Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 14 July 2020
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding the Importance of Temporal Coupling of Neural Activities in Information Processing Underlying Action and Perception View all 21 articles

The Low-Frequency Fluctuation of Trial-by-Trial Frontal Theta Activity and Its Correlation With Reaction-Time Variability in Sustained Attention

  • 1Institute of Psychological Sciences, College of Education, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China
  • 2Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders, The Affiliated Hospital of Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China
  • 3Zhejiang Key Laboratory for Research in Assessment of Cognitive Impairments, Hangzhou, China
  • 4Institute of Mental Health, The Sixth Hospital, Peking University, Beijing, China

Reaction-time variability is a critical index of sustained attention. However, researchers still lack effective measures to establish the association between neurophysiological activity and this behavioral variability. Here, the present study recorded reaction time (RT) and cortical electroencephalogram (EEG) in healthy subjects when they continuously performed an alternative responding task. The frontal theta activity and reaction-time variability were examined trial by trial using the measures of standard deviation (SD) in the time domain and amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation (ALFF) in the frequency domain. Our results showed that the SD of reaction-time variability did not have any correlation with the SD of trial-by-trial frontal theta activity, and the ALFF of reaction-time variability has a significant correlation with the ALFF of trial-by-trial frontal theta activity in 0.01–0.027 Hz. These results suggested the methodological significance of ALFF in establishing the association between neurophysiological activity and reaction-time variability. Furthermore, these findings also support the low-frequency fluctuation as a potential feature of sustained attention.

Introduction

The capacity of sustained attention is of great importance. It refers to focusing on a certain task for a long period of time (Posner et al., 2014). Many occupations in our daily lives require a high level of sustained attention, e.g., driving vehicles, industrial control, and air traffic control (Aricò et al., 2016; Reinerman-Jones et al., 2016; Sebastiani et al., 2020). Declined sustained attention was documented in studies of several physiological states, e.g., alcoholism (Coles et al., 2002), sleep deprivation (Gunzelmann et al., 2009), and fatigue (Gunzelmann et al., 2011). Moreover, the deficit of sustained attention is usually identified as symptoms of various neuropsychiatric disorders, e.g., attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Barkley, 1997), and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Christakou et al., 2013). The practical importance of sustained attention, therefore, attracted interests from the research community, and numerous studies have been devoted to the behavioral and neurophysiological explorations of sustained attention.

Behavioral studies on sustained attention always employ examine stimulus-response tasks, e.g., alternative responding task (Helps et al., 2010), Go/NoGo task (Kirmizi-Alsan et al., 2006), and Eriksen flanker task (Castellanos et al., 2005). Subjects performing these tasks were requested to continuously detect stimulus, and their behavioral data of response and reaction time (RT) were recorded simultaneously. Several measures of the behavioral data were used to assess the level of sustained attention, e.g., error rate and intraindividual variability of RT. The error rate did not exhibit good test–retest reliability, and it was suggested to be more related with the response strategy (Liu et al., 2017; Steinborn et al., 2018). Therefore, the measure of intraindividual variability of RT was employed more extensively. For the individual, RT variability could be assessed by calculating the standard deviation of the reaction time (RT-SD) (Castellanos et al., 2005; Flehmig et al., 2007), and this measure in some studies was also standardized with the mean value of reaction time (RT-Mean) (Epstein et al., 2011; Tamm et al., 2012). RT-SD has good test–retest reliability (Liu et al., 2017), and their functional significance has been demonstrated by many clinical investigations. Observation from these investigations confirmed the linking between increased RT variability and the symptoms of sustained attention deficit, e.g., ADHD (Di Martino et al., 2008; Epstein et al., 2011), bipolar disorder (Brotman et al., 2009), and traumatic brain injury (TBI) (Burton et al., 2002). This functional significance of RT variability not only attracted increasing number of clinical trials (Rosch et al., 2013; James et al., 2016; Salum et al., 2019) but also promoted the neurophysiological explorations.

Cortical electroencephalogram (EEG) explorations have contributed many insightful evidences for the neurophysiological underpinning of sustained attention. Studies examining EEG identified several rhythmic activities, e.g., theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–14 Hz), and beta (14–30 Hz) (Buzsaki, 2006; Clayton et al., 2015), and some experimental evidences suggested that these EEG rhythmic activities were in response to distinct cognitive processes of sustained attention. Alpha was mainly in response to the inhibition of task-irrelevant processes (Klimesch et al., 2007; Uusberg et al., 2013), and beta could affect the attentional engagement to stimulus (Oswal et al., 2012; Coelli et al., 2015; Li et al., 2017; Shapiro et al., 2017). Theta was suggested to play a role in attentional control (Cohen and Donner, 2013; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014) and action monitoring process (Cavanagh et al., 2012; Pandey et al., 2016). Furthermore, studies attempt to establish the association between EEG rhythmic activity and behavior, and theta has been proven to be an important target (Helfrich et al., 2018; Fiebelkorn and Kastner, 2019). Increased theta activity, specifically the theta activity of the frontal brain regions, was identified in the conditions requiring higher level of sustained attention (Jensen and Tesche, 2002; Mitchell et al., 2008; Sauseng et al., 2010). More importantly, several studies reported that frontal theta activity successfully predicted the behavior during attention-demanding tasks and was related to prolonged task performance (Clayton et al., 2015; Helfrich et al., 2018). It was observed that increased frontal theta activity was in response of greater error rates and prolonged RTs (Boksem et al., 2005; Cavanagh and Shackman, 2015; Cooper et al., 2019); nevertheless, these results were not consistent across studies (Loo et al., 2004; van Driel et al., 2012; Wascher et al., 2014). Similar to RT variability, continuous recording of EEG activity typically exhibited a pattern of waxing and waning across trials of tests (Ratcliff et al., 2009; VanRullen et al., 2011). Inspired by this, recent studies explored the trial-by-trial fluctuation of EEG activity (Truccolo et al., 2002; Fox et al., 2006; McLoughlin et al., 2014; Adamo et al., 2015). Studies used the measure of SD to assess the frontal theta activity across different trials and directly examined the correlation between the SD of trial-by-trial frontal theta activity and RT-SD; however, they did not identify any significant results (McLoughlin et al., 2014).

