- 1Faculty of Psychology, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
- 2Information Systems and Decision Sciences, California State University, Fullerton, CA, United States
- 3Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing, China
- 4Chongqing Collaborative Innovation Center for Brain Science, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
Disgust is an emotion that drives food avoidance. People vary in their responses to disgust, which is captured by their disgust sensitivity. Disgust sensitivity is clinically significant because it can influence eating behaviors, and indirectly people’s body mass index (BMI). Inhibitory control can also influence BMI through the role that such reflective abilities play in governing food intake. In this study, we relied on neural models of disgust to suggest that disgust and inhibitory control are intertwined, and that inhibitory control facilitates the translation of disgust sensitivity into BMI. Mediation analyses applied to 46 subjects, including 29 normal body weight [BMI = 18.34 kg/m2 (SD = 1.58)] and 17 overweight/obese [BMI = 26.03 kg/m2 (SD = 2.58)] subjects, were used to test the hypothesis. Subjects completed the Chinese version of the Disgust Scale-Revised, and an inhibition control test (Food-Specific Stop-Signal Task). There were negative correlations between the disgust sensitivity score (DS) and body mass index (BMI), and between DS and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). Moreover, BMI was positively correlated with SSRT. The mediation model results showed that disgust sensitivity was associated with BMI and that this relationship was mediated via inhibition control. There was no significant effect of DS on BMI, while the effect of SSRT on BMI was significant. This suggested that the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI was fully mediated through food-specific inhibitory control. This supports our hypothesis that BMI is affected by disgust sensitivity and that this relationship is mediated by inhibition control. These findings reveal a key mechanism that underlies disgust sensitivity-BMI association and point to future research and potential interventions aimed at food intake management.
Introduction
Disgust is an emotion that captures revulsion at the prospect of oral incorporation of offensive objects (Rozin and Fallon, 1987). It is an important protective mechanism that discourages humans from consuming things that could harm them; it ultimately prevents contamination and disease spread (Rozin et al., 1999). It also serves other avoidance behaviors, such as inhibiting the breaking of social norms (Pond et al., 2012). Disgust sensitivity is the trait-like predisposition of a person to become disgusted in response to a particular group of stimuli, known as disgust elicitors (Merckelbach et al., 1999). Disgust sensitivity is clinically significant because it can help individuals avoid external dangerous stimuli; however, individuals high in disgust sensitivity are prone to anxiety, fear of death, and unnecessarily avoiding risk-taking (Haidt et al., 1994). As such, disgust sensitivity can be associated with a variety of psychiatric disorders, including eating disorders (Aharoni and Hertz, 2012; Ammann et al., 2018), phobias (Bianchi and Carter, 2012), and obsessive-compulsive disorders (Ólafsson et al., 2013).
It has been demonstrated that disgust sensitivity is associated with eating disorders and body mass index (BMI), which is a reliable measure of body fat content (Troop et al., 2000; Griffiths and Troop, 2006; Mayer et al., 2008; Davey and Chapman, 2009; Houben et al., 2013; Watkins et al., 2015; Vicario and Rafal, 2017; Ammann et al., 2018). For example, Troop et al. (2000) and Mayer et al. (2008) found that eating disorder symptoms (as captured with thinness and bulimia scores) were related to higher levels of disgust sensitivity, including to food. Similarly, Davey and Chapman (2009) showed that the experience of disgust may be heightened in individuals with eating disorder symptomatology; and Houben and Havermans (2012) found that more restrained eaters have increased core disgust, increased contamination disgust, and a decreased appetite for high-calorie food, compared to women with higher BMIs.
Importantly, disgust is rooted in brain activity in the insular cortex (i.e., the insula) (Wicker et al., 2003; Wright et al., 2004; Chen et al., 2009), especially in its anterior parts (Krolak-Salmon et al., 2003; Jabbi et al., 2008). The sensitivity of the insula to stimuli can vary among individuals, which makes it a potential contributor to disgust sensitivity (Schienle et al., 2005; Stark et al., 2005; Calder et al., 2007; Mataix-Cols et al., 2008; Schafer et al., 2009). Indeed, it has been shown that the insula has decreased activation in an obese group as compared with a lean group when viewing contaminated food images (Watkins et al., 2015). Nevertheless, the effect of disgust sensitivity on behavior inhibition (including presumably food intake inhibition) is likely indirect, as inhibition tasks are executed in the prefrontal cortex (Shackman et al., 2009; Stramaccia et al., 2015). Indeed, the insula has trajectories, especially from its superior anterior parts to the prefrontal cortex (Flynn et al., 1999; Droutman et al., 2015), which afford disturbing self-control tasks by hijacking the prefrontal cortex (Naqvi et al., 2007; Craig, 2009; Naqvi and Bechara, 2009, 2010; Turel et al., 2018). Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that disgust sensitivity, which is manifested through insular activity, indirectly exerts an effect on eating behaviors, through its effect on self-control abilities, which manifest in prefrontal cortex activity.
