category, we excluded it from further analysis.
Qualitative Analysis
Occurrence analysis was used to determine how many times a specific category was assigned to a sentence in t0 and t1 time points. This kind of analysis allows comparison of each macro-category and sub-unit occurrence between time points (taking all participants into account). Co-occurrence defines how many times two sub-units are coded together in the same sentence. Two main measures were utilized: the number of times a specific co-occurrence between two units was found and the strength of the relationship between two units (c-coefficient). C-coefficient varies between 0 (no co-occurrence) and 1 (full co-occurrence). It is calculated as follows:
where n12 is the co-occurrence frequency of two codes, while n1 and n2 are their occurrence frequency.
Quantitative Analysis
To conduct quantitative and correlation analyses between the first person reports and DTI data, we quantified the scores of the macro-categories and sub-units for each participant. As indicated above, each participant answered three questions. We assigned one point to a sub-unit every time it was coded in one of the answers. Thus, the score range for each sub-unit was from 0 (never coded) to 3 (coded in each answer). Finally, we grouped the scores based on macro-category (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017; see Table 1). In this way, we obtained quantified scores that represented the salience of each macro-category for each participant. We then calculated a delta score by subtracting t0 scores from t1 scores. A positive delta indicates increased reference to a category from t0 to t1 while a negative delta indicates a decrease between the two time points.
All quantitative analyses were conducted using STATISTICA 10 (StatSoft Inc., Tulsa, United States). We compared t0 and t1 using the Wilcoxon matched pairs test for non-parametric datasets. We used Spearman’s correlation to investigate the possible association between Silencing score and Attentional Effort and Mindfulness scores.
MRI Data Acquisition
Imaging data were acquired using a Siemens 1.5-T MAGNETOM Avanto (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) whole body scanner equipped with a 12-element designed Head Matrix coil, as part of the standard system configuration. Diffusion-weighted images (DWIs) were acquired using an axial pulsed-gradient spin-echo echo-planar sequence (7600/103; 38 sections; section thickness, 3.0 mm with no intersection gap), with diffusion-encoding gradients applied in 12 non-collinear directions (b factor 0 and 1000 s/mm2; number of acquired signals, four). A 2D fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) T2-weighted scan was also used to exclude the presence of small vessel ischemic disease and other supra- or infra-tentorial brain lesions [Repetition Time (TR) = 11,460 ms, Echo Time (TE) = 102 ms, Inversion Time (TI) = 2360 ms, Field of View (FOV) = 280 × 330 mm, Number of Excitations (NEX) = 2, matrix = 248 × 320, 1.00 × 1.00 mm2 in-plane resolution, horizontal slices with a slice thickness of 3.0 mm and no gap].
MRI Data Analysis
To avoid a type I error induced by the effect of WM hyperintensities on brain connectivity results, two expert radiologists managed by CCQ examined all MRIs. Participants were excluded when more than three lesions with a maximum diameter of 5 mm were detected in the subcortical or periventricular WM on axial FLAIR images (Quattrocchi et al., 2015).
Preprocessing of Diffusion Data
All DWIs were visually inspected for artifacts and preprocessed using different tools from FDT (FMRIB Diffusion Toolbox, part of FSL (FMRIB’s Software Library v.5.0.81; Smith et al., 2004). Images were corrected for eddy current distortion and head motion using a 12 parameter affine registration to the first no-diffusion-weighted volume of each participant, and the gradient directions were rotated accordingly (Leemans and Jones, 2009). Corrected images were skull-stripped using Brain Extraction Tool (Smith, 2002). Diffusion tensor images were then generated for each participant and each time point using the Diffusion Tensor Imaging ToolKit software package (DTI-TK2; Zhang et al., 2006). An unbiased longitudinal analysis approach was chosen for the registration of DTI data (Keihaninejad et al., 2013) using DTI-TK, which applies a registration algorithm that leverages the full diffusion tensor information to drive the registration, improving the alignment of WM structures (Wang et al., 2011). At the end of the registration procedure, each participant’s DTI data were normalized to the ICBM-152 template (Zhang et al., 2011), and FA maps were generated for each participant using the normalized tensor images (for details on the DTI data analysis, see Piervincenzi et al., 2017). FA data from each participant were further analyzed using selected modules of the Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (Smith et al., 2006) toolbox, available in FSL. The mean FA image was created and thinned to create a mean FA skeleton, which represents the centers of all tracts common to the group. Each participant’s FA image was then projected onto this common skeleton to minimize any residual misalignment of tracts.
