Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 30 July 2020
Sec. Environmental Psychology

Children’s Microsystems and Their Relationship to Stress and Executive Functioning

\r\nJose Antonio Campos-Gil*Jose Antonio Campos-Gil1*Patricia Ortega-Andeane*Patricia Ortega-Andeane1*Delfino VargasDelfino Vargas2
  • 1School of Psychology, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico
  • 2University Program of Development Studies, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico City, Mexico

Microsystems are described as contexts formed by a subject, their roles, their interactions, and a specific physical space and time, such as housing and the school environment. Although several studies suggest the importance of studying this type of environment and its repercussion on children’s development, in a Latin American context, few studies integrate the interaction of two primary settings in the development of executive functioning. The present study explores the effects of the quality of housing and school environments on the perception of stress, decision making, and planning among children. A total of 114 children (43% girls and 57% boys, Mage = 10.57) from a primary school located in a community classified as poor participated in the study. The following was measured: the environmental quality of classrooms, housing, stress, and executive functioning of children. The results reveal a model linking environmental quality levels in children’s homes and schools and executive functioning. We also obtain a mediating role of stress between microsystems and performance, finding a deficit in executive performance when children experienced higher levels of stress as a result of poor environmental quality both in their homes and in their schools.

Introduction

Although studying the effects of environmental settings on cognitive development has become in recent years a field of extensive research in various domains such as economics (World Bank, 2014), psychology (Montero and Evans, 2010), and epidemiology (Garcia-Coll et al., 1998), it is still to be studied comprehensively in Mexico, although the specific context makes the topic a national priority.

Indeed, according to the latest report of the National Council for the Evaluation of Social Development Policy in Mexico (CONEVAL), the percentage of people in poverty is 43.6% of the total population (CONEVAL, 2013; CONEVAL, 2015). According to the National Institute of Geography and Statistics (INEGI, 2011), by 2015, 27.26% of the population was between the ages of 0 and 14, representing a large group in the country; of these, 21.4 million were children and teenagers. The United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF, 2015) reported that 4.6 million were in extreme poverty, making children one of the most vulnerable groups, who experience deprivation throughout their development (UNICEF, 2015). These deprivations can be harmful, in health, including even physical deprivation in their main environments.

Given the magnitude of the phenomenon and the context in which these children and young people are immersed, it is necessary to extend systematic research on the effect of the physical and psychosocial elements involved in housing, schools, and neighborhoods (Lawrence, 2010).

At present, it is understood that child development is influenced by not only epigenetic factors but also environmental factors. Various investigations have found evidence of the impact of the socio-physical environment on the life span of a child, within the first years of life (Ferguson et al., 2013), especially if we refer to the family environment, which is the first microsystem with which the child interacts.

Similarly, Maxwell (2009, 2016) provided evidence on the importance of educational settings, such as the number of students and teachers and the available space for occupation within the school. Indeed, the performance and well-being of both students and teachers are affected by overcrowding, interestingly not only within the classroom but also in the entire school environment. Maxwell considers the role of the planning and control of enrollment, the size of classrooms, the arrangement of furniture, and the rotation of teachers. She finds that all these elements tend to generate environmental inconsistencies that stress the children. Similarly, the negative impact of noise near the school has been established, not only because of the stress and hearing impairment it produces but also because of the difficulty in concentration and understanding and its impact on problem solving and decision making (Evans et al., 2005; Evans, 2006; Estrada, 2007).

Bioecological Model of Development

Bronfenbrenner (1986) offers a theoretical perspective for human development and especially of the evolving interaction between the person and ecological environment, conceived as a set of nested structures, each inside the next, like a set of Russian dolls. As Bronfenbrenner (1979) said, “at the inner most level is the immediate setting contained developing person this can be the home or the classroom” (p. 3). A system is understood as a set of elements formed by a day, roles, a process, a socio-physical context, and time. The first is the “microsystem.” The second is the “mesosystem” comprising the interrelationships of two or more environments in which the developing person actively participates, for example, the child and the relationship between home and school, which comprises any alteration in the home that will impact the school and vice versa; for example, the death of a family member at home will affect his or her development at school.

Bronfenbrenner (1986) postulates that development is a mutual and progressive adaptation between human beings and the changing properties of their immediate environments. Therefore, the change of one element in the system will have ramifications in all the others and finally “the development of themselves,” which must be understood as the result of the biopsychosocial alterations of a person throughout his/her life (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2006).

Finally, at the final level, learning and psychological functioning are influenced by multiple and nested layers in a macrosystem, where such layers or microsystems are connected through the individual, and although these microsystems differ from each other, both affect an individual. Therefore, a second environment could moderate the experiences of the original microsystem. For example, playing an important role here is how children cope with stress, that is, the use of both individual and external resources to cope with problems.

However, it is important to expand the evidence regarding deficits in executive functioning; this construct is responsible for integrating many of the processes that allow us an adequate response to the environment that surrounds us, with other individuals, or an adequate cognitive goal functioning (Hackman et al., 2015).

This is the case of those who live and grow up in poverty, in insecurity, or without access to quality formal education; socioeconomic status (SES) also appears to affect school attendance and the number of years of schooling completed. The impact on years completed appears to be less than the impact on school achievement. Also, low-SES children more often manifest symptoms of psychiatric disturbance and maladaptive social functioning than do children from higher-SES circumstances (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002).