Inhibitory control regulates eating behavior (Allom and Mullan, 2014; Carbine et al., 2017, 2018; Zhou et al., 2018), because it helps people make flexible choices in the face of changing environments (Miyake et al., 2000; Lavagnino et al., 2016) and help resisting impulsions toward reward-generating behaviors that are sub-optimal (Turel and Bechara, 2016; Turel and Qahri-Saremi, 2016, 2018; He et al., 2017a,b, 2018; Chen et al., 2018). Indeed, less effective inhibition control is associated with increased food intake and overeating, increased weight and obesity, and lower weight loss during treatment (Houben et al., 2013; Giel et al., 2017; Oomen et al., 2018). In the context of food intake, the stop-signal task is an effective means to capture food-specific inhibitory control (Bartholdy et al., 2016). Inhibitory control is significantly impaired in obese adults and children compared to individuals with normal body weight (Lavagnino et al., 2016). From a brain activation standpoint, Hendrick et al. (2012) observed that obese females had lower activity in the insula, inferior parietal cortex, cuneus, and supplementary motor area compared to lean females. Furthermore, the brain activations in these regions inversely correlated with BMI scores. As such, inhibitory control ability is critical for the regulation of food intake and may therefore influence body weight.
Synthesizing these views and relying on the neural model according to which insular activity, which mediates disgust, affects behavior through trajectories to brain centers that govern inhibitory control (Naqvi et al., 2007, 2014; Naqvi and Bechara, 2009, 2010; He et al., 2014; Droutman et al., 2015; Turel et al., 2018), we hypothesize that the effect of disgust sensitivity on body mass index is mediated through inhibitory control.
Materials and Methods
Participants
We recruited 52 participants (34 females, all university students) via class announcements. Their height and weight were measured; these were used for BMI calculation (weight to squared height in kg/m2). There were 18 overweight (BMI > 23, averaged BMI = 26.14 ± 2.61, 13 females) and 34 normal weight subjects (BMI < 23, averaged BMI = 19.27 ± 1.60, 22 females). All subjects were right-handed, with normal or corrected-to-normal vision. They had no self-reported history of neurological or psychiatric disorders. Six participants were excluded for further analysis because they did not follow the directions of the stop-signal tasks. This left us with 46 participants, out of which 17 were overweight (BMI = 26.03 ± 2.58, 12 females) and 29 were normal weight (BMI = 18.34 ± 1.58, 20 females). The ratio of overweight to normal weight participants in this sample is consistent with previous large sample studies of Chinese young adults (He et al., 2015). The study procedure was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Southwest University (SWU-IRB-17-031).
Measures
Disgust Scale-Revised Chinese Version
The scale was derived from the modified Disgust Scale-Revised (DS-R) (Olatunji et al., 2009). DS-R has 25 items along three dimensions: a 12-item core disgust scale, an 8-item animal-reminder scale, and a 5-item contamination disgust scale. Core disgust is a basic disgust elicitor. It focuses on disgust related to oral consumption of offensive items such as rotting foods. Animal reminder disgust is based on reminding people of their own animalistic nature; examples include disgust associated with corpses. Lastly, contamination disgust relates to interpersonal contagion that reflects threat of disease transmission from other people (Olatunji et al., 2008; Hamerman, 2016). DS-R has been shown to be valid and reliable across cultures and languages (Olatunji et al., 2009). The translation of the DS-R to Chinese followed a forward-backward translation procedure by four bilingual psychology professionals. It also included adjustments for cultural differences. The final iteration produced a scale that presumably accurately reflects the context-adjusted English items of DS-R.