To investigate the potential association between Silencing and training-induced DTI effects, a voxelwise correlation analysis was carried out between ΔSilencing (t1–t0) and ΔFA maps (t1–t0) using permutation-based non-parametric statistics via the FSL randomize tool (Nichols and Holmes, 2002) with 5,000 permutations. Resulting statistical maps were thresholded using FDR q = 0.05. Correlation analysis was performed inside the mask of previously reported longitudinal FA changes (for details on DTI analysis, see Piervincenzi et al., 2017).
Results
QMT Elicitation of Silence and Mind-Wandering Content
Following a session of QMT (at t0), 36% of the participants reported experiences related to Silence and 27% reported Reduced Mind-wandering. In addition, 95% of the participants reported experiences related to Mindfulness and 63% to Attentional Effort. After 6 weeks of training (t1), 9% of the participants reported experiences related to Silence and 13% to Reduced Mind-wandering. Also at t1, 95% of participants reported experiences related to Mindfulness and 72% to Attentional Effort. Thus, content in the Silencing macro-category significantly decreased between t0 and t1 (p < 0.04; see Figure 1A). Changes in the other macro-categories were not statistically significant (all p > 0.23).
Figure 1. Bar graph presenting results of the occurrence analysis comparing t0 and t1 (A) for each macro-category and (B) for each sub-unit. (C) Pie charts presenting the score distribution within each category at t0 and at t1. *p < 0.05.
When we analyzed the two sub-units of the Silencing macro-category, we observed that both Reduced mind-wandering and Silence were higher at t0 (6 and 9, respectively) than at t1 (both 3). However, only the decrease in the Silence sub-unit was significant (p < 0.03; Figure 1B).
The Attention and concentration sub-unit increased from 48 to 74% of the responses in the Attentional Effort macro-category, while those of Tiredness and Distraction both decreased, from 39 to 21% and from 13 to 5%, respectively, (see Figure 1C).
Correlation Between Silencing and DTI Changes
We observed a significant correlation between changes in Silencing and changes in FA maps following 6 weeks of QMT (p < 0.05 FDR corrected). More specifically, a positive correlation was found between change in Silencing and regions showing longitudinal increments of FA in left uncinate fasciculus (MNI peak coordinates: x = −33, y = 9, z = −6; t = 2.65; r = 0.57, p < 0.01) (Figure 2). Thus, greater FA increase was associated with a higher number of reports related to the experience of silence.
Figure 2. Scatterplot and results of voxel-wise correlation analysis between longitudinal changes in fractional anisotropy (FA) maps and concomitant changes in Silencing score (red-yellow color) between t0 and t1 (p < 0.05 FDR corrected). Only clusters showing a spatial extent of at least 30 voxels are reported. The study-specific FA skeleton, representing the centers of principal white matter tracts, is displayed in green, overlaid on the mean FA map. Anatomical localizations of peak MNI coordinates (mm) were established according to the JHU White-Matter Tractography and the JHU ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Labels atlases.
Correlations Between Silencing, Attentional Effort, and Mindfulness
A significant negative correlation between change (t1–t0) in Silencing and change in Attentional Effort (r = −0.47, p < 0.05) suggested that participants who show greater change in Attentional Effort exhibit less change in Silencing, and vice versa (Figure 3A). All other correlations were non-significant (all p > 0.30).
Figure 3. Scatterplot of correlation between change in (A) Silencing (y-axis) and Attentional Effort (x-axis); and (B) Reduced Mind-wandering and Silence (both on y-axis) and Attentional Effort (x-axis).
We observed a strong significant positive correlation between Reduced Mind-wandering and Silence (components of the Silencing category) (r = 0.72, p < 0.001), a significant negative correlation between Attentional Effort and Reduced Mind-wandering (r = −0.49, p < 0.05), and a correlation that approached significance between Attentional Effort and Silence (r = -0.40, p = 0.058) (Figure 3B). All other correlations were non-significant (all p > 0.21).
