Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 March 2019
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

The INventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits and Antisocial Behavior (INCA) for Young People: Development and Validation in a Community Sample

  • Research Center for Behavior Assessment, Department of Psychology, Universitat Rovira i Virgili, Tarragona, Spain

Callous-unemotional traits are defined as potential markers of psychopathy in children and adolescents. Previous studies with the most widely used instrument designed specifically to assess these traits, the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU), have shown major methodological problems. For this reason, the purpose of the present study was to develop a valid and reliable test to assess callous-unemotional traits for the adolescent population free of the response biases social desirability (SD) and acquiescence (AC). In order to obtain responses free of these biases, we used SD item markers as well as content balanced items to identify a factor related to SD and AC, so that SD and AC effects can be removed from the individual scores on content factors. As well as the CU traits (unemotional, callousness, and uncaring scales), this new questionnaire also contains an additional scale for assessing antisocial behaviors. The test was administered to 719 adolescents between 13 and 19 years old. Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis yielded the following expected four dimensions with a good fit: Unemotional, Callousness, Uncaring, and Antisocial Behavior. These scales also showed good psychometric properties with good reliability, and convergent, discriminant and criterion validity.

Introduction

Psychopathic behavior is a complex phenomenon that, together with aggressive behavior, raises noticeable concerns in society and is exacerbated by the spread of violent images in the media. For this reason, a considerable amount of research has been carried out to understand and describe psychopathy. However, there is still some debate about some issues related to psychopathy – for example, the relevance of genetic and environmental influences (e.g., Viding et al., 2005; Henry et al., 2016), the possible role of criminal behavior as a central component of psychopathy (Skeem and Cooke, 2010), or the structure of psychopathy (Blackburn, 2007) – but there is basic agreement on its most important emotional and behavioral characteristics (Hare et al., 2000). More specifically, in the area of interpersonal relationships, psychopaths can be defined as insensitive, arrogant, domineering, and manipulative; on the emotional level they show a lack of remorse and empathy, which makes it difficult for them to establish strong emotional relationships, and their lifestyle is characterized by impulsive and irresponsible behavior and a tendency to ignore and violate social norms (Abalos et al., 2004).

Psychopathy manifests at a relatively early age and it tends to be relatively stable throughout life (Lynam, 1996; Frick et al., 2003b). In fact, there is evidence that the origins of aggressive behavior can be traced back to preschool years (Loeber and Farrington, 2000), and psychopathic traits can emerge in children as young as 4 years old (Dadds et al., 2005). However, its biological basis and the environmental factors involved in its development and which favor its relative stability throughout life are still not well understood (Frick et al., 2003b).

Psychopathy in children and adolescents has traditionally been studied using the approach of callous-unemotional (CU) traits. These traits are the potential markers that might be the precursors to the development of psychopathy: lack of empathy, lack of guilt and regret, the manipulation of others, irresponsible attitude regarding self-performance, and poverty of emotional expression (Frick, 2004). Children with high levels of CU traits are deficient in emotional empathy but not in cognitive empathy, as they seem to understand the perspective of the others but they do not emotionally resonate with their feelings (Waller et al., 2015; Waller and Hyde, 2018). Moreover, many studies have found that CU traits are related to a range of dysfunctional behaviors in adolescents. For instance, young people with higher scores in CU tend to show greater antisocial behavior and repeated behavior problems (Frick et al., 2003a, 2005). In this regard, the latest version of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) includes CU traits as severity markers in the diagnostic criteria for Conduct Disorder. Youngsters with CU traits also show higher levels of criminal behavior involving aggression and sexual violence (Caputo et al., 1999; Frick and White, 2008), and higher levels of premeditated, instrumental violence (Frick et al., 2003a; Pardini et al., 2003). Moreover, higher levels of delinquency related to substance abuse are observed in these young people (Taylor and Lang, 2006). The origin of these antisocial behaviors shown by adolescents with CU traits may be related to deficits in processing negative emotional stimuli (Blair, 1999; Blair et al., 2001; Kimonis et al., 2006). In fact, they are less inhibited by fear and anxiety (Frick et al., 1999; Lynam et al., 2005) and less affected by threat of punishment (Fisher and Blair, 1998; Barry et al., 2000). All these studies show the relevance of CU traits and how important it is that they are assessed reliably.

In 2004, Paul J. Frick developed the 24-item Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU), on the basis of the four items of the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD; Frick and Hare, 2001) which loaded consistently on the CU scale. Although there was another instrument with a CU scale, the Youth Psychopathic trait Inventory (YPI; Andershed et al., 2002), the ICU is the only questionnaire that specifically assesses CU traits.

