Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 25 October 2019
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic How Children Learn from Parents and Parenting Others in Formal and Informal Settings: International and Cultural Perspectives - Volume I View all 11 articles

Father Involvement and Cognitive Development in Early and Middle Childhood: A Systematic Review

  • Department of Psychology, University of Torino, Turin, Italy

This systematic review aims to examine the existing literature concerning the association between father involvement and the development children's cognitive skills during early and middle childhood. Specifically, it analyzes: (1) how the number of researches developed across years; (2) which are the main socio-demographic characteristics of the samples; (3) which are the main focuses examined; and (4) which operational definitions were used to assess father involvement and children cognitive skills. Following the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement, the articles were searched through PubMed and EBSCO (PsycInfo, PsycArticles, Education Source, Social Sciences Abstract, Family Studies Abstracts, Gender Studies Database and CINAHL complete). The findings suggest that, although each research used a different operational definition of the father involvement construct, in recent years there was a wide and constant interest increase about this issue. Most of the empirical studies utilized quantitative methods, whereas relatively few used qualitative and only one mixed methods. As regards the analysis of socio-demographic characteristics of the samples there is a great evidence that most of them included biological and residential fathers: it may reflect that this type of sample is easier to recruit than non-residential and non-biological fathers. Regarding the socio-economic status and the ethnicity of families, the data highlighted how in recent years the literature on father involvement is starting to look at differences in ethnic and cultural backgrounds, in contrast to past researches. The findings revealed that the main focus is the impact of father involvement on children's cognitive skills and the most of the studies highlighted that it is positive and statistically significant. Regarding to the assessment of father involvement and children's cognitive skills, the literature is quite heterogeneous

Introduction

Cognitive skills development is a broad concept that involves the maturing of a variety of abilities and is defined by the American Psychological Association (VandenBos, 2015) as “the skills involved in performing the tasks associated with perception, learning, memory, understanding, awareness, reasoning, judgment, intuition, and language” (p. 202). In this process, as Bandura highlighted in 1993, parents can play a crucial role, contributing to stimulating and supporting children's self-regulatory and cognitive development (Bandura, 1993). Indeed, parents who undertake verbal interactions and structure activities and games with their children allow them to live the rich linguistic communication and contexts of shared attention and meanings. These experiences encourage children's active exploration and engagement with their environments, improving children's academic skill and their success in school (Grossmann et al., 2002). Paternal involvement has been also situated within Bronfenbrenner's bioecological theory (1979) which highlighted that both proximal (e.g., paternal involvement) and distal (e.g., socioeconomic status, race/ethnicity, school context) factors must be considered to fully understand paternal influences on children's cognitive skills development. In line with these findings, various meta-analyses and reviews investigated the link between parental involvement during early and middle childhood and student achievement (e.g., Fan and Chen, 2001; Jeynes, 2007) and proximal and distal factors influence levels of fathers' involvement in literacy activities (e.g., Varghese and Wachen's, 2016). Even though various proximal and distal factors (e.g., fathers' education, income level, residencial status, relationship with the child's mother) are indirectly associated with children's language development and literacy activities (Varghese and Wachen, 2016), research confirmed a direct, positive and relatively strong association between paternal involvement and children's cognitive skills development (Fan and Chen, 2001; Jeynes, 2007). For instance, Varghese and Wachen (2016) found that fathers made unique and direct contributions to their children's literacy outcomes through their engagement in reading and writing activities and the use of complex language and responsive parenting behaviors.

However, notwithstanding the extensive literature on the topic, there is a great difficulty in finding an agreed definition of the construct of parental involvement, as it is conceptualized in a variety of ways (Harris and Goodall, 2007). For example, some authors distinguish between school- and home-based involvement: the first refers to the communications between parents and school personnel and to the parental engagement in activities children must perform at school (Grolnick and Slowiaczek, 1994; e.g., Hill, 2001), while the second (home-based) includes all the school activities that both mothers and fathers perform with their children at home (Hill, 2001). Other researchers investigated a further form of parental involvement not directly related to school (e.g., playing sports and other games, going to the cinema or a museum) that can influence school achievement (Nord et al., 1997).

Furthermore, while the first studies on the influence of parenting on children's outcomes focused mostly on mother–child dyadic interaction, nowadays, given the increasing number of working mothers and the changing social climate regarding male and female gender roles, the assumption that mothers were the gatekeepers and that father–child relationships had little impact on children's development is diminished. Consequently, various current studies are focused on the construct of coparenting as “the ways that parents work together in their roles as parents” (Feinberg, 2003, p. 1499) and to explore the determinants of fathers' involvement with their children's education (Lamb et al., 1985; Volling and Belsky, 1991). One of the most widespread and accredited models of the construct of father involvement was developed by Lamb et al. (1985). It includes: (a) engagement, the amount of time fathers spends in direct contact with their children (e.g., verbal stimulation, caregiving, and physical play); (b) accessibility, fathers' attendance and availability; and (c) responsibility, the ability to plan activities specifically adapted to the age and needs of the child (Lamb et al., 1987). Although this model allowed an increase in the understanding of the ways in which fathers are involved in their children's lives, it showed various gaps (Fagan et al., 2014). First, given the lack of systematic development regarding the different heuristic categories, there are only a few validated and reliable tools. Second, of the three types of involvement suggested by Lamb et al. (1985), researches have generally focused more on paternal engagement than on accessibility and responsibility, resulting in an overfocus on the amount of time fathers spend with their children (Pleck, 2012). As a consequence, Pleck (2010) widens the definition of “engagement,” identifying two distinctive elements: (a) positive engagement activities, the amount of time fathers and children are involved in interactive activities, such as reading books and teaching, that may promote learning and development; and (b) the dimensions of parenting, how fathers engage with their children, including the dimensions of Baumrind's (1967) parenting style model (responsiveness and demandingness). This contribution showed that the originally engagement construct may be not well-defined for studying the effects of fathers' parenting on children. However, many gaps related to the original tripartite model remained (Fagan et al., 2014). One of these gaps is that positive engagement was still largely focused on the amount of time fathers are involved with their children; another is related to the lack of a valid theory that guided the selection of the different dimensions of positive father engagement. In addition, the multidimensional construct has not been integrated into a comprehensive conceptual framework that could enable an understanding of what fathering is, why fathers parent in the manner they do, and how paternal actions directly and indirectly help determine children's development (Palkovitz, 2002; Pleck, 2007, 2010). To overcome this research gap, Cabrera and Volling (2019) proposed a “Developmental Ecological Systems Framework for understanding fathering and children's development.” This approach considers fathers as part of dynamic systems characterized by interconnected relationships between and among caregivers and children and explains how these relationships evolve and change through time and social and contextual factors (Cabrera et al., 2014, 2018). Previously, based on this ecological approach, Cabrera et al. (2014) developed a model to acknowledge the flow and actions affecting the quality (and quantity) of fathering on child functioning. According to this model, the father–child relationship results are influenced by the personality, personal characteristics, and behaviors of the father and other family members (e.g., mother, child, siblings) and the overall context of the family system by the family relationship (e.g., coparenting), the different family households, socioeconomic statuses (SES), and cultures, and the child's and family's development (Volling and Belsky, 1991; Cabrera et al., 2014; Cabrera and Volling, 2019). The strengths of this model are the inclusion of interpersonal and contextual variables in determining the level of father involvement (Volling and Belsky, 1991) and, also, the consideration of the transactional and reciprocal relationships between fathers and children (Sameroff, 2010). In line with complexity put in light by this model, the literature about father involvement is still very heterogeneous. As a consequence, in order to design innovative and rigorous research, it becomes necessary to increase knowledge about what has been previously investigated, particularly in relation to the following issues: how should father involvement, especially in early and middle childhood, be conceptualized and measured? What is the relationship between the different components of father involvement and children's cognitive skills? Which are the main focuses investigated? Are non-residential, step-, low-income, and minority fathers included in the research samples? How is it possible to increase father involvement in children's education?