We also observed an increase in co-occurrence of Positive Emotion and Attention and Concentration (from 3 to 8; c-coefficient: from 0.09 to 0.17) and a contextual decrease of Positive Emotion and Silence (from 6 to 1; c-coefficient: from 0.20 to 0.02).
Discussion
The aim of the current study was to study the potential connection between silence-related experience during the practice of QMT and microstructural changes previously reported in Piervincenzi et al. (2017). While self-report techniques have their limitations (Rappert et al., 2017), they offer the possibility to capture the subjective dimension intrinsic to the personal experience, and thus a deeper understanding of the relationship between the different constructs, disentangling the subtle differences in the relationships among silence, attentional effort, and mindfulness.
Our results showed that 36% of participants reported experiences related to silence and 27% reported reduced mind-wandering during the first session of QMT practice. After 6 weeks of training, only 9% of the participants reported experiences related to silence and 13% reported reduced mind-wandering during practice execution. Hence, contrary to our expectation, participants reported fewer experiences related to silence and reduced mind-wandering after the 6 weeks period of daily training. It is possible that the participants experienced a sort of “normalization” or habituation to silence-related experience, reducing its salience. Namely, the same experiences of silence and reduced mind-wandering might have felt stronger and more salient to participants when they were new to QMT than when after 6 weeks of practice. In support of this interpretation, t0 reports were more complex and seemed aimed at explaining the experience of reduced thoughts (e.g., “strange absence of thoughts, more silence,” “no thoughts”), while t1 reports were simpler and more direct (e.g., “silence,” “quietness”) – as if participants had become more familiar with this kind of experience. Of course, “silence” or “quietness” might simply designate personally significant labels for more complex experiences. In this context, we note Varela’s (1996) proposition that greater integration of self-report techniques in empirical research requires that participants receive adequate training to increase their ability to report accurately. This is in line with the Tibetan tradition of denoting meditation practice with the term gom, which literally means to familiarize with (Saggar et al., 2012).
In line with our hypothesis, silence-related experiences were positively correlated with changes in the uncinate fasciculus. In humans, the uncinate fasciculus is engaged in tasks that involve naming, single word comprehension, response inhibition, face processing, and monitoring of outcomes (Arnodio and Frith, 2006; Catani et al., 2013). Disconnection of the uncinate fasciculus, on the other hand, causes impairment of object-reward association learning, and reduced performance in memory tasks involving temporally complex visual information (Gaffan and Wilson, 2008). The bidirectionality of information flow in the uncinate fasciculus allows orbitofrontal cortex-based reward and punishment history to rapidly modulate temporal lobe-based mnemonic representations (Von Der Heide et al., 2013). In accordance, while disruption of uncinate fasciculus activity might cause memory and learning deficits, enhancing its activity through training may foster the ability to evaluate past experiences, especially those related to reward and punishment (Von Der Heide et al., 2013). This, in turn, might result in more effective monitoring and management of ongoing performance, as participants in this and previous studies have reported increased attention and concentration (Ben-Soussan et al., 2017), possibly mediated by the silence required to correctly perform QMT.
Given that there are a number of functions associated with the uncinate fasciculus, one should keep in mind that the observed changes might be due to other effects of QMT training besides the construct of silence. For example, the reported changes in uncinate fasciculus might be ascribed to associative learning (Thomas et al., 2012) particularly that involving visual-motor associations (Thomas et al., 2015) to perform the QMT. In fact, the subjective experience of silence could reflect reduced cognitive processing associated with the transition from early cognitive to middle associative motor learning stages to a later autonomous stage. Thus, as one of our reviewers has suggested, the current changes in uncinate fasciculus correlated with subjective experience of silence, is so because both are reflective of motor learning. This latter view is of interest to mindfulness theory as it suggests that performing repetitive sequenced movements can establish mindfulness states and provide mindfulness training benefits after learning of the sequenced movements has progressed to the later stages of motor learning.
The current results also showed a negative correlation between the Silencing and Attentional Effort macro-categories. Moreover, the Silence and Reduced Mind-wandering sub-units showed a strong positive correlation between them, and were both negatively correlated with Attentional Effort, but not with the Mindfulness category.
