- 1School of Education Science, Xinyang Normal University, Xinyang, China
- 2Key Laboratory of Cognition and Personality, Southwest University, Chongqing, China
Previous research demonstrated the associations between cognitive biases toward food cues and binge eating (BE) behavior. To determine the characteristics of conflict monitoring to food cues in women who binge eat and non-eating disordered controls, a flanker task featured high-caloric food and low-caloric food images was used to examine conflict monitoring with measures of accuracy and reaction time. Women who binge eat displayed longer reaction times (RTs) to incongruent trials (i.e., flanked by pictures from the different category) than to congruent trials (i.e., flanked by pictures from the same category), while controls showed no such difference. This finding demonstrated women who binge eat displayed a general flanker effect toward food-related stimuli compared to controls. Faster reaction times in response to high-caloric food images disturbed by low-caloric food images predicted lower self-reported motor impulsiveness in the women who binge eat, but not in controls. These data suggest a relative conflict monitoring deficit in women with BE pathology.
Introduction
Binge eating (BE) refers to consumption of an objectively large amount of food within a short period of time, accompanied by a perceived loss of control over eating. Frequent BE is a core diagnostic feature of clinical eating disorders, and persistent symptoms increase risk for Binge-Eating Disorder (BED) according to the 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Diagnosis of Bulimia Nervosa (BN) requires regular binge eating and recurrent compensatory responses to such episodes. BE is prevalent among people seeking to lose or maintain weight (Coker et al., 2015), and affects up to 40% of college-age women in United States (Saules et al., 2009). Furthermore, binge eating behavior is increasing in China (Chen and Jackson, 2008; Tong et al., 2014), and over one third of Chinese adolescents and young adults have reported BE (Chen and Jackson, 2008). Indeed, individuals with binge eating often experience depression, interpersonal problems and reduced quality of life (Ambwani et al., 2015; Rosenbaum and White, 2015).
Cognitive models of obesity suggest reduced inhibitory control is an important causal and maintenance factor in obesity and eating disorders (Jansen et al., 2015). Recent studies have linked cognitive impairment toward food-related stimuli to disordered eating behavior, such as binge eating (Mobbs et al., 2011; Svaldi et al., 2014a,b; Manasse et al., 2016; Kollei et al., 2018; Leehr et al., 2018). An essential component of cognitive control is conflict monitoring (Luna et al., 2015). Conflict monitoring refers to the ability to detect information conflict and reactively increase cognitive control recruitment (Botvinick et al., 2001; Teubner-Rhodes et al., 2016). Poor conflict monitoring might contribute to the frequent initiation of eating episodes, which is associated with obesity. Moreover, poor ability to inhibit an already-initiated motor response (e.g., eating) might contribute to the development of BE (Manasse et al., 2016). However, the knowledge about the conflict monitoring in those with BE is still scarce.
The Stroop color-word interference task has been used as a measure of conflict monitoring and response selection among individuals with eating disorders (Duchesne et al., 2010; Galioto et al., 2012; Balodis et al., 2013; Kittel et al., 2017), though mixed findings have been observed between individuals with BE/BED and controls. For example, Kittel et al. (2017) investigated the conflict monitoring in obese individuals with BED as compared to obese individuals without BED and normal weight participants using a Stroop color-word interference task. They found obese individuals with BED and without BED performed worse than normal weight participants, while the obese individuals with BED and obese individuals without BED did not differ (Kittel et al., 2017). The results hint at general impaired conflict monitoring in youth with BED and obesity in comparison with the normal weight group. However null effects are also existed (Duchesne et al., 2010; Galioto et al., 2012). One potential explanation for the mixed findings is that neutral stimuli (e.g., letters of the alphabet) are probably less rewarding and emotionally arousing compared to food-related images or actual food stimuli, reducing the ecological validity of Stroop tasks (Berner et al., 2017). A meta-analysis of the Stroop task on eating disorders demonstrated that individuals with binge/purge eating disorders showed more cognitive control deficits when food-specific stimuli were used compared to controls (Wu et al., 2014). Cognitive control deficits in those with BE may be especially pronounced when relevant stimuli are used, i.e., food cues (Manasse et al., 2016). In a functional magnetic resonance imaging study, Lee et al. (2017) modified the Stroop match-to-sample task using two different conditions, food-related condition and neutral condition, to investigate the effect of food stimuli on cognitive controls in individuals with eating disorder. The cues with different colors consisted of three letters (i.e., XXX). Participants matched the color of the cue (i.e., “XXX”) to the written color of a Stroop word target or to the color that the Stroop word means, which appeared after an interference stimulus (e.g., food-related and neutral pictures). No significant differences were observed in accuracy and reaction time between the BED group and control group (Lee et al., 2017). However, BED group demonstrated stronger activations in the ventral striatum in response to food images compared to control group, indicating BED patients exhibited increased reward sensitivity without inhibitory control (Lee et al., 2017). Together, these findings suggest that individuals with BE/BED are likely to show conflict monitoring deficits though mixed findings have been existed.