We used this scale in test-retest procedure with 520 college students (328 females, averaged age = 21.4 ± 2.24 years), and found that the scale was reliable (Cronbach alpha coefficient, total scale = 0.82, core disgust subscale = 0.76, animal-reminder subscale = 0.87, contamination disgust subscale = 0.91) and valid (confirmatory factor analyses fit indices: χ2/df = 3.76, RMR = 0.06, GFI = 0.92, AGFI = 0.94, NFI = 0.92, TLI = 0.91, CFI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.04). There was a moderate correlation between the three sub-dimensions, with correlation coefficients ranging from 0.34 to 0.53 (all with p < 0.05); there were high correlations between the sub-dimension scores and the total score (correlation coefficients are 0.78, 0.74, and 0.87, respectively, all with p < 0.001). We therefore used the total score of the Chinese DS-R (DS-RC) as a measure of disgust sensitivity. Differences in DS-RC scores among gender, grade, region, and BMI groups in the sample are given in Table 1. Consistent with prior research, obese individuals had lower disgust sensitivity scores.
Food-Specific Stop-Signal Task
The food-specific stop-signal task was adapted from the general Stop-Signal Task (SST) to measure response inhibition specifically for food. The task flow is consistent with the traditional SST, and the only difference is that the background pictures involve food items. These pictures were selected from the Chinese Affective Picture System. The high- and low-calorie food images were matched in terms of arousal and valence, and this was verified with a panel of Chinese students.
The task was generated by Stop-it software (Verbruggen et al., 2008). After a 500-ms fixation cross on the black screen, a stimulus in the form of either square (1 cm side length) or circle (1 cm radius) appeared against the background of either high- and low-calorie food images (Figure 1). The subjects were asked to press the left or right response key (counterbalanced across participants) for the square or circle as fast as possible. Participants were instructed not to respond when the sound stimuli was presented (the stop signal, 75-Hz pure tone, lasted for 75 ms). The stop signal was presented in 25% of the trials, consistent with the traditional SST. The delay between the go stimulus and the stop signal (stop-signal delay, SSD) was initially set at 250 ms and was subsequently dynamically changed using a tracking procedure to enable participants to correctly inhibit 50% of the stop trials: following a successful inhibition, the SSD was increased by 50 ms; while following a failed inhibition, the SSD was decreased by 50 ms. The dependent variable, stop-signal reaction time (SSRT), was calculated by subtracting the mean SSD from mean reaction times. Higher SSRTs indicate decreased inhibitory control (Wessel and Aron, 2015). Each subject completed four blocks of the task, and each block consisted of 64 trials. Before the experiment, each subject completed one practice block. The process is depicted in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Illustration of the stop-signal task. For go trials, subjects were instructed to press left key when the stimulus was a square and right key when it was a circle. For a quarter of all trials, the go stimulus was followed by an auditory stop signal (pure tone) after a variable stop-signal delay (SSD). SDT, duration of the stop signal; MAXRT, maximum reaction time.
Procedure
In order to reduce the effect of hunger on the study, the experiment was uniformly arranged between 12:00~14:00 and 17:30~19:30, which was right after meals (lunch and dinner breaks, respectively). In addition, each participant rated his or her hunger level on a 1 (not hungry at all) to 10 (very hungry) scale. All participants completed the DS-RC, followed by the food-specific stop-signal task, and the measurement of their height and weight.
Results
Descriptive Statistics of Variables
Descriptive statistics and correlations among disgust sensitivity score (DS), body mass index (BMI), and stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) are given in Table 2; correlations are further depicted in Figure 2. There was a negative correlation between DS and BMI and between DS and SSRT, indicating that subjects with low disgust sensitivity had high BMI and poor control ability. BMI was positively correlated with SSRT, indicating that higher BMI subjects had poorer inhibitory control compared to normal-BMI subjects.
Figure 2. Correlation between disgust sensitivity score (DS), inhibition control index (SSRT), and body mass index (BMI). (A) There was a negative correlation between DS and BMI (r = −0.365, p < 0.05), indicating that subjects with low disgust sensitivity had high BMI. (B) DS was negatively correlated with SSRT (r = −0.335, p < 0.05), indicating that subjects with low disgust sensitivity had poor control ability. (C) BMI was positively correlated with SSRT (r = 0.393, p < 0.01), indicating that higher BMI subjects had poorer inhibitory control.