In this study, we explored for the first time self-reported subjective phenomenological experiences of silence during QMT practice and their correlations with changes in structural brain connectivity. However, it is not clear what is the mechanism underling these changes. Future studies should examine the causal relationship between QMT experience-related reports of silence and changes in FA by means of more specific assessments related to silence, as well as comparing it to additional training groups. The uncinate fasciculus matures later and more slowly than other brain fiber connections, and might continue developing beyond the age of 30 years (Lebel et al., 2008). This might enable greater structural plasticity in response to a variety of internal or external (De Fano et al., 2019) environmental influences (Markowitsch and Staniloiu, 2011), such as induced internal and external silence.
Limitations and Conclusion
The lack of a control group is one of the main limitations of this study. However, neural, neuronal, and cognitive changes related to QMT have been compared to different control groups in the past, controlling for both cognitive and motor load, demonstrating its specificity (see Ben-Soussan et al., 2013, 2014b; Venditti et al., 2015; Paoletti et al., 2017).
More in detail, QMT related changes have been studied by comparing groups performing QMT with active control groups performing verbal or motor tasks (Ben-Soussan et al., 2013). This study demonstrated that cognitive and neurophysiological changes can be specifically induced by QMT practice (Ben-Soussan et al., 2013). Moreover, QMT’s impact on emotional regulation has been studied comparing two groups of meditators, one performing a breathing meditation + QMT and the other one performing only the breathing meditation (Paoletti et al., 2017). In this case, increased affect balance was attributed to the group which also performed the QMT practice, while self-efficacy increased in both groups.
For these reasons, in our opinion, the lack of a control group in the present study represents a limitation but taking in consideration (1) the observed reliable specificity of QMT-related cognitive, neurophysiological and emotional modulation, (2) the results of other studies showing improvements in self-efficacy, emotional and cognitive modulations following mindfulness or meditative practices (Creswell et al., 2007; Garland et al., 2009, 2011; Wendt et al., 2015), we can associate the here presented results about silence reports and their correlation with uncinate fasciculus connectivity to the prolonged performance of QMT as a meditative practice.
Moreover, several studies have already demonstrated the longitudinal reliability of DTI measures, including previous learning studies where control groups did not show FA changes (Scholz et al., 2009; Taubert et al., 2010). Nevertheless, despite demonstrations from previous work, the present findings are limited by the lack of a control condition. Another possible interpretation which cannot be excluded by the present work, involves the expectation due to receiving the intervention.
It is important to underline that the content of this manuscript goes beyond the main aim pursued by the original longitudinal study, of studying the cognitive, emotional, and physical experiences related to QMT performance. The exploration of silence-related experience to QMT practice was not part of the experimental design, and participants have never been directly asked or cued to mention it. Nonetheless, it emerged spontaneously from first-person reports, suggesting that silence could represent a particularly relevant component of this meditative practice.
This study provided interesting insights, but further investigations specifically designed to assess the impact of the experience of silence in meditative sensorimotor trainings and their neurophysiological correlates are required.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethical Committee of the Università Campus Bio-Medico di Roma. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
TB-S and FC designed the research. CQ performed the research. FM, AD, and FA analyzed the first-person reports. CP and FC analyzed the DTI data and DTI correlations. TB-S, FM, CP, JG, and FC wrote the manuscript. AD, FA, and CQ contributed to the writing process. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The handling Editor declared a shared affiliation, though no other collaboration, with several of the authors CP and FC the at time of the review.
Footnotes
References
Abbiss, C. R., and Laursen, P. B. (2007). Is part of the mystery surrounding fatigue complicated by context? J. Sci. Med. Sport 10, 277–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2006.07.015
Anālayo (2017). Early Buddhist Meditation Studies. Scotts Valley, CA: CreateSpace Independent Publishing.