From early childhood onward, the number of settings in which the growing person actively intervenes increases gradually (for instance, home, school, and work). As a consequence, this growing participation is both a cause and a consequence of development (Bronfenbrenner, 1986); interactions must occur on a regular basis and over prolonged periods of time (Bronfenbrenner and Evans, 2000; Coley et al., 2015; Berry et al., 2016). The lack of routines and structure in the home has been found to be negatively associated with psychological adjustment in children, family satisfaction, and school performance (Fiese, 2006; Flouri, 2009; Fennis and Wiebenga, 2015).

Household Environmental Quality

The housing includes indoor and outdoor areas. Some aspects of housing and health are directly affected by the quality of infrastructure, as well as the conditions of the communities in which they are located (Ferguson et al., 2013). The main health effects include respiratory diseases related to indoor air quality, mortality related to thermal comfort, diseases transmitted due to exposure to pests, airborne infections, waterborne diseases, and domestic injuries. In terms of mental health, the sources are effects from noise, lead poisoning or exposure to other hazardous wastes such as asbestos, poor urban design, high residential density, the immediate environment of the dwelling (insufficient hygiene and sanitation), and type of dwelling.

To promote optimal development, according to the literature (Ferguson et al., 2009; Hackman et al., 2015), the household environment should (1) sustain the child (ensuring those elements that guarantee the biological integrity of the child), (2) stimulate activity in the child (aimed at the improvement of the child), (3) support the child’s self-sustaining capacities and trends, and (4) control the quantity and pattern of experiences for the child.

Caldwell and Bradley (1984) identified five basic functions that are performed by housing: (1) sustenance, (2) stimulation, (3) support, (4) structure, and (5) supervision. For the purposes of this paper, only four are highlighted.

School Environment

In the particular case of Mexico, not all schools have the possibility of moderating their social density through district mechanisms such as those used, for example, in the United States. In 2006, Mexico had an average of 21 children per classroom in public sector schools, reaching groups of 35 students in some states (INEE, 2006); Maxwell (2006) refers that one way to reduce the perception of overcrowding is by regulating enrollment or by building schools that provide more space per child; however, this implies higher economic costs, making it more feasible to address other variables, such as noise, whose presence in classrooms has been proven to hinder the intelligibility of words in children (Estrada, 2007); also lack of concentration affects not only academic performance but also the development of adequate emotional regulation (Maxwell and Evans, 2000; Zimmerman, 2005; Blair, 2010) and makes it impossible to attend to the content of the classes (Evans and Lepore, 1993).

The school is the space most important after home; initially, the school environment was not considered a relevant variable in the teaching–learning process, but lately, research has sought to break down the elements of the school environment and its impact on children’s development (Evans, 2004; Estrada, 2007; Maxwell, 2009; Abry et al., 2017).

In Mexico, important achievements have been made in terms of coverage of primary education; nevertheless, there are important disparities in education and school settings causing a significant proportion of the poor or most vulnerable sectors to not have access to them and causing many of those who enter education to not be able to conclude their studies (Bulotsky-Shearer et al., 2012; Hackman et al., 2015; Maxwell, 2016). This inequality in supply extends to various states, between rural and urban areas, and to private and public schools (UNICEF, 2015).

Today, UNESCO believes that quality must go beyond ensuring that children enter and remain in the classroom; rather, it is the “combination of teaching–learning conditions and student achievement” (UNESCO, 2013).

Research has revealed the importance of various aspects of the school, each having specific effects on development. First, the availability of school facilities and equipment or the sufficiency (concrete or perceived) of educational facilities, services, furniture, materials, and equipment as a necessary condition for school processes to take place (Comité Administrador del Programa Federal de Construcción de Escuelas [CAPFCE], 2001; NCERT, 2001; INEE, 2006; Walden, 2015). The physical conditions of facilities and equipment, that is, the design, operation, maintenance, and age of facilities, can have a positive impact on student experiences and consequently on educational performance (Bulotsky-Shearer et al., 2012). Research has also shown the importance of physical comfort in the classroom, for example, thermal comfort, ventilation, acoustics, lighting, and the quality of the furniture (Hannah, 2013; Hatfield et al., 2016). Maxwell (2009) argued that a school with high levels of noise, overcrowding, confusion, changes in teachers or classmates, poor-quality infrastructure, or an absence of routines might negatively affect children’s development. In spaces with quality deprivations, it is common for both children and teachers to experience overcrowding, due to density, temperature, and lighting (Maxwell, 2006); a student or teacher may then feel overcrowded when he or she observes that his or her ability to control interaction with others is affected or when others interfere with his or her ability to perform an activity such as reading and studied conversation (Fennis and Wiebenga, 2015).

Indeed, schools with higher enrollment have been identified as having reduced opportunities for meaningful student participation, higher dropout rates, and disruptive behaviors among students (Maxwell, 2006).

In contrast, schools with lower social density, with low-income students (Johnson, 2000), found better student attitudes, greater attention span, fewer behavioral problems, greater participation in extracurricular activities, and greater leadership development (Maxwell, 2006; Buckrop et al., 2016).

