Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 21 March 2019
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Perceptions of People: Cues to underlying Physiology and Psychology View all 17 articles

The Interplay Between Economic Status and Attractiveness, and the Importance of Attire in Mate Choice Judgments

  • 1Evolution and Ecology Research Centre, School of Biological, Earth and Environmental Sciences, The University of New South Wales, Sydney, NSW, Australia
  • 2School of Psychology, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD, Australia

Desirable characteristics of “opposite sex others,” such as physical attractiveness and economic status, can influence how individuals are judged, and this is different for men and women. However, under various social contexts where cues of higher or lower economic status is suggested, sex differences in judgments related to mate choice have not been fully explored. In two studies, ratings of economic status and attractiveness were quantified for male and female targets that were presented under various social contexts. Study 1 assessed judgments (n = 1,359) of images of nine male and nine female targets in different sized groups containing only opposite-sex others (i.e., group size). While we found no significant effects of group size on male and female attractiveness, target female economic status increased when surrounded by two or more men. An ad hoc analysis controlling for the attire of the targets (business or casual) found that the association between target female economic status and group size occurred when females were in business attire. Study 2 investigates this effect further by presenting images of 12 males and 12 females, in higher and lower status attire (i.e., business and casual clothing) and measured judgments of attractiveness and economic status among women and men (n = 1,038). Consistent with the results of Study 1, female economic status was only affected when women were in business attire. However, female economic status decreased when in the presence of other men in business attire. There were no sex differences in judgments of economic status when judging stimuli in casual attire. Additionally, negative associations between attractiveness and economic status were found for males presented in casual attire. We discuss these results in the light of evolutionary sexual conflict theory by demonstrating how the asymmetrical importance of status between men and women can influence mate choice judgments.

General Introduction

Evolved mate preferences often target attributes that signal dimensions of reproductive health (Buss and Schmitt, 1993; Puts, 2010). In women, age-related physical cues such as feminine facial shape, breast morphology, and an hourglass distribution of body fat are attractive to men (Jasieńska et al., 2004; Singh et al., 2010; Dixson et al., 2011, 2015; Marcinkowska et al., 2014), ostensibly because they signal fecundability. In men, muscularity, vocal pitch, and facial masculinity provide information regarding health, age, social status, dominance, and formidability, that enhance mating success (Archer, 2009; Puts, 2010; Hill et al., 2013; Dixson et al., 2014).

Judgments of physical attractiveness are also shaped by factors other than physical attributes (Dixson, 2019; Luoto, 2019). For instance, men are more likely to be rated as more physically attractive when they are presented with high status cues such as an expensive car (Dunn and Searle, 2010; Shuler and McCord, 2010) or an upscale apartment (Dunn and Hill, 2014). While these cues may not influence ratings of women’s physical attractiveness, high status may drive intersexual competition between women (Wang and Griskevicius, 2013). Judgments of physical attractiveness also increase with the addition of other people, an effect known as “mate choice copying” (Waynforth, 2007). Men are more likely to be rated as more attractive and to have higher economic status when in the presence of women, whereas mate choice copying effects are negligible when women are in the presence of men (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016, 2018).

The associations between sex, economic status, and physical attractiveness may reflect evolved sex differences in mate choice (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2018). Throughout human evolution, resource acquisition positively influenced male reproductive success (Low, 1990, 2000; Smith, 2004), so that sexual selection may have favored status seeking behavior in men (Betzig, 1986; Dixson, 2016; Von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016). An association between men’s status and reproductive success has been reported across many small-scale (Von Rueden and Jaeggi, 2016) and several industrialized societies (Li et al., 2002; Hopcroft, 2006), which may have implications regarding the formation of social perceptions of women and men. For instance, participants judged female economic status comparatively lower than that of the males they were presented alongside (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016). Unlike men, women’s success can be judged negatively, as high status or successful women are more frequently derogated (Heilman et al., 2004), particularly when dressed in short skirts or shirts displaying cleavage (Glick et al., 2005; Howlett et al., 2015). Conversely, physical attractiveness is beneficial to women as attractive individuals receive favorable treatment (Rosenblat, 2008) and are more likely to find jobs and get promoted (Hamermesh and Biddle, 1993; Pfann et al., 2000), which benefits women more than men within employment scenarios (Busetta et al., 2013). However, the interplay between physical attractiveness, economic status, and attire in mixed social contexts that are more comparable to real-world scenarios has yet to be explored.

The current research assesses how modifiable cues of economic status influence judgments of an individual’s physical attractiveness and economic status. Previous studies reported that high status men were more likely to receive respect and praise compared to women in high status roles (Forsythe, 1990; Brase and Guy, 2004). Studies have also shown that high status women within mixed sex groups were just as likely as men to attain leadership positions (Goktepe and Schneier, 1989). However, high status women were judged to be less attractive and approachable than high status men (Howlett et al., 2013). Additionally, ratings of women’s economic status are lower relative to men they are presented alongside (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016). However, whether this association persists when women are presented as higher in economic status than men or if physical attractiveness influences ratings of economic status remains unknown.

Based on evolutionary theories regarding the importance of status in male reproductive success (Hopcroft, 2006; Von Rueden et al., 2010) and mate choice copying theory (Waynforth, 2007; Gouda-Vossos et al., 2018), we predicted that cues of higher social status should have a stronger positive effect on ratings of economic status and physical attractiveness in men than in women. We also predicted that women’s economic status would be rated lower than the men they were presented alongside, even if women appeared to be higher in economic status than men (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016). We conducted two studies, both of which manipulated economic status via clothing in male and female stimuli and measured participant attractiveness and monetary earnings ratings of the stimuli. We first tested the effects of the presence of “opposite sex others” by manipulating mixed sex group sizes (Study 1). Based on the results of Study 1, we designed Study 2 wherein various forms of attire were used to manipulate social status, including the presence and absence of “opposite sex others” in various forms of attire.

