- 1Work and Organizational Psychology, Medical School Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
- 2Work, Organizational and Business Psychology, Johannes Gutenberg University Mainz, Mainz, Germany
Purpose: We investigated the role of gender in abusive leadership practices, along with the effects of abusive leadership on employee health. We tested two hypotheses regarding the relationship between abusive leadership practices and subordinates’ health outcomes.
Design: At two points of measurement, 663 participants in Germany rated their 158 direct team leaders on abusive supervision and stated their own levels of emotional exhaustion and somatic stress. To test our hypotheses, we used a mixed model approach.
Findings: The results show no gender differences between the ratings for female and male leaders regarding abusive supervision but do confirm that the leaders’ gender did play a role in employees’ perceptions of abuse; perceived abusive supervision is more strongly related to increased emotional exhaustion and somatic stress when the leader is male.
Limitations: The generalizability of the study is limited due to a majority of females in the sample.
Practical Implications: Organizations should review their policies and procedures to first identify abusive supervision, then to offer adequate support programs for both leaders and subordinates.
Originality/Value: The study integrates gender into research on leadership and health, shifting the focus from previous studies that investigated constructive to destructive leadership. A further strength of the study is the application of a multilevel design and two separate points of measurement.
Introduction
This study addresses the question of whether the relationship between abusive supervision and subordinate health, differs depending on the gender of the supervisor. To investigate this issue, we first turn to leadership and health. Leadership can be regarded as a key factor in the well-being and health of subordinates (Kuoppala et al., 2008; Lyons and Schneider, 2009; Skakon et al., 2010; Mullen and Kelloway, 2011), particularly concerning destructive leadership behaviors, such as abusive supervision (Nyberg et al., 2011; Martinko et al., 2013; Schyns and Schilling, 2013). It is important to note that leadership is not unidirectional, but instead a bidirectional interaction between a leader and a subordinate (Van Dierendonck et al., 2004). Thus, perceptions of leadership may be as important as actual behavior (Schyns, 2007; Brees et al., 2016; Schyns et al., 2018). Here, gender comes into play. The gender of a leader is a salient characteristic that shapes the perceptions and attitudes of subordinates (e.g., Eagly and Karau, 2002; Parks-Stamm et al., 2008; Schieman and McMullen, 2008). As past research on constructive leadership has demonstrated, people perceive, evaluate and acknowledge similar behavior between female and male leaders differently (Eagly et al., 1992; Schein, 2001; Sczesny, 2003; Johnson et al., 2008; Kulich et al., 2011; Amanathulla and Tinsley, 2013). The consequences of these different perceptions are highly significant for female leaders, who must often overcome greater barriers to reach leadership positions (e.g., Schein, 2001), face harsher standards (Ryan and Haslam, 2007) and receive less acknowledgment, fewer rewards, and lower pay (cf. Kulich et al., 2007) than their male counterparts. The Role Congruity Theory suggests that, due to similar social norms of men and leaders, men are perceived as more legitimate in leadership positions (Eagly and Karau, 2002; Heilman and Haynes, 2005). However, what happens to role congruity when the leader is not a shining example and behaves destructively? In this study, we investigated how gendered perceptions of leaders, shape the relationship between abusive supervision and health.
This study extends previous research in several ways. Firstly, two different theoretical roots of the Role Congruity Theory were re-evaluated with respect to destructive leadership: the Stereotype Content Model (Fiske et al., 2002) and the Expectations State Theory (Ridgeway, 2001, 2011). Secondly, by shifting the focus to destructive leadership behaviors, we provide new insights on the interplay between leadership and gender. Thirdly, choosing health outcomes as a dependent variable is an asset of the study, revealing the impact leadership behavior has on the individual. Thus, unlike variables such as performance or effectiveness, our outcomes were not entangled with the actual tasks or achievements of the leader. Fourth, since the study integrated research on leadership and gender, Germany, which is considered a country emphasizing ambition and differentiated gender roles (Hofstede et al., 2010), provided an interesting research setting (Chhokar et al., 2013).