Stress

Psycho-Environmental Perspective on Stress

There are a variety of factors that can mediate the reaction of stress for which environmental stress is defined as the product of psychological aspects that arise from the relationship of the individual with the environment; its study is carried out in previously defined contexts, where the dynamics and relationship between the demands of the environment toward an individual and the resources available to face them have been reflected (Baum et al., 1981; Kaminoff and Proshansky, 1982; Evans and Cohen, 1991; Ortega et al., 2016).

Environmental Stressors

1. Temperature (Evans and Stecker, 2004; Halpern, 1995): An inverted “U” effect has been observed, indicating that at higher levels of heat, aggressive and hostile behaviors increase with temperature, but this relationship begins to reverse to a certain point where temperature continues to rise.

2. Noise: Noise is a factor that has serious psychological effects on individuals. It is important to specify that noise is that sound that is undesirable and annoying to the listener; therefore, not everyone will experience noise in the same way. It has been found that sounds at high decibels are linked to effects on mental health, interference in tasks, the intelligibility of words, and attention (Evans, 2006; Estrada, 2007).

3. Overcrowding: The subjective perception that available space is insufficient depends on the person’s assessment of the environment which can be independent of density, which is the objective measurement of a person’s physical space, where, for study, social density (the variation of the number of people in a fixed space) is distinguished from spatial density (the variation of space in relation to a number of people) (Lawrence, 2010).

While overcrowding is closely related to both spatial and social density (Maxwell, 2006), a student or teacher may then feel overcrowded when he or she observes that his or her ability to control interaction with others is affected or when others interfere with his or her ability to perform an activity such as reading, studying, or talking.

Developmental Stress

Many studies have found that receiving low-quality care at an early age can serve as a primary source of stress for children. Indeed, households more likely to be overcrowded, disorganized, and lacking appropriate stimulation and resources for an adequate quality of life can result in physiological stress markers being found in young people, who had presented a high allostatic load during their childhood (Smith and Ezzati, 2005; Evans and Kim, 2012). This association is mediated by the time of exposure: the longer a child grows under poor physical conditions, the more the cumulative damage increases (Curby et al., 2010).

Garcia-Coll et al. (1998) have evaluated cognitive functions such as memory and problem solving in low-income children. Most longitudinal studies have found that young people who experienced less poverty or had a lower allostatic load during childhood obtained better grades and retained more information than their peers with longer exposure times.

Executive Functions

Suchy (2009) explained that executive functions (EFs) are neuropsychological constructs that can be defined as the ability to form, maintain, and change mental states; this implies reasoning skills, the generation of goals and plans, the maintenance of concentration, and the flexibility to alternate between those goals and plans in response to contingencies that may arise. Although EFs can be studied from a purely functional approach, considering their anatomical substrate provides valuable information regarding their organization and functioning; it is therefore paramount to clarify that in anatomical terms, the prefrontal cortex (CPF) is a primordial area in charge of EF, by means of which it has been studied that one of these functions have been the establishment of goals and planning, defined as the capacity to reach goals or objectives in the short or long term (Baker et al., 1996; Lozano and Ostrosky, 2011).

Environment and EFs

The effects of both the natural and constructed environments have been explored in relation to the broad range of functioning, including the cognitive process (Hygge and Knez, 2001), affection (Loewen and Suedfeld, 1992), and health (Evans et al., 2003); among the main settings evaluated have been educational institutions where they have evaluated the amount of light, temperature, and sound and its relationship to learning or predecessor functions to learning (Choi et al., 2016). The main findings contribute to considering noise as an irrelevant stimulus that diverts and even filters the attention that a person pays to a specific goal (Hygge and Knez, 2001; Hannah, 2013); likewise, it has been observed that the absence or interruption of learning resources in the home or school classroom is a predictor of decreased inhibitory control and cognitive flexibility (Hackman et al., 2015).

While the variety of settings in which a human being can grow up is wide, environments that are physically and socially impoverished can have long-term detrimental effects on the child, such as stress and problems in planning, attention, or decision making. It is reasonable to believe that children exposed to multiple stressful contexts experience greater vulnerability compared to children exposed to fewer stressful contexts; therefore, the objective was to explore the relationships among quality of housing and school with the perception of stress, planning, and decision making in children.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Setting

To obtain the sample, the research project was presented in a public school in the State of Mexico, and specifically in the municipality of Chalco, where the percentage of poverty is 54.5% and that of extreme poverty is 8.7% and which is classified nationally as one of the municipalities in “average” poverty according to the study by Cortés and Vargas (2019).

In this school, they were interested in the research, so a meeting was held with the director of each institution to establish a work route, which consisted of a round of information to parents, that is, informing in a group session the objectives of the research and requesting consent as well as ensuring the privacy of the information obtained.

The sample consisted of 114 children, of whom 43% were girls and 57% were boys with an average age of 10.57 years (9–11 years old, SD = 0.51), coming from a charitable school which differs from the socioeconomic level of children’s housing because it has a better infrastructure than other schools in the same area. Participants’ house and school were in the poorest community of Valle of Chalco, which has a population of 357,645 (as of 2010), where at least 20% experience educational backwardness, up to 30% has low-quality housing, and up to 50% do not have access to social security (CONEVAL, 2015).

Instruments

Physical Measurements

Spatial density

Spatial density was measured with a laser distance meter, and the dimensions of the classroom were then calculated as the area in square meters over the number of students at the time of evaluation.

a r e a s t u d e n t s
Noise

A digital decibel meter (Model HER-403, with a measurement range of 30–130 dB) was used to record the noise levels inside the classroom, calculating the average decibels for 20 min.

