Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 04 June 2019
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards Tokyo 2020: What will contribute to Optimal Olympic Athlete Performance? View all 29 articles

Conventional and Alternative Strategies to Cope With the Subtropical Climate of Tokyo 2020: Impacts on Psychological Factors of Performance

  • Laboratory ACTES (UPRES-EA 3596), University of the French West Indies, Pointe-à-Pitre, France

The thermal discomfort caused by a hot or hot-wet climate can have negative effects on human performance. The 2020 Summer Olympic and Paralympic Games will take place in Tokyo’s hot and humid summer period, possibly exposing athletes to severe environmental stressors. In addition to technical, tactical, physical and nutritional preparation, Olympians and Paralympians need an optimal psychological state to turn in their best performances, especially in terms of emotional control, concentration and motivation. Yet, the tropical climate can have many negative effects on these factors. Better understanding of the negative effects of this climate and the strategies to manage them might be crucial for competitors, coaches and their teams in Japan. At the psychological level, cooling interventions before, during and/or immediately after exercise were mainly studied on perceptual responses. However, the effects of these interventions on other psychological components such as cognitive abilities or psychological states and the use of psychological techniques have been little explored, especially in hot-wet climate. Thus, this article proposes to take stock of the knowledge on the conventional and alternative strategies that help athletes to psychologically cope with the subtropical climate of Tokyo.

Introduction

Competing at the Olympic/Paralympic Games requires high level physical, technical, tactical, nutritional, and mental preparation. Athletes have to manage event stressors like anxiety (Nicholls and Levy, 2016) and competitive stressors like preparation, expectations, and opponents (Fletcher and Sarkar, 2012). Tokyo, lying in a humid subtropical zone, will add another important stressor: the hot and wet climate in the summer season. The Köppen Climate Classification subtype for this climate is warm temperature, fully humid and hot summer (i.e., subtropical climate). The hottest month of the year is August with an average temperature of 27.4°C (81.3°F) corresponding to the month of the Games. The average temperatures in Tokyo have risen by three degrees Celsius in the last one hundred years. Two degrees of these three are attributed to the urban heat island (UHI) effect and one to global warming. UHI is a kind of heat accumulation phenomenon in which temperatures in urban areas are markedly higher than those in surrounding areas (Matsumoto et al., 2017). One of the causes of this effect is asphalt concrete (i.e., the most common pavement surfacing materials) exposed to solar irradiation (Mohajerani et al., 2017). During the competitions, athletes from certain disciplines will be directly exposed (e.g., triathlon, road cycling, marathon, 20 and 50 km race walk). The particularities of Tokyo 2020 are required to mobilize all strategies to succeed in achieving sports performance in high thermal stresses without risk for health. Associated with intense physical activity, if not regulated, these high thermal stresses may provoke hyperthermia, which disrupts brain function (e.g., cerebral blood flow reduction, brain temperature increase, and cerebral oxygenation compromising), which in turn alters the cognitive abilities (Bain et al., 2015). Therefore, it is important to determine strategies that allow better regulation (e.g., cooling intervention) and better adaptation to this hyperthermia (e.g., acclimatization, mental preparation) without risk to health. In addition to the heat-related physiological problems that affect athletes’ health, notably heat stroke (Brotherhood, 2008), the thermal environment can have negative consequences on performance (Hue, 2011). During prolonged aerobic exercise, this stressor is known to negatively impact both physiological (e.g., Maughan et al., 2012) and psychological performances (e.g., Vasmatzidis et al., 2002). The literature proposes cooling strategies to deal with these deleterious effects of heat (Bongers et al., 2017). Regarding the psychological components, most studies are interested in the perceptual responses of heat stress with or without cooling intervention. However, the effects of these interventions on other psychological components such as cognitive abilities (e.g., reaction time, attention, executive function) or psychological states (i.e., mood, emotional, expectation, and motivational effects), and the use of psychological interventions, are little known, specifically in the tropical climate (i.e., TropC). This brief review deals with the available strategies to cope with the impact of hot and hot-wet climates on perceptual responses and cognitive abilities. We first focus on the effects of conventional strategies (i.e., training in the thermal conditions and cooling interventions). We then focus on alternative or complementary strategies (i.e., menthol ingestion techniques and mental techniques). We also aimed to study the impact of these different strategies on other psychological components that have not been examined so far (e.g., self-confidence, motivation, flow states). In addition to the theoretical interest, this article provides information to coaches on how best to prepare their athletes for the climate conditions of Tokyo 2020 but also for the upcoming events (e.g., 2022 FIFA World Cup Qatar). To establish this mini-review, citations from Pubmed and Sciencedirect were identified from the earliest record until April 2019 using the following search terms: hot climate, hot-wet climate, strategies, sport, performance, and psychology. Included studies required using adult participants (≥18 years) and ambient temperatures (≥35°C and ≤ 40% rH for studies conducted in hot climates and around 31°C and ≥ 50% rH for studies conducted in TropC). All studies without any link or involvement in competitive physical activities were excluded.