It was worthy to note that an increasing number of studies documented the periodical variability of RT in sustained attention (Castellanos et al., 2005; Di Martino et al., 2008; Helps et al., 2011). This observation was firstly reported by Castellanos et al. (2005). They employed frequency-dependent analyses to examine RT fluctuation of children with ADHD when they perform a continuous stimulus–response test. It was found that the RT variability of the children with ADHD had peak amplitude around 0.05 Hz (about 20 s a cycle), and this peak amplitude of RT could be eliminated by using the medications of methylphenidate. These results, for the first time, revealed the periodical variability of RT for sustained attention. Since this periodical fluctuation was observed in the relatively low-frequency band (<0.2 Hz, >5 s/cycle), some studies employed the term amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation to depict this measure of reaction time (RT-ALFF). RT-ALFF as a measure of the frequency domain exhibited good test–retest reliability (ICC = 0.69) (Liu et al., 2017), and it has been repeatedly employed as a validity measure in examining the subjects with sustained attention deficits (Johnson et al., 2007; Vaurio et al., 2009; Karalunas et al., 2013). Studies usually investigated RT-ALFF in several frequency bands, including <0.01, 0.01–0.027, 0.027–0.073, and 0.073–0.167 Hz (Di Martino et al., 2008; Adamo et al., 2014, 2015). These frequency bands are defined by Penttonen and Buzsáki (2003) based on specific properties and physiological function. It has been found that RT-ALFF in three specific frequency bands (0.01–0.027 Hz; 0.027–0.073 Hz; 0.073–0.20 Hz) was strongly related to the ratings of ADHD symptoms (Mairena et al., 2012). These behavioral findings suggested that low-frequency fluctuation may be a feature of sustained attention, which encouraged us to investigate the ALFF of trial-by-trial frontal theta activity. We expected that the ALFF of trial-by-trial frontal theta activity could exhibit association with the ALFF of RT variability.

To verify this view, we performed an exploratory investigation. Data of RT and EEG activity were recorded simultaneously when subjects performing a sustained attention test. Trial-by-trial fluctuation of frontal theta activity and RT variability were examined with the measures of SD and ALFF (in the frequency bands of 0.01–0.027, 0.027–0.073, and 0.073–0.167 Hz). Then, EEG-behavior correlations were further assessed in each frequency band, respectively.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Seventy healthy participants (37 females; 21 ± 2 years old) were recruited in this study. All of the participants were right-handed, and no individual reported any history of brain injury or mental disorders. The RT data and EEG recording were collected from all participants during a continuously performed test, i.e., alternative responding task. According to the inclusion criteria of previous studies (Geurts et al., 2008; Liu et al., 2017), 4 participants (3 females) showing extreme mean RT values (longer than 3 SDs beyond the mean RT for all the participants) were excluded from subsequent analyses. At last, data from 66 participants (34 females; 21 ± 2 years old) were included in this study. All participants gave written informed consent prior to their participation. The study was approved by the Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders (CCBD) Ethics Committee of Hangzhou Normal University.

Experimental Paradigm

The whole experiment involves two sessions of test, i.e., resting session and sustained attention task session, and the order of the two sessions was counterbalanced across all participants. Since the present study focused on trial-by-trial fluctuation, data from the resting session were not involved in the analysis. In the sustained attention task session, all participants performed the alternative responding task for 8 min (Helps et al., 2010). In the task, two kinds of stimuli, i.e., “>” and “<,” were pseudo-randomly presented for 500 ms interleaved with a fixation cross, and the inter-trial-interval (ITI) was 3000 ms. Each participant was instructed to determine the direction (left or right) of the arrow and to press “F” or “J” on the computer keyboard with the index finger. Before this session, a practice involving six trials was employed to ensure each participant was familiar with the procedure of the task.

Electrophysiological Recording and Preprocessing

The EEG data were recorded using a 32-channel (Brain Products, Germany) extended 10–20 system montage. The original recording reference was positioned at FCz. A sampling rate of 500 Hz was used. The filter bandwidth at recording was 0.016–250 Hz. All impedances were kept below 5 kΩ. The EEG data from the task session was preprocessed using Vision Analyzer software (Brain Products, Germany). Specifically, the channel signals were firstly re-referenced to average reference. After applying a notch filter (50 Hz) and band pass filtering (0.1–70 Hz), eye movement artifacts were corrected using independent component analysis (ICA, Jung et al., 2000). Moreover, the stimulus-locked segments ranged from 0 to 3000 ms according to the ITI of the task. If the amplitude of the EEG data exceeded ± 100 mV at any electrode, a segment of 3 s around this artifact was excluded from further analyses; lastly, an average number of 158 (range from 137 to 160) artifact-free trials out of a total of 160 trials were available.