Although some attempts of synthesis have already been produced, many are prior to 2010 (Saracho, 2007a,b; Downer et al., 2008), while the more recent are focused on specific themes, such as educational achievement (Lipscomb, 2011; Jeynes, 2015; Kim and Hill, 2015), and none were published after 2016. Consequently, the previously presented questions call for a systematic examination of the existing research concerning the association between father involvement and children's cognitive skills during early and middle childhood, to widen our knowledge about the definition and measurement of father involvement, the pathways of its influence on children's cognitive skills development, and the differences from/similarities to mother involvement.

Aims

The aims of the present review are to systematically examine the literature about father involvement in the development of children's cognitive skills and, in particular, to analyze: (1) how the number of researches and the design/methods developed across years; (2) if non-residential, step-, low-income, and minority fathers are included in the research samples; (3) which are the main focuses of the examined literature; (4) which operational definitions of the construct and measures were used to assess father involvement and children's cognitive skills.

Methods

Data Source and Search Strategy

We followed the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher et al., 2009). Articles were searched through PubMed and EBSCO (PsycInfo, PsycArticles, Education Source, Social Sciences Abstract, Family Studies Abstracts, Gender Studies Database, and CINAHL Complete).

We examined titles and abstracts to find eligible studies published in English, from the first publication in 1964 to November 2018. The keywords used for the query were “Father*” and “learning” or “cognitive skills” and “children.” Of the 2,215 papers resulting from this first search, after duplicates removal, 777 met the criteria of being published in English in peer-reviewed journals. We selected, by titles and abstracts, 178 papers. As a second step, we selected the papers to be included in the data synthesis by reading the full-text. A total of 40 papers was finally retained (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram of study selction.

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

The inclusion criteria for the papers were: (a) the presence of the father involvement construct and a specific reference to cognitive skills; (b) age of children not above thirteen; (c) written in English; and (d) published in academic journals. Both qualitative and quantitative articles were selected as well as reviews, with the aim of looking at the different approaches and methodologies used in this field.

The exclusion criteria were: (a) clinical samples to avoid bias in focusing more on father involvement as related to the outcomes associated with disease (e.g., ADHD, autism); (b) studies that were not specifically focused on the father involvement construct; (c) adolescent samples (2 studies with 10-year-old children were excluded because they were part of samples aged between 10 and 16 years); and (d) studies focused on other children's outcomes (e.g., physical, diet). Two independent reviewers selected the articles by title and abstract. Afterwards, they compared the results to determine a common list of selected papers; each choice that was different between the two reviewers was discussed in order to find an agreement.

Strategy for Data Synthesis

The selected articles were independently entered by each reviewer into two classification tables: one dedicated to analyzing the reviews and meta-analyses, and the other to analyzing the qualitative, quantitative, and mixed-methods studies. The papers that, when reading the full text, did not match our selection criteria were further excluded. Each reviewer independently completed the tables, and discussed each discrepancy in the selection in order to find an agreement.

We subsequentely developed a dataset of the selected papers and conducted a thematic analysis (TA) with the purpose of defining the main focuses of the reviewed articles, using a semantic approach, according to the guidelines provided by Braun and Clarke (2006). The reviewers discussed the TA results as well.

To avoid a thematic overlap, each article was linked to a single topic, in particular, the main focus of each study.

Results

Table 1 shows the frequency of the different types of papers. Quantitative methods were mostly used, whereas there were few qualitative and only one utilized mixed methods.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Type of document.

Table 2 (meta-analyses and reviews) and Table 3 (empirical studies, case reports, and theoretical articles) describe the researches' primary characteristics—authors and year, study design, objective, content focus, determinants, and key findings—in relation to general information and our review question. Other features of the reviewed literature will be discussed in detail in the following four paragraphs: growing interest with the role of father involvement in children's education, and the samples' sociodemographic characteristics, main focuses, and measurement methods.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reviewes and meta-analysies included in the study.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Studies included in the meta-analysis listed by author, year of study, type of document, objects, and other.

Growing Interest in the Role of Father Involvement in Children's Education

This paragraph's main objective is to expand on the state of the literature about father involvement in children's cognitive skills development. One of the more widespread problems was that, in the literature on caregiving and children, fathers' parenting has been studied less than mothers' (Downer et al., 2008).

However, in the 70s and 80s the scholarly interest in fatherhood grew (Lamb, 2004) and, during the 90s, there was an increase in the researches on various fatherhood aspects, developing a large and heterogeneous body of studies that emphasized the unique role of fathers in children's development. For example, Marsiglio et al. (2000), in their review about fatherhood, examined the relationships between the dimensions of the father–child relationship (e.g., time spent with children, emotional support, everyday encouragement, and overseeing children's behaviors) and children outcomes. The increasing trend of the number of articles specifically focused on the construct of “father involvement” in children's education is shown in Figure 2. In the next paragraphs, we will focus on the main sociodemographic characteristics and measurement methods used in this growing literature.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Published articles (included in this review) across years.

Samples' Demographic Characteristics

Regarding age, father involvement research was mostly conducted on groups of children aged 0–6 (27.5%), preschoolers (3–5) (22.5%), and infants/toddlers (0–3) (20%); mixed age (7.5%) and middle childhood samples (15%) were less frequent. However, reviews and meta-analyses focused on a wider age range, from early years to 20 years old.

Concerning fathers' characteristics, it is possible to see a predominance of biological and residential fathers: 35% of the reviewed studies collected samples of only biological fathers and more than 35% of only residential fathers. This trend may reflect that this sample type is easier to recruit. Indeed, only two studies used samples mostly comprised of non-residential fathers (Black et al., 1999; Baker, 2018). In the first quantitative study Baker (2018) investigated, in a sample of 74% non-resident fathers, father–school involvement was a predictor of improving academic achievement and social-emotional skills.

The results showed that, although mothers were always more engaged in their children's education, father–school involvement was positively associated with children's math and reading skills and with teacher-related approaches to learning during early childhood. In the second study with a sample composed of non-residential biological and non-biological African American fathers (who were involved at least monthly). Black et al. (1999) assessed the relationship between paternal roles (e.g., nurturance, emotional and economic support) and children's well-being.

The results showed that children whose fathers are satisfied with their parenting and economically supportive of their families have better language competences and cognitive skills, proving the unique fathers' contributions.