Study 1: Judgements of Attractiveness and Economic Status Within Mixed-Sex Groups

Dynamics within groups can vary depending on the size of social groups and the distributions of gender therein. All-male groups tend to be more aggressive and competitive toward other group members than all-female groups (Schopler et al., 2001). Additionally, all-male groups form more stable hierarchies faster than all-female groups (Anderson et al., 2001) and are more likely to collaborate intra-sexually when an outside threat is present (Vugt et al., 2007). Sharing cooperatively produced resources is also an important factor within collective groups (Mangel, 1990; Melis and Semmann, 2010) and the dynamics within groups can vary if some members are more likely to obtain larger portions of resources (relative to other members) and subsequently gain direct benefits (Williams, 2002). Among men, resource acquisition and holding potential enhance mating opportunities and mating success (Betzig, 1986; Von Rueden et al., 2010). As a result, expectations and opportunities vary between men and women within mixed-sex groups (Eagly and Johnson, 1990). Mate choice copying studies have also found that men are judged to be more physically attractive when presented within a group of women, while women presented alongside men are not (Milonoff et al., 2007; Hill and Buss, 2008; Dunn and Doria, 2010).

Behaviors within groups may be driven by similar mechanisms associated with sex differences in mate choice, especially in reference to associations between status (social or economic) and physical attractiveness. In men, cues of social status, dominance, and formidability that enhance male physical attractiveness (Hill and Buss, 2008; Archer, 2009; Puts, 2010; Dixson et al., 2017, 2019) and mating success (Hill et al., 2013; Kordsmeyer et al., 2018) may also predict assertiveness and group leadership (Anderson et al., 2001; Geniole et al., 2015). While cues of status may also predict the emergence of female leaders in groups (Anderson et al., 2001) they may not augment women’s physical attractiveness and mating success (Puts, 2010). To test the effects of social group size on ratings of male and female attractiveness and economic status, we presented images of women and men in the presence of social groups varying in the number of opposite sex targets. Thus, each male and female was rated alone and again alongside opposite sex targets in increments of 1, 2, and 4 additional opposite sex individuals.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were recruited via Facebook, Twitter, and internal student email lists within the research institution, resulting in 1359 participants in total. All participants were over 18 years old and were not aware of the purpose of the study. Each participant provided details of their biological sex, age, and sexual orientation using a Kinsey Scale (Kinsey et al., 1948, 1953). As sexual orientation impacts on judgments of attractiveness of opposite sex targets (Petterson et al., 2015, 2016, 2018; Valentova et al., 2017), only participants who were heterosexual or bisexual were retained in the analyses (i.e., Kinsey scale 0–3). The final analysis included 569 (women = 494; men = 75) participants who completed surveys including target males and 598 (women = 357; men = 241) who completed the survey’s including target females. The age range of participants was 29.04 years ± 9.4. The majority of participants listed their country of origin as Australia (38.7%), followed by USA (20%), then UK (17.7%). The majority identified as North Western European, British, or Irish (51.2%), followed by European Mixed Race (13.8%), then Southern European (3.8%) with 8% stating they “did not wish to report ethnicity.”

Stimuli

Images of nine male and nine female targets surrounded by four members of the opposite sex (females and males, respectively) were chosen from a stock photo website1. This resulted in a total of 18 original images. Each target was presented in four group size conditions [alone, one opposite sex, two opposite sex, and four opposite sex others; for examples, see Electronic Supplementary Material S1 (ESM 1)]. Overall, 72 images were constructed and used in this study, with the target pose and facial expression identical between treatments. The targets ages ranged from 22 to 56 years (males: mean = 40, SD ±12.3, females: mean = 38, SD ±12.7). All images were professionally taken under standardized lighting and filters. Photographs were taken in workplaces and casual settings with positions of targets and opposite sex others varying from image to image.

Procedure

Experiments were conducted on-line via www.socialsci.com. Each participant entering the study was randomly assigned to one of four experiments in which they rated either male or female targets for either attractiveness or monetary earning (i.e., economic status). The number of participants for each experiment was as follows: Attractiveness/target female: 202 women, 150 men; Earnings/target female: 155 women, 91 men; Attractiveness/target males: 259 women, 39 men; Earnings/target males: 235 women, 36 men. The study employed a “Within Target – Between Treatment” design where participants saw all nine targets in random order with the treatment (target alone, one opposite sex other, two opposite sex others, and four opposite sex others) for each target drawn at random. Similar designs have been used in past research on physical attractiveness (Janif et al., 2014, 2015; Brooks et al., 2015; Dixson et al., 2016). This research was approved by the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Advisory Board (Psychology) (HREAP 1880).

Participants were informed that they would be shown a range of images of people. In each image, the target was indicated with an arrow. If assigned to rate physical attractiveness, participants were asked to rate each target using a percentile scale from 0 to 100 where “50” indicated that the individual is more physically attractive than 50% of other individuals of the same sex (i.e., of median attractiveness). If rating economic status, participants were asked to rate each target using a percentile scale from 0 to 100 where “50” indicated the individual earns more than 50% of other same sex individuals in full time work (i.e., median income in full-time work).

Analysis

Multilevel modeling was used where data were organized so that each row represented one participants rating of one target in one treatment. Using the statistical software SPSS, separate general linear mixed models (MLMs) were fitted for the two dependant variables (physical attractiveness or economic status). In each of these models, Model ID was a repeated-measures factor, Participant ID was a random factor. Participant sex and Group Size (alone, +1 opposite sex individual, +2 opposite sex individual, and +4 opposite sex individuals) were included as fixed factors. SPSS does not calculate effect sizes for mixed models. Thus, we calculated approximate effect sizes as partial Eta-squared, from the F-test and degrees of freedom, although this practice has not been formally validated for multi-level models. When interpreting effect sizes, by convention, effects of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 are interpreted as small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.

Results

The Effect of Group Size on Male and Female Attractiveness and Economic Status

There were no significant main effect or interactions involving Group Size; suggesting no differences in the ratings of attractiveness across treatment (Figures 1A,B and Table 1a). The significant main effects of participant sex on ratings of target females were due to women rating target females as more attractive (mean = 59.85, SE ±0.391) than men (mean = 58.66, SE ±0.456).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Data are mean ratings of attractiveness (A,B) and economic status (C,D) ratings (±1 SEM) split by four group size (alone, 1,2,4 opposite sex others). P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, determined by post hoc least significance difference tests.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. MLMs for target male and female rated attractiveness and economic status.

Like the results for attractiveness, male ratings of economic status were not affected by Group Size as there was no significant main effect or interactions with participant sex (Figure 1C and Table 1b). However, ratings for target females revealed a significant main effect of Group Size (Table 1b) as female economic status increased incrementally with the addition of two and four males (Figure 1D). There was no significant Participant Sex × Group Size interaction, suggesting that men and women were rating target females similarly (Table 1b).