Figure 3. The mediated model of the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI. Results suggested that the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI is fully mediated through food-specific inhibitory control. BMI, body mass index; DS, disgust sensitivity; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time.
Hypothesis Testing
We followed common mediation testing procedures (Baron and Kenny, 1986) with the PROCESS macro in SPPS (Hayes, 2013) to test our hypothesis. In these analyses, disgust sensitivity was the independent variable, obesity degree was the dependent variable, and inhibition control was the mediator variable. First, DS, BMI, and SSRT were mean-centered. Second, the effects of centered DS on BMI were examined with a regression model. The results showed that DS significantly reduces BMI (Table 3). Third, the effect of centered DS on SSRT was examined with a regression model. As shown in Table 3, DS significantly reduced SSRT on the food-specific SST. Lastly, the joint predictive effect of DS and SSRT on BMI was examined; see Model C in Table 3; the mediated model is further depicted in Figure 3. Results showed no significant effect of DS on BMI, while the effect of SSRT on BMI was significant. This suggests full mediation. That is, the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI was fully mediated through food-specific inhibitory control. This lends support to our hypothesis and the neural model of disgust translation into food consumption behavior. Accounting for both DS and SSRT explained 22% of the variance in BMI, suggesting that these two factors are important but not exclusive determinants of BMI.
Discussion
This study sought to unravel a key mechanism through which disgust sensitivity affects food intake and indirectly, BMI. To theoretically develop this mechanism, we relied on insights from the neurocognitive science literature that suggest that the insula, which is a center for processing disgust, exerts influence on behavior via its trajectory to the prefrontal cortex, which is a center for inhibitory control. Based on these insights, we suggested that inhibitory control mediates the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI. Our results support a full mediation and lend support for this perspective.
These results reaffirm the findings in previous studies in which disgust sensitivity scores were negatively associated with BMI; and participants with lower DS had a significantly higher BMI compared to participants with a higher DS (Houben et al., 2013; Watkins et al., 2015). This by itself is important because BMI abnormalities have many adverse consequences. For example, excessively high BMI is associated with a higher risk of diabetes; cardiovascular, heart disease; hypertension; stroke; and sleep apnea (Narayan et al., 2007; Freedman and Sherry, 2009; Qizilbash et al., 2015; Turel et al., 2016). Similarly, excessively low BMI in anorexic individuals can adversely affect people’s health and normal functioning (Bulik et al., 2005; Nickols-Richardson, 2008; Sanford, 2017). Our Model A (Table 3) shows that DS alone explains 13% of the variance in BMI. Thus, it can be an important intervention target for scholars and professionals trying to help obese and overweight populations to return their BMIs to a normal desirable range.
Our result extends this view and explains one facet of how this association works. Specifically, it demonstrates that one key mechanism through which disgust sensitivity influences BMI is through a reduction in inhibitory control that accompanies increases in disgust sensitivity. This perspective provides another layer of support, albeit behavioral, to the neurocognitive perspective of decision-making. According to this perspective, the insula mediates interoceptive signals such as disgust, and these hijack prefrontal cortex activity and prevent proper inhibition (Naqvi et al., 2007, 2014; Naqvi and Bechara, 2009, 2010; Clark et al., 2014; Droutman et al., 2015; Turel et al., 2018). These findings also corroborate previous evidence linking inhibition control with BMI (Guerrieri et al., 2009; Hendrick et al., 2012; Lavagnino et al., 2016). Thus, future research may further focus on the insula-prefrontal cortex interaction, and/or on inhibition-disgust interactions as a means to examine and intervene in BMI abnormalities. The mediation model with both disgust sensitivity and food-specific inhibitory control (Model C in Table 3) explained 22% of the variance in BMI. Thus, disgust sensitivity and food-specific inhibitory control can be important metrics and targets in future research on interventions aimed at restoring BMI normality.