Data Analysis

Behavioral Data Analyses

Behavioral measures, including several conventional measures and RT-ALFF, were calculated. The conventional measures including RT-Mean, RT-SD, and error rate (including commission error and omission error) were firstly acquired from each participant. Then, RT-ALFF was assessed through the following steps: (1) Missing and anticipatory responses (RT < 100 ms) were interpolated by linear interpolation to reconstruct an integrated time series. (2) The RT time series (divided by RT-Mean) were transformed from the time domain to the frequency domain through fast Fourier transformation (FFT), and the amplitude at each frequency point was obtained. (3) RT-ALFF was calculated as the mean amplitude in a fixed frequency band. The examinable frequency band was 0.002–0.167 Hz according to Nyquist’s sampling theorem (sampling rate is 0.33 Hz corresponding to the ITI of the task). RT-ALFF was calculated in three sub-frequency bands including 0.01–0.027, 0.027–0.073, and 0.073–0.167 Hz, which were widely explored in previous studies (Di Martino et al., 2008; Adamo et al., 2015).

Electrophysiological Data Analyses

Electroencephalogram recording of the whole testing was analyzed through FFT, and the theta activity (4–8 Hz) mainly located in the frontal area was acquired at the electrodes of F3, FZ, and F4, respectively (Figures 1A,B). In the same way, trial-by-trial theta activity of each electrode was acquired based on the EEG recording of each single trial (each trial lasting 3000 ms) (Figure 1C).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The procedure of EEG data analyses. (A) EEG topographic map showing the theta activity mainly located in the frontal area during the sustained attention task. (B) Frontal theta EEG activity was analyzed from 4 to 8 Hz at F3, FZ, and F4. (C) The procedure of the trial-by-trial analysis of EEG activity.

Trial-by-trial theta activity (F3, FZ, and F4) was examined through two measures, Theta-SD and Theta-ALFF. For each frontal electrode, Theta-SD was calculated as the SD value of theta activity across all available trials. Theta-ALFF for each frontal electrode was assessed using the same analysis procedure of RT-ALFF (see details in Figure 2). Concretely, the theta activity of missing trials was replaced using linear interpolation between the theta activity of adjacent trials. Then, the time series of theta activity were divided by the mean value and were further transformed from the time domain to the frequency domain through FFT. At last, the average amplitude for a fixed frequency band was calculated as Theta-ALFF. Here, Theta-ALFF was examined in three frequency bands, i.e., 0.01–0.027, 0.027–0.073, and 0.073–0.167 Hz, corresponding to the analysis procedure of RT-ALFF. All of the analyses were implemented through our own MATLAB code.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The procedure of the frequency-dependent analysis of reaction time and trial-by-trial theta activity.

Behavior-EEG Correlation Analyses

The correlations between behavioral measures and EEG measures were examined using Kendall rank-correlation analysis, since the data is not normally distributed (Shapiro–Wilk test, p-values < 0.045). The extreme values (longer than 3 SDs beyond the mean value of measure for all the participants) of RT data and EEG data were not involved in the analyses. The correlations between RT-SD and Theta-SD were first investigated, and then the correlations between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF were assessed in the frequency bands (0.01–0.027, 0.027–0.073, and 0.073–0.167 Hz). Moreover, we also examined the correlation between theta activity of whole testing and the above RT measures. All of these correlation analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 20.0).

Results

Behavioral Results

Descriptive statistics of conventional behavioral measures were depicted in Table 1, including RT-Mean, RT-SD, commission error rate, and omission error rate, and the measures of RT-ALFF in all frequency bands are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Conventional behavioral measures for the task performance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. RT-ALFF in each frequency band for the task performance.

Correlations Between RT-SD and Theta-SD

The time series of RT are manifested in Figure 3D, and Figures 3A–C exhibited the time series of trial-by-trial theta activity at F3, FZ, and F4. The values of Theta-SD are shown in Table 3. The correlations between RT-SD and Theta-SD were assessed, and no significant correlation between RT-SD and Theta-SD was identified (each r < 0.01, p > 0.87) (Table 3). Moreover, no significant correlation between the theta activity of whole testing and RT-SD was identified (each r < 0.05, p > 0.56).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The time series of trial-by-trial theta activity and reaction time (RT). The time series of trial-by-trial theta activity at F3 (A), FZ (B), and F4 (C) and the time series of RT (D).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Theta-SD and its correlations (Kendall rank correlation) with RT-SD.

Correlations Between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF

The frequency-dependent amplitude of reaction-time fluctuation is shown in Figure 4D, and Figures 4A–C exhibited the frequency-dependent amplitude of trial-by-trial theta activity at F3, FZ, and F4. Moreover, the values of Theta-ALFF in each frequency band are shown in Table 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The frequency-dependent amplitude of trial-by-trial theta activity and reaction time (RT). The amplitude of trial-by-trial theta activity at F3 (A), FZ (B), and F4 (C) and the amplitude of RT fluctuation (D) in the sampled frequencies (0.01–0.167 Hz).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Theta-ALFF in each frequency band.