Despite the significance of CU traits and the importance of assessing them, many methodological issues affect the development of this test. As far as the factor structure is concerned, it should be taken into account that Frick (2004) developed the ICU to assess four different facets of psychopathy: Careless, Uncaring, Unemotional, and Callous behaviors. However, he did not provide any evidence about the dimensionality of the instrument. Several authors have carried out studies on the factor structure of the ICU, most of which have found a three-factor structure consisting of Unemotional, Callousness, and Uncaring traits (Essau et al., 2006; Kimonis et al., 2008, 2013; Fanti et al., 2009; Roose et al., 2010; Ciucci et al., 2014; López-Romero et al., 2015), but this factor structure presents several problems. In fact, the exploratory factor analyses were done using Pearson correlation coefficients instead of polychoric correlations, which are more advisable with Likert-type items (Muthen and Kaplan, 1985), and most of them used procedures such as the scree test to determine the number of factors to retain instead of more appropriate methods such as parallel analysis. In some cases, however, (for example, the study by Feilhauer et al., 2012), principal component analysis was used instead of factor analysis. The confirmatory analyses carried out by various studies show no clear consensus, and many of them reached a marginal fit only after applying such highly undesirable procedures as allowing a high number of error terms to correlate after the modification indexes had been inspected (Essau et al., 2006; Houghton et al., 2013; Ciucci et al., 2014), or some items (Kimonis et al., 2008; López-Romero et al., 2015) or even scales have been removed (Feilhauer et al., 2012; Houghton et al., 2013). These “ad hoc” procedures often lack substantive and theoretical foundation, and are likely to capitalize on chance (Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva, 2000).

There are also some doubts about the content of the scales found by the factor analyses of most studies (e.g., Essau et al., 2006; Fanti et al., 2009; Ciucci et al., 2014; López-Romero et al., 2015), because some of the items in a specific scale seem to be related to a different scale. For instance, the Uncaring scale contains some items that seem to be more related to callousness, because they refer to a lack of empathy or remorse (i.e., I feel bad or guilty when I do something wrong, or I try not to hurt others’ feelings). In fact, some items in the Callousness scale also refer to a lack of empathy or remorse, like those other items in the Uncaring scale (i.e., I do not feel remorseful when I do something wrong, or I am concerned about the feelings of others). Likewise, the Callousness scale also contains some items that seem to be more related to uncaring, because they refer to a lack of responsibility (i.e., I do not care about doing things well, or I do not care about being on time). Also, some items in the Uncaring scale refer to responsibility (i.e., I work hard on everything I do, or I care about how well I do at school or work).

Other studies have proposed a different factorial structure. For instance, Houghton et al. (2013) proposed that fit was best using a two-factor model and removing the items of the Unemotional scale, while others, such as Feilhauer et al. (2012), failed to confirm the previously reported factorial structures and proposed a new exploratory analysis, which yielded a five-factor structure. Taking all this into account, these factorial structure problems seem to indicate that the three-factor structure proposed initially by Essau et al. (2006) may not be suitable, which may also explain why in many cases researchers have used only the ICU’s overall score instead of the scores of specific scales (e.g., Stickle et al., 2009; Viding et al., 2009; White et al., 2009).

We also have to take into account that some characteristic features of psychopathy, such as manipulating others for one’s own ends, are considered to be socially undesirable. For this reason, like other personality traits such as aggressiveness, the items that assess this construct are expected to be deeply impacted by social desirability (SD) (Rogers et al., 2002; Vigil-Colet et al., 2012). Several studies show that controlling acquiescence (AC) in personality questionnaires provides a simpler and more congruent factor structure (Rammstedt and Farmer, 2013; Navarro-González et al., 2016; Morales-Vives et al., 2017). Therefore, response bias control is highly recommended for assessing these kinds of construct, although the ICU test does not control any response bias.

Since CU traits play a major role in the prediction of a wide range of antisocial and problematic behaviors in adolescents and given the scarcity of the measures available and their problems, the main purpose of the present study is to develop a new CU traits questionnaire for the adolescent population free of the response biases SD and AC. As well as the CU traits (unemotional, callousness, and uncaring scales), which are also included in the questionnaire ICU, the new questionnaire also contains an additional scale for assessing antisocial behaviors (challenge to authority and breaking social rules). This scale was included because, according to Hare et al. (2000), one of the key aspects that defines the psychopath is an unstable and antisocial lifestyle. Moreover, including this scale means that the same instrument has a measure of the precursors of psychopathy as well as how they lead to dysfunctional and detrimental behaviors in society, which can be very useful in fields such as forensic, education, etc.

Materials and Methods

Development of the INventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits and Antisocial Behavior (INCA)

We wanted the INCA to assess four correlated traits: Unemotional (UE), Callousness (CA), Uncaring (UC), and Antisocial Behavior (AB). Unemotional refers to deficient emotional affect, which implies a lack of emotional expression. Callousness is defined as the lack of empathy and sensitivity to the needs and suffering of others, and involves a tendency to manipulate others for one’s own benefit, feeling little guilt and remorse when others are harmed. Uncaring refers to a lack of responsibility and effort, with a tendency to be lax about one’s own obligations and duties (for example not respecting deadlines or not finishing tasks). Antisocial Behavior refers to the violation of norms and social rules, challenging authority, engaging in behaviors which are illegal or breaking social rules which may harm others or the community.