Arnodio, D. M., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884
Baars, B. J. (2013). A scientific approach to silent consciousness. Front. Psychol. 4:678. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00678
Ben-Soussan, T. D., Avirame, K., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Harpaz, Y., and Ben-Shachar, M. (2014a). Changes in cerebellar activity and inter-hemispheric coherence accompany improved reading performance following Quadrato Motor Training. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:81. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00081
Ben-Soussan, T. D., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2014b). A suspended act: increased reflectivity and gender-dependent electrophysiological change following Quadrato Motor Training. Front. Psychol. 5:55. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00055
Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2017). Attentional effort, mindfulness, and altered states of consciousness experiences following Quadrato Motor Training. Mindfulness 8, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0469-5
Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., De Fano, A., Mauro, F., Marson, F., Modica, M., et al. (2019). Embodied time: time production in advanced Quadrato and Aikido practitioners. PsyCh. J. 8, 8–16. doi: 10.1002/pchj.266
Ben-Soussan, T. D., Glicksohn, J., Goldstein, A., Berkovich-Ohana, A., and Donchin, O. (2013). Into the square and out of the box: the effects of Quadrato Motor Training on creativity and alpha coherence. PLoS One 8:e55023. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055023
Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front. Psychol. 4:912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912
Cahn, B. R., and Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 132, 180–211. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180
Catani, M., Mesulam, M. M., Jakobsen, E., Malik, F., Martersteck, A., Wieneke, C., et al. (2013). A novel frontal pathway underlies verbal fluency in primary progressive aphasia. Brain 136, 2619–2628. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt163
Clark, D., Schumann, F., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2015). Mindful movement and skilled attention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:297. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00297
Coull, J. T. (1998). Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 343–361. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(98)00011-2
Creswell, J. D., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., and Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural correlates of dispositional mindfulness during affect labeling. Psychosom. Med. 69, 560–565. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e3180f6171f
Dane, E., and Pratt, M. G. (2007). Exploring intuition and its role in managerial decision making. Acad. Manag. Rev. 32, 33–54. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.23463682
d’Arbeloff, T. C., Kim, M. J., Knodt, A. R., Radtke, S. R., Brigidi, B. D., and Hariri, A. R. (2018). Microstructural integrity of a pathway connecting the prefrontal cortex and amygdala moderates the association between cognitive reappraisal and negative emotions. Emotion 18:912. doi: 10.1037/emo0000447
Davies, O., and Turner, D. (eds) (2002). Silence and the Word: Negative Theology and Incarnation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
De Fano, A., Leshem, R., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2019). Creating an internal environment of cognitive and psycho-emotional well-being through an external movement-based environment: an overview of Quadrato Motor Training. Int. J. Environ. Res. Publ. Health 16:2160. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16122160
Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2017). “Fundamental questions surrounding efforts to improve executive functions (including working memory),” in An Integrative Approach to Cognitive and Working Memory Training: Perspectives from Psychology, Neuroscience, and Human Development (New York, NY: Oxford University Press).
Diamond, A., and Ling, D. S. (2019). Aerobic-exercise and resistance-training interventions have been among the least effective ways to improve executive functions of any method tried thus far. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 37:100572. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.05.001
Farb, N. A., Segal, Z. V., Mayberg, H., Bean, J., McKeon, D., Fatima, Z., et al. (2007). Attending to the present: mindfulness meditation reveals distinct neural modes of self-reference. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 313–322. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm030
Gaffan, D., and Wilson, C. R. (2008). Medial temporal and prefrontal function: recent behavioural disconnection studies in the macaque monkey. Cortex 44, 928–935. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.03.005
Garland, E., Gaylord, S., and Fredrickson, B. (2011). Positive reappraisal mediates the stress-reductive effects of mindfulness: an upward spiral process. Mindfulness 2, 59–67. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0043-8
Garland, E., Gaylord, S., and Park, J. (2009). The role of mindfulness in positive reappraisal. Explore 5, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2008.10.001
Harmat, L., de Manzano, Ö, Theorell, T., Högman, L., Fischer, H., and Ullén, F. (2015). Physiological correlates of the flow experience during computer game playing. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 97, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.05.001