The flanker task is another paradigm that has been widely used to assess the ability to inhibit distraction and adapt to conflict (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974). The Stroop-like tasks elicited responses to non-symbolic information (e.g., color of a letter), whereas flanker task elicited responses to symbolic information (e.g., arrow meaning) (Freitas et al., 2007). In the flanker task, participants respond to the “target” stimulus displayed in the middle of the screen, which are flanked on each side by either the same as the target (i.e., congruent, > > > > > ) or different from the target (i.e., incongruent, > > < > >). Participants are instructed to attend to the target stimuli and ignore the non-target stimuli. Because of the interference elicited by the incongruent non-target stimuli, participants generally required longer reaction times to incongruent trials than to congruent trials (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974; Coles et al., 1985). This “flanker effect” phenomenon, that is, slower responses to incongruent trials, provides an index of the conflict monitoring. Due to the lower levels of cognitive control being successfully applied, the flanker effect will be larger, which indicates a greater conflict monitoring deficit (Husted et al., 2016).
Conflict monitoring to food-related stimuli has been examined using flanker task in previous research (Forestell et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2012; Husted et al., 2016). For example, using a flanker task involved high caloric food-cues and neutral pictures, Meule et al. (2012) found restrained eaters responded faster to high-calorie food targets as compared to neutral targets than unrestrained eaters, suggesting a low self-regulatory ability in restrained eaters (Meule et al., 2012).
Based on the literature outlined above, a food-related flanker task with high- and low-calorie food images was introduced to examine the interference from food stimuli on cognitive control in women who binge eat. We hypothesized that the women who binge eat would show greater deficits in response conflict than the control group on both stimulus types of the flanker task.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Thirty-one undergraduate women with BE and 33 healthy control participants were recruited from a large Chinese university. Average age was M = 20.91 years (SD = 1.52), and the mean BMI was M = 20.68 kg/m2 (SD = 2.66). All women who binge eat reported at least one binge eating episode per week over the past 3 months (M = 2.52, SD = 2.12, range: 1–12) on the Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (EDDS; Stice et al., 2000) as well as an absence of compensatory behavior following BE episodes. Healthy control participants reported no current nor past eating disorder according to DSM-5 criteria. In order to standardize hunger levels, all participants were instructed to refrain from eating and drinking caffeinated beverages for 12 h before the study which occurred between 8 and 11 am the next day.
Materials
Forty-eight colorful images each of high-calorie foods (e.g., hamburger, doughnuts, and fried chicken wings) and low-calorie foods (e.g., tomatoes, carrots) were used. All images were taken from a set previously used in our studies (Lyu and Jackson, 2016; Lyu et al., 2016, 2017), and edited to be homogeneous with respect to background color.
Procedure
This study was performed in accordance with the guidelines of the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. The study was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Xinyang Normal University. Potential volunteers were recruited via the campus electronic bulletin board system and flyers. Screening materials including demographic items and the Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (EDDS) were completed (Stice et al., 2000). Participants arrived at the lab individually for their scheduled appointment. Upon arriving, volunteers were informed of the general research focus (i.e., attention toward different kinds of food images) and gave the informed consent prior to their participation. Subjective feeling of hunger were assessed individually on a visual analog scale from 1 to 9 (1: not hungry at all; 9: very hungry).