Temperature

The temperature inside the room was recorded with a digital thermometer with a humidity sensor (Model TER-150).

Environmental Variables

The Environmental Quality Scale for Housing (EQSH) that was created for the study consists of 26 items and had a total Cronbach alpha of 0.886. With this scale, participants have to indicate the presence or absence of certain characteristic in their houses with four response options (Never to Always); the items are distributed in four factors: support, stimulation, social integration, and structure (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Reactive example of ECAV: “Have a space where I can be alone if I wish?”

The School Environmental Quality Scale (SEQS) that was created for the study consists of 24 reagents and had a total Cronbach alpha of 0.880. With this scale, participants have to indicate the presence or absence of certain characteristics in schools with four response options coded as 0–3 (Never–Always); the reagents are distributed in four factors: support, stimulation, social integration, and structure (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Reactive example of ECAE: “Is the classroom tidy?”

For both scales, a graphic representation was designed by item, looking for the illustrations to facilitate the comprehension of the children; these scales were applied through the software PEBL (The Psychology Experiment Building Languaje, Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. PEBL screen.

Psychological Measure

Child Stress Scale consists of 44 multiple-choice items (“nothing,” “little,” “enough,” and “much”) by means of a pictorial scale; these are grouped into eight factors: lack of family acceptance, verbal aggression and mockery, lack of trust and respect, school pressure, family demand, punishment, family conflicts, and fears. Finally, the scale has a global alpha of 0.91 (Lucio et al., unpublished manuscript).

Executive Functioning Tasks

In the case of decision making, the complexity is higher; however, neuropsychologists have focused on working with conditions of ambiguity and uncertainty, using a system of rewards and punishments (Iowa Gambling Task) where the key variable is the degree of consistency in the pattern of decisions (Verdejo-García and Bechara, 2010).

The “Hungry Donkey Task” (HDT) is an isomorphism of the “Iowa Game of Chance” (Mueller and Piper, 2014). The HDT consists of four blocks of 70 trials for a total of 280 trials presented where a donkey chooses from four doors, each with a cost or reward in apples; the task will be that the child plans and makes decisions from the feedback of the doors, the ultimate goal being to give the donkey as many apples as possible (Crone and Van Der Molen, 2004).

In the “Simón Task,” children are presented with congruent stimuli: arrows in the right or left field of vision; these will be pointing to the left or right, so children will respond through the button that goes according to the direction of the arrow. However, there will also be incongruent stimuli: arrows pointing to the left will appear on the right side, and vice versa. The number of trials is 140, which are presented randomly in two sets. The time it takes to run the experiment is about 15 min.

Procedure

The inventories were applied alternating between the application of psychometric instruments (using pencil and paper), followed by those where respondents answered in a computer (ECAV and ECAE) and at the end the HDT and “Simón Task.” Each set of tests was applied on different days, starting with psychometric instruments, followed by the ECAV and ECAE on the second day and the HDT and “Simón task” on the third day, with the purpose of diminishing the fatigue of the evaluation (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Student responding to computer tests.

Once all the instruments were applied, the information was coded and entered into the statistical program STATA, for the preparation of the database, to finally analyze the data with the Smart PLS data package.

Plan of Analysis

We opted for a partial least squares (PLS) model in which the focus is on the analysis of variance as opposed to models [structural equation model (SEM)], which worked with covariance for this type of analysis using the software Smart PLS. A partial model is a modeling technique with latent variables, such as the quality of the spaces, which allows the incorporation of multiple dependent constructs, such as the factors that build these scales, like support, stimulation, structure, and social integration, explicitly recognizing the measurement error. However, the least restrictive is in terms of normal distribution assumptions, allowing us to simultaneously estimate/explore both a measurement model and a structural model from a small sample.

Results

It is important to mention that by internal regulations of the school, the classes are not mixed in terms of gender. There were 114 students who were grouped into four groups according to sex and school year: fifth-year girls (n = 14), fifth-year boys (n = 32), sixth-year girls (n = 35), and sixth-year boys (n = 33). The sample was selected intentionally, where the groups were previously formed. We applied the instruments to all the students from the fifth and sixth grades.

In relation to the physical conditions of the classrooms, the following scores were obtained per group (Table 1), finding that the boys’ classrooms were larger (t = −85.104, p < 0.0001) and at the same time warmer (t = −33.732, p < 0.0001) than the girls’ classrooms and also noisier (t = −2.745, p = 0.007) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Physical characteristics of classrooms in the function of the gender and the grade.

As for the perception of the quality that the children (Table 2) had of their microsystems, although this does not differ from gender, it does so from one school year to another. Both at home and at school, significant differences were found where the fifth-grade group indicates greater support both in their classroom (F = 4.53DF=78, p = 0.035) and in their homes (F = 5.41, p = 0.022). On the other hand, sixth-grade children reported that their classrooms provided more of a structure, unlike their younger congeners (Figure 5).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Environmental quality factors by grades.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Formative model.

Stress

Girls in both fifth (F = 6.230, p = 0.017) and sixth grades (F = 5.526, p = 0.022) experienced higher levels of stress compared to boys, related to family conflicts and fear. Compared with boys of the same age, in the rest of factors, although they obtained higher scores, they did not differ enough on statistical terms (Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Stress score by gender.