Impacts of Conventional Strategies to Deal With Thermal Stress

The main usual strategies to limit the negative effects of heat or TropC are training in the thermal stress and cooling interventions (for a review, see Bongers et al., 2017). For greater clarity, we distinguished the studies conducted in hot climate from those conducted in TropC and distinguished the factors related to perceptual responses from those related to cognitive abilities.

Training in Hot and Tropical Climate

In Hot Climate

Thermal adaptation encompasses physiological factors, behavioral adjustments, and psychological factors (Marialena and Koen, 2003). According to Pryor et al. (2019), heat acclimation in a hot environment is induced by repeated exercise-heat exposures that result in temporary physiological adaptations. This reduces thermal load and cardiovascular strain, and improves heat dissipation mechanisms during exercise. In addition, Taylor (2014) reviewed that the adaptations previously evoked can improve aerobic performance, enhance exercise-heat tolerance, and reduce the risk of exertional heat illness. Likewise, a series of studies investigated whether training in the same thermal conditions as the competition would enable athletes or soldiers to better adapt to heat stress (see Heathcote et al., 2018, for a review). Several studies showed an improved thermal comfort after acclimation (Sunderland et al., 2008; Costa et al., 2014). Recently, Malgoyre et al. (2018) compared the effects of a 15-day aerobic training program on soldiers in a hot-dry versus a temperate environment. They showed that the physiological modifications (e.g., rectal temperature) were mostly the same in the two environments but that thermal discomfort and the rating of perceived exertion (RPE) were much lower in the heat-training group than in the control group. Equivalent results were obtained for thermal comfort, sensation and perceived exertion in athletes prior to the Marathon des Sables following short-term heat acclimation (Willmott et al., 2017). Being accustomed to withstanding high training loads and high environmental stresses, athletes perceive the difficulty of an exercise as less onerous than the objective evaluation of it, especially compared to less well-accustomed athletes.

Several studies have shown the positive effects of thermal adaptation on cognitive abilities. Radakovic et al. (2007) examined the effects of exertional heat stress and acclimation status on the physiological and cognitive abilities of soldiers. The participants performed an exertional heat stress test in a cool environment, a hot environment to which they were not acclimatized, or a hot environment after undergoing 10 days of passive or active acclimation. The results showed that participants in the acclimatized group did not experience any adverse effects of heat stress, unlike the group of non-acclimatized participants who experienced a slight decrease in attention. Although different, these populations of athletes and soldiers may have commonalities in coping with a stressful environment (e.g., rigor, resilience, determination).

In Tropical Climate

The literature on the impact of acclimation to TropC on psychological factors is far sparser. Schmit et al. (2017), testing the impact of an 8-day training camp, with or without a cooling vest, on 13 triathletes who performed two 20-km cycling time-trials in TropC (35°C and 50% rH), found positive effects on thermal comfort. Then, it can be assumed that acclimation to the environment reduces the effects of heat stress and thus avoids the risk of cognitive disturbances. Thus, training in TropC may be one of the most important strategies to sensitize the delegations to Tokyo 2020. In this regard, Racinais et al. (2015) recommended repeated exercise-heat exposures over 1–2 weeks, as well as incentives for hydration during the athletes’ effort.