The flanker task procedures were programmed using E-Prime 2.0. The flanker task comes from prior food flanker studies (Forestell et al., 2012; Meule et al., 2012; Husted et al., 2016). In the food flanker task, the central targets were pictures of either high-calorie foods or low-calorie foods images, which were flanked by pictures either from the same category (congruent condition) or distractors from the other category (incongruent condition). Participants were instructed to respond to the centrally presented picture (i.e., target) as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing a left or right button to indicate whether the target was a high-calorie food or a low-calorie food item (mapping was counterbalanced across participants). A 2 min practice session commenced first to ensure that participants become familiar with the procedure before the formal study. Each trial consisted of a prestimulus baseline during which a fixation cross was presented in the middle of the screen for 1000 ms. This was followed by a stimulus array contained one target picture and two pictures on either side of the target (i.e., flankers) followed by a blank screen. After the target appeared in the center of the screen, participants were instructed to respond as quickly as possible. The target remained on the screen until a response was detected or 1500 ms passed. Pictures for flankers and targets were randomly drawn from the same food image set of high or low calorie. In the congruent condition, target and flanker pictures were the homogeneous: (1) all the pictures were high-calorie foods (HHH); (2) all the pictures were low-calorie foods (LLL). In the incongruent condition, target and flanker pictures differed: (3) the target was a high-calorie food picture and the flankers were two low-calorie food pictures (LHL); (4) the target was a low-calorie food picture and the flankers were two high-calorie food pictures (HLH). Inter-trial intervals varied randomly from 1000 to 3000 ms to avoid time conditioning. The task included 192 trials in total divided in three blocks.
After the flanker task, the self-report scales described below, except the EDDS, were completed in a quiet room. Finally, participants were required to make a guess regarding the main research purpose, and then were debriefed about the study hypotheses. No participants identified BE or response conflict as foci of the experiment. Participants received 20 yuan as compensation.
Questionnaires
Eating Disorder Diagnostic Scale (EDDS; Stice et al., 2000)
The EDDS is a 22-item self-report scale that based on Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-IV criteria for Anorexia Nervosa (AN), BN, and BED. In the present study, EDDS was used to identify individuals with BE as well as to rule an eating disorder diagnosis among the control group members. An overall composite calculated from the sum of z-scores of the first 18 items also provided a symptom severity rating. The scale has satisfactory test–retest reliability, a high level of internal consistency, and excellent concordance with diagnoses based on structured interviews and other self-report measures of eating disturbances (Stice et al., 2000, 2004). The Chinese version of the EDDS also has satisfactory reliability and validity in samples of mainland Chinese adolescents and young adults (Jackson and Chen, 2014, 2015). The consistency was α = 0.76 in this sample.
Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Chinese (BIS-C; Li et al., 2011)
The BIS-C, consisting of attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness subscales, was used to assess rash-spontaneous behavior (Lyu et al., 2016, 2017). BIS-C has satisfactory psychometrics among Chinese undergraduates (Li et al., 2011). For this study, the internal consistency of the overall BIS-C was α = 0.74. Alphas values were acceptable for the three subscales (αs ≥ 0.71).
Uncontrolled Eating Scale (UES; Karlsson et al., 2000)
The binge eating level was assessed by summing seven items from the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R18 in the formal study. The questionnaire has been found to have sound reliability and validity (Anglé et al., 2009; Lyu et al., 2017). Its alpha value was 0.84 in the current study.
Data Analyses
Data were analyzed with IBM SPSS Statistics 20.0. Group differences on demographics, impulsivity, and uncontrolled eating were assessed via t-tests. A 2 (Group: binge eating versus non-binge eating) × 2 (Food Type: high-calorie food versus low-calorie food) × 2 (Flanker: congruent versus incongruent) ANOVA was performed to assess accuracy and RT differences. Individual trials on the flanker task with errors and response times more than three standard deviations above or below the mean (i.e., 7% of trials) were excluded from analysis (Forestell et al., 2012). Mean accuracy and reaction times were calculated for each of the 3-picture array combinations (i.e., HHH, LLL, LHL, and HLH) for each participant. Mauchly’s test of sphericity was violated in ANOVA analyses; thus, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were used to reduce risk for Type I errors.
Results
Group Differences on Demographics and Characteristics
As show in Table 1, women who binge eat displayed higher levels of motor impulsiveness and uncontrolled eating relative to controls. However, no group differences were observed on age, BMI, year in university, or hunger ratings.