Executive Functioning

It can be seen in Table 3 that although the groups, in terms of gender, had results in terms of planning below 0, the t-test (1,246, p = 0.213) indicates that there were no significant differences.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. EF scores.

We fit an SEM-PLS model in this investigation. First, we estimated a second-order factor model for the latent variable called environmental quality of the home EQSH (support, structure, stimulation, and integration). Similarly, eight factors conform to the stress latent variable (lack of family acceptance, verbal aggression, lack of trust and respect, school pressure, family demand, punishment, family conflicts, and fears). Similarly, executive functioning is constructed using four manifest variables (congruent hits, incongruent hits, total HDT, and total good choices). The latent variable SEQS is constructed using four manifest variables (support SEQS, stimulation SEQS, structure SEQS, and social integration SEQS). The path connections of the SEM-PLS model are shown in Figure 7.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Structural equation model using partial least squares (SEM-PLS).

SEMs, in general, are not designed for testing causal relations among the constructs but provide useful information of the path relations supported by a framework (Cortés, 2019, pp. 113–115).

Additionally, we need to provide some criteria that ensure the model has a good fit. In SEM-PLS, we need to show that there is discriminant validity among the various instruments (Hair et al., 2014).

Once the model parameters have been estimated through the statistical analysis software Smart PLS, the main components of the variables were identified and are listed in Table 3; the average variance extracted (AVE) values greater than 0.50 indicated that environmental quality constructs are valid at a convergent level (Wold, 1985).

The AVE for SEQS is 0.523, indicating that about 52% of the variance is explained by this construct. Similarly, EQSH explains 62.1% of the variance. The composite reliability is similar to Cronbach’s alpha; for example, EQSH has a 0.83 Cronbach’s alpha and 0.86 composite reliability, both numbers indicating good reliability measures. We noticed that EF has the lowest reliability values.

Once the model parameters have been estimated through the statistical analysis software Smart PLS, the main components of the variables were identified and are listed in Table 3; the AVE values greater than 0.50 indicated that environmental quality constructs are valid at a convergent level.

To verify the discriminant validity of the measurement model, we use the Fornell–Larcker criteria (Hair et al., 2014) that compares AVE to the correlation between the variables of the model. Table 4 shows along the diagonal theAVE. For example, 0.523=0.723, which is greater than the absolute value of correlation of the latent variables EQSH (0.28), stress (0.18), and EF (0.05). Similarly, the AVE for EQSH is 0.621=0.788, which is greater than the correlations of EQSH (0.28), stress (0.11), and EF (0.09). Therefore, the discriminant validity is supported (see Table 5).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Quality criteria of the model.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Fornell–Larcker criterion for discriminant validity.

To verify the discriminant validity of the measurement model, the square root of the AVE was compared with the correlation between the variables of the model. When the square root is greater than 0.5 in all cases, it is assumed that the model is valid in a discriminant way (Wold, 1985).

The latent variable stress is formed by lack of acceptance (4.655), verbal aggression (4.080), lack of confidence (3.99), school pressure (3.26), family demand (4.335), punishment (1.60), family problems (3.09), and fear (2.135), while the latent variable of executive functioning is positively shaped by congruent hits (1.602), incongruent successes (1.674), good choices (0.947), and total score (0.098).

The criteria used to evaluate an internal model involve the revision of the coefficient of determination R2, the standardized trajectory coefficients, the statisticians t, and the levels of significance (p = 0.05); there are paths that explain a negative impact of housing quality on executive functioning (planning and decision making) (-0.063), while the quality of the school environment has a positive impact (0.012). The mediating role of stress stands out, being negative between the quality of housing (-0.061) and school quality (-0.170), over executive functioning (-0.190).

Discussion

From the results about the physical characteristics of the school environment, we observe that a warm environment, high-density conditions in the classrooms according to the guidelines (Maxwell, 2006), and the noise levels experienced by the students influence overstimulation, being able to generate overcrowding. Later on, it will be necessary to integrate to the model the perception of the students about this phenomenon.

Some of the main characteristics in housing (Evans et al., 1989) are the type of construction, whether attached or not (it refers to houses that are built at the same time and that share at least one wall); the number of levels or floors; and finally the environmental quality, which in itself implies a broad set of characteristics, from structural quality to the presence of stimulating materials for learning and linguistic stimulation and even the safety of the physical environment (Bradley et al., 1989). It was possible to identify that the quality experienced by the different age groups is present in their homes, with those having low grades experiencing conditions that make it difficult to satisfy an adequate structure, sources of stimulation, and even support from their homes (CONAVI, 2012).

Several studies confirm the influence that environmental deficiencies can have throughout the growth of children, related to low SES (Schmitt and Lipscomb, 2016) or on the contrary that a home without deprivation favors the training of skills and competencies in children, which are maintained throughout adulthood (Evans and Kim, 2012). Therefore it is worrying that the youngest are experiencing these levels of quality in their microsystems. In Mexico, although Article 4 of the Constitution establishes “the right of every family to have a decent and decorous home,” in this article, not even the Housing Law specifies the minimum characteristics it must have; only some criteria formulated by the National Housing Commission (CONAVI) are available for the quality indicator, and housing spaces include two dimensions: the construction material of the home and the sizes.