As shown in Figure 5, a significant correlation was observed in 0.01–0.027 Hz, and RT-ALFF was negatively correlated with Theta-ALFF at F3 (r = −0.26, p = 0.003, p < 0.05 after Bonferroni correction for three bands across three electrodes), not at F4 and FZ (Figure 5A). Then, the frequency band of 0.01–0.027 Hz was divided into two sub-frequency bands, i.e., 0.01–0.019 and 0.020–0.027 Hz. Correlations between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF could be reserved in the two bands (0.01–0.019 Hz, r = −0.22, p = 0.009 and 0.020–0.027 Hz, r = −0.22, p = 0.01).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Correlations between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF in each frequency band. The correlation (Kendall rank correlation) between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF (at the electrodes of F3, FZ, and F4) is shown in 0.01–0.027 Hz (A), 0.027–0.073 Hz (B), and 0.073–0.167 Hz (C). The amplitude of reaction time fluctuation and trial-by-trial theta activity (at the electrodes of F3, FZ, and F4) were also depicted in the frequencies of 0.01–0.027 Hz (A), 0.027–0.073 Hz (B), and 0.073–0.167 Hz (C).

No other significant correlations were found between RT-ALFF and Theta-ALFF in the frequency bands of 0.027–0.073 and 0.073–0.167 Hz (each r < 0.11, p > 0.18) (Figures 5B,C). Moreover, no significant correlation between the theta activity of whole testing and RT-ALFF in any frequency bands was identified (each r < 0.06, p > 0.46). In addition, the trial-by-trial ALFF of delta (1–4 Hz), alpha (8–14 Hz), beta (14–30 Hz), and gamma (30–50 Hz) and its correlation with RT-ALFF in each frequency band was examined separately, and no significant correlation was observed (each r < 0.13, p > 0.12) (see details in Supplementary Table S2).

Discussion

The present study explored the association between trial-by-trial fluctuation of frontal theta activity and RT variability. Measures of SD and ALFF were used to establish this association in the time domain and frequency domain, respectively. It was observed that RT variability did not show any relationship with the trial-by-trial frontal theta activity when we used SD as the measure. In contrast, a frequency-dependent correlation between RT variability and trial-by-trial frontal theta activity was revealed by the measure of ALFF. These results provided a methodological insight for future studies on the neural underpinning of sustained attention.

Reaction-time variability was considered as a critical index of the level of sustained attention (Betts et al., 2006; Flehmig et al., 2007). Albeit intensive investigation, measures for establishing the association between the RT variability and neurophysiological activity was lacking (Rabbi et al., 2009; Clayton et al., 2015; Fortenbaugh et al., 2017; O’Halloran et al., 2018). It is always failed to identify significant results when the researcher directly examined the correlation between EEG activity, e.g., beta, theta, and RT variability (Buyck and Wiersema, 2015). Similarly, the present study also did not find any significant association between theta activity at each electrode and RT variability. Experimental observations indicated that the EEG activity always fluctuates across trials in a similar way as RT variability (Ratcliff et al., 2009; VanRullen et al., 2011). Thus, recent EEG explorations highlighted the trial-by-trial fluctuation of EEG activity (Fox et al., 2006; McLoughlin et al., 2014; Adamo et al., 2015). These studies mostly focused on the frontal theta activity because experimental evidences indicated the functional role of frontal theta activity in attentional control (Cohen and Donner, 2013; Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; McLoughlin et al., 2014). Consistent with their finding, the present study did not identify any significant correlation results when using SD to assess the RT variability and trial-by-trial fluctuation of frontal theta activity (McLoughlin et al., 2014). Methodically, SD is a measure of time domain reflecting the overall fluctuation of the testing data. It is worthy to note, however, that RT variability for sustained attention may occur periodically (Castellanos et al., 2005; Di Martino et al., 2008; Helps et al., 2011). Overall fluctuation potentially masked the periodical variability. This is a possible explanation for the findings on SD, and it further encouraged us to examine the trial-by-trial frontal theta activity and RT variability in frequency domain.