Hernández, S. E., Barros-Loscertales, A., Xiao, Y., Gonzalez-Mora, J. L., and Rubia, K. (2018). Gray matter and functional connectivity in anterior cingulate cortex are associated with the state of mental silence during sahaja yoga meditation. Neuroscience 371, 395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.12.017
Hinterberger, T., Schmidt, S., Kamei, T., and Walach, H. (2014). Decreased electrophysiological activity represents the conscious state of emptiness in meditation. Front. Psychol. 5:99. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00099
Hölzel, B. K., Brunsch, V., Gard, T., Greve, D. N., Koch, K., Sorg, C., et al. (2016). Mindfulness-based stress reduction, fear conditioning, and the uncinate fasciculus: a pilot study. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:124. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00124
Jacob, J., Jovic, E., and Brinkerhoff, M. B. (2009). Personal and planetary well-being: mindfulness meditation, pro-environmental behavior and personal quality of life in a survey from the social justice and ecological sustainability movement. Soc. Indic. Res. 93, 275–294. doi: 10.1007/s11205-008-9308-6
Josipovic, Z. (2014). Neural correlates of nondual awareness in meditation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1307, 9–18. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12261
Josipovic, Z. (2019). Nondual awareness: consciousness-as-such as non-representational reflexivity. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 273–298. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.021
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2009). Full Catastrophe Living: Using the Wisdom of Your Body and Mind to Face Stress, Pain, and Illness. New York, NY: Delacourt.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am. Psychol. 58, 697–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.58.9.697
Keihaninejad, S., Zhang, H., Ryan, N. S., Malone, I. B., Modat, M., Cardoso, M. J., et al. (2013). An unbiased longitudinal analysis framework for tracking white matter changes using diffusion tensor imaging with application to Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage 72, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.044
Kimmel, M., and Rogler, C. R. (2018). Affordances in interaction: the case of aikido. Ecol. Psychol. 30, 195–223. doi: 10.1080/10407413.2017.1409589
Koechlin, E., and Hyafil, A. (2007). Anterior prefrontal function and the limits of human-decision making. Science 318: 594–598. doi: 10.1126/science.1142995
Krippendorff, K. (2018). Content Analysis: An Introduction to its Methodology. New York, NY: Sage publications.
Lasaponara, S., Mauro, F., Carducci, F., Paoletti, P., Tombini, M., Quattrocchi, C. C., et al. (2017). Increased alpha band functional connectivity following the Quadrato Motor Training: a longitudinal study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:282. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00282
Lebel, C., Walker, L., Leemans, A., Phillips, L., and Beaulieu, C. (2008). Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. NeuroImage 40, 1044–1055. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.053
Leckart, B. T., Levine, J. R., Goscinski, C., and Brayman, W. (1970). Duration of attention: the perceptual deprivation effect. Percept. Psychophys. 7, 163–164. doi: 10.3758/bf03208649
Leemans, A., and Jones, D. K. (2009). The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Mag. Reson. Med. 61, 1336–1349. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21890
Lin, P., Chang, J., Zemon, V., and Midlarsky, E. (2008). Silent illumination: a study on Chan (Zen) meditation, anxiety, and musical performance quality. Psychol. Music 36, 139–155. doi: 10.1177/0305735607080840
Lindahl, J. R., Kaplan, C. T., Winget, E. M., and Britton, W. B. (2014). A phenomenology of meditation-induced light experiences: traditional Buddhist and neurobiological perspectives. Front. Psychol. 4:973. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00973
Luders, E., Clark, K., Narr, K. L., and Toga, A. W. (2011). Enhanced brain connectivity in long-term meditation practitioners. NeuroImage 57, 1308–1316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.075
Markowitsch, H. J., and Staniloiu, A. (2011). Memory, autonoetic consciousness, and the self. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 16–39. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.005
Mrazek, M. D., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2012). Mindfulness and mind-wandering: finding convergence through opposing constructs. Emotion 12, 442–448. doi: 10.1037/a0026678
Nichols, T. E., and Holmes, A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058
Niven, K. (2013). “Affect,” in Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine, eds M. D. Gellman and J. R. Turner (New York, NY: Springer).
Paoletti, P. (2002). Flussi, Territori, Luogo [Flows, Territories, Place]. Madeira, PT: M.E.D. Publishing.
Paoletti, P., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2019). The sphere model of consciousness: from geometrical to neuro-psycho-educational perspectives. Log. Univer. 13, 395–415. doi: 10.1007/s11787-019-00226-0
Paoletti, P., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2020). Reflections on silence and consciousness without contents according to the sphere model of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 11:1807. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01807
Paoletti, P., Ben-Soussan, T. D., and Glicksohn, J. (2020). “Inner navigation and theta activity: from movement to cognition and hypnosis according to the sphere model of consciousness,” in Hypnotherapy and Hypnosis, ed. C. Mordeniz (London: IntechOpen), 1–27.