As was also observed in the PLS model, other environments besides housing that may lag due to lack of resources, in this case, are school environments; those who enter an institution of this type from a very young age are more vulnerable, compared to those who entered at an older age, since they are more likely to recover as they have already consolidated aspects of their growth in other possibly healthier environments such as their homes (Bronfenbrenner and Morris, 2006).

Although classrooms offer the possibility of conversation and interactive activities, as a consequence, noise is generated inevitably, so these activities must be regulated, because high levels of stimulation require higher levels of psychic energy to maintain a certain concentration. The WHO recommends a maximum noise level of 35 dB in schools; however, the lowest noise level in this sample was 53 dB. This phenomenon can eventually lead to attention overload or the so-called cognitive fatigue, which inhibits learning (Estrada, 2007; Galindo and Sheldon, 2012).

In conclusion, it is important to focus efforts to be able to provide spaces of environmental quality for the positive development of children even when housing is important. It is very important that they develop and learn in a school environment that alleviates and generates a positive effect that compensates for their family microsystem (Choi et al., 2016), since intervening with or modifying the family environment with the necessary control is unlikely.

However, the house should also be one of the primary environments to intervene thinking that it represents a model of physical, social, and temporal stability, and ideally, it should not present elements of chaos that obstruct such stability according to numerous investigations (Wenner et al., 1985).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

JC-G, PO-A, and DV contributed conception and design of the study. JC-G and DV organized the database and performed the statistical analysis. JC-G wrote the first draft of the manuscript. JC-G and PO-A wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to conception and design of the study, manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The present project was financed by the Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología (CONACyT) through scholarship #336144 granted to the first author with the number CVU 621825.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abry, T., Bryce, C. I., Swanson, J., Bradley, R. H., Fabes, R. A., and Corwyn, R. F. (2017). Classroom-level adversity: associations with children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors across elementary school. Dev. Psychol. 53, 497–510. doi: 10.1037/dev0000268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C., Rogers, D., Owen, M., Frith, D., Dolan, J., Frackowiak, S., and Robbins, W. (1996). Neural systems engaged by planning: A PET study of the Tower of London task. Neuropsychologia 34, 515–526. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(95)00133-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baum, A., Singer, J. E., and Baum, C. (1981). Stress and the environment. J. Soc. 37, 4–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1981.tb01056.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, D., Blair, C., Willoughby, M., Garrett-Peters, P., Vernon-Feagans, L., Mills-Koonce, W. R., et al. (2016). Household chaos and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development in early childhood: does childcare play a buffering role? Early Childh. Res. Q. 34, 115–127. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C. (2010). Stress and the development of self-regulation in context. Child Dev. Perspect. 4, 181–188. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00145.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R., Caldwell, B., Rock, S., Barnard, K., Gray, C., and Hammond, M. (1989). Home environment and cognitive development in the first 3 years of life: a collaborative study involving six sites and three ethnic groups in North America. Dev. Psychol. 25, 217–235. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.2.217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Introduction socioeconomic status. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399.

Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: research perspectives. Dev. Psychol. 22, 723–742. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.22.6.723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U., and Evans, G. (2000). Developmental science in the 21st century: emerging theoretical models, research designs, and empirical findings. Soc. Dev. 9, 115–125. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00114

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U., and Morris, P. (2006). “The bioecological model of human development,” in Theoretical Models Of Human Development, Handbook Of Child Psychology, eds W. Damon and R. M. Lerner (New York, NY: Wiley), 793–828.

Google Scholar

Buckrop, J., Roberts, A., and LoCasale-Crouch, J. (2016). Children’s preschool classroom experiences and associations with early elementary special education referral. Early Childh. Res. Q. 36, 452–461. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bulotsky-Shearer, R. J., Wen, X., Faria, A.-M., Hahs-Vaughn, D. L., and Korfmacher, J. (2012). National Profiles of classroom quality and family involvement: a multilevel examination of proximal influences on Head Start children’s school readiness. Early Childh. Res. Q. 27, 627–639. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2012.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caldwell, B., and Bradley, R. (1984). Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment. Administration Manual. Revised edition.

Google Scholar

Choi, J. Y., Elicker, J., Christ, S. L., and Dobbs-Oates, J. (2016). Predicting growth trajectories in early academic learning: evidence from growth curve modeling with Head Start children. Early Childh. Res. Q. 36, 244–258. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.12.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coley, R. L., Lynch, A. D., and Kull, M. (2015). Early exposure to environmental chaos and children’s physical and mental health. Early Childh. Res. Q. 32, 94–104. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Comité Administrador del Programa Federal de Construcción de Escuelas [CAPFCE] (2001). Planeación, Programación Y Evaluación. Normas y Especificaciones Para Estudios, Proyectos, Construcción E Instalaciones. Libro 2. Normas Para Servicios Técnicos. México: CAPFCE.

Google Scholar

CONAVI (2012). Guía Para La Redensificación De Las Ciudades. Mexico: CONAVI.

Google Scholar

CONEVAL (2013). Informe de Pobreza En México 2012. México: CONEVAL.

Google Scholar

CONEVAL (2015). Informe de evaluación de la Política de Desarrollo Social en México 2014. México: CONEVAL.