We wrote an initial pool of 72 items, which was assessed by external judges with experience in personality test development and adolescents. These judges were asked to indicate, for each item, if the content was suitable for assessing the corresponding dimension, if the statement was clear, if the length of the item was adequate and if the response format was compatible with the item statement. The items considered by the external judges to be the most appropriate were administered to 244 adolescents in a pilot study to determine whether they were clear and easy to understand. In this pilot study, those items with loadings lower than λ = 0.30 or with complex loadings (greater than λ = 0.30 on more than one content factor) were removed. The resulting version had 38 content items, plus four markers of SD and a dummy item that we included as the first item of the test so that it could be used as a training item in computer administrations (Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva, 2005). Therefore, the final version of the test includes a total of 43 items. Item responses are made using a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). Moreover, the test is content balanced (half of the items measured in one direction of the trait whereas others measured in the opposite direction). The use of SD markers and reversed items allowed us to implement the procedure developed by Ferrando et al. (2009) and Lorenzo-Seva and Ferrando (2009) to control for potential response biases of SD and AC, which has been successfully implemented in several questionnaires of personality and psychological maturity (Morales-Vives et al., 2013; Vigil-Colet et al., 2013; Ruiz-Pamies et al., 2014). This procedure consists of using SD item markers as well as content balanced items to identify a factor related to SD and AC, so that SD and AC effects can be removed from the individual scores on content factors. More specifically, this procedure has three main steps. The first one consists of identifying a factor related to SD, using the set of markers of SD. The inter-marker correlation matrix is analyzed using factor analysis to obtain the corresponding loading values of each marker in the SD factor. These loading values are then used to compute the loading values of the content items in the SD factor using the Instrumental Variables Technique (Hägglund, 1982), and the variance explained by the SD factor is removed from the inter-item correlation matrix. The second step consists on identifying a factor related to AC. The residual inter-item correlation matrix obtained from the previous step is analyzed to remove the variance due to acquiescent responding from the content items using a totally or partially balanced scale (Lorenzo-Seva and Ferrando, 2009). Finally, the resultant residual inter-item correlation matrix is analyzed using factor analysis to identify the content variables of interest.

Participants

The participants were 719 adolescents (318 males and 401 females) aged between 13 and 19 years old (M = 15.22; SD = 1.62) from four high schools in the province of Tarragona (Spain).

Measures

In addition to the INCA, the following questionnaires were used to assess convergent and discriminant validity:

The Inventory of Callous Unemotional Traits (ICU; Frick, 2004)

The Inventory of Callous Unemotional traits (ICU; Frick, 2004), a questionnaire specifically designed to evaluate precursors of psychopathy in youth populations through three dimensions: Callousness (CA), Uncaring (UC), and Unemotional (UE) traits. We used the self-report Spanish adaptation of the ICU developed by López-Romero et al. (2015), which consists of 24 items with a 4-point response format (0 = never/almost never; 3 = always/almost always) with internal consistencies of α = 0.76, α = 0.82, and α = 0.78 for CA, UC, and UE, respectively.

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 for Children (BIS-11c; Chahin et al., 2010)

The BIS-11c consists of 30 items with a 4-point response format (0 = never/almost never; 3 = always/almost always). The questionnaire measures motor (MI), non-planning (N-PI), and cognitive (CI) impulsivity and shows internal consistencies of α = 0.80, α = 0.73, and α = 0.68, respectively.

The Overall Personality Assessment Scale (OPERAS; Vigil-Colet et al., 2013)

This 40-item test provides SD- and AC-free scores with good reliability and temporal stability for the factors Extraversion (EX; ρθθ = 0.86; rtt = 0.70), Emotional Stability (ES; ρθθ= 0.86; rtt = 0.70), Conscientiousness (CO; ρθθ= 0.77; rtt = 0.75), Agreeableness (AG; ρθθ= 0.71; rtt = 0.73), and Openness to experience (OE; ρθθ= 0.81; rtt = 0.79). Temporal stability was calculated from the correlations between the scores obtained by a sample at two different moments in time, 4 weeks apart.

Procedure

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Spanish organic law 15/1999 and the Spanish Agency for Data Protection, which regulate the fundamental right to the protection of data. This project and the protocol was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Faculty of Educational Sciences and Psychology of the Universitat Rovira i Virgili. Moreover, we obtained the parental written informed consent from all subjects. All parents gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

The questionnaires were administered collectively during regular school hours, in groups of 15–30 students, by professional psychologists. Students were guaranteed anonymity and confidentiality, and participation was voluntary. School approval and parental written informed consent were obtained before the study.

Data Analysis

The sample of 719 participants was split into two equally representative subsamples of the same population (i.e., all possible sources of variance are enclosed in both subsamples) using the DUPLEX algorithm (Snee, 1977). We computed an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with the first sample and a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) with the second (Ferrando and Lorenzo-Seva, 2000). As both analyses led to the same conclusions, a final EFA was carried out with the overall sample to obtain the factorial weights required to compute the participants’ factor scores. These factor analyses were carried out using Psychological Test Toolbox (Navarro et al., in press) and LISREL 8.5 (Jorkeskog and Sorbom, 2001). The structural model was performed using LISREL 9 (Jorkeskog and Sorbom, 2001). We also used SPSS 23.0 to compute sex differences and the convergent and discriminant validity of the INCA questionnaire.

Results

Exploratory Factor Analysis

The polychoric correlation matrix was computed for the first sample using 42 items (the first item in the test was excluded from the analyses because it is a dummy item). We used polychoric correlations because they are more suitable for Likert-type scales, and because the skewness or kurtosis of some items was greater than one in absolute value. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO; Kaiser, 1974) index value was 0.86, which indicates that the correlation matrix is suitable for factor analysis.

We computed Optimal Implementation of Parallel Analysis (Timmerman and Lorenzo-Seva, 2011) using 500 random datasets. In this implementation of Parallel Analysis, the decision of the optimal number of factors to be extracted is not based on the eigenvalues themselves, but on the proportion of explained common variance related to the eigenvalues. The results suggested that, as expected, the data had four underlying factors.