Several limitations of this study are noteworthy. First, the Chinese version of Disgust Scale-Revised (DS-RC) used in this study had good reliability and validity; however, it may fail to consider the cultural differences between China and the west. Disgust sensitivity can be culturally dependent (what may be seen as disgusting in one culture may not be perceived as such in another). Thus, the social context in which an individual is located may affect his or her experience of disgust stimuli. Therefore, we call for future research to consider a Chinese-specific disgust sensitivity scale that reflects common Chinese taste preferences, cultural elements, and social conditions, and to examine the generalizability of our findings to other cultures. Second, while we relied on a neurocognitive model to develop the hypothesis, we used only behavioral and anthropometric measures in this study. Even though the link between the insula and prefrontal cortex has been demonstrated (Droutman et al., 2015), and the understanding of the neural basis of responses to gustatory and food stimuli has evolved (Yeung et al., 2019), future research should corroborate the link between brain activation and our measures. Third, we focused on a limited set of predictors of obesity for theoretical and simplicity reasons. Future research can expand our model to account for more predictors of obesity (e.g., genetics, family environment). Lastly, this study used a small sample and the participants were not excessively abnormal in terms of BMI. This may impact the generalizability of the findings. Future research should therefore replicate our results with samples containing excessively high BMI (morbidly obese) and excessively low BMI (e.g., anorexic) participants.
Conclusions
This is the first study to our knowledge to use the Disgust Scale-Revised Chinese Version (DS-RC) to examine individual differences in disgust sensitivity and to explore the relationship between disgust sensitivity and obesity. The results showed that the DS-RC had high reliability and validity and was suitable for the measurement of Chinese college students’ disgust sensitivity. Consistent with prior research, individuals with low disgust sensitivity had a high BMI and poor control ability to suppress the desire to eat food, and higher BMI individuals had poorer inhibitory control compared to normal-BMI individuals. This study innovatively used inhibition control as a mediator variable, to examine the predictive effect of disgust sensitivity on obesity. We found that the effect of disgust sensitivity on BMI is fully mediated through food-specific inhibitory control.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Southwest University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
JL and QH designed the study. JL and RC performed the study. XL, JL, and QH analyzed the data. XL, JL, OT, RC, and QH wrote the manuscript. All authors made final approval of the draft submitted to the journal.
Funding
This study was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31972906), Fundamental Research Funds for Central Universities (SWU1809003), Open Research Fund of the Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences (KLMH2019K05), and the High-end Foreign Expert Introduction Program (G20190022029).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Aharoni, R., and Hertz, M. M. (2012). Disgust sensitivity and anorexia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 20, 106–110. doi: 10.1002/erv.1124
Allom, V., and Mullan, B. (2014). Individual differences in executive function predict distinct eating behaviours. Appetite 80, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.05.007
Ammann, J., Hartmann, C., and Siegrist, M. (2018). Does food disgust sensitivity influence eating behaviour? Experimental validation of the food disgust scale. Food Qual. Prefer. 68, 411–414. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.12.013
Baron, R. M., and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator variable distinction in social psychological-research – conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 51, 1173–1182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173
Bartholdy, S., Dalton, B., O’Daly, O. G., Campbell, I. C., and Schmidt, U. (2016). A systematic review of the relationship between eating, weight and inhibitory control using the stop signal task. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 64, 35–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.010
Bianchi, K. N., and Carter, M. M. (2012). An experimental analysis of disgust sensitivity and fear of contagion in spider and blood injection injury phobia. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 753–761. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.06.004
Bulik, C. M., Reba, L., Siega-Riz, A. M., and Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2005). Anorexia nervosa: definition, epidemiology, and cycle of risk. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 37, S2–S9. doi: 10.1002/eat.20107
Calder, A. J., Beaver, J. D., Davis, M. H., van Ditzhuijzen, J., Keane, J., and Lawrence, A. D. (2007). Disgust sensitivity predicts the insula and pallidal response to pictures of disgusting foods. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3422–3428. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05604.x