Differences in Accuracy
The repeated measures ANOVA performed on accuracy revealed an interaction effect of Food Type × Flanker, F(1, 62) = 14.42, p < 0.001, = 0.19. Simple effects analyses indicated that a better performance of judgment was observed for congruent trials (M = 0.94, SE = 0.02) compared to incongruent trials (M = 0.91, SE = 0.02) with low-calorie food images as targets (p = 0.01), while a better performance of judgment was shown for incongruent trials (M = 0.94, SE = 0.01) than congruent ones (M = 0.92, SE = 0.02) with high-calorie food images as targets (p = 0.01). No other main effect or interaction effect was significant (p’s > 0.05).
Differences in Reaction Time (RT)
A main effect for Food Type, F(1, 62) = 5.16, p = 0.02, = 0.20, indicated the sample responded more quickly to low-calorie food compared to high-calorie food images (M = 726.27 ms versus M = 739.99 ms, p = 0.03). As expected, the main effect for Flanker, F(1, 62) = 14.36, p < 0.001, = 0.19, evidenced a general flanker effect: congruent trials elicited significantly shorter RTs in the entire sample (M = 727.28 ms versus M = 738.98 ms, p < 0.001).
Simple effects analyses of the Group × Flank interaction, F(1, 62) = 6.69, p = 0.01, = 0.10, indicated women who binge eat displayed a significant flanker effect (p < 0.001), while no such effect was observed in the controls (p = 0.39) (see Figures 1, 2). The Food Type × Flanker interaction, F(1, 62) = 8.35, p = 0.01, = 0.12, indicated a strong flanker effect to low-calorie food images (p < 0.001), but not to high-calorie food images (p = 0.45). Furthermore, shorter RTs were observed to low-calorie food images compared to high-calorie food images in congruent trials (M = 716.16 ms versus M = 738.39 ms, p = 0.01), but no such difference was found in incongruent trials (M = 736.37 ms versus M = 741.58 ms, p = 0.43). For the Food Type × Group interaction, F(1, 62) = 4.11, p = 0.04, = 0.06, controls displayed shorter RTs to low-calorie food images than to high-calorie food images (M = 712.68 ms versus M = 738.65 ms, p = 0.01), but no such difference was found in women who binge eat (M = 741.32 ms versus M = 739.85 ms, p = 0.87). No other differences were observed significant.
Figure 1. Reaction time data for women with binge eating. Bars represent standard error of the mean (SE). Hi-Cal, High-calorie; Lo-Cal, Low-calorie.
Figure 2. Reaction time data for controls. Bars represent standard error of the mean (SE). Hi-Cal, High-calorie; Lo-Cal, Low-calorie.
To disentangle significant relations between flank task responses and self-reported measures, supplementary correlation analyses were performed within group. Women who binge eat displayed negative correlations between self-reported motor impulsiveness score and accuracy rates in different conditions (HHH: r = -0.41, p = 0.02; LHL: r = -0.46, p = 0.01; LLL: r = -0.47, p = 0.01), though a marginally significant correlation was observed (HLH: r = -0.35, p = 0.05). Conversely, control group members showed no such correlations between self-reported motor impulsiveness and accuracy rates for HHH (r = 0.01, p = 0.99), LHL (r = -0.01, p = 0.98), LLL (r = -0.01, p = 0.99), and HLH (r = -0.03, p = 0.88). Shorter RTs in response to high-caloric food images disturbed by low-caloric food images (i.e., LHL) were related to higher motor impulsiveness in women who binge eat (r = 0.44, p = 0.01) but not in controls (r = 0.03, p = 0.87). No other correlations were observed significant.
Discussion
The present study investigated the conflict monitoring in women who binge eat during exposure to food cues. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that used food-related flanker task to assess conflict monitoring in women with BE and controls. Regarding the central research focus, women with BE show greater deficits in response conflict on the food-related flanker task, which support the main hypothesis.
Compared to controls, participants with BE reported higher trait impulsivity and uncontrolled eating, in line with the criterion of BE and previous research (Schag et al., 2013; Svaldi et al., 2014a), especially on motor impulsivity (Nasser et al., 2004; Galanti et al., 2007; Lyu et al., 2016, 2017). The motor impulsivity is defined as acting without thinking (Stanford et al., 2009), and could be used to distinguish eating disorder subtypes (Tillman and Wiens, 2011). Previous research have found the motor impulsivity was positively correlated with test meal intake and mood rated before consuming the test meal (Nasser et al., 2004; Galanti et al., 2007), and positively predicted binge eating and general eating pathology (Meule and Platte, 2015). Motor impulsivity involves the ability to suppress a prepotent yet inapposite motor response (Chamberlain and Sahakian, 2007). On this basis, the negative correlations between motor impulsiveness and performance of judgment indexed with accuracy may reflect poor response inhibition for women who binge eat, which may make them vulnerable to binge eating.