Google Scholar

Cortés, F. (2019). “Métodos estadísticos no experimentales y causalidad,” in Aspectos metodológicos para la investigación social: Modelos de ecuaciones estructurales, ed. D. Vargas (Ciudad de México: Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México), 113–128.

Google Scholar

Cortés, F., and Vargas, D. (2019). “El cambio de la pobreza municipal en México entre 2010 y 2015,” in A diez años de la Gran Recesión: Desastres y Desarrollo en México, eds R. Cordera and E. Provencio (Ciudad de México: Programa Universitario de Estudios del Desarrollo UNAM), 69–76.

Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., and Van Der Molen, M. W. (2004). Developmental changes in real life decision making: performance on a gambling task previously shown to depend on the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Dev. Neuropsychol. 25, 251–279. doi: 10.1207/s15326942dn2503_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curby, T. W., Grimm, K., and Pianta, R. C. (2010). Stability and change in early childhood classroom interactions during the first two hours of a day. Early Childh. Res. Q. 25, 373–384. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Estrada, C. (2007). Efectos Psicológicos de la Contaminación Por Ruido En Escenarios Educativos. Tesis Doctoral. México: UNAM.

Google Scholar

Evans, G. (2004). The environment of childhood poverty. Am. Psychol. 59, 77–92.

Google Scholar

Evans, G. (2006). Child development and the physical environment. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 57, 423–451. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G., and Cohen, S. (1991). “Environmental stress,” in Handbook of Environmental Psychology, eds D. Stokols and I. Altman (Malabar: Krieger Publishing Company), 571–610.

Google Scholar

Evans, G., and Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty and young adults’ allostatic load the mediating role of childhood cumulative risk exposure. Psychol. Sci. 23, 979–983. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G., and Lepore, S. (1993). Nonauditory effects of noise children: a critical review. Child. Environ. 10, 31–51.

Google Scholar

Evans, G., Wells, N., and Moch, A. (2003). Housing and mental health: a review of the evidence and a methodological and conceptual critique. J. Soc. Issues 59, 475–500. doi: 10.1111/1540-4560.00074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Gonnella, C., Marcynyszyn, L. A., Gentile, L., and Salpekar, N. (2005). The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychol. Sci. 16, 560–565. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., Palsane, M. N., Lepore, S. J., and Martin, J. (1989). Residential density and psychological health: The mediating effects of social support. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 994–999. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.6.994

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Stecker, R. (2004). Motivational consequences of environmental stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 24, 143–165. doi: 10.1016/s0272-4944(03)00076-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fennis, B. M., and Wiebenga, J. H. (2015). Disordered environments prompt mere goal pursuit. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 226–237. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, K., Cassells, R., MacAllister, J., and Evans, G. (2013). The physical environment and child development. An international review. Intern. J. Psychol. 48, 437–468. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2013.804190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, K. T., Kim, P., Dunn, J. R., and Evans, G. W. (2009). “An ecological model of urban child health,” in Urban Health and Society: Interdisciplinary Approaches to Research and Practice, eds N. Freudenberg, S. Klitzman, and S. Saegert (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 63–91.

Google Scholar

Fiese, B. (2006). Family Routines And Rituals. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Google Scholar

Flouri, E. (2009). Strong families, tidy houses, and children’s values in adult life: are “chaotic”, “crowded” and “unstable” homes really so bad? Intern. J. Behav. Dev. 33, 496–503. doi: 10.1177/0165025409340090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galindo, C., and Sheldon, S. B. (2012). School and home connections and children’s kindergarten achievement gains: the mediating role of family involvement. Early Childh. Res. Q. 27, 90–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Coll, C., Buckner, J., Brooks, M., Weinreb, L., and Bassuk, E. (1998). The developmental status and adaptive behavior of homeless and low-income housed infants and toddlers. Am. J. Public Health 88, 1371–1374. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.9.1371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D., Gallop, R., Evans, G., and Farah, M. (2015). Socioeconomic status and executive function: developmental trajectories and mediation. Dev. Sci. 18, 686–702. doi: 10.1111/desc.12246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M., and Sarstedt, M. (2014). A Primer on Partial Least Squares Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM). Washington D.C: SAGE.

Google Scholar

Halpern, D. (1995). Mental Health and the Built Environment. Milton Park: Taylor & Francis.

Google Scholar

Hannah, R. (2013). The Effect of classroom environment on student learning. Hon. Theses Pap. 2375, 1–22. doi: 10.1167/iovs.07-0356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatfield, B. E., Burchinal, M. R., Pianta, R. C., and Sideris, J. (2016). Thresholds in the association between quality of teacher-child interactions and preschool children’s school readiness skills. Early Childh. Res. Q. 36, 561–571. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2015.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hygge, S., and Knez, I. (2001). Effects of noise, heat and indoor lighting on cognitive performance and self-reported affect. J. Environ. Psychol. 21, 291–299. doi: 10.1006/jevp.2001.0222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

INEE (2006). Estimaciones a Partir Del Sistema De Estadísticas Continuas de la DGPP/SEP, Inicio del ciclo Escolar 2004/2005. New York, NY: INEE.

Google Scholar

INEGI (2011). Censo de Población y Vivienda 2010. Consulta Interactiva De Datos, Recuperado en 9 de Abril de 2015. Aguascalientes: INEGI.