Carbine, K. A., Christensen, E., LeCheminant, J. D., Bailey, B. W., Tucker, L. A., and Larson, M. J. (2017). Testing food-related inhibitory control to high-and low-calorie food stimuli: electrophysiological responses to high-calorie food stimuli predict calorie and carbohydrate intake. Psychophysiology 54, 982–997. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12860
Carbine, K. A., Duraccio, K. M., Kirwan, C. B., Muncy, N. M., LeCheminant, J. D., and Larson, M. J. (2018). A direct comparison between ERP and fMRI measurements of food-related inhibitory control: implications for BMI status and dietary intake. NeuroImage 166, 335–348. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.008
Chen, R., Li, D. P., Turel, O., Sørensen, T. A., Bechara, A., Li, Y., et al. (2018). Decision making deficits in relation to food cues influence obesity: a triadic neural model of problematic eating. Front. Psychiatry 9, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00264
Chen, Y. H., Dammers, J., Boers, F., Leiberg, S., Edgar, J. C., Roberts, T. P. L., et al. (2009). The temporal dynamics of insula activity to disgust and happy facial expressions: a magnetoencephalography study. NeuroImage 47, 1921–1928. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.093
Clark, L., Studer, B., Bruss, J., Tranel, D., and Bechara, A. (2014). Damage to insula abolishes cognitive distortions during simulated gambling. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 6098–6103. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1322295111
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel - now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555
Davey, G. C., and Chapman, L. (2009). Disgust and eating disorder symptomatology in a non-clinical population: the role of trait anxiety and anxiety sensitivity. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 16, 268–275. doi: 10.1002/cpp.623
Droutman, V., Read, S. J., and Bechara, A. (2015). Revisiting the role of the insula in addiction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 19, 414–420. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.05.005
Flynn, F. G., Benson, D. F., and Ardila, A. (1999). Anatomy of the insula – functional and clinical correlates. Aphasiology 13, 55–78. doi: 10.1080/026870399402325
Freedman, D. S., and Sherry, B. (2009). The validity of BMI as an indicator of body fatness and risk among children. Pediatrics 124, S23–S34. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-3586E
Giel, K., Teufel, M., Junne, F., Zipfel, S., and Schag, K. (2017). Food-related impulsivity in obesity and binge eating disorder–a systematic update of the evidence. Nutrients 9:1170. doi: 10.3390/nu9111170
Griffiths, J., and Troop, N. A. (2006). Disgust and fear ratings of eating disorder-relevant stimuli: associations with dieting concerns and fat intake. Anxiety Stress Coping 19, 421–433. doi: 10.1080/10615800601066771
Guerrieri, R., Nederkoorn, C., Schrooten, M., Martijn, C., and Jansen, A. (2009). Inducing impulsivity leads high and low restrained eaters into overeating, whereas current dieters stick to their diet. Appetite 53, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.05.013
Haidt, J., Mccauley, C., and Rozin, P. (1994). Individual differences in sensitivity to disgust: a scale sampling seven domains of disgust elicitors. Pers. Individ. Differ. 16, 701–713. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90212-7
Hamerman, E. J. (2016). Cooking and disgust sensitivity influence preference for attending insect-based food events. Appetite 96, 319–326. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.09.029
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. J. Educ. Meas. 51, 335–337.
He, Q., Chen, C., Dong, Q., Xue, G., Chen, C., Lu, Z. L., et al. (2015). Gray and white matter structures in the midcingulate cortex region contribute to body mass index in Chinese young adults. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 319–329. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0657-9
He, Q. H., Huang, X. L., Turel, O., Schulte, M., Huang, D., Thames, A., et al. (2018). Presumed structural and functional neural recovery after long-term abstinence from cocaine in male military veterans. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 84, 18–29. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2018.01.024
He, Q., Turel, O., and Bechara, A. (2017a). Brain anatomy alterations associated with social networking site (SNS) addiction. Sci. Rep. 7:45064. 1–8. doi: 10.1038/srep45064
He, Q., Turel, O., Brevers, D., and Bechara, A. (2017b). Excess social media use in normal populations is associated with amygdala-striatal but not with prefrontal morphology. Psychiatry Res., Neuroimaging 269, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.09.003
He, Q., Xiao, L., Xue, G., Wong, S., Ames, S. L., Schembre, S. M., et al. (2014). Poor ability to resist tempting calorie rich food is linked to altered balance between neural systems involved in urge and self-control. Nutr. J. 13:92. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-13-92
Hendrick, O. M., Luo, X., Zhang, S., and Li, C. S. (2012). Saliency processing and obesity: a preliminary imaging study of the stop signal task. Obesity 20, 1796–1802. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.180