Cooling Interventions

Cooling interventions, such as cold-water immersion/ingestion or cooling garments, have been developed to prevent the physiological and psychological consequences of heat and TropC (for a review, see Jones et al., 2012; Bongers et al., 2017).

In Hot Climate

Ruddock et al. (2017) reviewed the effectiveness of cooling strategies during continuous exercise in heat. The results were from studies in a hot climate with lower relative humidity, but they are nonetheless interesting and indicate that cooling decreases the RPE and thermal perception during fixed-intensity exercise, which would improve endurance performance. Several studies have also found that head cooling decreases the RPE and thermal discomfort during hyperthermic exercise (e.g., Armada-da-Silva et al., 2004; Mündel et al., 2007). For example, Simmons et al. (2008) tested nine physically active, non-heat-acclimated volunteers. Participants performed two exercises (i.e., 12-min constant-load cycling tests at 70% VO2max) separated by a 90-min period of passive heating in two conditions: with or without head and face cooled. They showed that head cooling during passive heating reduced RPE and improved thermal comfort during subsequent exercise in the heat.

While cooling interventions in the heat appear to have positive effects on the RPE and thermal comfort, the benefits to cognitive abilities are less clear-cut. Their effectiveness seems limited and dependent on the timing of implementation (Schmit et al., 2017) and the type of task (Shibasaki et al., 2017). Along this line, Gaoua et al. (2011) assessed whether attention and memory task performance would be positively affected by cold pack application to the head during passive heating compared to passive heating without cold packs and a control situation. Head cooling had a more positive effect on working memory capacity, and rapid visual processing was no longer negatively impacted, which was the case for passive heating without the cold packs. However, head cooling showed no beneficial effect on pattern recognition memory. In another experiment with three conditions (hot, 50°C vs. hot, 50°C with cold packs vs. control, 20°C), Racinais et al. (2008) investigated whether cooling (i.e., cold packs to the head) would limit the alterations in motor training and cognitive function induced by the passive hyperthermia. They showed that cooling preserved memory capacity but not visual memory.

In Tropical Climate

Arngrimsson et al. (2004) showed that wearing a cooling vest during warm-up in TropC reduces thermal discomfort at the start of a 5-km run race compared to control condition, but this decrease fades after 3.2-km of running. That said, according to the authors, reducing perceived thermal discomfort and cardiovascular strain at the beginning of the race seems to play a role in improving performance in TropC (i.e., 32°C and 50% rH). Moreover, Cleary et al. (2014) evaluated the effectiveness of intermittent superficial cooling (i.e., 5-min of wearing a cooling vest) in 10 college students over 60-min of intense American football training in TropC. Thermal sensation was significantly decreased by this technique but not thirst, RPE, nor heat illness symptoms.

Regarding the impact of cooling strategies on cognitive abilities, Lee et al. (2014) evaluated the efficacy of neck cooling on cognitive task (i.e., symbol digit matching, search and memory, digit span, choice reaction time and psychomotor vigilance test), following exercise-induced hyperthermia. They showed that the neck-cooling collar seemed to enhance performance only in the high-complexity tasks (i.e., search and memory test). Ando et al. (2015) also examined the influence of neck-cooling interventions (with a wet towel and fanning) on cognitive functions during an exercise in TropC (35°C and 70% rH). They particularly focused on working memory and executive function in eight participants at rest and after cycling 10-min in both conditions (i.e., TropC vs. TropC+cooling intervention) but found that the cooling intervention in TropC did not effectively attenuate the impairment of either. Bandelow et al. (2010) used another cooling method to examine the cognitive effects of exercising (i.e., complex visuo-motor, fine motor speed, visual/auditory and visuo-spatial working memories) in TropC in a series of three matches between two soccer teams. The environmental conditions were about 34°C and 63% rH. The players sat under a canopy in 25°C temperature for 15-min before the match and for 10-min during the half-time break. Of the four cognitive functions assessed, only complex visuo-motor speed was improved.

Thus, some cooling interventions may not be efficient enough (e.g., Shibasaki et al., 2017) or may even be counterproductive. Indeed, internal cooling by ingesting large volumes of cold water or ice during exercise might cause gastrointestinal discomfort, allergy or cold-stimulus headache in some individuals (Stevens et al., 2013). Given the disadvantages of certain conventional interventions and the complexity of implementing them during sports competitions, other less invasive and easier to implement techniques should be considered to avoid the inconvenience of overly restrictive or embarrassing cooling interventions and yet maintain the psychological benefits.