Nevertheless, regarding the families' SESs and ethnicities, we found a great heterogeneity across all the studies. Between the studies that distinguished minority, non-minority, and mixed ethnicity samples, 12.5% of the selected articles recruited only minority fathers, 30% exclusively focused on non-minority men, and 40% had a mixed ethnicity sample. More specifically, most of the studies that focused on minority or mixed ethnicity included African American (Black et al., 1999; Cabrera et al., 2011) and Hispanic fathers (Ortiz, 2000; Saracho, 2007a, 2008).

Despite the well-established relevance of household income for children's academic success, the research and knowledge about father involvement and children's cognitive skills in low-income families were lacking. Among the reviewed articles that studied the SESs of the families with children in early and middle childhood, 20% centered on low-income fathers while 42% included a mixed sample with family income ranging from low to high.

Main Research Focuses

As shown in Figure 3, half the empirical studies and all the reviews and meta-analyses focused on the relationship between father involvement and children's outcomes; a good number also focused on the effectiveness of interventions to increase fathers' engagement with their children's education and on the comparison between mother and father involvement, while a few articles examined the determinants, and only one study addressed the issue of assessment. In the following subparagraphs, we will synthetize the literature for each research focus, except the assessment, which we will discuss in the next paragraph.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Main researches focuses.

Extent of the Association Between Father Involvement and Children's Cognitive Skills

The findings of the reviewed articles (including reviews and meta-analyses), focusing in particular on the association between father involvement and children's cognitive skills, showed a positive and statistically significant association during early childhood (Flouri and Buchanan, 2004; McBride et al., 2005, 2009; Roopnarine et al., 2006; Saracho, 2007b; Downer et al., 2008; Coley et al., 2011; Fagan and Lee, 2012; McWayne et al., 2013; Duursma, 2014; Jeynes, 2015; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2017, 2018) beyond mothers' parenting (Roopnarine et al., 2006). More specifically, Fagan and Lee (2012) argued that this relationship was significantly strengthened for children living in single-mother households, whereas Coley et al. (2011) found that family characteristics were trifling.

However, it is important to underline that several studies highlighted that this pattern generally remains significant across ethnicity and SES (Downer et al., 2008; Jeynes, 2015; Baker, 2017).

For example, Cabrera et al. (2011) examined father engagement—verbal stimulation, caregiving, and physical play—across race, monitoring marital status, parental conflict, parental education, and depressive symptoms. The findings showed that while Caucasian fathers demonstrate a lower involvement in care and physical play than African American and Latino fathers, there were no differences in verbal stimulation across ethnicity. Furthermore, fathers' education (i.e., college level) was linked with more verbal activities, whereas couple conflict was associated with less engagement in care and physical activities. In conclusion, the authors argued that, although the level of involvement differed across ethnicity, the general model did not change.

Conversely, some scholars found that the relationship between father involvement and children's cognitive skills diverges across children's and parents' gender (Tan and Goldberg, 2009; Eng et al., 2014), race, and SES (McBride et al., 2013).

For example, Baker et al. (2018) found that, although poverty negatively influences more fathers' than mothers' parenting, fathers' engagement was a stronger moderator between poverty and children's cognitive skills than mothers' involvement.

Comparison Between Maternal and Paternal Involvement

In addition to the studies that specifically focused on the relationship between father involvement and children's outcomes, another great part of research aimed at comparing maternal and paternal involvement in children's education. Most studies on this topic highlighted that, although the mean level of mothers' involvement was higher than fathers' (Duursma, 2014; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2018), fathers' involvement and children's academic skills had a positive association.

For example, Keown and Palmer (2014) collected a sample of 94 two-parent families, with the aim of comparing father and mother involvement with their young son. The findings revealed that, although the mothers were more available to their sons on workdays, and fathers were more involved in activities with their children on weekend days, both mother– and father–child conversations were rich resources for children during their school ages.

Another study, specifically focused on the comparison between maternal and paternal involvement, was conducted by Foster et al. (2016). The authors investigated the home learning environment (HLE) during early childhood and how mothers' and fathers' parenting practices predict children's academic outcomes. The findings showed that mothers provided HLE activities more frequently than fathers although, in families in which mothers had at most a high school diploma, fathers' contributions were a significant predicator of children's early academic skills. However, other studies indicate that also fathers' education levels were often associated with mother engagement and that parental education indirectly influences children's language development through multiple pathways (Pancsofar et al., 2010).

Furthermore, Duursma (2014) examined the association between book-reading frequency of low-income fathers and mothers and children's cognitive and literacy skills and found that, although mothers read to their children more frequently than fathers, approximately 55% of fathers reported that they read to their children weekly. Moreover, this study highlighted that fathers' book reading significantly predicted children's language competences, book knowledge, and cognitive skills.

In line with mentioned studies, Giallo et al. (2013) found quite a few differences between mothers' and fathers' involvement, despite both of them were influenced by parenatal self-efficacy (PSE), considering it one of the determinants of father involvement.

Determinants of Father Involvement

Another group of articles specifically focused on the determinants of father involvement, examining both contextual, and personal factors.

Ball (2009), in a qualitative study about the involvement of 80 Canadian Indigenous fathers, introduced a conceptual model that identified six key ecological and psychological factors of fathers' involvement in circles of care for children. The six found factors are: (a) individual health, (b) learning fathering (direct father–child interaction, role models, and direct instructions), (c) socioeconomic inclusion, (d) social support for positive involvement, (e) legislative and policy support for involvement, and (f) cultural continuity.

In the same direction, Bradley and Corwyn (2000) investigated the influence of factors related to context (e.g., income), child characteristics (e.g., temperament), marital quality, and mother's and father's occupation on paternal socioemotional investment toward their children. The results highlighted that paternal investment is a multidetermined construct: it was not possible to identify one single factor that played a predominant role.

In recent years, Nordhal et al. (2016) suggested a conceptual model based on the “social interaction learning model” (SIL; Patterson, 1982), regarding the protective and risk factors of father involvement. Given that the interactions between children and parents can be considered a two-way process (Patterson, 1982; Patterson and Fisher, 2002). Nordhal et al. (2016) distinguished protective factors for fathers, including mental functioning and contextual and personal resources, and protective factors for children, including typical development, male gender, and easy behavior. While the negative factors related to fathers were mental dysfunction and personal and contextual strains, and the negative factors for children were development difficulties, female gender, and difficult behaviors. According to the findings, given that protective and negative factors were positively associated to fathers' positive involvement and to negative reinforcement, respectively, these last two dimensions could be considered different parenting components.

Another study that focused on the determinants of father involvement in Cambodian American families showed that parents' social networks, academic ambitions, trust, gender attitudes, and fatalistic convictions can be considered predictors of parental involvement (Eng et al., 2014). As a consequence, the authors highlighted the need for educators who work with Cambodian American parents to consider mothers' and fathers' beliefs systems and to identify personal and contextual resources to increase parents' involvement in children's education.

To sum up, the examined studies make reference to the following factors as determinants of father involvement: fathers' and mothers' education, families' income level, residencial status, race-ethnicity, characteristics of fathers and children, mother-father relation and social support for positive involvement. Indeed, although the studies in this field are characterized by a great heterogeneity, there is a general agreement in the literature about the necessity to consider how personal (e.g., mental health, child temperament, personality), interpersonal (e.g., marital quality, coparenting), and contextual factors (e.g., social support, culture) influence one another on their impact on father involvement.