We applied the procedure proposed by Ferrando et al. (2009) explained above to determine the response bias factors. Four factors were retained using unweighted least squares (ULS) and an oblique rotation, more specifically a Promin rotation (Lorenzo-Seva, 1999), because we expected the factors to be correlated. We obtained the four content factors that we expected: Unemotional, Callousness, Uncaring, and Antisocial Behavior.

To assess the fit of the rotated loading matrix, the congruence index (Tucker, 1951) was computed between the rotated loading matrix and the ideal loading matrix (i.e., a loading matrix where, for each item, a single loading is one and the other loadings are zero). The congruence values ranged between 0.88 and 0.98. As the coefficients were all above the threshold of 0.85, the factor similarity between the rotated loading matrix and the ideal loading matrix was fair (Lorenzo-Seva and Ten Berge, 2006). We also computed the Goodness of Fit Index (GFI) and the Root Mean Square of Residuals (RMSR), obtaining values of 0.95 and 0.054, respectively. As a GFI equal or higher than 0.95 is considered to be good and Kelly’s criterion indicates that the expected mean value of RMSR for an acceptable model is 0.053, we can conclude that both indices show that the model has a good fit.

Finally, in order to evaluate the simplicity of the factor solution, Bentler’s Simplicity index (S; Bentler, 1977) and the Loading Simplicity index (LS; Lorenzo-Seva, 2003) were computed. The values of the S index and the LS index were 0.99 and 0.60, respectively. These values suggest that the factor simplicity is high, which suggests that each item is mainly related to only one dimension.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

We performed a CFA to examine the replicability of the factor structure obtained in the first sample. First the variance due to SD and AC was partialized out following the procedure proposed by Ferrando et al. (2009). ULSs estimates were computed from the residual covariance. It was proposed that the model should retain four correlated factors, as the EFA explained above suggested. The ideal pattern matrix proposed was also the one expected, as in the exploratory analysis.

The values obtained for the fit indexes were CFI = 0.99, GFI = 0.97 and RMSEA = 0.033, so the data seemed to show an acceptable fit to the proposed model. Chi-square test was significant, χ2(565) = 1433.85, p < 0.01. However, this statistic is very sensitive to sample size, so it cannot be relied upon as a basis for acceptance or rejection (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1988). The chi-square to degrees of freedom ratio was 2.5. Taking into account that values lower than 2 indicate an excellent fit and values lower than 3 indicate an acceptable fit, our result suggests an acceptable fit (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1988).

Final Exploratory Factor Analysis

As both EFA and CFA led to similar conclusions, we used the whole sample (N = 719) to estimate the factor loadings and the weights to estimate the participants’ factor scores. We repeated the same analysis as in EFA, but with all the participants so that the sample was the largest possible and the estimates of the weights used to compute individual factor scores were the best possible. The KMO value of 0.88 again indicated that the correlation matrix was suitable for the factor analysis. The GFI index was now 0.95 and the value of RMSR was 0.049. The congruence index values ranged between 0.91 and 0.98. Finally, S and LS were 0.99 and 0.66, respectively. The values obtained in the final factor analysis indicated a good fit to the model.

Table 1 shows the loading values after rotation on both the content scales and the control scales (SD and AC). As can be seen, several items loaded more than λ = 0.30 on the AC factor, two items had loadings greater than λ = 0.30 and six had loadings greater than λ = 0.20 on the SD factor, which justifies using methods to control response biases when assessing these traits. In fact, controlling these biases allowed us to obtain the loadings of the items on the four content factors free of SD and AC (see Table 1). Moreover, as can be seen in this table, each item loads on the expected factor and has low loadings on the other factors. Table 2 shows the interfactor correlation matrix. As can be seen, correlation values among content factors ranged from r = 0.02 to r = 0.25.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Loading matrix obtained in the final factor analysis, factor reliabilities and congruence with expected factor solution.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Interfactor correlation matrix.

Item Analyses

We computed the descriptive statistics and discrimination indices for the 42 items. The means ranged between 1.51 and 4.26, and the standard deviations between 0.78 and 2.47. Discrimination indices reached values higher than 0.14 with a maximum of 0.65, which indicates that the items of each INCA subscale are correlated with each other.

Scale Analyses

Likewise, we computed the reliability estimates on the basis of the factor scores for each scale (see, e.g., Mellenbergh, 1994). As can be seen in the penultimate line of Table 1, the reliabilities for the content subscales ranged between ρθθ = 0.90 and ρθθ = 0.96, which indicates a good reliability for the factor scores.

Regarding the convergent and discriminant validity of INCA, Table 3 shows the correlations between the INCA factor score estimates and the ICU, BIS-11c, and OPERAS questionnaires. It should be taken into account that, according to Cohen (1988), correlations of 0.50 or higher have a large effect size, correlations around 0.30 have a medium effect size and correlations around 0.10 have a small effect size. As can be seen, the unemotional, callousness and uncaring scores of INCA have the highest correlation with their corresponding ICU scales. The antisocial behavior subscale presents moderate correlations with INCA’s callousness and uncaring scores. Moreover, as expected, there are significant correlations between the INCA scores and impulsivity. More specifically, the INCA factor scores that are most related to impulsivity are uncaring and antisocial behavior, which are positively correlated with motor and non-planning impulsivity of BIS-11c, while uncaring is negatively correlated with cognitive impulsivity. In addition, callousness has a positive although small correlation with motor impulsivity. There are also significant correlations with some personality traits of the test OPERAS. More specifically, callousness and antisocial behavior factor scores are negatively correlated with the traits conscientiousness and agreeableness. Callousness also has a significant negative correlation with openness to experience.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients and structural validity coefficients between INCA, ICU, BIS-11c, and OPERAS.