The Effect of Attire and Group Size on Target Attractiveness and Economic Status; an ad hoc Analysis

Images included targets in either casual or business attire, which may have affected ratings. To test this, targets were classified as wearing either business or casual attire using methods from Forsythe (1990). Business attire referred to dark, angular, traditional business suits whereas casual attire referred to light, informal, everyday wear. There were four business and five casual attired target males and five business and four casual attired target females. Full analysis can be found in Electronic Supplementary Materials S2, S3 (ESM 2: Male and Female Attractiveness and ESM 3: Male and Female Economic Status).

Another series of MLMs were conducted, with attire included as a fixed factor. We found no effects of Attire on target male attractiveness (F1,2555 = 0.851, P = 0.356, ηp2 = 0.00033), Group Size (F3,2555 = 0.091, P = 0.965, ηp2 = <0.001), and Participant Sex (F1,2555 = 0.961, P = 0.339, ηp2 = <0.001). The effect sizes for all main effects were small for male attractiveness (i.e., less than 0.2), suggesting that both Attire and Group Size do not strongly impact on male attractiveness.

There was a main effect of Attire on target male economic status (F1,1877 = 533.83, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.220), but not Group Size (F3,1853 = 1.408, P = 0.239, ηp2 = 0.002), or Participant Sex (F1,1877 = 2.156, P = 0.142, ηp2 = 0.001). Both men and women rated male economic status higher when in business attire (mean = 65.32, SE±0.713) than when in casual attire (mean = 39.43, SE ±0.865). We did not find mate choice copying effects, which suggest that the type of attire men were wearing influences ratings of target males more than the presence of other females.

There was a main effect of Attire on attractiveness ratings of target females (F1,2963 = 68.13, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.023) but no main effect of Group Size (F3,2952 = 1.937, P = 0.121, ηp2 = 0.002) or Participant Sex (F1,2963 = 2.982, P = 0.084, ηp2 = 0.010). Target females were rated as more attractive when in business (mean = 61.46, SE ±0.404) than Casual Attire (mean = 56.52, SE ±0.442), although the effect size was small (i.e., below 0.2) and comparable to Group Size, suggesting that the impact of Attire on female attractiveness is small.

Ratings of female earnings were also significantly affected by Attire (F1,2075 = 356.54, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.150) and Group Size (F2,2057 = 3.181, P = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.005), although both effect sizes were small (i.e., below 0.2). A significant Attire × Participant Sex interaction (F1,2057 = 18.68, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.009) occurred due to women rating females higher in economic status when in business attire than men (women mean = 63.57, SE ±0.566; men mean = 61.6, SE ±0.740; P = 0.043) and lower when in casual attire than men (women mean = 47.58, SE ±0.622; men mean = 51.89, SE±0.785; P < 0.001). There was also a significant Attire × Group Size interaction (F1,2057 = 6.369, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.009), which reflects that ratings of female economic status increased incrementally with the addition of two and four males when females were presented in business but not casual attire (Figures 2A,B). This suggests that the original results of female economic status were likely driven by the responses toward target females in business attire, as opposed to casual attire.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Data are mean economic status ratings (±1 SEM) of target males (A) and females (B), split by target attire (business/casual) for four group size treatments (alone, 1,2,4 opposite sex others). P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, ∗∗∗P < 0.001, determined by post hoc least significance difference tests.

In order to determine the model of best fit, we used the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). We monitored the AIC between the original models and the ad hoc analysis. If AIC changes downward by more than 2 units, the model is significantly better (Bozdogan, 1987). Using both the level of significance of interactions and the AIC allows us to test the validity of different restrictions of a model and to choose a model with the smallest probability of rejection to be the best fitting model as opposed to choosing based on a priori ground (Bozdogan, 1987). Across all four ad hoc models, the AIC was significantly lower than the original models constructed (Female Attractiveness, Original AIC: 26858.21, ad hoc AIC: 26,768.36; Male Attractiveness, Original AIC: 23,691.96, ad hoc AIC: 23,657.82; Female Earnings, Original AIC: 18,833.71, ad hoc AIC: 18,405.63; Male Earnings, Original AIC: 22,008.02, ad hoc AIC: 21,434.76). This suggests that entering Attire as a fixed factor improves all the models.

Discussion

Contrary to our predictions, ratings of attractiveness for target male and females were not influenced by social group size. Further, the economic status of males was unaffected when in the presence of an opposite sex other (i.e., female). Whereas the addition of opposite sex others positively influenced the economic status of target females. Previous studies have found that woman’s task proficiency (Balkwell and Berger, 1996), economic status (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016), and social status (Eagly and Karau, 2002) were rated lower than the men they were compared with, suggesting that the mere presence of a man can lower perceptions of women’s status within an economic hierarchy. However, the type of attire women wear may reduce these effects as women dressed in more masculine attire (i.e., traditional business attire) are seen as having better managerial characteristics (Forsythe et al., 1984, 1985) are more likely to get hired for leadership positions (Forsythe, 1990), and are just as likely as men to emerge as leaders within a mixed sex group (Goktepe and Schneier, 1989). Taken together with the results of the current studies, women’s perceived economic status appears to be heavily influenced by high status and masculine cues such as business attire and the number of men within their immediate presence.

However, we are limited in making these assumptions regarding attire, as the numbers of targets used were too small after separation for ad hoc analysis (i.e., four business and five casual attired target males and five business and four casual attired target females). Further, the position of the target in each photo was not randomized or controlled and therefore we could not conclude whether participants perceived targets as leaders, which could have influenced ratings of attractiveness and economic status. It is also possible that even though female economic status increased with the addition of “male others,” ratings of female status may still be made relative to men. Unfortunately, we did not obtain ratings of earnings and attractiveness of the male “opposite sex others” to confirm this. Thus, we designed a second study focused on the impact of attire of various social status (business/casual) and measured effects of male and female attractiveness and economic status in targets presented individually and when paired (Study 2).