Programs to Increase Father Involvement

As seen, although many studies highlighted that mothers were engaged in their children's education more frequently than fathers (Duursma, 2014; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2018), at the same time, there is strong evidence that fathers can play a unique role in children's cognitive skills development (Flouri and Buchanan, 2004; McBride et al., 2005, 2009; Roopnarine et al., 2006; Saracho, 2007b; Downer et al., 2008; Fagan and Lee, 2012; McWayne et al., 2013; Duursma, 2014; Jeynes, 2015; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2017, 2018).

Consequently, in recent years, there was an increase in studies focused on the efficacy of father involvement interventions. Indeed, among the 40 selected articles, the focus most investigated, after the relationship between father involvement and children's outcomes, was the benefits and strategies that can improve father involvement in children's education.

For example, Lipscomb (2011) reviewed various intervention types and found that different programs exist, each designed for specific population groups (programs related to incarcerated fathers, minority ethnic groups, and fathers with low income or literacy skills). The following are some examples: The My Baby's Father (MBF) Involvement Model; Dads at School; the Alliance of Concerned Men (Abridging); 100 Black Men; Long Distance Dads; and the Incarcerated Fathers Program. The study of Saracho (2008) can be considered an example of research on the evaluation of an intervention specifically related to fathers with low literacy. The results demonstrated that, when fathers improve literacy strategies, this can help and support their children's literacy skills development.

There are other important contributions in the same direction. Baskwill (2008) identified three main benefits after participating in the Picture It, Dads! (PID) literacy initiative: (1) fathers improved their literacy skills; (2) participants increased their knowledge about the relevance of father involvement in children's well-being; and (3) men acquired a wide range of dad-friendly strategies and increased their ability to engage with their children in learning and intellectual activities.

Chawla-Duggan (2006) identified two ways in which father development workers (FDWs) can help improve men's involvement with their children's education: (a) by encouraging fathers within the group to increase their self-confidence and (b) by helping fathers use both indirect and direct learning approaches to improve their children's intellectual abilities. Potter et al. (2012) examined the findings of fathers' participation in the Father's Transition Project (FTP), which aims to increase the involvement of fathers who live in deprivation areas in children's education during their transition to school. The more successful strategies in engaging fathers were: the focus on strengths rather than weakness, cooperation within the group, constant follow-up, the use of male activities, and mothers' engagement. Moreover, the main program participation benefits can be identified as follows: closer relationships with their children and a higher level of father involvement in playing and in learning activities.

Definition and Measurement of Father Involvement and Children's Cognitive Skills

Definition and Measurement of Children's Cognitive Skills

Most of the reviewed studies examined children's academic/student achievement, cognitive skills, and literacy skills. Regarding the studies that focused on cognitive skills, on one hand, some that assessed memory, vocabulary, problem-solving, enumeration, and the competence to form generalizations and classifications used the Bayley Short Form (BSF) or the Bayley Mental Development Index (MDI)1. On the other hand, other studies about cognitive skills investigated particular math and reading skills with the Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery (WJ): this measure is used in the studies that focused on academic achievement as well. Moreover, the studies that focused on father involvement and children's literacy skills assessed the relationship between the amount of time fathers spend reading books to their children and developing reading skills (Ortiz, 2000; Saracho, 2008). For instance, in his review, Saracho (2007a) found that fathers support and increase their children literacy skills by reading books, involving children in book discussions, recording which books have already been read, and stimulating children to enhance their reading (Saracho, 2007b).

Definition and Measurement of Father Involvement

To address the way father involvement is studied in the different researches, it is necessary to deepen the construct's characteristics: Table 4 shows that there are many types of father involvement and, for each, there is no single definition or component. Father involvement is a multidimensional construct that has been conceptualized and measured in various ways. For example, 40% of the articles focused on “father involvement” in general, defining and measuring the construct differently, without any other specification (e.g., related to school, home, etc.). Most of the studies used self-reports (40%), and only a few utilized interviews and observations (30%) or mothers' reports (10%); however, each self-report focused on different aspects of father involvement. For instance, Baker (2017) used a self-report questionnaire to investigate the frequency (1 = “never” to 5 = “always”) of fathers' participation in warm interactions and in three home learning activities with their children. Furthermore, the kind of control/discipline that fathers used with their children was assessed with a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 = “not at all like me” and 5 = “exactly like me.”

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Type of construct.

In another study, Tan and Goldberg (2009) investigated parental school involvement using a 26-item scale adapted from Tan and Goldberg (2009) About Parental Involvement.

It aimed to assess how frequently mothers and fathers are engaged in four different kinds of school/educational-related activities: direct school involvement, homework involvement, interpersonal involvement, and extracurricular activity involvement.

Each item was evaluated using a 5-point scale, where 1 = “never” and 5 = “always.”

These two studies are useful for understanding the great difference in the way in which the construct of father involvement is defined and measured.

Discussion

Growing Interest in the Role of Father Involvement in Children's Education

The first aim was related to the progression of the amount and nature of the researches on this theme over the years. The findings suggest that, although each research used a different operational definition of the father involvement construct, in recent years there has been a wide and constant interest increase about this issue. Moreover, most of the examined articles were empirical studies and, in particular, a sizable number had a quantitative design.

Fathers' Living Conditions

Regarding our second aim, the analysis of fathers' research samples revealed that most included biological and residential fathers: this trend may reflect that this sample type is easier to recruit than one comprised of non-residential and non-biological fathers.

Regarding the SES and the ethnicity of the families, the data highlighted how, in comparison with past researches, in recent years the literature on father involvement is starting to consider cultural differences and different educational and economic levels.

Indeed, while the most of past researches collected samples of Caucasian and middle-class men, failing to grasp the families' diversity, more and more articles, recently, include minority populations or diversified samples both in terms of the cultural level and the SES of the family (Black et al., 1999; Ortiz, 2000; Saracho, 2007a, 2008; Cabrera et al., 2011; Sun et al., 2018).

Main Research Focuses

The third objective was to analyze the main focuses of the examined literature. The findings revealed that the main focus is the impact of father involvement on children's cognitive skills. The studies highlighted a positive association between father involvement and children's cognitive skills2 in early and middle childhood (Flouri and Buchanan, 2004; McBride et al., 2005, 2009; Roopnarine et al., 2006; Saracho, 2007b; Downer et al., 2008; Fagan and Lee, 2012; McWayne et al., 2013; Duursma, 2014; Jeynes, 2015; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2017, 2018). Moreover, it is important to underline that several studies highlighted that this positive association between father involvement and children's cognitive skills remains significant across ethnicity and SES (Downer et al., 2008; Jeynes, 2015; Baker, 2017). Instead, regarding the family household, some authors argued that this association is lower for children living in two-parent families than for children living with a single mother (Fagan and Lee, 2012), whereas others supported the strength of this association, regardless of the family characteristics (Coley et al., 2011). In line with these findings, Volling and Belsky (1991), in their study of multiple determinants of father involvement in dual- and single-earner families, found that even the fathers' personality characteristics had a significant impact on their responsibility for child care in single-earner but not in dual-earner families; contextual factors (e.g., marital quality and work) were potential influents in both dual and single families.