Houben, K., and Havermans, R. C. (2012). A delicious fly in the soup. The relationship between disgust, obesity, and restraint. Appetite 58, 827–830. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.01.018
Houben, K., Nederkoorn, C., and Jansen, A. (2013). Eating on impulse: the relation between overweight and food-specific inhibitory control. Obesity 22:E6. doi: 10.1002/oby.20670
Jabbi, M., Bastiaansen, J., and Keysers, C. (2008). A common anterior insula representation of disgust observation, experience and imagination shows divergent functional connectivity pathways. PLoS One 3:e2939. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002939
Krolak-Salmon, P., Henaff, M. A., Isnard, J., Tallon-Baudry, C., Guenot, M., Vighetto, A., et al. (2003). An attention modulated response to disgust in human ventral anterior insula. Ann. Neurol. 53, 446–453. doi: 10.1002/ana.10502
Lavagnino, L., Arnone, D., Cao, B., Soares, J. C., and Selvaraj, S. (2016). Inhibitory control in obesity and binge eating disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis of neurocognitive and neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 714–726. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.041
Mataix-Cols, D., An, S. K., Lawrence, N. S., Caseras, X., Speckens, A., Giampietro, V., et al. (2008). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity modulate neural responses to aversive/disgusting stimuli. Eur. J. Neurosci. 27, 3050–3058. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06311.x
Mayer, B., Muris, P., Bos, A. E., and Suijkerbuijk, C. (2008). Disgust sensitivity and eating disorder symptoms in a non-clinical population. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 39, 504–514. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.11.007
Merckelbach, H., Muris, P., De Jong, P. J., and De Jongh, A. (1999). Disgust sensitivity, blood–injection–injury fear, and dental anxiety. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 6, 279–285. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0879(199910)6:4<279::AID-CPP210>3.0.CO;2-R
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Naqvi, N. H., and Bechara, A. (2009). The hidden island of addiction: the insula. Trends Neurosci. 32, 56–67. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.09.009
Naqvi, N. H., and Bechara, A. (2010). The insula and drug addiction: an interoceptive view of pleasure, urges, and decision-making. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 435–450. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0268-7
Naqvi, N. H., Gaznick, N., Tranel, D., and Bechara, A. (2014). The insula: a critical neural substrate for craving and drug seeking under conflict and risk. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1316, 53–70. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12415
Naqvi, N. H., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, H., and Bechara, A. (2007). Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science 315, 531–534. doi: 10.1126/science.1135926
Narayan, K. M. V., Boyle, J. P., Thompson, T. J., Gregg, E. W., and Williamson, D. F. (2007). Effect of BMI on lifetime risk for diabetes in the US. Diabetes Care 30, 1562–1566. doi: 10.2337/dc06-2544
Nickols-Richardson, S. M. (2008). Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa During Pregnancy. Nutrition and Health 115–134. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.11.008
Ólafsson, R. P., Emmelkamp, P. M., Gunnarsdóttir, E. R., Snæbjörnsson, T., Ólafsson, D. T., and Kristjánsson, Á. (2013). Suppressing disgust related thoughts and performance on a subsequent behavioural avoidance task: implications for OCD. Behav. Res. Ther. 51, 152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.11.008
Olatunji, B. O., Haidt, J., McKay, D., and David, B. (2008). Core, animal reminder, and contamination disgust: three kinds of disgust with distinct personality, behavioral, physiological, and clinical correlates. J. Res. Pers. 42, 1243–1259. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2008.03.009
Olatunji, B., Moretz, M., Mackay, D., Bjorklund, F., Jong, P. J. D., Haidt, J., et al. (2009). Confirming the three-factor structure of the disgust scale-revised in eight countries. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 40, 234–255. doi: 10.1177/0022022108328918
Oomen, D., Grol, M., Spronk, D., Booth, C., and Fox, E. (2018). Beating uncontrolled eating: training inhibitory control to reduce food intake and food cue sensitivity. Appetite 131, 73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.09.007
Pond, R. S., Dewall, C. N., Lambert, N. M., Deckman, T., Bonser, I. M., and Fincham, F. D. (2012). Repulsed by violence: disgust sensitivity buffers trait, behavioral, and daily aggression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 175–188. doi: 10.1037/a0024296
Qizilbash, N., Gregson, J., Johnson, M. E., Pearce, N., Douglas, I., Wing, K., et al. (2015). BMI and risk of dementia in two million people over two decades: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol. 3, 431–436. doi: 10.1016/S2213-8587(15)00033-9
Rozin, P., and Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychol. Rev. 94, 23–41. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.1.23