Impacts of Alternative Strategies to Deal With Thermal Stress

In addition to the conventional interventions to deal with heat stress, other techniques can counter heat and TropC. Thus, these techniques may be used as alternatives or complements.

Menthol Ingestion Techniques to Create a Cooling Sensation

A series of studies has dealt with subjective body cooling through menthol. Although menthol does not lower the core or skin temperature of athletes (Barwood et al., 2015), it stimulates the cold receptors (Cheung, 2010) and induces a cool feeling (Mündel and Jones, 2010), which modifies thermal perceptions. Menthol-induced cold perception has been shown to increase exercise intensity and performance in hot climate (see Cheung, 2010; Mündel and Jones, 2010) and in TropC (e.g., Riera et al., 2014). For example, Stevens et al. (2016) examined the impacts of ice-slurry ingestion and menthol mouth rinse (25 mL of an L-menthol solution at 22°C at a concentration of 0.01% during 5 s) prior to an endurance running performance in the heat. Although ice-slurry reduced the core temperature, it neither decreased the thermal sensation during exercise nor improved the performance on a 5-km run. In contrast, the menthol mouth rinse improved the thermal sensation during exercise and the running performance. However, care is needed when using this technique. Indeed, a false thermal afferent signal, as when the actual temperature of the skin differs from the cold perception, can increase the risk of developing heat-related illnesses (see Valente et al., 2015), the brain being fooled by the menthol-induced cold signal. Moreover, a high menthol concentration could alter thermal perception to an extent that a hot deep body temperature could be ignored. This could lead to even more serious health risks such as heat stroke or hyperthermia, and subsequently to generated cerebral blood flow reduction, brain temperature increase or/and cerebral oxygenation compromising (Bain et al., 2015).

Although these results suggest that the modified perceptive signal is stronger than the physiological signal, it should be possible to find a technique that retains this perceptual advantage without the risk of a side effect on health. Other techniques, such as mental techniques, might avoid the downside of a menthol intervention while maintaining its psychological benefits for motivation and performance. These can be used to manage thermal stress and create a cold feeling that could help athletes to psychologically deal with the thermal environment.

Mental Techniques to Manage Thermal Stress

The techniques of mental preparation are useful to manage stress during competition, but it might be worthwhile to examine the classic mental preparation techniques to determine which ones would be most appropriate for coping with climate stressors. Many variables have been explored, including athletes’ resilience and adaptation (Fletcher and Sarkar, 2012). The question remaining: Which strategy is best for coping with climatic factors? In the field of sport and exercise, few studies have investigated psychological techniques in relation to ambient temperature. Barwood et al. (2008) tested whether a training package of four psychological skills (i.e., goal setting, arousal regulation, mental imagery, positive self-talk) would increase the distance covered during three maximal-effort runs of 90-min in the heat (30°C and 40% rH). They showed that the package lowered the temptation to reduce exercise intensity during the maximal-effort runs while increasing the distance covered by 8% in the last 90-min run. However, it was not possible to determine which of the skills had the beneficial influence. Wallace et al. (2016), therefore, used only one psychological skill (i.e., motivational self-talk) and showed that a 2-week intervention significantly improved endurance capacity and executive function in the heat. Similarly, it might also be interesting to examine how conventional mental interventions could be specifically adapted to coping with TropC. From this viewpoint and even though it has not been shown thus far, mindfulness might be an appropriate strategy. This technique has three components (awareness of current thoughts, emotions and bodily sensations; acceptance, which is a non-judgmental attitude toward one’s current thoughts, emotions, and bodily sensations; and commitment to goal-relevant attention focus and behavior) (Gardner and Moore, 2007; Thienot et al., 2014). Although the study did not take place in a particular climate condition, Haase et al. (2015) showed that a 7-week mindfulness intervention (2 full days+6 sessions of 90-min/week) changed the way high-level athletes treated interoceptive information and increased their ability to regulate the anxiety related to unpleasant feelings. This technique seems a more promising way to mentally prepare athletes for the Olympic and Paralympic Games in TropC.