Reaction times for incongruent trials in the flanker task were found to be significantly longer in women with BE, indicating a deficit of conflict monitoring in BE. The findings are comparable to the results of a study of combined electroencephalography (EEG) and eye tracking. In the food-related antisaccade task, Leehr et al. (2018) observed smaller N2 latencies in overweight individuals with BED compared with overweight individuals without BED, suggesting that the conflict processing might be less thorough in the overweight individuals with BED (Leehr et al., 2018). Our finding extends evidences of previous literature focusing on conflict monitoring in individuals with binge eat. In previous research, the Stroop color-word interference task was performed to assess the inhibitory control of individuals with eating disorders (Duchesne et al., 2010; Galioto et al., 2012; Balodis et al., 2013; Kittel et al., 2017). The mixed findings mentioned above could be explained by the methodological procedures employed. Corresponding to the differential mechanisms of the interference, the response of the flanker task and Stroop task may reflect different cognitive processes. For example, Tillman and Wiens (2011) assessed effects of variation in proportions of incongruent trials on response conflict in the Stroop and flanker task. ERP findings demonstrated that the flanker N200 and Stroop N450 may reflect different cognitive processes (Tillman and Wiens, 2011). The flanker N200 may reflect attentional control processes used to focus attention on task-relevant aspects of a situation, while the Stroop N450 may reflect the perceptual conflict processing (Tillman and Wiens, 2011). The flanker task contains two sources of conflict, one related to the responses, and the other related to the stimulus itself (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974). The flankers involve little semantic interference, so the motor conflict may occur faster than does the conflict in the Stroop, which requires also semantic processing (Pires et al., 2014). Women who binge eat with high level of motor impulsiveness displayed longer reaction times in responding to high-caloric food images flanked by low-caloric food images (i.e., LHL) may reflect the poor inhibition of responses in the women who binge eat when exposure to food cues.
One alternative explanation from an evolutionary perspective is that the binge eating behavior may be suitably conceptualized as an “evolutionary mismatch” condition arising from a maladaptive gene–environment interaction (Levitan et al., 2004; Davis, 2015; Abed, 2016; Mayhew et al., 2018). The strong hedonic response to food was an undeniable survival benefit during the huntergatherer era, which mismatched to our current environment (Davis, 2014). The deficit of conflict monitoring in women who binge eat when food stimuli present may reflect an adaptative strategy in early environments.
A number of study limitations should be mentioned. First, given possible gender differences in BE (Rosenbaum and White, 2015), results do not necessarily apply to BE men. In addition, findings may not generalize fully to a wider population. Second, it would be beneficial to replicate these findings in food-related Stroop task. In these tasks, the food or eating words (e.g., cake, cream, and diet) are presented. Participants must name the color in which each word is printed and ignore the meaning of the words (Black et al., 1998; Johansson et al., 2005; Rosenbaum and White, 2015). Some studies have found that the Stroop effect is enhanced when food-related stimuli are primed in restrained eaters (Cooper and Fairburn, 1992; Perpiñá et al., 1993). Further, only high-caloric and low-caloric food stimuli were used, the conflict monitoring of food versus non-food stimuli cannot be revealed. Future research should examine the comparison of food versus non-food stimuli to investigate the food-specific conflict monitoring in the cognitive control processes. Finally, higher impulsive responding was correlated with more calories consumed in average weight samples (Nederkoorn et al., 2009). It would be interesting to investigate the correlation between the response conflict to high- and low-calorie foods and the consumption of these foods in future research.
Conclusion
The results provide initial evidence that women who binge eat appear to exhibit greater response conflict deficits with food cues in a flanker task featured high-caloric food and low-caloric food images relative to those who do not have binge eating episodes. These findings suggest that individuals who binge eat tend to rely on automatic processing and respond to processing conflicting food stimuli less effectively. Such an understanding will assist to prevent disordered eating in binge-eating populations, and to develop psychological interventions more effectively.
Ethics Statement
This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the ethical guidelines of the American Psychological Association. The protocol was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the Xinyang Normal University. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.
Author Contributions
ZL designed the project. SL and MQ performed the experiment. ZL and PZ analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript.