The study of father involvement in different families' households allowed a wider understanding of the direct and indirect pathways of the fathers' influence on their children's development. Regarding the direct patterns of influence, many studies found that father–school involvement was positively and directly associated with children's reading, math, and approach to learning (Baker, 2018). Moreover, fathers' SES (including education level and income) is uniquely and directly associated with children's cognitive skills (Cabrera et al., 2007; Malin et al., 2012). Finally, depending on the level of involvement, fathers can positively affect children's development even when they do not live with them (Cabrera et al., 2000) while, according to the well-known contribution of Amato and Gilbreth (1999) about non-residential fathers, the complete absence of a father is associated with less success in school and impaired cognitive function. Moreover, with respect to indirect effects, fathers impact their children through their financial responsibilities by influencing the quality of children's home experiences (Cabrera et al., 2009; Kolak and Volling, 2013). Another indirect effect may be largely attributable to harmonious family contexts: there is empirical support for the hypothesis that a positive marital quality is associated with positive parent–child relationships and child adjustment (Gable et al., 1994), whereas marital conflict is associated with maladjustment (Emery, 1994; Cummings et al., 2004).

However, although these findings highlight the various pathways of fathers' involvement in influencing their children's development, they do not explain how fathers and mothers are similarly and differently involved in their children's development, and how this could similarly or differentially affect child outcomes. On one hand, some authors, in stressing the differences between parents, noted that fathers give a unique contribution to their children's development—one that is different from the mothers'. Most of the articles highlighted that, although mothers showed higher levels of involvement compared to fathers (Duursma, 2014; Kim and Hill, 2015; Baker, 2018), there is great evidence that fathers' involvement had a positive association with their children's academic skills, demonstrating a unique influence provided by the paternal contribution. In line with these results, several researches showed that fathers are more likely than mothers to engage their children, especially sons, in rough-and-tumble play (Hossain and Roopnarine, 1994; Panksepp et al., 2003; Paquette et al., 2003), to encourage them in risk taking (Hagan and Kuebli, 2007) and dealing with scary experiences (Sandseter and Kennair, 2011).

On the other hand, other researches have focused on the similarities between mothers and fathers and found that children may benefit from parental support regardless of which parent provides it, as long as it is frequent and of high quality (Ryan et al., 2006; Cabrera et al., 2007). To conclude, given the evidence of both the similarities and differences in father– and mother–child relationships, Cabrera et al. (2014) suggested considering fathers and mothers as a complements to each other, where each person's behavior can help strengthen or weaken the bond between them. Consequently, sometimes fathers will enact roles played by mothers, and vice versa, in response to environmental conditions that require adaptation (e.g., both parents working, single-parent fathers). Given these findings, a model that attempts to capture the complexities of father involvement must consider contextual and individual factors that may move fathers to being more similar to or different from mothers. For this reason, the examined articles that focused on the determinants of father involvement tried to identify which possible risk and protective factors are related to fathering.

Although these studies are characterized by a great heterogeneity, there is a general agreement in the literature about the necessity to consider how personal (e.g., mental health, child temperament, personality), interpersonal (e.g., marital quality, coparenting), and contextual factors (e.g., social support, culture) influence one another on their impact on father engagement. A current model that tried to compile these factors, overcoming linear and static approaches considering the transactional and reciprocal nature of the father–child relationship, is the Ecology of Father–Child Relationships: An Expanded Model, developed by Cabrera et al. (2014). It considers fathers as part of dynamic systems characterized by interconnected relationships between and among caregivers and children and explains how these relationships evolve and change through time and social and contextual factors. This model also considers the personal, interpersonal, and contextual variables in determining the level of father involvement (Volling and Belsky, 1991) and the transactional and reciprocal nature of the relationship between fathers and children (Sameroff, 2010).

As a consequence, if future studies enable the achievement of a shared knowledge and understanding of such factors, then researchers and professionals will be able to enhance father involvement through specific programs based on such protective and risk factors. Indeed, a sizable number of articles focused on the effectiveness of programs to improve paternal engagement. It is possible to discriminate two kinds of programs: one aimed toward samples with specific characteristics (e.g., Dads at School, 100 Black Men, Long Distance Dads, Incarcerated Fathers Program, programs for fathers with low-literacy skills); the other concerns programs that can be applied to a more generic sample (e.g., Father-to-Father Mentoring Program, Father in Training (FIT): Empowering Men to Become Better Fathers).

In the end, it is possible to summarize that, although the literature about father involvement is characterized by a great heterogeneity, there is a general agreement about the necessity to consider how personal (e.g., mental health, child temperament, father's personality, father and mother level of education,), interpersonal (e.g., marital quality, coparenting), and contextual factors (e.g., social support, culture) influence one another on their impact on father involvement.

Definition and Measurement of Father Involvement and Children's Cognitive Skills

The last aim of the present review concerns the operational definition of the construct and the measurement instruments used to assess father involvement and their children's cognitive skills. Regarding the assessment of cognitive skills, the literature is quite heterogeneous: on one hand, some studies examined cognitive skills by evaluating problem-solving, memory, math ability, vocabulary, and competence to create generalization and categorization (Fagan and Lee, 2012); on the other hand, other studies investigated, in particular, math and reading skills as well as the studies focused on “academic/student achievement” (Baker, 2017, 2018). For this reason, it is clear that the most investigated children's outcomes are math and reading skills, also referred to as “cognitive skills” and “academic achievement.” As for father involvement, the selected researches used different types of constructs and tools to understand the father–child relationship, focusing on some aspects of father involvement and neglecting others.

Most of the selected researches used quantitative methods, in particular self-report questionnaires, usually developed or specifically adapted to assess mother–child relationships. In contrast, the use of interviews, observations, or diaries in qualitative studies to explore father–child relationships allow the assessment of more specific aspects. For instance, Keown and Palmer (2014), in their qualitative study, used semistructured interviews, which evaluated both the frequency and nature of parents' involvement with their children. Such heterogeneity related to the measuring instruments reduces the possibilities of comparing the results of each study: this remains one of the main limits in most empirical studies. Moreover, the use of only self-reported data does not make it possible to understand how the ecology of children's lives changes, how fathers interact with their children, how they engage in different activities, and what circumstances bring fathers into and remove them from their children's lives (Cabrera and Volling, 2019).

In conclusion, it could be argued that parental involvement assessment requires overcoming the traditional developmental models focused on dyadic interactions, usually mother–child, by using a broader multidimensional perspective, and thus a comprehensive methodological approach based on a developmental ecological system framework (Cabrera and Volling, 2019), to evaluate the father–child relationship in their family system, including a multidimensional, multi-informant assessment.

Limitations

The main limitation across studies is related to generalizability. This aspect could reflect the recruitment process and the configuration of the samples: confined to residential, American and middle-class fathers. For example, in their study Fagan and Lee (2012) identified a limitation in the low response rate of non-resident fathers. Furthermore, given the increase of non-residential fathers in the general population, their involvement in study designs becomes more necessary.