Study 2: The Effects of Attire on Men and Women

Attire communicates information relating to identity, social status, and position within a hierarchy (Roach-Higgins and Eicher, 1992). Molloy and Potter (1975) suggested that attire may play a pivotal role in judgments of an individual’s credibility, likeability, interpersonal attractiveness, and dominance. Bassett et al. (1979) found that high status clothing positively influenced judgments of credibility. However, females were rated lower than males across all four measurements that composed credibility (i.e., potency, character, composure, and competence). Unlike men, women’s success can be judged negatively, as high status or successful women are more frequently derogated (Heilman et al., 2004) and are judged more negatively when dressed in short skirts (Glick et al., 2005; Howlett et al., 2015). However, physically attractive women receive better treatment (Rosenblat, 2008), are more likely to find employment, and are more likely to get promoted (Hamermesh and Biddle, 1993; Pfann et al., 2000), which may not be the case among men (Busetta et al., 2013).

In Study 2, we measured associations between rated physical attractiveness and economic status in male and female targets in different attires. We also assessed if the presence of opposite sex others in various attire influenced judgments of male and female targets. Based on mate choice copying theories (Waynforth, 2007; Gouda-Vossos et al., 2018), we hypothesized that males would attain high ratings of physical attractiveness and economic status when presented in high status attire (i.e., business attire) regardless of the attire the opposite sex other. As economic status may not have had strong effects on female reproductive success during human evolution (Betzig, 1986), we did not predict a positive association between attractiveness and economic status with target females. Based on the results of Study 1, we predicted that female economic status will be limited to the ratings of males when in casual but not business attire.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 1,035 participants were recruited. All participants were over 18 years old and were not aware of the purpose of the study. Each participant provided details of their biological sex, age, and sexual orientation using the Kinsey Scale (Kinsey et al., 1948, 1953). Participants received $1US. As in Study 1, only participants who were heterosexual or bisexual were retained in the analyses (i.e., Kinsey scale 0–3). A total of 459 females and 578 males (Age 32 ± 10.5) were included in the final analysis. The majority of participants listed their country of origin as United States (82.4%), followed by Southern Europe (4.4%), then Australia (3.4%). The majority identified as ethnically North Western European, British, or Irish (40.4%), followed by European Mixed Race (17.2%), Southern European (7.2%), and 10% elected not to state their ethnicity.

Stimuli

Full body, color photographs of 12 male and 12 female targets were obtained from a stock photo website2. All photographs were taken using standardized lighting and filters and were on a white background. Two sets of images for each male and female target were obtained (i.e., either in casual attire or business attire), comprised of 24 male and 24 female target images. We then created composite images, where each target (in both business and casual attire) was paired with opposite sex others (six in business and six in casual attire), resulting in a total of 144 composite images for male targets and 144 composite images for female targets (Figure 3). Business attire included suits, collared shirts, and pencil skirts (for females not wearing pant suits). Casual attire included t-shirts, jeans, shorts, or skirts (Figure 3). PeopleImages.com collects information on the targets they recruit, including biological sex, age, and ethnicity. The targets ages ranged from 20 to 30 years (Males Target mean = 25, SD ±4.8 years; Females Target Mean = 24, SD ±4.3) and the majority were Caucasian (66%) followed by multi-ethnic (16%), then African and Latino (9% each).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Example images used for targets presented alone (A) and with opposite sex others (B). Film Media courtesy of Yuri Arcurs Photography Aps, Used by Permission.

Procedure

Studies were conducted online using the SocSci platform3 and participants were recruited via MTurk. Participants each rated two batches of 12 images (24 in total). One batch of 12 images included either male or female targets alone. The other batch included male or female targets with opposite sex others. The order of the batches as well as the target sex was fully randomized, so that participants were presented with four possible combinations (i.e., female alone/male with other; female with other/male alone; male alone/female with other; and male with other/female alone). Within each batch, each target was drawn and shown once, in random order and either in casual or business attire (drawn at random with equal probability) (see Figures 3A,B for examples). Thus, each target was presented to the participant either alone or with an opposite sex other, in either business or casual attire. This design ensured participants saw all possible targets (male and female) in only one type of attire (i.e., Within Target – Between Treatment design). By designing the study in this manner, participants do not see the same target more than once in different scenarios, which minimizes possible carry-over effects and avoided participants deciphering the true nature of the study that previous studies have shown to influence ratings of targets (Chen, 2008). Experimental designs like this have been previously employed to test preferences for physical appearance (Janif et al., 2014, 2015; Brooks et al., 2015; Dixson et al., 2016). Participants rated targets using a sliding scale (from 0 to 100) provided below each image for physical attractiveness and economic status using the same scales as in Study 1. This research was approved by the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Advisory Board (HREA 155047).

Analysis

Using the statistical software, SPSS, we first tested the influence of individual sex and attire on ratings of attractiveness and economic status by focusing on the ratings of attractiveness and economic status of targets when presented alone. This allowed us to determine how target attire (business/casual) and target sex (female/male) influence these ratings. This 2 × 2 between-subject design (Sex of target – Male/Female) × (Attire of Target – business/casual) employed MANOVAs, with rated attractiveness and economic status as dependant variables.

We then assessed if the attire of the opposite sex other influenced male and female attractiveness and economic status by focusing on ratings of targets when presentenced with opposite sex others. This was a 2 × 2 × 2 × 2 between-subject design (Sex of target – Male/Female) × (Attire of TARGET – business/casual) × (Attire of OTHER – business/ casual) × (Participant Sex – Men/Women). We analyzed the results of targets when presented with an opposite sex other of varying attire (business/casual), using separate general linear mixed models (MLM) for ratings of physical attractiveness and economic status for each study. Target ID, Subject ID, and OtherID were included as random factors to specify the covariance structure for the residuals. Target Attire, Target Sex, Other Attire, and Participant Sex were fixed factors. All main effects and interactions were assessed.

Results

Effects of Individual Attire and Sex on Attractiveness and Economic Status Ratings

The multivariate analysis revealed significant main effect of Target Sex, Target Attire, and their interaction (Table 2). For female targets, there were positive associations between Attractiveness and Economic status ratings when presented in both casual and business attire (Figure 4). In contrast, male targets were rated negatively for attractiveness and economic status when in casual attire (Figure 4). There was a significant Target Sex × Target Attire interaction (Table 2), which reflects target females were rated lower for economic status (target female mean = 54.03, SD ±11.27; target male = 55.33, SD ±12.04) but higher for attractiveness (target female = 67.178, SD ±5.33; target male = 56.32, SD ±5.27) than male targets. There was also a significant Target Sex × Target Attire interaction (Table 2), so that target females were rated as more attractive in casual attire (mean = 69.28, SD ±4.72) than business attire (mean = 65.08, SD ±5.91).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. MANOVA for rated economic status and attractiveness for targets presented alone.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The association between rated attractiveness and economic status of target males and females when presented alone in business and casual attire.