Uncaring scores have a negative correlation with emotional stability and openness to experience, but these correlations have a small effect size because they are lower than 0.30. Uncaring also has a negative correlation with conscientiousness, and a large effect size because this correlation is higher than 0.50. Finally, the unemotional factor scores have a negative correlation only with extraversion, and a medium effect size because this correlation is lower than 0.50.

In order to improve the raw validity estimates described above, we also extended the measurement model into a full structural model that includes the relations between the factors and the relevant external variables (the subscales of the questionnaires ICU, BIS-11c, and OPERAS). Given the complexity and size of the full model, however, we decided to include these additional variables as single indicators. In this setting, and provided that the unique parts of the original items are uncorrelated with the additional variables (which is expected because they do not share specific content), the relative fit of the full structural model is expected to be the same as that of the measurement model, and that is the result we obtained. However, the validity path estimates can now be interpreted as disattenuated correlations in which the measurement errors of both the factor score estimates and the relevant related variables have been corrected. Therefore, Table 3 provides both the uncorrected validity coefficients and the structural validity coefficients (free from measurement error). Moreover, Figure 1 shows the structural part of the model in which the latent variables or factors are related to the external variables derived from ICU, BIS-11c, and OPERAS. However, for the sake of clarity, only the weights that can be considered as substantial are shown in the figure. Otherwise, the graph of the structural sub-model would be too complex to be clearly interpreted. Given that the structural weights in this case can be essentially interpreted as product-moment correlations, we have considered the 0.30 to be a reasonable threshold for considering a weight to be substantial. This choice stems from the fact that the effect size for a correlation coefficient is the correlation itself, and, according to Cohen (1988), effect sizes below 0.30 should be considered as small.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Structural part of the model with the substantial relations between the factors and the external variables from ICU, BIS-1lc, and OPERAS.

Table 4 shows the means of the factor scores (on a T scale metric) in boys and girls. Because the individual measurement errors of the factor score estimates are expected to cancel, the difference tests on Table 4 can be regarded as structural comparisons based on latent means. It should be pointed out that the direct structural estimation of the latent mean differences would have been a theoretically superior to the procedure that we used here. However, this approach would have involved fitting a very complex multiple-group model likely to lead to unstable results. So, we believe that our chosen approach is a good trade-off that, as discussed above, is expected to provide consistent results. As can be seen, there were significant sex differences only for callousness, with lower scores for women [t(716)= -5.46; p < 0.01] and a medium effect size (d = 0.41).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean scores for men and women on INCA scales and effect sizes.

Discussion

The results presented above indicate that the INCA has suitable psychometric properties, good reliability, the expected factor structure and good validity coefficients. The traditional CU scales measured by the INCA are similar to those proposed by the ICU, but their content shows a clear and replicable factor structure, as is shown by the fact that the same factor structure has been found in two different subsamples. Furthermore, all scales provide scores that are free of SD and AC response biases. As we mentioned in the introduction, this kind of scale is often affected by SD, and the loadings of some items on SD justified controlling this bias. On the other hand, several studies have shown that controlling AC makes the factor structure simpler and more congruent (Rammstedt and Farmer, 2013; Navarro-González et al., 2016; Morales-Vives et al., 2017), which is the case of INCA. In addition, since these results were obtained when the test was administered anonymously, we assume that when the test is not anonymous, such as in forensics (penitentiary centers, social services, etc.), adolescents would show a greater SD and hide their psychopathic traits even more. Therefore, it would be particularly advisable to control response biases in these situations.

INCA also has good convergent and discriminant validity. In fact, the test’s subscales had the expected relationships with the measures of several constructs traditionally linked with psychopathy, such as impulsivity and agreeableness. Our findings support the associations between high levels of CU traits and impulsivity found in previous studies (Roose et al., 2010; López-Romero et al., 2015). These associations might be explained by the lack of inhibition common in the aggressive and impulsive behaviors that psychopaths usually display. The criteria validity of the INCA was also supported by its relationships with the Big Five personality traits. As previous studies have pointed out, conscientiousness and agreeableness were the dimensions that were most related to CU traits (Miller and Lynam, 2001; Lynam et al., 2005; Essau et al., 2006). More specifically, there was a negative correlation between agreeableness and CA and between conscientiousness and UC. The negative relationship between conscientiousness and UC was expected, because UC involves a lack of responsibility and effort, which implies a low level of conscientiousness. Therefore, young people with higher UC traits tend to show irresponsible behavior characterized by lack of perseverance, disorganization and poor work orientation. Likewise, we also expected to find a negative relationship between agreeableness and CA, because both dimensions share empathy as the key feature. This implies that adolescents with higher levels of CA are less nice and considerate with others. Another noteworthy result is the negative relationship between the OE scale and the UC traits, specifically CA and UC. These findings are similar to those of Duran-Bonavila et al. (2017), in which juvenile delinquents and young people at risk of social exclusion scored lower than the community sample on OE, probably because this personality trait is related to intellectual curiosity and cultural interests which may be less stimulated in the marginal environments of society.