The second limitation is related to measurement. Indeed, the ways in which father involvement is measured are criticized for more than one reason: (a) the simplicity of the construct's measurement (e.g., assessing the extent rather than the quality of interaction); (b) the wide variability of instruments; (c) instruments validation testing of the mothers; (d) the use of mothers' reports about fathers' involvement; and (e) the overuse of self-report questionnaires. In particular, the self-report measure of father involvement is not considered the best way to assess paternal engagement because it may reflect fathers' aspirations to look better than they might be (because of a social desirability bias), and they cannot grasp the dynamic and transactional nature of father–child interactions, which is a complex phenomenon that could be investigated across several dimensions (Cabrera et al., 2018; Cabrera and Volling, 2019). Thus, the integration of self-reported data with qualitative tools (e.g., observations) in father–child interactions should be preferred.

Further developments in evaluation procedures are needed for broader comprehension of fathers' involvement and their impact on their children's well-being: in fact this limitation on the source of information (e.g., parent report) could have and effect on the estimation of children's enjoyment of school and cognitive skills. The use of the same data source for both the independent and dependent variables may lead to overestimate the correlation between the variables, which, in turn, compromise the possibility of making causal inferences starting from parental involvement to understand the children's outcomes. For this reason, researches that utilize a multi-informant approach can be more informative compared to studies based on a single data source.

We must also take into consideration that, to avoid bias, we excluded the articles involving clinical samples, which would need to be addressed with a specific literature review.

A further limitation is about the design of the reviewed studies, since the majority of them used a cross-sectional design, which limits the inferences of causality.

Finally, in many studies, the reported interaction effects and sample sizes were quite small. However, although the great heterogeneity of the examined studies represented a limiting condition also for conducting the present review, it was possible to outline many important points in relation to the examined literature and to draw on future research directions.

Implication for Future Researches

Given the previously discussed limits, the first implication for future researches is related to the need to expand the research samples, including more ethnic groups and different geographical contexts (e.g., suburban, urban, or rural areas)and low-income, non-resident, social, and step-fathers in the studies that are addressed to deepen the knowledge about father involvement. This is necessary because it could enhance both the internal and external validity of the existing studies about father involvement in the literature. Indeed, parenting occurs in a social context (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and, for this reason, fathering patterns may vary by race or social context.

Another implication for future researches is related to the necessity of extending and deepening the conceptualizations of father involvement in children's cognitive skills development, not focusing exclusively on fathers engagement. Currently, there is vast evidence in the literature that father involvement is a multidimensional construct influenced by personality, family history, child characteristics, marital quality, and the father's sociocultural context (Volling and Belsky, 1991). For this reason, the use of one single source of information and one method of measurement of father involvement is reductive, and there is a necessity for studies to incorporate multiple informants and methods (e.g., observations, surveys carried out by both fathers and mothers, diaries) in the assessment of paternal involvement (Cabrera et al., 2018; Cabrera and Volling, 2019). Furthermore, future research should create and validate measures of parenting practices for both low-income and minority families and use longitudinal designs to better understand the association between father involvement and children's outcomes.

In conclusion, the consideration of the above indications in future research designs could make it possible to increase the knowledge about mothers' and fathers' involvement in their children's cognitive skills development.

Author Contributions

LR, GG, and AC took overall responsibility for the creation of the frame used in this review and the selection of the papers. TT, LC, EG, and PB searched for the articles discussed in the review. LR and AC supervised the entire work. All authors were involved in the discussion, the write and the revision of the manuscript and they gave the final approval of the version to be published.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^The MDI is the most used measure to assess toddlers' cognitive development, in which high scores reveal high levels of cognitive development.

2. ^Further ahead, we will discuss the construct of the operational definition of “cognitive skills” which, in some articles, is also referred to as academic/student achievement.

References

Amato, P. R., and Gilbreth, J. G. (1999). Nonresident fathers and children's well-being: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 61, 557–573. doi: 10.2307/353560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, S., Aller, T. B., Piercy, K. W., and Roggman, L. A. (2015). ‘Helping us find our own selves': exploring father-role construction and early childhood programme engagement. Early Child Dev. Care. 185, 360–376. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2014.924112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. E. (2017). Father-son relationships in ethnically diverse families: links to boys' cognitive and social emotional development in preschool. J. Child Fam. Stud. 26, 2335–2345. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0743-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. E. (2018). When daddy comes to school: father–school involvement and children's academic and social–emotional skills. Early Child Dev. Care. 188, 208–219. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2016.1211118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, C. E., Kainz, K. L., and Reynolds, E. R. (2018). Family poverty, family processes and children's preschool achievement: understanding the unique role of fathers. J. Child Fam. Stud. 27, 1242–1251. doi: 10.1007/s10826-017-0947-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ball, J. (2009). Fathering in the shadows: indigenous fathers and Canada's colonial legacies. Ann. Am. Acad. Pol. Soc. Sci. 624, 29–48. doi: 10.1177/0002716209334181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1993). Perceived self-efficacy in cognitive development and functioning. Educ. Psychol. 28, 117–148. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep2802_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baskwill, J. (2008). Picture it, Dads! Can. Child. 33, 26–31.

Google Scholar

Baumrind, D. (1967). Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 75, 43–88.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scale for Infant Development, (BSID-II), 2nd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Black, M. M., Dubowitz, H., and Starr, R. H. Jr. (1999). African American fathers in low income, urban families: development, behavior, and home environment of their three-year-old children. Child Dev. 70, 967–978. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2000). Fathers' socioemotional investment in their children. J. Men Stud. 8, 333–347. doi: 10.3149/jms.0803.333

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, V., and Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual. Res. Psychol. 3, 77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Cabrera, N., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bradley, R. H., Hofferth, S., and Lamb, M. E. (2000). Fatherhood in the twenty-first century. Child Dev. 71, 127–136. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Fitzgerald, H. E., Bradley, R. H., and Roggman, L. (2014). The ecology of father–child relationships: an expanded model. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 6, 336–354. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Hofferth, S. L., and Chae, S. (2011). Patterns and predictors of father–infant engagement across race/ethnic groups. Early Child. Res. Q. 26, 365–375. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2011.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J., Mitchell, S., and West, J. (2009). Mexican American mothers and fathers' prenatal attitudes and father prenatal involvement: links to mother–infant interaction and father engagement. Sex Roles 60, 510–526. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9576-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers' influence on their children's cognitive and emotional development: from toddlers to pre-k. Appl. Dev. Sci. 11, 208–213. doi: 10.1080/10888690701762100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., and Volling, B. L. (2019). Moving research on fathering and children's development forward: priorities and recommendations for the future. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 84, 107–117. doi: 10.1002/mono.12404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabrera, N. J., Volling, B. L., and Barr, R. (2018). Fathers are parents, too! Widening the lens on parenting for children's development. Child Dev. Perspect. 12, 152–157. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chawla-Duggan, R. (2006). Exploring the role of father development workers in supporting early years learning. Early Years 26, 93–109. doi: 10.1080/09575140500507918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coley, R. L., Lewin-Bizan, S., and Carrano, J. (2011). Does early paternal parenting promote low-income children's long-term cognitive skills? J. Fam. Issues 32, 1522–1542. doi: 10.1177/0192513X11402175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (1988). Who does what when partners become parents: implications for men, women, and marriage. Marr. Family Rev. 12, 105–131.