Mental Techniques to Create a Cold Feeling

Few studies in the health field have examined the effect of mental techniques such as hypnosis on thermal factors. Langlade et al. (2002) investigated the influence of hypnotic suggestion and showed that heat detection and heat-pain thresholds were increased. Younus et al. (2003) studied the effect of hypnosis (4 × 1-h/week) on hot flashes in ten healthy volunteers and four breast cancer patients and showed that the frequency, duration, and severity of the hot flashes were significantly reduced. Elkins et al. (2013) obtained equivalent results. However, another question is whether this technique is also effective for coping with the discomfort of a hot and wet environment. For this reason, it would be interesting to examine whether an intervention in TropC consisting of hypnotic cold suggestion would have specific effects on psychological markers (i.e., thermal comfort, thermal sensation, affect) and the motivation to perform exercise compared with a control situation (in TropC with a neutral intervention). Finally, the scientific literature has not yet studied the effects of TropC and cooling strategies on other psychological factors like motivation, self-confidence or flow states. It would therefore be interesting to examine the effects on these psychological factors that are directly involved in performance. Likewise, no studies have compared high performance athletes with mid-level athletes in terms of their ability to cope with heat stress. Thus, it would be interesting to examine whether psychological components such as motivation, resilience or achievement goals allow high performance athletes to feel perceptions (e.g., thermal comfort, RPE) more bearable than mid-level athletes.

Conclusion

Although the conventional strategies are of interest for managing physiological responses during long-duration exercise (see Bongers et al., 2017), they have more relative effects on psychological factors, particularly in TropC. In addition to the disadvantages of implementing pre-cooling (cold-water immersion, cooling garments, cold packs applied to the head) or per-cooling (cold-water, ice-slurry, and/or menthol ingestion), there are also the psychological costs. Indeed, if the physiological benefits (central temperature decrement) are less than the psychological cost (increase in cognitive disturbance), the strategy in real performance situations will prove counterproductive particularly in highly demanding cognitive tasks.

The results concerning the efficacy of these techniques on cognitive abilities depend of the intervention used and the factor investigated. It seems very important to match the expected benefits of a particular technique (conventional or alternative) with the type of performance. Although all psychological factors are important in high-level practice, psychological needs differ across sports. On the one hand, precision sports (e.g., 50-m pistol, archery), team sports (e.g., football, basketball) and duel sports (e.g., tennis, fencing) will primarily require cognitive resources (e.g., attention, decision-making) through subjective intervention (e.g., cold suggestion), and the use of objective strategies (e.g., a cooling vest during timeouts) could be beneficial if they do not disturb the athlete. On the other hand, aerobic sports (e.g., the marathon, 50-km race walk) will mainly require motivational resources through training adaptation in TropC, the use of objective strategies (e.g., cooling equipment before the race), and the use of mental techniques (e.g., mindfulness intervention) should be beneficial.

Last, a parameter that deserves more study is inter-individual variability. Some high-level athletes may be much more supportive of certain strategies, whereas, others prefer one or a combination of different strategies. Not all athletes are inclined to drink crushed ice or take an ice bath before a race. Cooling techniques are not necessarily adapted to all sports modalities and all athletes. We need to focus on the strategy (not the method), which will depend on the severity of the environment and the athletes’ perceptions. As perceptions differ from one athlete to another, the psychological feelings are essential. This aspect is enhanced by the fact that the majority of the studies presented in this review are with weak samples and with mid-level athletes as there are few studies of high-level athletes related to the climate. For this reason, we encourage delegations to have their athletes train in TropC a few weeks before the Tokyo Games (i.e., acclimation training during preparation camp) and to arrive on site early enough to have the time to test strategies and find the most effective one in terms of perceptions (e.g., thermal comfort, RPE), cognitive abilities (e.g., reaction time, attention) and feelings and emotions (e.g., self-confidence, motivation or flow states). With this goal in mind, we also encourage delegations to consider easy-to-use alternative strategies to preserve the psychological benefits in TropC without constraining the athletes to cooling interventions that are potentially deleterious for their performance (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Practical implications for dealing with thermal stress.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the manuscript redaction, from the plan conception to the review of literature to the corrections.