The Influence of Target Attire and the Attire of Opposite Sex Other on Target Attractiveness and Economic Status

There was a significant main effect of Target Sex (Table 3a), so that female targets were rated as more attractive than male targets (Target Female mean = 67.32, SE ±1.20; Target Male Mean = 56.32, SE ±1.16; P < 0.001). The Target Sex × Participant Sex interaction was not statistically significant (Table 3a). There was a significant Target Attire × Target Sex × Participant Sex interaction for attractiveness ratings that was driven by the ratings from men, who rated female attractiveness lower when in business attire than casual; and male attractiveness higher in business than casual (Table 3a and Figure 5). There were no significant effects due to women’s ratings (Figure 5).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. MLMs for target male and female rated attractiveness and economic status.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Data are mean attractiveness ratings (±1 SEM) from women and men (participant sex), split by target sex for two attire treatments (business/casual). ∗∗P < 0.01, determined by post hoc least significance difference tests.

There were no significant main effects or interactions involving Other Attire on rated attractiveness (Table 3a and Figure 6A). However, there was a significant Other Attire × Target Attire interaction for ratings of economic status (Table 3b and Figure 6B). Targets in business attire presented alongside “others” in business attire were rated higher in economic status than when presented alongside “others” in casual attire (Figure 6B), which did not vary with Target Sex (Table 3b). A significant Target Sex × Target Attire interaction reflected men in business attire were rated significantly higher for economic status than women in business attire (Table 3b and Figure 7). Ratings of males and females in casual attire did not differ significantly (Figure 7). There were no significant main effects or interactions involving participant sex (Table 3b). For additional analyses see Supplementary Tables S4, S5.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Data are mean ratings (±1 SEM) for (A) attractiveness and (B) economic status, split by other attire (business/casual) and target attire (business/casual). P < 0.05, ∗∗P < 0.01, determined by post hoc least significance difference tests.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Data are mean economic status ratings (±1 SEM) split by participant sex (women and men), target attire (business/casual), and target sex (male/female). ∗∗P < 0.01, determined by post hoc least significance difference.

Discussion

As predicted, economic status ratings were higher when male and female targets were presented in business than casual attire. Additionally, male and female economic status ratings increased when presented alongside others in business attire. In contrast to Study 1, target females received lower attractiveness ratings in business than casual attire, which was driven by men’s ratings. The reduction in female attractiveness ratings when presented in business attire is consistent with previous research reporting high status women were judged negatively, less attractive, and less approachable than lower status women (Bassett et al., 1979; Forsythe, 1990; Heilman et al., 2004; Lavin A. et al., 2009). We also report female economic status was rated lower than the men they were presented alongside, indicating that perceptions of women’s economic status are influenced by the men they are presented with (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016). Interestingly, the effects of economic status and sex disappeared when target males and females were presented in casual attire, so that sex differences in perceptions of status were specific to status-related social cues.

Our predictions regarding positive associations between attractiveness and economic status in male targets were supported, but only in males presented in business and not casual attire. Unexpectedly, we found positive associations between attractiveness and economic status ratings for women in both types of attire whereas previous research reported strong associations between economic status and attractiveness in males, with mixed results in women (Townsend and Levy, 1990; Hanson et al., 1991; Shuler and McCord, 2010). Our findings suggest that the influence of status-related clothing on judgments of attractiveness among women and men may be less robust than previously reported.

General Discussion

A “Wall Street” article discussing attire and business practices stated that “traditional business dress is seen as a uniform…it simplifies decision making and makes hierarchies easy to read.” (Binkley, 2008). Our findings reinforce this sentiment, as participants made clear distinctions in physical attractiveness and economic status judgments based on clothing. We also report that women’s economic status is judged relative to and lower than the men they are presented alongside. These sex differences in judgments of economic status disappeared when target males and females were presented in casual attire, demonstrating that judgments of women and men’s economic status are most influenced by traditionally masculine clothing.

Status seeking is positively associated with men’s mating and reproductive success (Betzig, 1986; Hopcroft, 2006) with high economic status associated more with ideals surrounding maleness and masculinity than femininity (Akerlof and Kranton, 2000). Past studies have found that, regardless of sex, group members expressing masculine gender roles or dress in masculine attire are more likely to emerge as leaders and are judged as more forceful and aggressive than those expressing outwardly feminine characteristics (Goktepe and Schneier, 1989; Forsythe, 1990). A limitation of the current study was that we did not compare the effects within same sex groups. In our previous study (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016), we reported that the economic status of target males was judged to be highest when presented with another man. High status men form same-sex alliances and partnerships (Von Rueden et al., 2010; Von Rueden, 2011), and the presence of two men not obviously in conflict may give the appearance that targets were forming same-sex partnerships. This was not the case when target females were presented alongside another female, as economic status was rated lower than when female targets were presented alone (Gouda-Vossos et al., 2016). In order to fully understand how men and women’s economic status are perceived within various group dynamics, and if economic status is truly judged within a masculine hierarchy, comparisons within same-sex groups would be a worthwhile extension of the current research.

In male dominated social environments characterized by defined hierarchies (Anderson et al., 2001; Schmid Mast, 2004) business attire is associated with more masculine and socially dominant attributes (Forsythe, 1990). Without clothing that clearly communicates economic status, it may be difficult for people to judge where others fall within a hierarchy, which may be why economic status ratings were more neutral when targets were presented in casual attire. It was also unsurprising that female attractiveness was rated lower when presented in business attire. However, this directly contradicts studies reporting no negative influence on female physical attractiveness when presented with high status cues such as cars (Brase and Richmond, 2004) or luxury apartments (Dunn and Hill, 2014). This suggests that judgments of female attractiveness are more likely to vary when women are presented as being of higher status rather than alongside high status cues. It could be argued that participants did not believe that the women in the study actually own the high-status cues (i.e., cars, luxury apartments, etc.); with attire being a more convincing indicator of earned status. By presenting target females as high status individuals, it may communicate economic independence and decrease the attractiveness of female targets to men.