Paoletti, P., Glicksohn, J., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2017). Inner Design Technology: Improved Affect by Quadrato Motor Training. The Amygdala–Where Emotions Shape Perception, Learning and Memories. London: IntechOpen, 27–41.
Pfeifer, E., Fiedler, H., and Wittmann, M. (2019). Enhanced relaxation in students after combined Depth Relaxation Music Therapy and silence in a natural setting. Arts Psychother. 63, 68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.aip.2019.02.006
Pfeifer, E., Fiedler, H., and Wittmann, M. (2020). Increased relaxation and present orientation after a period of silence in a natural surrounding. Nord. J. Music Ther. 29, 75–92. doi: 10.1080/08098131.2019.1642374
Piervincenzi, C., Ben-Soussan, T. D., Mauro, F., Mallio, C. A., Errante, Y., Quattrocchi, C. C., et al. (2017). White matter microstructural changes following Quadrato Motor Training: a longitudinal study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:590. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00590
Quattrocchi, C. C., De Pandis, M. F., Piervincenzi, C., Galli, M., Melgari, J. M., Salomone, G., et al. (2015). Acute modulation of brain connectivity in Parkinson disease after automatic mechanical peripheral stimulation: a pilot study. PLoS One 10:e0137977. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0137977
Rappert, B., Colombetti, G., and Coopmans, K. (2017). What is absent from contemplative neuroscience? Rethinking limits within the study of consciousness, experience, and meditation. J. Conscious. Stud. 24, 199–225.
Rudrauf, D., Lutz, A., Cosmelli, D., Lachaux, J. P., Le Van, and Quyen, M. (2003). From autopoiesis to neurophenomenology: Francisco Varela’s exploration of the biophysics of being. Biol. Res. 36, 27–65.
Russell, T. A., and Arcuri, S. M. (2015). A neurophysiological and neuropsychological consideration of mindful movement: clinical and research implications. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:282. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00282
Ryan, R. M., and Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Pers. 65, 529–565. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x
Saggar, M., King, B. G., Zanesco, A. P., MacLean, K. A., Aichele, S. R., Jacobs, T. L., et al. (2012). Intensive training induces longitudinal changes in meditation state-related EEG oscillatory activity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:256. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00256
Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (2007). Cerebral white matter—historical evolution of facts and notions concerning the organization of the fiber pathways of the brain. J. Hist. Neurosci. 16, 237–267. doi: 10.1080/09647040500495896
Scholz, J., Klein, M. C., Behrens, T. E., and Johansen-Berg, H. (2009). Training induces changes in white-matter architecture. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1370–1371. doi: 10.1038/nn.2412
Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., Slade, K., and Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Mindfulness and other Buddhist-derived interventions in correctional settings: a systematic review. Aggress. Viol. Behav. 18, 365–372. doi: 10.1016/j.avb.2013.01.002
Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychol. Bull. 132, 946–958. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946
Smith, S. M. (2002). Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 143–155. doi: 10.1002/hbm.10062
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. NeuroImage 31, 1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024
Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23, S208–S219.
Steffens, D. C., Taylor, W. D., Denny, K. L., Bergman, S. R., and Wang, L. (2011). Structural integrity of the uncinate fasciculus and resting state functional connectivity of the ventral prefrontal cortex in late life depression. PLoS One 6:e22697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022697
Stratton, S. P. (2015). Mindfulness and contemplation: secular and religious traditions in Western context. Counsel. Values 60, 100–118. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-007x.2015.00063.x
Tang, Y. Y., Lu, Q., Fan, M., Yang, Y., and Posner, M. I. (2012). Mechanisms of white matter changes induced by meditation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S. A. 109, 10570–10574. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1207817109
Taubert, M., Draganski, B., Anwander, A., Müller, K., Horstmann, A., Villringer, A., et al. (2010). Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J. Neurosci. 30, 11670–11677. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2567-10.2010
Teschner, G. (1981). The relation between mind and body in the “Surangama Sutra”. J. Indian Philos. 9, 77–83.