Previous studies have obtained contradictory results in terms of sex differences in CU traits. In fact, while some studies found higher scores in boys for CA, UC, and UE traits (Essau et al., 2006; Fanti et al., 2009), others only found higher scores in boys for UC and UE traits (Ciucci et al., 2014). Houghton et al. (2013) did not report significant differences between boys and girls in a sample of community children. We only found significant sex differences in the CA subscale, with higher scores in boys, which is congruent with the studies by Essau et al. (2006) and Fanti et al. (2009). This may be because CA is strongly associated with low AG (Miller and Lynam, 2001; Lynam et al., 2005; Essau et al., 2006), a dimension in which women tend to score higher than men (Schmitt et al., 2008). Further studies are needed to elucidate which of the CU traits are most affected by sex differences.

It should be mentioned that the INCA scores have to be interpreted as factor scores, not as raw additions of individuals’ answers to the items (raw scores). To compute factor scores, the individuals’ answers to items must be standardized by using the means and standard deviations of the items, and the standardized responses must be added as a weighted addition. Finally, the total addition should be transformed from typical scores to T scores (i.e., mean 50 and standard deviation 10). In addition, normalized percentiles should be computed as part of a proper psychological report. Although this procedure is not complex, it is not yet straightforward for applied psychologists. To solve this drawback, we have developed a public internet application that applied psychologists can use to obtain factor scores and normalize percentiles. It is free share software available at http://psico.fcep.urv.cat/links/INCAS_CORRECTOR.zip.

This study has some limitations that should be addressed in future research. In fact, it is based only on a community sample, so future studies should analyze the results in samples with high levels of antisocial behavior, such as juvenile offenders. This kind of sample would also be particularly useful in providing information about the predictive validity of the INCA to identify young people who might have severe behavioral and psychosocial maladjustment. Moreover, further studies should be carried out in order to assess the correlations between some specific facets of the Big Five, such as warmth, positive emotions, assertiveness or excitement, and the dimensions of the test.

Despite these limitations, the study provides evidence about the psychometric properties of INCA, and shows that this instrument is a useful measure of CU traits in adolescents. In this regard, INCA might be especially valuable in assessment as well as in the prevention and intervention programs of some applied fields such as forensics, education and mental health.

Author Contributions

FM-V contributed to the design of the study and collection of data, carried out most of the statistical analyses, supervised the research and revised it critically, wrote part of the article, and provided the final approval of the version to be published. SC contributed to the collection of data and contacted with some centers and wrote part of the article. UL-S contributed to the design of the study, carried out part of the statistical analyses, and revised the research critically. AV-C formulated the research question, was responsible for the statistical design of the study, supervised the research and revising it critically, and provided the final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

This research was supported by a grant from the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitivity (PSI2017-82307-P) and a grant from the Catalan Ministry of Universities, Research and the Information Society (2017 SGR 97).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abalos, C., Esquivel, N., and Gallardo, C. (2004). Adaptación de la Escala de Calificación de la Psicopatía Revisada (PCL-R) de Robert Hare en Población Reclusa del Centro de Detención Preventiva de San Miguel. Santiago: Universidad de Chile, 1–168.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andershed, H., Kerr, M., Stattin, H., and Levander, S. (2002). “Psychopathic traits in non-referred youths: initial test of a new assessment tool,” in Psychopaths: Current International Perspectives, eds E. Blau and L. Sheridan (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 131–158.

Barry, C. T., Frick, P. J., DeShazo, T. M., McCoy, M., Ellis, M., and Loney, B. R. (2000). The importance of callous-unemotional traits for extending the concept of psychopathy to children. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 109, 335–340. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1977). Factor simplicity index and transformations. Psychometrika 42, 277–295. doi: 10.1007/BF02294054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackburn, R. (2007). Personality disorder and antisocial deviance: comments on the debate on the structure of the psychopathy checklist-revised. J. Pers. Disord. 21, 142–159. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2007.21.2.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R. (1999). Responsiveness to distress cues in the child with psychopathic tendencies. Pers. Individ. Dif. 27, 135–145. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00231-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, R. J. R., Colledge, E., Murray, L., and Mitchell, D. G. V. (2001). A selective impairment in the processing of sad and fearful expressions in children with psychopathic tendencies. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 29, 491–498. doi: 10.1023/A:1012225108281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caputo, A. A., Frick, P. J., and Brodsky, S. L. (1999). Family violence and juvenile sex offending. Crim. Justice Behav. 26, 338–356. doi: 10.1177/0093854899026003004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chahin, N., Cosi, S., Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Vigil-Colet, A. (2010). Stability of the factor structure of Barrat’s Impulsivity Scales for children across cultures: a comparison of Spain and Colombia. Psicothema 22, 983–989.

Google Scholar

Ciucci, E., Baroncelli, A., Franchi, M., Golmaryami, F. N., and Frick, P. J. (2014). The association between callous-unemotional traits and behavioral and academic adjustment in children: further validation of the inventory of callous-unemotional traits. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 36, 189–200. doi: 10.1007/s10862-013-9384-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Google Scholar

Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., and Hawes, D. J. (2005). Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: a community study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 73, 400–410. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duran-Bonavila, S., Vigil-Colet, A., Cosi, S., and Morales-Vives, F. (2017). How individual and contextual factors affects antisocial and delinquent behaviors: a comparison between young offenders, adolescents at risk of social exclusion and a community sample. Front. Psychol. 8:1825. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Essau, C. A., Sasagawa, S., and Frick, P. J. (2006). Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment 13, 454–469. doi: 10.1177/1073191106287354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanti, K. A., Frick, P. J., and Georgiou, S. (2009). Linking callous-unemotional traits to instrumental and non-instrumental forms of aggression. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 31, 285–298. doi: 10.1007/s10862-008-9111-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feilhauer, J., Cima, M., and Arntz, A. (2012). Assessing callous-unemotional traits across different groups of youths: further cross-cultural validation of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 35, 251–262. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2012.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrando, P. J., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2000). Unrestricted versus restricted factor analysis of multidimensional test items: some aspects of the problem and some suggestions. Psicológica 21, 301–323.