Clinical investigations indicated that children with ADHD could show increased RT variability in the low-frequency band (<0.2 Hz) (Castellanos et al., 2005; Karalunas et al., 2013). This observation was confirmed by many studies (Johnson et al., 2007; Di Martino et al., 2008; Vaurio et al., 2009; Helps et al., 2011), and these evidences indicated that the low-frequency fluctuation was a potential feature of sustained attention. Thus, the association between trial-by-trial frontal theta activity and RT variability may be frequency-dependent. This idea was confirmed by our findings of the ALFF analyses. RT-ALFF exhibited a significant correlation with the Theta-ALFF in 0.01–0.027 Hz. Behaviorally, frequency-dependent RT variability has been linked with the attention lapse (Castellanos et al., 2005; Johnson et al., 2007; Adamo et al., 2014). It has been reported that RT-ALFF in 0.01–0.027 Hz could be used as a predictor to explain scale ratings of inattention of ADHD (Mairena et al., 2012). Here, our results may provide some preliminary evidences for understanding the neural underpinning of these behavioral observations. Nevertheless, the functional significance of the frequency-dependent fluctuation of frontal theta activity required to be further clarified. Notably, the frequency-dependent correlation in the present study had spatial specificity, and the correlation was only identified at the left frontal electrode (F3). The importance of frontal theta activity in sustained attention has been discussed (Missonnier et al., 2006; Gongora et al., 2015); however, the functional differences between the theta activity of the left and right frontal areas were less addressed in these EEG studies. It was reported that theta activity in the frontal brain areas may function in attention maintenance (Cavanagh and Frank, 2014; Wascher et al., 2014). Here, we found that the greater Theta-ALFF at the left electrode (F3) was negatively correlated with lower RT-ALFF. This finding reinforced the role of frontal theta activity in attention maintenance. Source location analysis further linked these findings with previous neuroimaging studies. The origin of the theta activity was implicated in the ventral medial frontal gyrus (vmPFC) (see details in the Supplementary Material), and this region serving as a role of state monitoring has been intensively reported by the functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies on sustained attention (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Hasenkamp et al., 2012; Sepede et al., 2012; Clayton et al., 2015). Nevertheless, the EEG data, collected from 32 electrodes, could not provide robust localization results (Michel et al., 2004). Therefore, the linking between theta activity, vmPFC, and behavior variability should be examined with more experiments in the future. Moreover, significant correlation only appears on the left frontal electrode (F3). This spatial specificity suggested the functional differences between the theta activity of left and right areas, which should be taken into account by further explorations.

The present study sheds light on the low-frequency fluctuation of trial-by-trial EEG activity. Actually, the low-frequency fluctuation of brain activity has been documented in many previous studies. However, most of these explorations focused on fMRI but not on EEG activity (Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos, 2007; Zang et al., 2007). The findings of the present study supported the feature of low-frequency fluctuation of sustained attention, and thus, ALFF as a measure of frequency domain will subserve the establishment of the association between RT variability and EEG activity. Notably, the present study focused on EEG rhythmic activity, and we believe that it is also an interesting issue whether low-frequency fluctuation could be identified in other EEG components, e.g., event-related potential (ERP). Adamo et al. (2015) have attempted to explore this issue, and they reported that the trial-by-trial variability of P3 and RT variability coupled at 0.073–0.167 Hz when subjects performed the Go/NoGo tasks (Adamo et al., 2015). However, their findings may be confounded by many experimental factors. For example, the restriction of the signal-to-noise ratio makes it challengeable to extract robust ERP from data of a single trial. ERP is usually acquired from tasks with jitter in ITI, and the jitter results in more frequency complexity. So, this issue still needs to be examined with well-designed experiments in future studies. Moreover, the low-frequency fluctuation of trial-by-trial EEG activity, in the present study, is derived from the specific task, i.e., alternative responding task. This task has obvious advantages for frequency-dependent analysis. It has continuous response, and the time series does not need to be reconstructed statistically as is required by many non-responding trials in more complex tasks. Behavioral evidences suggested that the task complexity could induce the variation in the frequency band of fluctuation for sustained attention (Johnson et al., 2007; Di Martino et al., 2008; Helps et al., 2011; Karalunas et al., 2013; Salum et al., 2019). Therefore, further experimentation with different tasks is required to verify the band specificity of our findings.

Several limitations existed in the present study. First, the subjects were all healthy college students; further studies on the subjects with attention deficits were required to examine whether the trial-by-trial fluctuation of frontal theta activity has clinical significance. Second, the ITI of the task paradigm for the current study was 3000 ms, and thus, the frequencies that we could analyze only ranged from 0.002 to 0.167 Hz, according to the Nyquist sampling theorem. The explorations on higher frequencies (>0.167 Hz) are necessary, and tasks with fast behavior recording may be helpful for this issue. Third, our finding has spatial specificity in F3, and this electrode is spatially close to the original reference, FCz. This reference during data collection is a fixed setting for the EEG device. Therefore, it remains to be investigated whether this setting affects our findings in F3.

Conclusion

The present study is an exploratory investigation and, for the first time, reported that the correlation between RT variability and trial-by-trial frontal-theta activity was frequency-dependent. ALFF as a measure of the frequency domain exhibited methodological significance in establishing the association between RT variability and EEG activity. These findings supported the low-frequency fluctuation as a feature of sustained attention. Further explorations on this feature may facilitate the understanding of the neuro underpinning of sustained attention.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Center for Cognition and Brain Disorders (CCBD) Ethics Committee of the Hangzhou Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

HZ conceived and designed the experiment. Y-YW, Y-WL, and J-HP collected the data. Y-YW, Y-WL, J-HP, and Y-MZ performed the data analysis. LS, Y-FW, and Y-FZ provided advice on the analysis and interpretation of the results. Y-YW and HZ wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Key R&D Program of China (2018YFC1312600 and 2018YFC1312603), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31471084, 81520108016, and 81771479), the National Basic Research Program of China (973 program 2014CB846104), and the Natural Science Foundation of Zhejiang Province (LY20H090006). HZ was supported by Scientific Research Staring Foundation from the Hangzhou Normal University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01555/full#supplementary-material