Funding
This study was supported by Chinese National Natural Science Foundation (31800925) and the Nanhu Scholars Program for Young Scholars of XYNU to the corresponding author.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Abed, R. T. (2016). Evolutionary theories in disordered eating psychopathology. Br. J. Psychiatry 209, 351–352. doi: 10.1192/bjp.209.4.351a
Ambwani, S., Roche, M. J., Minnick, A. M., and Pincus, A. L. (2015). Negative affect, interpersonal perception, and binge eating behavior: an experience sampling study. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 48, 715–726. doi: 10.1002/eat.22410
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Anglé, S., Engblom, J., Eriksson, T., Kautiainen, S., Saha, M.-T., Lindfors, P., et al. (2009). Three factor eating questionnaire-R18 as a measure of cognitive restraint, uncontrolled eating and emotional eating in a sample of young finnish females. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 6, 41–47. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-41
Balodis, I. M., Molina, N. D., Kober, H., Worhunsky, P. D., White, M. A., Sinha, R., et al. (2013). Divergent neural substrates of inhibitory control in binge eating disorder relative to other manifestations of obesity. Obesity 21, 367–377. doi: 10.1002/oby.20068
Berner, L. A., Winter, S. R., Matheson, B. E., Benson, L., and Lowe, M. R. (2017). Behind binge eating: a review of food-specific adaptations of neurocognitive and neuroimaging tasks. Physiol. Behav. 176, 59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.037
Black, C. M. D., Wilson, G. T., Labouvie, E., and Heffernan, K. (1998). Selective processing of eating disorder relevant stimuli: does the stroop test provide an objective measure of bulimia nervosa? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 22, 329–333. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199711)22:3<329::AID-EAT13>3.0.CO;2-T
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol. Rev. 108, 624–652. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.108.3.624
Chamberlain, S. R., and Sahakian, B. J. (2007). The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 20, 255–261. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989
Chen, H., and Jackson, T. (2008). Prevalence and sociodemographic correlates of eating disorder endorsements among adolescents and young adults from China. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 16, 375–385. doi: 10.1002/erv.837
Coker, E. L., von Lojewski, A., Luscombe, G. M., and Abraham, S. F. (2015). The difficulty in defining binge eating in obese women: how it affects prevalence levels in presurgical bariatric patients. Eat. Behav. 17, 130–135. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.01.014
Coles, M. G., Gratton, G., Bashore, T. R., Eriksen, C. W., and Donchin, E. (1985). A psychophysiological investigation of the continuous flow model of human information processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 11, 529–553. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.11.5.529
Cooper, M. J., and Fairburn, C. G. (1992). Selective processing of eating, weight and shape related words in patients with eating disorders and dieters. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 31, 363–365. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb01007.x
Davis, C. (2014). Evolutionary and neuropsychological perspectives on addictive behaviors and addictive substances: relevance to the “food addiction” construct. Subst. Abuse Rehabil. 5, 129–137. doi: 10.2147/SAR.S56835
Davis, C. (2015). The epidemiology and genetics of binge eating disorder (BED). CNS Spectr. 20, 522–529. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000462
Duchesne, M., Mattos, P., Appolinário, J. C., de Freitas, S. R., Coutinho, G., Santos, C., et al. (2010). Assessment of executive functions in obese individuals with binge eating disorder. Braz. J. Psychiatr. 32, 381–388. doi: 10.1590/S1516-44462010000400011
Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267
Forestell, C. A., Lau, P., Gyurovski, I. I., Dickter, C. L., and Haque, S. S. (2012). Attentional biases to foods: the effects of caloric content and cognitive restraint. Appetite 59, 748–754. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.07.006
Freitas, A. L., Bahar, M., Yang, S., and Banai, R. (2007). Contextual adjustments in cognitive control across tasks. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1040–1043. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02022.x
Galanti, K., Gluck, M. E., and Geliebter, A. (2007). Test meal intake in obese binge eaters in relation to impulsivity and compulsivity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 40, 727–732. doi: 10.1002/eat.20441
Galioto, R., Spitznagel, M. B., Strain, G., Devlin, M., Cohen, R., Paul, R., et al. (2012). Cognitive function in morbidly obese individuals with and without binge eating disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 53, 490–495. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.002