Google Scholar

Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (1990). “Who does what?” in Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques, eds J. Touliatos, B. F. Perlmutter, and M. A. Straus (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage), 447–448.

Google Scholar

Cox, M., and Crnic, K. (2003). Qualitative Ratings for Parent-Child Interaction at 3–15 Months. Center for Developmental Science, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.

Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., and Raymond, J. (2004). “Fathers in family context: effects of marital quality and marital conflict,” in The Role of the Father in Child Development, 4th Edn., ed M. E. Lamb (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 196–221.

Google Scholar

Downer, J., Campos, R., McWayne, C., and Gartner, T. (2008). Father involvement and children's early learning: a critical review of published empirical work from the past 15 years. Marriage Fam. Rev. 43, 67–108. doi: 10.1080/01494920802010264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duursma, A. E. (2014). The effects of fathers' and mothers' reading to their children on language outcomes of children participating in early head start in the United States. Fathering 12, 283–302. doi: 10.3149/fth.1203.283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emery, R. E. (1994). Renegotiating Family Relationships: Divorce, Child Custody, and Mediation. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Eng, S., Szmodis, W., and Mulsow, M. (2014). Cambodian parental involvement: the role of parental beliefs, social networks, and trust. Elem. School J. 114, 573–594. doi: 10.1086/675639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagan, J., Day, R., Lamb, M. E., and Cabrera, N. J. (2014). Should researchers conceptualize differently the dimensions of parenting for fathers and mothers? J. Fam. Theory Rev. 6, 390–405. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagan, J., and Lee, Y. (2012). Effects of fathers' and mothers' cognitive stimulation and household income on toddlers' cognition: variations by family structure and child risk. Fathering 10, 140–159. doi: 10.3149/fth.1002.140

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., and Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students' academic achievement: a meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 13, 1–22. doi: 10.1023/A:1009048817385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, M. E. (2003). The internal structure and ecological context of coparenting: a framework for research and intervention. Parenting 3, 95–131. doi: 10.1207/S15327922PAR0302_01

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flouri, E., and Buchanan, A. (2004). Early father's and mother's involvement and child's later educational outcomes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 74, 141–153. doi: 10.1348/000709904773839806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, T. D., Froyen, L. C., Skibbe, L. E., Bowles, R. P., and Decker, K. B. (2016). Fathers' and mothers' home learning environments and children's early academic outcomes. Read. Writing 29, 1845–1863. doi: 10.1007/s11145-016-9655-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gable, S., Crnic, K., and Belsky, J. (1994). Coparenting within the family system: influences on children's development. Fam. Relat. 43, 380–386. doi: 10.2307/585368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giallo, R., Treyvaud, K., Cooklin, A., and Wade, C. (2013). Mothers' and fathers' involvement in home activities with their children: psychosocial factors and the role of parental self-efficacy. Early Child Dev. Care 183, 343–359. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2012.711587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grolnick, W. S., and Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents' involvement in children's schooling: a multidimensional conceptualization and motivational model. Child Dev. 65, 237–252. doi: 10.2307/1131378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grossmann, K., Grossmann, K. E., Fremmer-Bombik, E., Kindler, H., Scheuerer-Englisch, H., and Zimmermann, A. P. (2002). The uniqueness of the child–father attachment relationship: fathers' sensitive and challenging play as a pivotal variable in a 16-year longitudinal study. Soc. Dev. 11, 301–337. doi: 10.1111/1467-9507.00202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagan, L. K., and Kuebli, J. (2007). Mothers' and fathers'socialization of preschoolers' physical risk taking. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 28, 2–14. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, A., and Goodall, J. (2007). Engaging Parents in Raising Achievement: Do Parents Know They Matter?: A Research Project Commissioned by the Specialist Schools and Academies Trust. Warwick: Department for Children, Schools and Families.

Google Scholar

Hernandez, D. C., and Coley, R. L. (2007). Measuring father involvement within low-income families: who is a reliable and valid reporter? Parenting 7, 69–97. doi: 10.1080/15295190709336777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, N. E. (2001). Parenting and academic socialization as they relate to school readiness: the roles of ethnicity and family income. J. Educ. Psychol. 93:686–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.4.686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hossain, Z., and Roopnarine, J. (1994). AfricanAmerican fathers' involvement with infants: relationship to their functioning style, support, education, and income. Infant Beh. Dev. 17, 175–184. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(94)90053-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeong, J., McCoy, D. C., Yousafzai, A. K., Salhi, C., and Fink, G. (2016). Paternal stimulation and early child development in low-and middle-income countries. Pediatrics 138:e20161357. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeynes, W. H. (2007). The relationship between parental involvement and urban secondary school student academic achievement: a meta-analysis. Urban Educ. 42, 82–110. doi: 10.1177/0042085906293818

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeynes, W. H. (2015). A meta-analysis: the relationship between father involvement and student academic achievement. Urban Educ. 50, 387–423. doi: 10.1177/0042085914525789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S., and Kaufman, N. L. (1993). K-SEALS: Kaufman Survey of Early Academic and Language Skills. American Guidance Service.

Google Scholar

Kelly, D. (2018). Generative fatherhood and children's future civic engagement: a conceptual model of the relationship between paternal engagement and child's developing prosocial skills. J. Hum. Behav. Soc. Environ. 28, 303–314. doi: 10.1080/10911359.2017.1418469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keown, L. J., and Palmer, M. (2014). Comparisons between paternal and maternal involvement with sons: early to middle childhood. Early Child Dev. Care 184, 99–117. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2013.773510

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. W., and Hill, N. E. (2015). Including fathers in the picture: a meta-analysis of parental involvement and student's academic achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 107, 919–934. doi: 10.1037/edu0000023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolak, A. M., and Volling, B. L. (2013). Coparenting moderates the association between firstborn children's temperament and problem behavior across the transition to siblinghood. J. Fam. Psychol. 27, 355–364. doi: 10.1037/a0032864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamb, M. E. (2004). The Role of the Father in Child Development. New York, NY: Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Lamb, M. E., Pleck, I. H., Charnov, E. L., and Levine, J. A. (1985). Parental behavior in humans. Am. Zool. 25, 883–894. doi: 10.1093/icb/25.3.883

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamb, M. E., Pleck, J. H., Charnov, E. L., and Levine, J. A. (1987). “A biosocial perspective on paternal behavior and involvement,” in Parenting Across the Lifespan: Biosocial Dimensions, eds J. B. Lancaster, J. Altman, A. S. Rossi, and L. R. Sherrod (New York, NY: Aldine de Gruyter), 111–142.