References

Adamo, N., Baumeister, S., Hohmann, S., Wolf, I., Holz, N., Boecker, R., et al. (2015). Frequency-specific coupling between trial-to-trial fluctuations of neural responses and response-time variability. J. Neural Trans. 122, 1197–1202. doi: 10.1007/s00702-015-1382-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamo, N., Di Martino, A., Esu, L., Petkova, E., Johnson, K., Kelly, S., et al. (2014). Increased response-time variability across different cognitive tasks in children with ADHD. J. Attent. Disord. 18, 434–446. doi: 10.1177/1087054712439419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aricò, P., Borghini, G., Di Flumeri, G., Colosimo, A., Bonelli, S., Golfetti, A., et al. (2016). Adaptive automation triggered by EEG-based mental workload index: a passive brain-computer interface application in realistic air traffic control environment. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:539. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121:65. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betts, J., Mckay, J., Maruff, P., and Anderson, V. (2006). The development of sustained attention in children: the effect of age and task load. Child Neuropsychol. 12, 205–221. doi: 10.1080/09297040500488522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotman, M. A., Rooney, M. H., Skup, M., Pine, D. S., and Leibenluft, E. (2009). Increased intrasubject variability in response time in youths with bipolar disorder and at-risk family members. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48, 628–635. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181a27527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burton, C. L., Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., and Hunter, M. A. (2002). Intraindividual variability in physical and emotional functioning: comparison of adults with traumatic brain injuries and healthy adults. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 264–279. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.3.264.13854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buyck, I., and Wiersema, J. R. (2015). Task-related electroencephalographic deviances in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 29:433. doi: 10.1037/neu0000148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsaki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press, doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301069.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Scheres, A., Di Martino, A., Hyde, C., and Walters, J. R. (2005). Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individual variability. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1416–1423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Frank, M. J. (2014). Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 414–421. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., and Shackman, A. J. (2015). Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence. J. Physiol. Paris 109, 3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2014.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cavanagh, J. F., Zambrano-Vazquez, L., and Allen, J. J. (2012). Theta lingua franca: a common mid-frontal substrate for action monitoring processes. Psychophysiology 49, 220–238. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01293.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christakou, A., Murphy, C. M., Chantiluke, K., Cubillo, A. I., Smith, A. B., Giampietro, V., et al. (2013). Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during sustained attention in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and with autism. Mol. Psychiatry 18:236. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, M. S., Yeung, N., and Kadosh, R. C. (2015). The roles of cortical oscillations in sustained attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 188–195. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coelli, S., Sclocco, R., Barbieri, R., Reni, G., Zucca, C., and Bianchi, A. M. (2015). “EEG-based index for engagement level monitoring during sustained attention,” in Proceedings of the 2015 37th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 1512–1515. doi: 10.1109/embc.2015.7318658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M. X., and Donner, T. H. (2013). Midfrontal conflict-related theta-band power reflects neural oscillations that predict behavior. J. Neurophysiol. 110, 2752–2763. doi: 10.1152/jn.00479.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coles, C. D., Platzman, K. A., Lynch, M. E., and Freides, D. (2002). Auditory and visual sustained attention in adolescents prenatally exposed to alcohol. Alcoholism 26, 263–271. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2002.tb02533.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, P. S., Karayanidis, F., McKewen, M., McLellan-Hall, S., Wong, A. S., Skippen, P., et al. (2019). Frontal theta predicts specific cognitive control-induced behavioural changes beyond general reaction time slowing. Neuroimage 189, 130–140. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Martino, A., Ghaffari, M., Curchack, J., Reiss, P., Hyde, C., Vannucci, M., et al. (2008). Decomposing intra-subject variability in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 64, 607–614. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, J. N., Langberg, J. M., Rosen, P. J., Graham, A., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., et al. (2011). Evidence for higher reaction-time variability for children with ADHD on a range of cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology 25, 427. doi: 10.1037/a0022155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiebelkorn, I. C., and Kastner, S. (2019). A rhythmic theory of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 23, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flehmig, H. C., Steinborn, M., Langner, R., Scholz, A., and Westhoff, K. (2007). Assessing intraindividual variability in sustained attention: reliability, relation to speed and accuracy, and practice effects. Psychol. Sci. 49:132.

Google Scholar

Fortenbaugh, F. C., DeGutis, J., and Esterman, M. (2017). Recent theoretical, neural, and clinical advances in sustained attention research. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1396:70. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Zacks, J. M., and Raichle, M. E. (2006). Coherent spontaneous activity accounts for trial-to-trial variability in human evoked brain responses. Nat. Neurosci. 9:23. doi: 10.1038/nn1616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geurts, H. M., Grasman, R. P., Verté, S., Oosterlaan, J., Roeyers, H., van Kammen, S. M., et al. (2008). Intra-individual variability in ADHD, autism spectrum disorders and Tourette’s syndrome. Neuropsychologia 46, 3030–3041. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gongora, M., Peressuti, C., Velasques, B., Bittencourt, J., Teixeira, S., Arias-Carrión, O., et al. (2015). Absolute Theta power in the frontal cortex during a visuomotor task: the effect of bromazepam on attention. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 46, 292–298. doi: 10.1177/1550059414535576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunzelmann, G., Gross, J. B., Gluck, K. A., and Dinges, D. F. (2009). Sleep deprivation and sustained attention performance: integrating mathematical and cognitive modeling. Cogn. Sci. 33, 880–910. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01032.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunzelmann, G., Moore, L. R., Gluck, K. A., Van Dongen, H., and Dinges, D. F. (2011). “Fatigue in sustained attention: generalizing mechanisms for time awake to time on task,” in Cognitive Fatigue: Multidisciplinary Perspectives on Current Research and Future Applications, Ed. P. L. Ackerman (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), doi: 10.1037/12343-004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasenkamp, W., Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Duncan, E., and Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Mind wandering and attention during focused meditation: a fine-grained temporal analysis of fluctuating cognitive states. Neuroimage 59, 750–760. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helfrich, R. F., Fiebelkorn, I. C., Szczepanski, S. M., Lin, J. J., Parvizi, J., Knight, R. T., et al. (2018). Neural mechanisms of sustained attention are rhythmic. Neuron 99, 854–865. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.07.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helps, S. K., Broyd, S. J., Bitsakou, P., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2011). Identifying a distinctive familial frequency band in reaction time fluctuations in ADHD. Neuropsychology 25:711. doi: 10.1037/a0024479