Funding

Some of the results obtained from the laboratory experimentations have been partially funded by PO-Feder 2014–2020, “ACTE-APPLI”, n°215-FED-213.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Ando, S., Komiyama, T., Sudo, M., Kiyonaga, A., Tanaka, H., and Higaki, Y. (2015). The effects of temporal neck cooling on cognitive function during strenuous exercise in a hot environment: a pilot study. BMC Res. Notes 8:202. doi: 10.1186/s13104-015-1210-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armada-da-Silva, P. A. S., Woods, J., and Jones, D. A. (2004). The effect of passive heating and face cooling on perceived exertion during exercise in the heat. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 91, 656–663. doi: 10.1007/s00421-003-1006-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arngrimsson, S. A., Petitt, D. S., Stueck, M. G., Jorgensen, D. K., and Cureton, K. J. (2004). Cooling vest worn during active warm-up improves 5-km run performance in the heat. J. Appl. Physiol. 96, 1867–1874. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00979.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bain, A. R., Nybo, L., and Ainslie, P. N. (2015). Cerebral vascular control and metabolism in heat stress. Compr. Physiol. 5, 1345–1380. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c140066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandelow, S., Maughan, R., Shirreffs, S., Ozgunen, K., Kurdak, S., Erso, G., et al. (2010). The effects of exercise, heat, cooling and rehydration strategies on cognitive function in football players. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 20, 148–160. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01220.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barwood, M. J., Corbett, J., Thomas, K., and Twentyman, P. (2015). Relieving thermal discomfort: effects of sprayed L-menthol on perception, performance, and time trial cycling in the heat. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 25, 211–218. doi: 10.1111/sms.12395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barwood, M. J., Thelwell, R. C., and Tipton, M. J. (2008). Psychological skills training improves exercise performance in the heat. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 40, 387–396. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31815adf31

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bongers, C. W., Hopman, T. E., and Eijsvogels, T. M. (2017). Cooling interventions for athletes: an overview of effectiveness, physiological mechanisms, and practical considerations. Temperature 4, 60–78. doi: 10.1080/23328940.2016.1277003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brotherhood, J. R. (2008). Heat stress and strain in exercise and sport. J. Sci. Med. Sport 11, 6–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2007.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, S. S. (2010). Interconnections between thermal perception and exercise capacity in the heat. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 20, 53–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01209.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cleary, M. A., Toy, M. G., and Lopez, R. M. (2014). Thermoregulatory, cardiovascular, and perceptual responses to intermittent cooling during exercise in a hot, humid outdoor environment. J. Strength Cond. Res. 28, 792–806. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e3182a20f57

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, R. J., Crockford, M. J., Moore, J. P., and Walsh, N. P. (2014). Heat acclimation responses of an ultra-endurance running group preparing for hot desert-based competition. Eur J Sport Sci. 14, 131–141. doi: 10.1080/17461391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elkins, G. R., Fisher, W. I., Johnson, A. K., Carpenter, J. S., and Keith, T. Z. (2013). Clinical hypnosis in the treatment of post-menopausal hot flashes: a randomized controlled trial. Menopause 20, 291–298. doi: 10.1097/GME.0b013e31826ce3ed

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, D., and Sarkar, M. (2012). A grounded theory of psychological resilience in olympic champions. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 13, 669–678. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaoua, N., Racinais, S., Grantham, J., and El Massioui, F. (2011). Alterations in cognitive performance during passive hyperthermia are task dependent. Int. J. Hyperthermia. 27, 1–9. doi: 10.3109/02656736.2010.516305

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, F. L., and Moore, Z. E. (2007). The Psychology of Enhancing Human Performance: The Mindfulness-Acceptance-Commitment (MAC) Approach. New York, NY: Springer Publishing.