Husted, M., Banks, A. P., and Seiss, E. (2016). Eating behaviour associated with differences in conflict adaptation for food pictures. Appetite 105, 630–637. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.07.003
Jackson, T., and Chen, H. (2014). Risk factors for disordered eating during early and middle adolescence: a two year longitudinal study of mainland Chinese boys and girls. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 791–802. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9823-z
Jackson, T., and Chen, H. (2015). Features of objectified body consciousness and sociocultural perspectives as risk factors for disordered eating among late-adolescent women and men. J. Couns. Psychol. 62, 741–752. doi: 10.1037/cou0000096
Jansen, A., Houben, K., and Roefs, A. (2015). A cognitive profile of obesity and its translation into new interventions. Front. Psychol. 6:1807. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01807
Johansson, L., Ghaderi, A., and Andersson, G. (2005). Stroop interference for food- and body-related words: a meta-analysis. Eat. Behav. 6, 271–281. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.11.001
Karlsson, J., Persson, L., Sjöström, L., and Sullivan, M. (2000). Psychometric properties and factor structure of the three-factor eating questionnaire (TFEQ) in obese men and women. Results from the Swedish obese subjects (SOS) study. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 24, 1715–1725. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801442
Kittel, R., Schmidt, R., and Hilbert, A. (2017). Executive functions in adolescents with binge-eating disorder and obesity. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 50, 933–941. doi: 10.1002/eat.22714
Kollei, I., Rustemeier, M., Schroeder, S., Jongen, S., Herpertz, S., and Loeber, S. (2018). Cognitive control functions in individuals with obesity with and without binge-eating disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 233–240. doi: 10.1002/eat.22824
Lee, J. E., Namkoong, K., and Jung, Y.-C. (2017). Impaired prefrontal cognitive control over interference by food images in binge-eating disorder and bulimia nervosa. Neurosci. Lett. 651, 95–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.04.054
Leehr, E. J., Schag, K., Dresler, T., Grosse-Wentrup, M., Hautzinger, M., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2018). Food specific inhibitory control under negative mood in binge-eating disorder: evidence from a multimethod approach. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 51, 112–123. doi: 10.1002/eat.22818
Levitan, R. D., Masellis, M., Basile, V. S., Lam, R. W., Kaplan, A. S., Davis, C., et al. (2004). The dopamine-4 receptor gene associated with binge eating and weight gain in women with seasonal affective disorder: an evolutionary perspective. Biol. Psychiatry 56, 665–669. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.013
Li, X. Y., Phillps, M. R., Xu, D., Zhang, Y. L., Yang, S. J., Tong, Y. S., et al. (2011). Reliability and validity of an adapted Chinese version of Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. Chin. Ment. Health J. 25, 610–615.
Luna, B., Marek, S., Larsen, B., Tervo-Clemmens, B., and Chahal, R. (2015). An integrative model of the maturation of cognitive control. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 38, 151–170. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071714-034054
Lyu, Z., and Jackson, T. (2016). Acute stressors reduce neural inhibition to food cues and increase eating among binge eating disorder symptomatic women. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10:188. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00188
Lyu, Z., Zheng, P., Chen, H., and Jackson, T. (2017). Approach and inhibition responses to external food cues among average-weight women who binge eat and weight-matched controls. Appetite 108, 367–374. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.10.025
Lyu, Z., Zheng, P., and Jackson, T. (2016). Attention disengagement difficulties among average weight women who binge eat. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 24, 286–293. doi: 10.1002/erv.2438
Manasse, S. M., Goldstein, S. P., Wyckoff, E., Forman, E. M., Juarascio, A. S., Butryn, M. L., et al. (2016). Slowing down and taking a second look: inhibitory deficits associated with binge eating are not food-specific. Appetite 96, 555–559. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.025
Mayhew, A. J., Pigeyre, M., Couturier, J., and Meyre, D. (2018). An evolutionary genetic perspective of eating disorders. Neuroendocrinology 106, 292–306. doi: 10.1159/000484525
Meule, A., and Platte, P. (2015). Facets of impulsivity interactively predict body fat and binge eating in young women. Appetite 87, 352–357. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.01.003
Meule, A., Vögele, C., and Kübler, A. (2012). Restrained eating is related to accelerated reaction to high caloric foods and cardiac autonomic dysregulation. Appetite 58, 638–644. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.023