The current study also found positive associations between attractiveness and economic status among male and female targets, except for males presented in casual attire. Judgments of men’s economic status and physical attractiveness are strongly positively correlated (Townsend and Levy, 1990; Hanson et al., 1991; Shuler and McCord, 2010) with competence, financial worth, and credibility being more consistently associated with men in business than casual attire (Bassett et al., 1979; Morris et al., 1996; Lavin A. M. et al., 2009). However, even subtle differences in attire within male-dominated business environments can lead to negative criticisms and attitudes toward men. For instance, men are perceived to be less confident, successful, and having lower salaries when presented in “off the peg” suits as opposed to “tailored suits” (Howlett et al., 2013). Further, men experience greater verbal harassment when presented in non-traditional business attire (i.e., business casual) than when presented in business attire (Kwantes et al., 2011). It could be argued that the economic status of more attractive male targets in casual attire was penalized in the current study, demonstrating how culturally malleable cues of status interplays with male attractiveness, possibly influencing women’s mate preferences.

Conclusion

Maestripieri et al. (2017) marshaled a comprehensive review on financial and prosocial biases and concluded that attractive individuals, especially women, were more likely to attain financial benefits and better treatment than their less attractive counterparts. The results of the current studies demonstrated that high status individuals, especially men, receive more favorable judgments relating to mate choice (i.e., attractiveness). However, whether this leads to better treatment or increased financial gain remains to be fully explored. Ostensibly, men and women both benefit from being highly attractive or high status, however, this benefit is not distributed equally. Although this is consistent with ideals surrounding the asymmetrical importance of status in males and physical attractiveness in women within mating contexts, the results of the current studies reflect how this may lead to unfair judgments and, possibly, unfair treatment of both men and women.

Author Contributions

AG-V, RB, and BD contributed conception and design of the study. AG-V carried out studies and organized the database. AG-V, RB, and BD performed the statistical analysis. AG-V wrote the first draft of the manuscript. AG-V, RB, and BD wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Australian Research Council awarded to RB and BD and a University of Queensland Post-Doctoral Fellowship to BD.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00462/full#supplementary-material

MATERIAL S1 | Example images used for targets presented alone and with opposite sex others.

MATERIAL S2 | Ad Hoc MLMs of rated attractiveness of male and female models by men and women, factoring in ‘target attire’.

MATERIAL S3 | Ad Hoc MLMs of rated economic status of male and female models by men and women, factoring in ‘target attire’.

MATERIAL S4 | Data for groups study 1 separated by target sex and measure (attractiveness, earnings).

MATERIAL S5 | Data for attire study 2 separated by measure (attractiveness, earnings).

Footnotes

  1. ^www.peopleimages.com
  2. ^www.peopleimages.com
  3. ^www.socialsci.com

References

Akerlof, G. A., and Kranton, R. E. (2000). Economics and identity. Q. J. Econ. 115, 715–753. doi: 10.1162/003355300554881

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C., John, O. P., Keltner, D., and Kring, A. M. (2001). Who attains social status? Effects of personality and physical attractiveness in social groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 116. doi: 10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.article.47256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behav. Brain Sci. 32, 249–266. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X09990951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balkwell, J. W., and Berger, J. (1996). Gender, status, and behavior in task situations. Soc. Psychol. Q. 59, 273–283. doi: 10.2307/2787023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassett, R. E., Staton-Spicer, A. Q., and Whitehead, J. L. (1979). Effects of source attire on judgments of credibility. Cent. States Speech J. 30, 282–285. doi: 10.1080/10510977909368022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Betzig, L. L. (1986). Despotism and Differential Reproduction: A Darwinian View of History. New York, NY: Aldine Publishing Co.

Google Scholar

Binkley, C. (2008). Business casual: all business, never casual. Wall Street J. D1–D8.

Google Scholar

Bozdogan, H. (1987). Model selection and Akaike’s information criterion (AIC): the general theory and its analytical extensions. Psychometrika 52, 345–370. doi: 10.1007/BF02294361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brase, G. L., and Guy, E. C. (2004). The demographics of mate value and self-esteem. Pers. Individ. Differ. 36, 471–484. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00117-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brase, G. L., Richmond, J. (2004). The white-coat effect: Physician attire and perceived authority, friendliness, and attractiveness. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 34, 2469–2481.

Google Scholar

Brooks, R. C., Jordan, A. L., Shelly, J., and Dixson, B. J. (2015). The multivariate evolution of female body shape in an artificial digital ecosystem. Evol. Hum. Behav. 36, 351–358. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busetta, G., Fiorillo, F., and Visalli, E. (2013). Searching for a Job is a Beauty Contest. Available at: https://mpra.ub.uni-muenchen.de/49392/2/

Google Scholar

Buss, D. M., and Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: an evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychol. Rev. 100, 204–232. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M. K. (2008). Rationalization and Cognitive Dissonance: Do Choices Affect or Reflect Preferences?. New Haven, CT: Yale University.

Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J. (2016). Masculinity and femininity. Encyclopedia of Evolutionary Psychological Science. Berlin: Springer.

Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J. (2019). Sexual selection and extended phenotypes in humans. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 5, 103–107. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0106-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Duncan, M., and Dixson, A. F. (2015). The role of breast size and areolar pigmentation in perceptions of women’s sexual attractiveness, reproductive health, sexual maturity, maternal nurturing abilities, and age. Arch. Sex. Behav. 44, 1685–1695. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0516-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Linklater, W. L., and Dixson, A. F. (2011). Eye-tracking of men’s preferences for waist-to-hip ratio and breast size of women. Arch. Sex. Behav. 40, 43–50. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9523-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J., Grimshaw, G. M., Ormsby, D. K., and Dixson, A. F. (2014). Eye-tracking women’s preferences for men’s somatotypes. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 73–79. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J. W., Rantala, M. J., and Brooks, R. C. (2019). Cross-cultural variation in women’s preferences for men’s body hair. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. doi: 10.1007/s40750-019-0107-x.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J. W., Rantala, M. J., Melo, E. F., and Brooks, R. C. (2017). Beards and the big city: displays of masculinity may be amplified under crowded conditions. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 259–264. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixson, B. J. W., Sulikowski, D., Gouda-Vossos, A., Rantala, M. J., and Brooks, R. C. (2016). The masculinity paradox: facial masculinity and beardedness interact to determine women’s ratings of men’s facial attractiveness. J. Evol. Biol. 29, 2311–2320. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, M. J., and Doria, M. V. (2010). Simulated attraction increases opposite sex attractiveness ratings in females but not males. J. Soc. Evol. Cult. Psychol. 4, 1–17. doi: 10.1037/h0099305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, M. J., and Hill, A. (2014). Manipulated luxury-apartment ownership enhances opposite-sex attraction in females but not males. J. Evol. Psychol. 12, 1–17. doi: 10.1556/JEP.12.2014.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, M. J., and Searle, R. (2010). Effect of manipulated prestige-car ownership on both sex attractiveness ratings. Br. J. Psychol. 101, 69–80. doi: 10.1348/000712609X417319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 108, 233–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.108.2.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagly, A. H., and Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol. Rev. 109, 573–598. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsythe, S., Drake, M. F., and Cox, C. E. (1985). Influence of applicant’s dress on interviewer’s selection decisions. J. Appl. Psychol. 70, 374–378. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.70.2.374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsythe, S. M. (1990). Effect of applicant’s clothing on interviewer’s decision to hire. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 20, 1579–1595. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1990.tb01494.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsythe, S. M., Drake, M. F., and Cox, C. A. (1984). Dress as an influence on the perceptions of management characteristics in women. Fam. Consum. Sci. Res. J. 13, 112–121. doi: 10.1177/1077727X8401300203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geniole, S. N., Denson, T. F., Dixson, B. J., Carré, J. M., and McCormick, C. M. (2015). Evidence from meta analyses of the facial width-to-height ratio as an evolved cue of threat. PLoS One 10:e0132726. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glick, P., Larsen, S., Johnson, C., and Branstiter, H. (2005). Evaluations of sexy women in low-and high-status jobs. Psychol. Women Q. 29, 389–395. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2005.00238.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goktepe, J. R., and Schneier, C. E. (1989). Role of sex, gender roles, and attraction in predicting emergent leaders. J. Appl. Psychol. 74, 165–167. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.74.1.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouda-Vossos, A., Dixson, B. J., and Brooks, R. C. (2016). Sexual conflict and gender gap effects: associations between social context and sex on rated attractiveness and economic status. PLoS One 11:e0146269. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0146269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouda-Vossos, A., Nakagawa, S., Dixson, B. J. W., and Brooks, R. C. (2018). Mate choice copying in humans: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 4, 364–384. doi: 10.1007/s40750-018-0099-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamermesh, D. S., and Biddle, J. E. (1993). Beauty and the labor market. Natl. Bureau Econ. Res. 84, 1174–1194. doi: 10.3386/w4518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanson, F. I., Olson, J., and Russell, J. (1991). ‘Attractiveness and Income for Men and Women. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 21, 1039–1057. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1991.tb00458.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., and Tamkins, M. M. (2004). Penalties for success: reactions to women who succeed at male gender-typed tasks. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 416–427. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, A. K., Hunt, J., Welling, L. L., Cárdenas, R. A., Rotella, M. A., Wheatley, J., et al. (2013). Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men’s traits. Evol. Hum. Behav. 34, 334–341. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01633.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hill, S. E., and Buss, D. M. (2008). The mere presence of opposite-sex others on judgments of sexual and romantic desirability: opposite effects for men and women. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 635–647. doi: 10.1177/0146167207313728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopcroft, R. L. (2006). Sex, status, and reproductive success in the contemporary United States. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 104–120. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howlett, N., Pine, K., Orakçıoğlu, I., and Fletcher, B. (2013). The influence of clothing on first impressions: rapid and positive responses to minor changes in male attire. J. Fashion Mark. Manage. 17, 38–48. doi: 10.1108/13612021311305128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howlett, N., Pine, K. J., Cahill, N., Orakçıoğlu, İ., and Fletcher, B. C. (2015). Unbuttoned: the interaction between provocativeness of female work attire and occupational status. Sex Roles 72, 105–116. doi: 10.1007/s11199-015-0450-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janif, J. Z., Brooks, R. C., and Dixson, B. J. (2014). Negative frequency-dependent preferences and variation in male facial hair. Biol. Lett. 10:20130958. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janif, J. Z., Brooks, R. C., and Dixson, B. J. (2015). Are preferences for women’s hair color frequency dependent? Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 1, 54–71. doi: 10.1007/s40750-014-0008-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasieńska, G., Ziomkiewicz, A., Ellison, P. T., Lipson, S. F., and Thune, I. (2004). Large breasts and narrow waists indicate high reproductive potential in women. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 271, 1213–1217. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.2712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., and Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.

Google Scholar

Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., and Sloan, S. (1948). Sexual Behavior in the Human Male, Vol. 1. Philadelphia, PA: Saunders.

Google Scholar

Kordsmeyer, T. L., Hunt, J., Puts, D. A., Ostner, J., and Penke, L. (2018). The relative importance of intra-and intersexual selection on human male sexually dimorphic traits. Evol. Hum. Behav. 39, 424–436. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.03.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwantes, C. T., Lin, I. Y., Gidak, N., and Schmidt, K. (2011). The effect of attire on expected occupational outcomes for male employees. Psychol. Men Masc. 12, 166–180. doi: 10.1037/a0020872

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavin, A., Carr, D., and Davies, T. (2009). Does image matter? Female and male student perceptions of the impact of the female professor’s attire. J. Bus. Account. 2, 93–106.

Google Scholar

Lavin, A. M., Carr, D. L., and Davies, T. L. (2009). The male professor’s attire and student perceptions of instructional quality. Res. High. Educ. J. 4:1.

Google Scholar

Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., and Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The necessities and luxuries of mate preferences: testing the tradeoffs. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 947–955. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, B. S. (1990). Sex, power, and resources: male and female strategies of resource acquisition. Int. J. Contemp. Soc. 27, 49–73.

Google Scholar

Low, B. S. (2000). “Sex, wealth, and fertility: Old rules, new environments,” in Adaptation and Human Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective, eds L. Cronk, N. Chagon, and W. Irons (New York, NY: Aldline), 323–344.