Google Scholar

Haase, L., May, A. C., Falahpour, M., Isakovic, S., Simmons, A. N., Hickman, S. D., et al. (2015). A pilot study investigating changes in neural processing after mindfulness training in elite athletes. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:229. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heathcote, S. L., Hassmén, P., Zhou, S., and Stevens, C. J. (2018). Passive heating: reviewing practical heat acclimation strategies for endurance athletes. Front. Physiol. 9:1851. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hue, O. (2011). The challenge of performing aerobic exercise in tropical environments: applied knowledge and perspectives. Int. J. Sports Physiol. Perform. 6, 443–454. doi: 10.1123/ijspp.6.4.443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, P. R., Barton, C., Morrissey, D., Maffulli, N., and Hemmings, S. (2012). Pre-cooling for endurance exercise performance in the heat: a systematic review. BMC Med. 10:166. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-10-166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langlade, A., Jussiau, C., Lamonerie, L., Marret, E., and Bonnet, F. (2002). Hypnosis increases heat detection and heat pain thresholds in healthy volunteers. Reg. Anesth. Pain. Med. 27, 43–46. doi: 10.1053/rapm.2002.29126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. K. W., Koh, A. C. H., Koh, S. X. T., Liu, G. J., Nio, A. Q., and Fan, P. W. (2014). Neck cooling and cognitive performance following exercise-induced hyperthermia. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 114, 375–384. doi: 10.1007/s00421-013-2774-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malgoyre, A., Tardo-Dino, P.-E., Koulmann, N., Lepetit, B., Jousseaume, L., and Charlot, K. (2018). Uncoupling psychological from physiological markers of heat acclimatization in a military context. J. Therm. Biol. 77, 145–156. doi: 10.1016/j.jtherbio.2018.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marialena, N., and Koen, S. (2003). Thermal comfort and psychological adaptation as a guide for designing urban spaces. Energ Build. 35, 95–101. doi: 10.1016/s0378-7788(02)00084-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsumoto, J., Fujibe, F., and Takahashi, H. (2017). Urban climate in the tokyo metropolitan area in Japan. J. Environ. Sci. 59, 54–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jes.2017.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maughan, R. J., Otani, H., and Watson, P. (2012). Influence of relative humidity on prolonged exercise capacity in a warm environment. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 112, 2313–2321. doi: 10.1007/s00421-011-2206-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohajerani, A., Bakaric, J., and Jeffrey-Bailey, T. (2017). The urban heat island effect, its causes, and mitigation, with reference to the thermal properties of asphalt concrete. J. Environ. Manage. 197, 522–538. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2017.03.095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mündel, T., Bunn, S. J., Hooper, P. L., and Jones, D. A. (2007). The effects of face cooling during hyperthermic exercise in man: evidence for an integrated thermal, neuroendocrine and behavioural response. Exp. Physiol. 92, 187–195. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2006.034934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mündel, T., and Jones, D. A. (2010). The effects of swilling an L(-)-menthol solution during exercise in the heat. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 109, 59–65. doi: 10.1007/s00421-009-1180-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, A. R., and Levy, A. R. (2016). The road to London 2012: the lived stressor, emotion, and coping experiences of gymnasts preparing for and competing at the world championships. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 14, 255–267. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2015.1020664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pryor, J. L., Pryor, R. R., Vandermark, L. W., Adams, E. L., VanScoy, R. M., Casa, D. J., et al. (2019). Intermittent exercise-heat exposures and intense physical activity sustain heat acclimation adaptations. JSAMS 22, 117–122. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2018.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racinais, S., Alonso, J. M., Coutts, A. J., Flouris, A. D., Girard, O., González-Alonso, J., et al. (2015). Consensus recommendations on training and competing in the heat. Br. J. Sports Med. 49, 1164–1173. doi: 10.1111/sms.12467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Racinais, S., Gaoua, N., and Grantham, J. (2008). Hyperthermia impairs short-term memory and peripheral motor drive transmission. J. Physiol. 586, 4751–4762. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2008.157420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radakovic, S. S., Maric, J., Surbatovic, M., Radjen, S., Stefanova, E., Stankovic, N., et al. (2007). Effects of acclimation on cognitive performance in soldiers during exertional heat stress. Mil. Med. 172, 133–136. doi: 10.7205/milmed.172.2.133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riera, F., Trong, T. T., Sinnapah, S., and Hue, O. (2014). Physical and perceptual cooling with beverages to increase cycle performance in a tropical climate. PLoS One 9:e103718. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruddock, A., Robbins, B., Tew, G., Bourke, L., and Purvis, A. (2017). Practical cooling strategies during continuous exercise in hot environments: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 47, 517–532. doi: 10.1007/s40279-016-0592-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmit, C., Le Meur, Y., Duffield, R., Robach, P., Oussedik, N., Coutts, A. J., et al. (2017). Heat-acclimatization and pre-cooling: a further boost for endurance performance? Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 27, 55–65. doi: 10.1111/sms.12629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shibasaki, M., Namba, M., Oshiro, M., Kakigi, R., and Nakata, H. (2017). Suppression of cognitive function in hyperthermia; from the viewpoint of executive and inhibitive cognitive processing. Sci. Rep. 7:43528. doi: 10.1038/srep43528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, S. E., Mündel, T., and Jones, D. A. (2008). The effects of passive heating and head-cooling on perception of exercise in the heat. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 104, 281–288. doi: 10.1007/s00421-007-0652-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C. J., Dascombe, B., Boyko, A., Sculley, D., and Callister, R. (2013). Ice slurry ingestion during cycling improves olympic distance triathlon performance in the heat. J. Sports Sci. 31, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2013.779740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C. J., Thoseby, B., Sculley, D. V., Callister, R., Taylor, L., and Dascombe, B. J. (2016). Running performance and thermal sensation in the heat are improved with menthol mouth rinse but not ice slurry ingestion. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 26, 1209–1216. doi: 10.1111/sms.12555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sunderland, C., Morris, J. G., and Nevill, M. E. (2008). A heat acclimation protocol for team sports. Br. J. Sports Med. 42, 327–333. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2007.034207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, N. A. (2014). Human heat adaptation. Compr. Physiol. 4, 325–365. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c130022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thienot, E., Jackson, B., Dimmock, J., Grove, J. R., Bernier, M., and Fournier, J. F. (2014). Development and preliminary validation of the mindfulness inventory for sport. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 15, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valente, A., Carrillo, A. E., Tzatzarakis, M. N., Vakonaki, E., Tsatsakis, A. M., Kenny, G. P., et al. (2015). The absorption and metabolism of a single L-menthol oral versus skin administration: effects on thermogenesis and metabolic rate. Food Chem. Toxicol. 86, 262–273. doi: 10.1016/j.fct.2015.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vasmatzidis, I., Schlegel, R., and Hancock, P. (2002). An investigation of heat stress effects on time sharing performance. Ergonomics 45, 218–239. doi: 10.1080/00140130210121941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallace, P. J., McKinlay, B. J., Coletta, N. A., Viaar, J. L., Taber, M. J., Wilson, P. M., et al. (2016). Effects of motivational self-talk on endurance and cognitive performance in the heat. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 49, 191–199. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willmott, A. G. B., Hayes, M., Waldock, K. A. M., Relf, R. L., Watkins, E. R., James, C. A., et al. (2017). Short-term heat acclimation prior to a multi-day desert ultra-marathon improves physiological and psychological responses without compromising immune status. J. Sports Sci. 35, 2249–2256. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2016.1265142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Younus, J., Simpson, I., Collins, A., and Wang, X. (2003). Mind control of menopause. Womens Health Issues 13, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/s1049-3867(02)00196-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive abilities, hot-wet climate, heat stress, cooling, thermal comfort, mental technique

Citation: Coudevylle GR, Sinnapah S, Robin N, Collado A and Hue O (2019) Conventional and Alternative Strategies to Cope With the Subtropical Climate of Tokyo 2020: Impacts on Psychological Factors of Performance. Front. Psychol. 10:1279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01279

Received: 04 December 2018; Accepted: 15 May 2019;
Published: 04 June 2019.

Edited by:

Toby Mündel, Massey University, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Martin Barwood, Leeds Trinity University, United Kingdom
Manabu Shibasaki, Nara Women’s University, Japan

Copyright © 2019 Coudevylle, Sinnapah, Robin, Collado and Hue. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Olivier Hue, T2xpdmllci5odWVAdW5pdi1hbnRpbGxlcy5mcg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.