Google Scholar

Johnson, K. A. (2000). Do Small Classes Influence Academic Achievement? What the National Assessment of Educational Progress Shows. Washington, DC: Heritage Foundation.

Google Scholar

Kaminoff, R., and Proshansky, H. (1982). “Stress as a consequence of the urban physical environment,” in Handbook of Stress: Theoretical and Clinical Aspects, ed. Y. S. Goldberg (New York, NY: Mc Millan Publising).

Google Scholar

Lawrence, R. J. (2010). Housing and health promotion: moving forward. Int. J. Public Health 55, 145–146. doi: 10.1007/s00038-010-0118-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loewen, L., and Suedfeld, P. (1992). Cognitive and arousal effects of masking office noise. Environ. Behav. 24, 381–395. doi: 10.1177/0013916592243006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lozano, A., and Ostrosky, F. (2011). Desarrollo de las funciones ejecutivas y de la corteza prefrontal. Rev. Neuropsicol. Neuropsiquiatr. Neurocien. 11, 159–172.

Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. (2006). “Crowding, class size, and school size,” in Safe and Healthy School Environments, eds H. Frumkin, R. Geller, I. L. Rubin, and J. Nodvin (Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press), 13–19. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195179477.003.0002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. (2009). “Chaos outside the home: the school environment,” in Chaos and its Influence On Children’s Development: An Ecological Perspective, eds G. W. Evans and T. D. Wachs (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 83–96.

Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. E. (2016). School building condition, social climate, student attendance and academic achievement: a mediation model. J. Environ. Psychol. 46, 206–216. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. E., and Evans, G. W. (2000). The effects of noise on preschool children’s prereading skills. J. Environ. Psychol. 20, 91–97.

Google Scholar

Montero, M., and Evans, G. (2010). “Perspectiva ecológica social, una opción heurística para el estudio de la pobreza,” in Ecología Social de la Pobreza: Impactos Psicosociales, Desafíos Multidisciplinarios, eds M. Montero and D. Mayer (México: UNAM).

Google Scholar

Mueller, S. T., and Piper, B. J. (2014). The psychology experiment building language (PEBL) and PEBL test battery. J. Neurosci. Methods 222, 250–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

NCERT (2001). National Curriculum Framework For School Education. National Council Of Educational Research And Training. New Delhi: NCERT.

Google Scholar

Ortega, P., Mercado, S., Reidl, L., and Estrada, C. (2016). Estrés Ambiental En Instituciones de Salud. Valoración Psicoambiental. México: UNAM.

Google Scholar

Schmitt, S. A., and Lipscomb, S. T. (2016). Longitudinal associations between residential mobility and early academic skills among low-income children. Early Childh. Res. Q. 36, 190–200. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K. R., and Ezzati, M. (2005). How environmental health risks change with development: the epidemiologic and environmental risk transitions revisited. Annu. Rev. Environ. Resour. 30, 291–333. doi: 10.1146/annurev.energy.30.050504.144424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suchy, Y. (2009). Executive functioning: overview, assessment, and research issues for non-neuropsychologists. Annu. Behav. Med. 37, 106–116. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9097-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

UNESCO (2013). Situación Educativa de América Latina y el Caribe: Hacia la Educación de Calidad Para Todos al 2015, Recuperado El 12 De Octubre de 2017. Paris: UNESCO.

Google Scholar

UNICEF (2015). Informe Anual 2014. México: UNICEF.

Google Scholar

Verdejo-García, A., and Bechara, A. (2010). Neuropsychology of executive functions. Psicothema 22, 227–235.

Google Scholar

Walden, R. (2015). Schools for the Future, Design Proposals from Architectural Psychology. Boston, MA: Hogrefe.

Google Scholar

Wenner, C., Altman, I., and Oxley, D. (1985). “Temporal aspects of homes a transactional perspective,” in Home Environments Human Behavior and Environment Advances in Theory Research, eds I. Altman and C. Wenner (Utah: Springer Science), 1–32. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-2266-3_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wold, H. (1985). “Partial least squares,” in Encyclopedia of Statistical Sciences, Vol. 6, eds S. Kotz and N. L. Johnson (New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons), 581–591.

Google Scholar

World Bank (2014). World Development Report 2015: Mind, Society, and Behavior. Washington, DC: The World Bank.

Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (2005). “Attaining self-regulation. A social cognitive perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds M. Boekaerts, R. Pintrich, and Y. M. Zeidner (San Diego: Elsevier Press), 13–39. doi: 10.1016/b978-012109890-2/50031-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: microsystems, executive functions, children, classroom, stress

Citation: Campos-Gil JA, Ortega-Andeane P and Vargas D (2020) Children’s Microsystems and Their Relationship to Stress and Executive Functioning. Front. Psychol. 11:996. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00996

Received: 27 December 2019; Accepted: 21 April 2020;
Published: 30 July 2020.

Edited by:

César O. Tapia-Fonllem, University of Sonora, Mexico

Reviewed by:

Laura Fernanda Barrera-Hernández, Instituto Tecnológico de Sonora (ITSON), Mexico
Lucía Poggio, Universidad Europea de Madrid, Spain

Copyright © 2020 Campos-Gil, Ortega-Andeane and Vargas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jose Antonio Campos-Gil, psi.tono@gmail.com; Patricia Ortega-Andeane, andeane@unam.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.