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., Mccauley, C., Dunlop, L., and Ashmore, M. (1999). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity: comparisons and evaluations of paper-and-pencil versus behavioral measures. J. Res. Pers. 33, 330–351. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1999.2251
Sanford, A. M. (2017). Anorexia of aging and its role for frailty. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 20, 54–60. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000336
Schafer, A., Leutgeb, V., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., and Schienle, A. (2009). Propensity and sensitivity measures of fear and disgust are differentially related to emotion-specific brain activation. Neurosci. Lett. 465, 262–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.09.030
Schienle, A., Schafer, A., Stark, R., Walter, B., and Vaitl, D. (2005). Relationship between disgust sensitivity, trait anxiety and brain activity during disgust induction. Neuropsychobiology 51, 86–92. doi: 10.1159/000084165
Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and Behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1500–1506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x
Stark, R., Schienle, A., Sarlo, M., Palomba, D., Walter, B., and Vaitl, D. (2005). Influences of disgust sensitivity on hemodynamic responses towards a disgust-inducing film clip. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.01.010
Stramaccia, D. F., Penolazzi, B., Sartori, G., Braga, M., Mondini, S., and Galfano, G. (2015). Assessing the effects of tDCS over a delayed response inhibition task by targeting the right inferior frontal gyrus and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 2283–2290. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4297-6
Troop, N. A., Murphy, F., Bramon, E., and Treasure, J. L. (2000). Disgust sensitivity in eating disorders: a preliminary investigation. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 27, 446–451. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(200005)27:4<446::AID-EAT9>3.0.CO;2-W
Turel, O., and Bechara, A. (2016). A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Front. Psychol. 7:601. 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00601
Turel, O., He, Q., Brevers, D., and Bechara, A. (2018). Delay discounting mediates the association between posterior insular cortex volume and social media addiction symptoms. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 694–704. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0597-1
Turel, O., and Qahri-Saremi, H. (2016). Problematic use of social networking sites: antecedents and consequence from a dual-system theory perspective. J. Manage. Inf. Syst. 33, 1087–1116.
Turel, O., and Qahri-Saremi, H. (2018). Explaining unplanned online media behaviors: dual system theory models of impulsive use and swearing on social networking sites. New Media Soc. 20, 3050–3067. doi: 10.1177/146144481774075
Turel, O., Romashkin, A., and Morrison, K. M. (2016). Health outcomes of information system use lifestyles among adolescents: videogame addiction, sleep curtailment and cardio-metabolic deficiencies. Plos One 11:e0154764. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154764
Verbruggen, F., Logan, G. D., and Stevens, M. A. (2008). STOP-IT: Windows executable software for the stop-signal paradigm. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 479–483. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.2.479
Vicario, C. M., and Rafal, R. D. (2017). Relationship between body mass index and moral disapproval rating for ethical violations. Pers. Individ. Differ. 104, 8–11. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.07.027
Watkins, T. J., Di, I. C., Olatunji, B. O., Benningfield, M. M., Blackford, J. U., Dietrich, M. S., et al. (2015). Disgust proneness and associated neural substrates in obesity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 11, 458–465. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv129
Wessel, J. R., and Aron, A. R. (2015). It's not too late: the onset of the frontocentral P3 indexes successful response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology 52, 472–480. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12374
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2
Wright, P., He, G., Shapira, N. A., Goodman, W. K., and Liu, Y. (2004). Disgust and the insula: fMRI responses to pictures of mutilation and contamination. Neuroreport 15, 2347–2351. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200410250-00009
Yeung, A. W. K., Wong, N. S. M., Lau, H., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2019). Human brain responses to gustatory and food stimuli: a meta-evaluation of neuroimaging meta-analyses. NeuroImage 202:116111. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116111
Keywords: disgust sensitivity, body mass index, obesity, inhibition control, mediation
Citation: Liu X, Li J, Turel O, Chen R and He Q (2019) Food-Specific Inhibitory Control Mediates the Effect of Disgust Sensitivity on Body Mass Index. Front. Psychol. 10:2391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02391
Edited by:
Tifei Yuan, Shanghai Mental Health Center (SMHC), ChinaCopyright © 2019 Liu, Li, Turel, Chen and He. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Qinghua He, heqinghua@gmail.com