Google Scholar

Ferrando, P. J., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2005). IRT-related factor analytic procedures for testing the equivalence of paper-and-pencil and Internet-administered questionnaires. Psychol. Methods 10, 193–205. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.2.193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrando, P. J., Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Chico, E. (2009). A general factor-analytic procedure for assessing response bias in questionnaire measures. Struct. Equ. Model. 16, 364–381. doi: 10.1080/10705510902751374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, L., and Blair, R. J. R. (1998). Cognitive impairment and its relationship to psychopathic tendencies in children with emotional and behavioral difficulties. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 26, 511–519. doi: 10.1023/A:1022655919743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J. (2004). The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits. New Orleans, LA: University of New Orleans.

Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., and Dane, H. E. (2003a). Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 31, 457–470. doi: 10.1023/A:1023899703866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., Kimonis, E. R., Dandreaux, D. M., and Farell, J. M. (2003b). The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non-referred youth. Behav. Sci. Law 21, 713–736. doi: 10.1002/bsl.568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., and Hare, R. (2001). Antisocial Process Screening Device: APSD. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.

Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., Lilienfeld, S. O., Ellis, M., Loney, B., and Silverthorn, P. (1999). The association between anxiety and psychopathy dimensions in children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 27, 383–392. doi: 10.1023/A:1021928018403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., Stickle, T. R., Dandreaux, D. M., Farrell, J. M., and Kimonis, E. R. (2005). Callous-unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 471–487. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-5728-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frick, P. J., and White, S. F. (2008). Research Review: the importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01862.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hägglund, G. (1982). Factor analysis by instrumental variable methods. Psychometrika 47, 209–222. doi: 10.1007/BF02296276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hare, R. D., Clark, D., Grann, M., and Thornton, D. (2000). Psychopathy and the predictive validity of the PCL-R: an international perspective. Behav. Sci. Law 18, 623–645. doi: 10.1002/1099-0798(200010)18:5<623::AID-BSL409>3.0.CO;2-W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henry, J., Pingault, J. B., Boivin, M., Rijsdijk, F., and Viding, E. (2016). Genetic and environmental aetiology of the dimensions of Callous-Unemotional traits. Psychol. Med. 46, 405–414. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715001919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houghton, S., Hunter, S. C., and Crow, J. (2013). Assessing callous unemotional traits in children aged 7- to 12-years: a confirmatory factor analysis of the inventory of callous unemotional traits. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 35, 215–222. doi: 10.1007/s10862-012-9324-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jöreskog, K. G., and Sörbom, D. (1988). LISREL 7 [Computer Software]. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

Google Scholar

Jorkeskog, K. G., and Sorbom, D. (2001). LISREL 8.5. (Scientific). Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc.

Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika 39, 31–36. doi: 10.1007/BF02291575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimonis, E. R., Branch, J., Hagman, B., Graham, N., and Miller, C. (2013). The psychometric properties of the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits in an undergraduate sample. Psychol. Assess. 25, 84–93. doi: 10.1037/a0029024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Fazekas, H., and Loney, B. R. (2006). Psychopathy, aggression, and the processing of emotional stimuli in non-referred girls and boys. Behav. Sci. Law 24, 21–37. doi: 10.1002/bsl.668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimonis, E. R., Frick, P. J., Skeem, J. L., Marsee, M. A., Cruise, K., Munoz, L. C., et al. (2008). Assessing callous–unemotional traits in adolescent offenders: validation of the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits. Int. J. Law Psychiatry 31, 241–252. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2008.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeber, R., and Farrington, D. P. (2000). Young children who commit crime: epidemiology, developmental origins, risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications. Dev. Psychopathol. 12, 737–762. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Romero, L., Gómez-Fraguela, J. A., and Romero, E. (2015). Assessing callous-unemotional traits in a Spanish sample of institutionalized youths: the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 37, 392–406. doi: 10.1007/s10862-014-9469-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U. (1999). Promin: a method for oblique factor rotation. Multivariate Behav. Res. 34, 347–365. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3403_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2003). A factor simplicity index. Psychometrika 68, 49–60. doi: 10.1007/BF02296652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Ferrando, P. J. (2009). Acquiescent responding in partially balanced multidimensional scales. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 62, 319–326. doi: 10.1348/000711007X265164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Ten Berge, J. M. F. (2006). Tucker’s congruence coefficient as a meaningful index of factor similarity. Methodology 2, 57–64. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.2.2.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynam, D. R. (1996). Early identification of chronic offenders: who is the fledgling psychopath? Psychol. Bull. 120, 209–234. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.2.209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynam, D. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Raine, A., Loeber, R., and Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (2005). Adolescent psychopathy and the big five: results from two samples. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 431–443. doi: 10.1007/s10648-005-5724-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellenbergh, G. J. (1994). A unidimensional latent trait model for continuous item responses. Multivariate Behav. Res. 29, 223–236. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2903_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, J. D., and Lynam, D. (2001). Structural models of personality and their relation to antisocial behavior: a meta-analytic review. Criminology 39, 765–798. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-9125.2001.tb00940.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales-Vives, F., Camps, E., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2013). Development and validation of the psychological maturity assessment scale (PSYMAS). Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 29, 12–18. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales-Vives, F., Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Vigil-Colet, A. (2017). How response biases affect the factor structure of Big Five personality questionnaires. An. Psicol. 33, 589–596. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.2.254841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muthen, B., and Kaplan, D. (1985). A comparison of some methodologies for the factor analysis of non-normal Likert variables. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 38, 171–189. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8317.1985.tb00832.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, D., Vigil-Colet, A., Ferrando, P. J., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (in press). Psychological Test Toolbox: a new tool to compute factor analysis controlling response bias. J. Stat. Softw.