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helps, S. K., Broyd, S. J., James, C. J., Karl, A., Chen, W., and Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2010). Altered spontaneous low frequency brain activity in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Res. 1322, 134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, S. N., Cheung, C. H., Rijsdijk, F., Asherson, P., and Kuntsi, J. (2016). Modifiable arousal in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and its etiological association with fluctuating reaction times. Biol. Psychiatry 1, 539–547. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Tesche, C. D. (2002). Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task. Eur. J. Neurosci. 15, 1395–1399. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01975.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, K. A., Kelly, S. P., Bellgrove, M. A., Barry, E., Cox, M., Gill, M., et al. (2007). Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity. Neuropsychologia 45, 630–638. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, T. P., Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., and Sejnowski, T. J. (2000). Removal of eye activity artifacts from visual event-related potentials in normal and clinical subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 1745–1758. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(00)00386-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karalunas, S. L., Huang-Pollock, C. L., and Nigg, J. T. (2013). Is reaction time variability in ADHD mainly at low frequencies? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 536–544. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirmizi-Alsan, E., Bayraktaroglu, Z., Gurvit, H., Keskin, Y. H., Emre, M., and Demiralp, T. (2006). Comparative analysis of event-related potentials during Go/NoGo and CPT: decomposition of electrophysiological markers of response inhibition and sustained attention. Brain Res. 1104, 114–128. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., and Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63–88. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Kang, C., Wei, Z., Qu, X., Liu, T., Zhou, Y., et al. (2017). Beta oscillations in major depression–signalling a new cortical circuit for central executive function. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–15. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18306-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, Z. Z., Qu, H. J., Tian, Z. L., Han, M. J., Fan, Y., Ge, L. Z., et al. (2017). Reproducibility of frequency-dependent low frequency fluctuations in reaction time over time and across tasks. PLoS One 12:e0184476. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0184476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo, S. K., Hopfer, C., Teale, P. D., and Reite, M. L. (2004). EEG correlates of methylphenidate response in ADHD: association with cognitive and behavioral measures. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 21, 457–464. doi: 10.1097/01.wnp.0000150890.14421.9a