Google Scholar

Lipscomb, R. C. (2011). Strategies to improve fathers' involvement with their children's development and academic achievement. Race Gender Class 18, 253–267. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/43496847

Google Scholar

Malin, J. L., Karberg, E., Cabrera, N. J., Rowe, M., Cristaforo, T., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2012). Father-toddler communication in low-income families: the role of paternal education and depressive symptoms. J. Fam. Sci. 3, 155–163. doi: 10.1080/19424620.2012.779423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marsiglio, W., Amato, P., Day, R. D., and Lamb, M. E. (2000). Scholarship on fatherhood in the 1990s and beyond. J. Marriage Fam. 62, 1173–1191. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01173.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, B. A., Dyer, W. J., and Laxman, D. J. (2013). Father involvement and student achievement: variations based on demographic contexts. Early Child Dev. Care 183, 810–826. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2012.725275

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, B. A., Dyer, W. J., Liu, Y., Brown, G. L., and Hong, S. (2009). The differential impact of early father and mother involvement on later student achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 498–508. doi: 10.1037/a0014238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McBride, B. A., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., and Ho, M. H. (2005). The mediating role of fathers' school involvement on student achievement. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 26, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2004.12.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McWayne, C., Downer, J. T., Campos, R., and Harris, R. D. (2013). Father involvement during early childhood and its association with children's early learning: a meta-analysis. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 898–922. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2013.746932

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., and PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, F. J., and Cooney, R. R. (2002). “Parenting and academic achievement: multiple paths to early literacy,” in Monographs in Parenting. Parenting and the Child’s World: Influences on Academic, Intellectual, and Social-Emotional Development, eds J. G. Borkowski, S. L. Ramey, and M. Bristol-Power (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 141–160.

Nord, C. W., Brimhall, D., and West, J. (1997). Fathers' Involvement in Their Children's Schools (NCES 98-091). Washington, DC: US Department of Education, National Center for Educational Statistics. ED 409 125.

Google Scholar

Nordhal, K. B., Zambrana, I. M., and Forgatch, M. S. (2016). Risk and protective factors related to fathers' positive involvement and negative reinforcement with 1-year-olds. Parenting 16:1–21. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2016.1116891

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ortiz, R. W. (2000). The many faces of learning to read: the role of fathers in helping their children to develop early literacy skills. Multicult. Perspect. 2, 10–17. doi: 10.1207/S15327892MCP0202_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palkovitz, R. (2002). “Involved fathering and child development: advancing our understanding of good fathering,” in Handbook of Father Involvement: Multidisciplinary Perspectives, eds C. S. Tamis-LeMonda and N. Cabrera (New York, NY: Routledge; Taylor Francis Group), 33–64.

Google Scholar

Pancsofar, N., Vernon-feagans, L., and Family Life Project Investigators. (2010). Fathers' early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Childh. Res. Q. 25, 450–463. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Burgdorf, J., Turner, C., and Gordon, N. (2003). Modeling ADHD-type arousal with unilateral frontal cortex damage in rats and beneficial effects of play therapy. Brain Cogn. 52, 97–105. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00013-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paquette, D., Carbonneau, R., Dubeau, D., Bigras, M., and Tremblay, R. E. (2003). Prevalence of father–child rough-and-tumble play and physical aggression in preschool children. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 18, 171–189. doi: 10.1007/BF03173483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, G. E. (1982). Coercive Family Process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Patterson, G. R., and Fisher, P. A. (2002). “Recent developments in our understanding of parenting: bidirectional effects, causal models, and the search for parsimony,” in Handbook of Parenting, Vol. 5, eds M. H. Bornstein (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 59–88.

Google Scholar

Pleck, J. H. (2007). Why could father involvement benefit children? Theor. Perspect. 11, 196–202. doi: 10.1080/10888690701762068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pleck, J. H. (2010). “Paternal involvement,” in The Role of the Father in Child Development, 5th Edn, eds M. E. Lamb (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons Inc., 58–93.

Google Scholar

Pleck, J. H. (2012). Integrating father involvement in parenting research. Parenting 12, 243–253. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.683365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, C., Walker, G., and Keen, B. (2012). Engaging fathers from disadvantaged areas in children's early educational transitions: a UK perspective. J. Early Child. Res. 10, 209–225. doi: 10.1177/1476718X12442065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potter, C., Walker, G., and Keen, B. (2013). ‘I am reading to her and she loves it': benefits of engaging fathers from disadvantaged areas in their children's early learning transitions. Early Years 33, 74–89. doi: 10.1080/09575146.2012.666959

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roopnarine, J. L., Krishnakumar, A., Metindogan, A., and Evans, M. (2006). Links between parenting styles, parent–child academic interaction, parent–school interaction, and early academic skills and social behaviors in young children of English-speaking Caribbean immigrants. Early Child. Res. Q. 21, 238–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2006.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Martin, A., and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2006). Is one good parent good enough? Patterns of mother and father parenting and child cognitive outcomes at 24 and 36 months. Parenting 6, 221–228. doi: 10.1207/s15327922par0602&3_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: a dialectical integration of nature and nurture. Child Dev. 81, 6–22. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01378.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandseter, E. B., and Kennair, L. E. (2011). Children's risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evol. Psychol. 9, 257–284. doi: 10.1177/147470491100900212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saracho, O. N. (2007a). Hispanic father-child sociocultural literacy practices. J. Hisp. High. Educ. 6, 272–283. doi: 10.1177/1538192707302878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saracho, O. N. (2007b). Fathers and young children's literacy experiences in a family environment. Early Child Dev. Care 177, 403–415. doi: 10.1080/03004430600563034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saracho, O. N. (2008). Fathers' and young children's literacy experiences. Early Child Dev. Care 178, 837–852. doi: 10.1080/03004430802352251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, J., Lau, C., Sincovich, A., and Rao, N. (2018). Socioeconomic status and early child development in East Asia and the Pacific: the protective role of parental engagement in learning activities. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 93, 321–330. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, E. T., and Goldberg, W. A. (2009). Parental school involvement in relation to children's grades and adaptation to school. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 30, 442–453. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.12.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

VandenBos, G. R. (2015). APA Dictionary of Psychology, 2nd Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Google Scholar

Varghese, C., and Wachen, J. (2016). The determinants of father involvement and connection to children's literacy and language outcomes: a review of literature. Marriage Fam. Rev 52, 331–359. doi: 10.1080/01494929.2015.1099587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volling, B. L., and Belsky, J. (1991). Multiple determinants of father involvement during infancy in dual-earner and single-earner families. J. Marriage Fam. 53, 461–474. doi: 10.2307/352912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walters, E. (2001). Parent-Teacher Involvement Questionnaire (Parent Version). Retrieved from http://www.fasttrackproject.org/techrept/p/ptp/ptp3tech.pdf

Google Scholar

Keywords: fathers, fathers involvement, cognitive skills, learning, parenting

Citation: Rollè L, Gullotta G, Trombetta T, Curti L, Gerino E, Brustia P and Caldarera AM (2019) Father Involvement and Cognitive Development in Early and Middle Childhood: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 10:2405. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02405

Received: 09 April 2019; Accepted: 09 October 2019;
Published: 25 October 2019.

Edited by:

Yvette Renee Harris, Miami University, United States

Reviewed by:

Nicola Carone, University of Pavia, Italy
E. Helin Yaban, Hacettepe University, Turkey

Copyright © 2019 Rollè, Gullotta, Trombetta, Curti, Gerino, Brustia and Caldarera. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Gullotta, giulia.gullotta@edu.unito.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.