Mobbs, O., Iglesias, K., Golay, A., and Van der Linden, M. (2011). Cognitive deficits in obese persons with and without binge eating disorder. Investigation using a mental flexibility task. Appetite 57, 263–271. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.04.023
Nasser, J. A., Gluck, M. E., and Geliebter, A. (2004). Impulsivity and test meal intake in obese binge eating women. Appetite 43, 303–307. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2004.04.006
Nederkoorn, C., Guerrieri, R., Havermans, R., Roefs, A., and Jansen, A. (2009). The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket. Int. J. Obes. 33, 905–912. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.98
Perpiñá, C., Hemsley, D., Treasure, J., and De Silva, P. (1993). Is the selective information processing of food and body words specific to patients with eating disorders? Int. J. Eat. Disord. 14, 359–366. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(199311)14:3<359::AID-EAT2260140314>3.0.CO;2-G
Pires, L., Leitão, J., Guerrini, C., and Simões, M. R. (2014). Event-related brain potentials in the study of inhibition: cognitive control, source localization and age-related modulations. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 461–490. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9275-4
Rosenbaum, D. L., and White, K. S. (2015). The relation of anxiety, depression, and stress to binge eating behavior. J. Health Psychol. 20, 887–898. doi: 10.1177/1359105315580212
Saules, K. K., Collings, A. S., Hoodin, F., Angelella, N. E., Alschuler, K., Ivezaj, V., et al. (2009). The contributions of weight problem perception, BMI, gender, mood, and smoking status to binge eating among college students. Eat. Behav. 10, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.07.010
Schag, K., Teufel, M., Junne, F., Preissl, H., Hautzinger, M., Zipfel, S., et al. (2013). Impulsivity in binge eating disorder: food cues elicit increased reward responses and disinhibition. PLoS One 8:e76542. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076542
Stanford, M. S., Mathias, C. W., Dougherty, D. M., Lake, S. L., Anderson, N. E., and Patton, J. H. (2009). Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: an update and review. Pers. Individ. Differ. 47, 385–395. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.008
Stice, E., Fisher, M., and Martinez, E. (2004). Eating disorder diagnostic scale: additional evidence of reliability and validity. Psychol. Assess. 16, 60–71. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.16.1.60
Stice, E., Telch, C. F., and Rizvi, S. L. (2000). Development and validation of the eating disorder diagnostic scale: a brief self-report measure of anorexia, bulimia, and binge-eating disorder. Psychol. Assess. 12, 123–131. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.12.2.123
Svaldi, J., Naumann, E., Trentowska, M., and Schmitz, F. (2014a). General and food-specific inhibitory deficits in binge eating disorder. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 534–542. doi: 10.1002/eat.22260
Svaldi, J., Schmitz, F., Trentowska, M., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Berking, M., and Naumann, E. (2014b). Cognitive interference and a food-related memory bias in binge eating disorder. Appetite 72, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.09.014
Teubner-Rhodes, S. E., Mishler, A., Corbett, R., Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Trueswell, J. C., et al. (2016). The effects of bilingualism on conflict monitoring, cognitive control, and garden-path recovery. Cognition 150, 213–231. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.02.011
Tillman, C. M., and Wiens, S. (2011). Behavioral and ERP indices of response conflict in Stroop and flanker tasks. Psychophysiology 48, 1405–1411. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01203.x
Tong, J., Miao, S., Wang, J., Yang, F., Lai, H., Zhang, C., et al. (2014). A two-stage epidemiologic study on prevalence of eating disorders in female university students in Wuhan, China. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 49, 499–505. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0694-y
Keywords: binge eating, flanker task, conflict monitoring, motor impulsivity, food cue
Citation: Lyu Z, Zheng P, Lu S and Qin M (2018) Impaired Conflict Monitoring to Food Cues in Women Who Binge Eat. Front. Psychol. 9:2585. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02585
Received: 28 April 2018; Accepted: 03 December 2018;
Published: 17 December 2018.
Edited by:
Yonghui Li, University of Chinese Academy of Sciences (UCAS), ChinaReviewed by:
Michael Grady Wheaton, Barnard College, Columbia University, United StatesMichael Leon, University of California, Irvine, United States
Copyright © 2018 Lyu, Zheng, Lu and Qin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Zhenyong Lyu, lyuzy@xynu.edu.cn; lvzheny@126.com
†These authors have contributed equally to this work as co-first authors