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mairena, M. Á, Di Martino, A., Domínguez-Martín, C., Gómez-Guerrero, L., Gioia, G., Petkova, E., et al. (2012). Low frequency oscillations of response time explain parent ratings of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 21, 101–109. doi: 10.1007/s00787-011-0237-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLoughlin, G., Palmer, J. A., Rijsdijk, F., and Makeig, S. (2014). Genetic overlap between evoked frontocentral theta-band phase variability, reaction time variability, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a twin study. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 238–247. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, C. M., Murray, M. M., Lantz, G., Gonzalez, S., Spinelli, L., and de Peralta, R. G. (2004). EEG source imaging. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 2195–2222. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Missonnier, P., Deiber, M. P., Gold, G., Millet, P., Pun, M. G. F., Fazio-Costa, L., et al. (2006). Frontal theta event-related synchronization: comparison of directed attention and working memory load effects. J. Neural Trans. 113, 1477–1486. doi: 10.1007/s00702-005-0443-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, D. J., McNaughton, N., Flanagan, D., and Kirk, I. J. (2008). Frontal-midline theta from the perspective of hippocampal “theta”. Progr. Neurobiol. 86, 156–185. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Halloran, L., Cao, Z., Ruddy, K., Jollans, L., Albaugh, M. D., Aleni, A., et al. (2018). Neural circuitry underlying sustained attention in healthy adolescents and in ADHD symptomatology. Neuroimage 169, 395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oswal, A., Litvak, V., Sauleau, P., and Brown, P. (2012). Beta reactivity, prospective facilitation of executive processing, and its dependence on dopaminergic therapy in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurosci. 32, 9909–9916. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0275-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pandey, A. K., Kamarajan, C., Manz, N., Chorlian, D. B., Stimus, A., and Porjesz, B. (2016). Delta, theta, and alpha event-related oscillations in alcoholics during Go/NoGo task: neurocognitive deficits in execution, inhibition, and attention processing. Progr. Neuro Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 158–171. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penttonen, M., and Buzsáki, G. (2003). Natural logarithmic relationship between brain oscillators. Thalamus Relat. Syst. 2, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/s1472-9288(03)00007-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., and Rueda, M. R. (2014). “Developing attention and self-regulation in childhood,” in The Oxford Handbook of Attention, Ed. S. Kastner (Oxford: Oxford University Press), doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199675111.013.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabbi, A. F., Ivanca, K., Putnam, A. V., Musa, A., Thaden, C. B., and Fazel-Rezai, R. (2009). “Human performance evaluation based on EEG signal analysis: a prospective review,” in Proceedings of the 2009 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (Piscataway, NJ: IEEE), 1879–1882. doi: 10.1109/iembs.2009.5333877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R., Philiastides, M. G., and Sajda, P. (2009). Quality of evidence for perceptual decision making is indexed by trial-to-trial variability of the EEG. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 6539–6544. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0812589106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinerman-Jones, L., Matthews, G., and Mercado, J. E. (2016). Detection tasks in nuclear power plant operation: vigilance decrement and physiological workload monitoring. Saf. Sci. 88, 97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.ssci.2016.05.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ridderinkhof, K. R., Ullsperger, M., Crone, E. A., and Nieuwenhuis, S. (2004). The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science 306, 443–447. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosch, K. S., Dirlikov, B., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2013). Increased intrasubject variability in boys with ADHD across tests of motor and cognitive control. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41, 485–495. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9690-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salum, G. A., Sato, J. R., Manfro, A. G., Pan, P. M., Gadelha, A., do Rosário, M. C., et al. (2019). Reaction time variability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: is increased reaction time variability specific to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Testing predictions from the default-mode interference hypothesis. ADHD 11, 47–58. doi: 10.1007/s12402-018-0257-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger, R., and Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: a possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34, 1015–1022. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastiani, M., Di Flumeri, G., Aricò, P., Sciaraffa, N., Babiloni, F., and Borghini, G. (2020). Neurophysiological vigilance characterisation and assessment: laboratory and realistic validations involving professional air traffic controllers. Brain Sci. 10:48. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10010048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepede, G., De Berardis, D., Campanella, D., Perrucci, M. G., Ferretti, A., Serroni, N., et al. (2012). Impaired sustained attention in euthymic bipolar disorder patients and non-affected relatives: an fMRI study. Bipolar Disord. 14, 764–779. doi: 10.1111/bdi.12007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, K. L., Hanslmayr, S., Enns, J. T., and Lleras, A. (2017). Alpha, beta: the rhythm of the attentional blink. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 24, 1862–1869. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1257-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 977–986. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinborn, M. B., Langner, R., Flehmig, H. C., and Huestegge, L. (2018). Methodology of performance scoring in the d2 sustained-attention test: cumulative-reliability functions and practical guidelines. Psychol. Assess. 30:339. doi: 10.1037/pas0000482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamm, L., Narad, M. E., Antonini, T. N., O’Brien, K. M., Hawk, L. W., and Epstein, J. N. (2012). Reaction time variability in ADHD: a review. Neurotherapeutics 9, 500–508. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0138-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Truccolo, W. A., Ding, M., Knuth, K. H., Nakamura, R., and Bressler, S. L. (2002). Trial-to-trial variability of cortical evoked responses: implications for the analysis of functional connectivity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 113, 206–226. doi: 10.1016/s1388-2457(01)00739-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uusberg, A., Uibo, H., Kreegipuu, K., and Allik, J. (2013). EEG alpha and cortical inhibition in affective attention. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 89, 26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Driel, J., Ridderinkhof, K. R., and Cohen, M. X. (2012). Not all errors are alike: theta and alpha EEG dynamics relate to differences in error-processing dynamics. J. Neurosci. 32, 16795–16806. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0802-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VanRullen, R., Busch, N., Drewes, J., and Dubois, J. (2011). Ongoing EEG phase as a trial-by-trial predictor of perceptual and attentional variability. Front. Psychol. 2:60. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaurio, R. G., Simmonds, D. J., and Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Increased intra-individual reaction time variability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across response inhibition tasks with different cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia 47, 2389–2396. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wascher, E., Rasch, B., Sänger, J., Hoffmann, S., Schneider, D., Rinkenauer, G., et al. (2014). Frontal theta activity reflects distinct aspects of mental fatigue. Biol. Psychol. 96, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, Y. F., He, Y., Zhu, C. Z., Cao, Q. J., Sui, M. Q., Liang, M., et al. (2007). Altered baseline brain activity in children with ADHD revealed by resting-state functional MRI. Brain Dev. 29, 83–91. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2006.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sustained attention, EEG, frontal theta activity, reaction-time variability, trial-by-trial fluctuation, frequency-dependent fluctuation

Citation: Wang Y-Y, Sun L, Liu Y-W, Pan J-H, Zheng Y-M, Wang Y-F, Zang Y-F and Zhang H (2020) The Low-Frequency Fluctuation of Trial-by-Trial Frontal Theta Activity and Its Correlation With Reaction-Time Variability in Sustained Attention. Front. Psychol. 11:1555. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01555

Received: 22 February 2020; Accepted: 10 June 2020;
Published: 14 July 2020.

Edited by:

Daya Shankar Gupta, Camden County College, United States

Reviewed by:

Pietro Aricò, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Ariel Telpaz, General Motors, United States

Copyright © 2020 Wang, Sun, Liu, Pan, Zheng, Wang, Zang and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Hang Zhang, a2V2aW5oYW5nYm51QGZveG1haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.