Navarro-González, D., Lorenzo-Seva, U., and Vigil-Colet, A. (2016). How response bias affects the factorial structure of personality self-reports. Psicothema 28, 465–470. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2016.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pardini, D. A., Lochman, J. E., and Frick, P. J. (2003). Callous/unemotional traits and social-cognitive processes in adjudicated youths. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 364–371. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200303000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rammstedt, B., and Farmer, R. F. (2013). The impact of acquiescence on the evaluation of personality structure. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1137–1145. doi: 10.1037/a0033323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R., Vitacco, M. J., Jackson, R. L., Martin, M., Collins, M., and Sewell, K. W. (2002). Faking psychopathy? An examination of response styles with antisocial youth. J. Pers. Assess. 78, 31–46. doi: 10.1207/S15327752JPA7801_03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roose, A., Bijttebier, P., Decoene, S., Claes, L., and Frick, P. J. (2010). Assessing the Affective features of psychopathy in adolescence: a further validation of the inventory of callous and unemotional traits. Assessment 17, 44–57. doi: 10.1177/1073191109344153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruiz-Pamies, M., Lorenzo-Seva, U., Morales-Vives, F., Cosi, S., and Vigil-Colet, A. (2014). I-DAQ: a new test to assess direct and indirect aggression free of response bias. Span. J. Psychol. 17:E41. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2014.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., and Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 94, 168–182. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skeem, J. L., and Cooke, D. J. (2010). Is criminal behavior a central component of psychopathy? Conceptual directions for resolving the debate. Psychol. Assess. 22, 433–445. doi: 10.1037/a0008512

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snee, R. D. (1977). Computer aided design of experiments. Technometrics 19, 415–428. doi: 10.1080/00401706.1977.10489581

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickle, T. R., Kirkpatrick, N. M., and Brush, L. N. (2009). Callous-unemotional traits and social information processing: multiple risk-factor models for understanding aggressive behavior in antisocial youth. Law Hum. Behav. 33, 515–529. doi: 10.1007/s10979-008-9171-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J., and Lang, A. R. (2006). “Psychopathy and substance use disorders,” in Handbook of Psychopathy, ed. C. J. Patrick (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 495–511.

Google Scholar

Timmerman, M. E., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assessment of ordered polytomous items with Parallel Analysis. Psychol. Methods 16, 209–220. doi: 10.1037/a0023353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tucker, L. R. (1951). A Method for Synthesis of Factor Analysis Studies (No. PRS-984). Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Services. doi: 10.21236/AD0047524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., and Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 46, 592–597. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00393.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viding, E., Simmonds, E., Petrides, K. V. V., and Frederickson, N. (2009). The contribution of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems to bullying in early adolescence. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 471–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.02012.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigil-Colet, A., Morales-Vives, F., Camps, E., Tous, J., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2013). Development and validation of the Overall Personality Assessment Scale (OPERAS). Psicothema 25, 100–106. doi: 10.7334/psicothema2011.411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigil-Colet, A., Ruiz-Pamies, M., Anguiano-Carrasco, C., and Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2012). The impact of social desirability on psychometric measures of aggression. Psicothema 24, 310–315.

Google Scholar

Waller, R., and Hyde, L. W. (2018). Callous-unemotional behaviors in early childhood: the development of empathy and prosociality gone awry. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 20, 11–16. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, R., Hyde, L. W., Grabell, A. S., Alves, M. L., and Olson, S. L. (2015). Differential associations of early callous-unemotional, oppositional, and ADHD behaviors: multiple domains within early-starting conduct problems? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 56, 657–666. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White, S. F., Cruise, K. R., and Frick, P. J. (2009). Differential correlates to self-report and parent-report of callous-unemotional traits in a sample of juvenile sexual offenders. Behav. Sci. Law 27, 910–928. doi: 10.1002/bsl.911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychopathy, callous-unemotional traits, antisocial behavior, adolescence, response biases

Citation: Morales-Vives F, Cosi S, Lorenzo-Seva U and Vigil-Colet A (2019) The INventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits and Antisocial Behavior (INCA) for Young People: Development and Validation in a Community Sample. Front. Psychol. 10:713. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00713

Received: 02 August 2018; Accepted: 14 March 2019;
Published: 27 March 2019.

Edited by:

Lynne D. Roberts, Curtin University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Georgiana Susa, Babeş-Bolyai University, Romania
Antonio Zuffiano, Liverpool Hope University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Morales-Vives, Cosi, Lorenzo-Seva and Vigil-Colet. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fabia Morales-Vives, ZmFiaWEubW9yYWxlc0B1cnYuY2F0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.