Google Scholar

Luoto, S. (2019). An updated theoretical framework for human sexual selection: From ecology, genetics, and life history to extended phenotypes. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 5, 48–102.

Google Scholar

Maestripieri, D., Henry, A., and Nickels, N. (2017). Explaining financial and prosocial biases in favor of attractive people: interdisciplinary perspectives from economics, social psychology, and evolutionary psychology. Behav. Brain Sci. 40:e19. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X16000340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mangel, M. (1990). Resource divisibility, predation and group formation. Anim. Behav. 39, 1163–1172. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80788-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcinkowska, U. M., Kozlov, M. V., Cai, H., Contreras-Garduño, J., Dixson, B. J., Oana, G. A., et al. (2014). Cross-cultural variation in men’s preference for sexual dimorphism in women’s faces. Biol. Lett. 10:20130850. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melis, A. P., and Semmann, D. (2010). How is human cooperation different? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 365, 2663–2674. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milonoff, M., Nummi, P., Nummi, O., and Pienmunne, E. (2007). Male friends, not female company, make a man more attractive. Ann. Zool. Fennici 44, 348–354.

Google Scholar

Molloy, J. T., and Potter, S. (1975). Dress for Success. Available at: https://dressforsuccess.org/

Google Scholar

Morris, T. L., Gorham, J., Cohen, S. H., and Huffman, D. (1996). Fashion in the classroom: effects of attire on student perceptions of instructors in college classes. Commun. Educ. 45, 135–148. doi: 10.1080/03634529609379043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petterson, L. J., Dixson, B. J., Little, A. C., and Vasey, P. L. (2015). Viewing time measures of sexual orientation in Samoan cisgender men who engage in sexual interactions with fa’afafine. PloS One 10:e0116529. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0116529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petterson, L. J., Dixson, B. J., Little, A. C., and Vasey, P. L. (2016). Reconsidering male bisexuality: sexual activity role and sexual attraction in Samoan men who engage in sexual interactions with Fa’afafine. Psychol. Sex. Orientat. Gend. Divers. 3, 11–26. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petterson, L. J., Dixson, B. J., Little, A. C., and Vasey, P. L. (2018). Viewing time and self-report measures of sexual attraction in Samoan cisgender and transgender androphilic males. Arch. Sex. Behav. 47, 2427–2434. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1267-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfann, G. A., Biddle, J. E., Hamermesh, D. S., and Bosman, C. M. (2000). Business success and businesses’ beauty capital. Econ. Lett. 67, 201–207. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1765(99)00255-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 157–175. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roach-Higgins, M. E., and Eicher, J. B. (1992). Dress and identity. Cloth. Text. Res. J. 10, 1–8. doi: 10.1177/0887302X9201000401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenblat, T. S. (2008). The beauty premium: physical attractiveness and gender in dictator games. Negotiat. J. 24, 465–481. doi: 10.1111/j.1571-9979.2008.00198.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmid Mast, M. (2004). Men are hierarchical, women are egalitarian: an implicit gender stereotype. Swiss J. Psychol. 63, 107–111. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.63.2.107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schopler, J., Insko, C. A., Wieselquist, J., Pemberton, M., Witcher, B., Kozar, R., et al. (2001). When groups are more competitive than individuals: the domain of the discontinuity effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 80, 632–644. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shuler, G. A., and McCord, D. M. (2010). Determinants of male attractiveness:“Hotness” ratings as a function of perceived resources. Am. J. Psychol. Res. 6, 10–23.

Google Scholar

Singh, D., Dixson, B. J., Jessop, T. S., Morgan, B., and Dixson, A. F. (2010). Cross-cultural consensus for waist–hip ratio and women’s attractiveness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 176–181. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. A. (2004). Why do good hunters have higher reproductive success? Hum. Nat. 15, 343–364. doi: 10.1007/s12110-004-1013-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Townsend, J. M., and Levy, G. D. (1990). Effects of potential partners’ physical attractiveness and socioeconomic status on sexuality and partner selection. Arch. Sex. Behav. 19, 149–164. doi: 10.1007/BF01542229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valentova, J. V., Varella, M., Bártová, K., Štìrbová, Z., and Dixson, B. J. W. (2017). Mate preferences and choices for facial and body hair in heterosexual women and homosexual men: effects of sex, population, homogamy, and imprinting-like effects. Evol. Hum. Behav. 38, 241–248. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Rueden, C., Gurven, M., and Kaplan, H. (2010). Why do men seek status? Fitness payoffs to dominance and prestige. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 278, 2223–2232. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Von Rueden, C. R. (2011). The Acquisition of Social Status by Males in Small-Scale Human Societies (with an Emphasis on the Tsimane of Bolivia). Santa Barbara, CA: University of California.

Von Rueden, C. R., and Jaeggi, A. V. (2016). Men’s status and reproductive success in 33 nonindustrial societies: effects of subsistence, marriage system, and reproductive strategy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 10824–10829. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606800113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vugt, M. V., Cremer, D. D., and Janssen, D. P. (2007). Gender differences in cooperation and competition: the male-warrior hypothesis. Psychol. Sci. 18, 19–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., and Griskevicius, V. (2013). Conspicuous consumption, relationships, and rivals: women’s luxury products as signals to other women. J. Consum. Res. 40, 834–854. doi: 10.1086/673256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waynforth, D. (2007). Mate choice copying in humans. Hum. Nat. 18, 264–271. doi: 10.1007/s12110-007-9004-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, P. M. (2002). Community strategies: mainstreaming sustainable development and strategic planning? Sustain. Dev. 10, 197–205. doi: 10.1002/sd.197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sex difference, status, attire, attractiveness, mate choice copying, economics

Citation: Gouda-Vossos A, Brooks RC and Dixson BJW (2019) The Interplay Between Economic Status and Attractiveness, and the Importance of Attire in Mate Choice Judgments. Front. Psychol. 10:462. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00462

Received: 05 September 2018; Accepted: 15 February 2019;
Published: 21 March 2019.

Edited by:

Alex L. Jones, Swansea University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Viktoria Mileva, University of Stirling, United Kingdom
Andrew Thomas, Swansea University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2019 Gouda-Vossos, Brooks and Dixson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barnaby J. W. Dixson, Yi5kaXhzb25AdXEuZWR1LmF1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.