Thomas, C., Avram, A., Pierpaoli, C., and Baker, C. (2015). Diffusion MRI properties of the human uncinate fasciculus correlate with the ability to learn visual associations. Cortex 72, 65–78. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.01.023
Thomas, C., Walker, L., Pierpaoli, C., and Baker, C. (2012). The role of the uncinate fasciculus in human visual-associative learning. J. Vis. 12, 1189–1189. doi: 10.1167/12.9.1189
Vago, D. R., and David, S. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296
Vago, D. R., and Zeidan, F. (2016). The brain on silent: mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1373, 96–113. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13171
Varela, F. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J. Conscious. Stud. 3, 330–349.
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (2016). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Venditti, S., Verdone, L., Pesce, C., Tocci, N., Caserta, M., and Ben-Soussan, T. D. (2015). Creating well-being: increased creativity and proNGF decrease following Quadrato Motor Training. BioMed. Res. Int. 2015:275062.
Vieten, C., Wahbeh, H., Cahn, B. R., MacLean, K., Estrada, M., Mills, P., et al. (2018). Future directions in meditation research: recommendations for expanding the field of contemplative science. PLoS One 13:e0205740. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0205740
Von Der Heide, R. J., Skipper, L. M., Klobusicky, E., and Olson, I. R. (2013). Dissecting the uncinate fasciculus: disorders, controversies and a hypothesis. Brain 136, 1692–1707. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt094
Wackermann, J., Pütz, P., Büchi, S., Strauch, I., and Lehmann, D. (2002). Brain electrical activity and subjective experience during altered states of consciousness: ganzfeld and hypnagogic states. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 46, 123–146. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00070-3
Wang, Y., Gupta, A., Liu, Z., Zhang, H., Escolar, M. L., Gilmore, J. H., et al. (2011). DTI registration in atlas based fiber analysis of infantile Krabbe disease. NeuroImage 55, 1577–1586. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.038
Wendt, S., Hipps, J., Abrams, A., Grant, J., Valosek, L., and Nidich, S. (2015). Practicing transcendental meditation in high schools: relationship to well-being and academic achievement among students. Contemp. Sch. Psychol. 19, 312–319. doi: 10.1007/s40688-015-0066-6
Wheeler, M. S., Arnkoff, D. B., and Glass, C. R. (2017). The neuroscience of mindfulness: how mindfulness alters the brain and facilitates emotion regulation. Mindfulness 8, 1471–1487. doi: 10.1007/s12671-017-0742-x
Winter, U., LeVan, P., Borghardt, T. L., Akin, B., Wittmann, M., Leyens, Y. M., et al. (2019). Content-free Awareness: EEG-fcMRI correlates of consciousness as such in an expert meditator. Front. Psychol. 10:3064. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03064
Wittmann, M. (2015). Modulations of the experience of self and time. Conscious. Cogn. 38, 172–181. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.008
Wittmann, M. (2020). Altered states of consciousness self and time during meditation. Int. J. Study Chan Buddh. Hum. Civiliz. Issue 7, 27–39.
Zhang, H., Yushkevich, P. A., Alexander, D. C., and Gee, J. C. (2006). Deformable registration of diffusion tensor MR images with explicit orientation optimization. Med. Image Anal. 10, 764–785. doi: 10.1016/j.media.2006.06.004
Keywords: silence, mind-wandering, attentional effort, DTI, uncinate fasciculus
Citation: Ben-Soussan TD, Marson F, Piervincenzi C, Glicksohn J, De Fano A, Amenduni F, Quattrocchi CC and Carducci F (2020) Correlates of Silence: Enhanced Microstructural Changes in the Uncinate Fasciculus. Front. Psychol. 11:543773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543773
Received: 18 March 2020; Accepted: 03 September 2020;
Published: 08 October 2020.
Edited by:
Antonino Raffone, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyReviewed by:
Maarten A. Immink, University of South Australia, AustraliaM. Justin Kim, Sungkyunkwan University, South Korea
Copyright © 2020 Ben-Soussan, Marson, Piervincenzi, Glicksohn, De Fano, Amenduni, Quattrocchi and Carducci. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Tal Dotan Ben-Soussan, research@fondazionepatriziopaoletti.org