Theoretical Background
Leadership and Well-Being
Leadership has been identified as important for the well-being and health of employees (Kuoppala et al., 2008; Mullen and Kelloway, 2011; Nyberg et al., 2011; Holstad et al., 2013). Leaders can influence their subordinates’ health either directly, by exhibiting supportive or obstructive leadership behavior (Skakon et al., 2010; Schyns and Schilling, 2013), or indirectly by influencing employee duties (Nielsen et al., 2008; Tuckey et al., 2012).
Abusive supervision is defined as “subordinates’ perception of the extent to which supervisors engage in a sustained display of hostile verbal and non-verbal behaviors, excluding physical contact” (Tepper, 2000, p. 178) and has been linked to increased health impairment and reduced well-being (for a review, see Martinko et al., 2013). Abusive behavior of leaders over time (Tepper, 2007) is likely to cause strain. Abusive supervision correlates with subordinates’ depression, anxiety, and emotional exhaustion (Tepper, 2000; Wu and Hu, 2009), as well as with more negative attitudes toward the job, the organization, and life in general, higher turnover intentions, and work–family conflict (Tepper, 2000, 2007). In their meta-analysis, Schyns and Schilling (2013, p. 11) note that “the effect size for the relationship of destructive leadership with well-being (rho = -0.35) even exceed those for constructive leadership (rho = 0.26).”
In the current study, we employed emotional exhaustion as a psychological indicator (Maslach and Leiter, 2008) and somatic stress as a physical (Pejtersen et al., 2010) indicator of health impairment. Drawing on previous findings regarding abusive supervision, it was expected that the hostile behaviors of abusive supervisors would be linked to higher levels of emotional exhaustion and somatic stress among their subordinates. Thus, we propose that:
H1: Abusive supervision positively relates to subordinates’ emotional exhaustion.
H2: Abusive supervision positively relates to subordinates’ somatic stress.
Since abusive supervision, by definition, relies on subordinates’ perceptions (Tepper, 2000), this relationship is likely influenced by the characteristics of the leader. Gender, one of the most salient characteristics, not only shapes perceptions but also affects the impact of leadership behavior (Eagly and Karau, 2002; Mohr and Wolfram, 2008; Parks-Stamm et al., 2008; Ye et al., 2016). To what extent behavior is seen as inappropriate or unjust differs for female and male leaders (Heilman and Eagly, 2008; Escartín et al., 2011; Amanathulla and Tinsley, 2013) and thus might cause differing health responses (Hoel et al., 2004). In the following section, we will therefore examine the role of gender, in the context of leadership and how it might shape the effects of abusive leadership on employee health.
Theories on Leadership and Gender
Most existing research has focused on desirable leadership behaviors. Prior research on gender, in the context of leadership, was built on three theories: the Stereotype Content Model (Fiske et al., 2002); the Expectations State Theory (Ridgeway, 2011); and the Role Congruity Theory (Eagly and Karau, 2002). All three theories are strongly interrelated but vary in their emphasis and scope.
The Stereotype Content Model concentrates on attributes ascribed to social groups (e.g., women, men, ethnic groups), which can be arranged into two dimensions; warmth and competence. For instance, the female gender stereotype would suggest that people rate women high on positive intentions (warmth) but low on agentic abilities (competence), whereas the opposite categorization would apply for men. Studies show that competence closely relates to status and lack of warmth closely relates to competition (Fiske et al., 2002). Both attributes are considered essential for leadership (Schein, 2001). The theory describes stereotypes of specific groups and respective reactions well, but sometimes falls short on explaining why reactions differ for individuals of social groups that fall into “conflicting” categories. For instance, a study by Cuddy et al. (2004) shows that working mothers are perceived less competent, whereas working fathers retain their competence and gain on perceived warmth. Thus, keeping in mind that stereotypes are also subject to change (Duehr and Bono, 2006), it seems necessary to take further factors into consideration.
The Expectations State Theory (Ridgeway, 2001, 2011) emphasizes beliefs about status that are associated with a variety of social roles in society. People in high-status roles are ascribed higher competence, as well as more valuable skills and emerge as leaders more often. Due to an institutionalized social hierarchy, men are considered the higher- and women the lower-status group. These implicit expectations drive the self’s, as well as others’, performance expectations.
The Role Congruity Theory (Eagly and Karau, 2002) integrates both aforementioned theories by incorporating social roles as well as social groups. The representation of social groups in occupational roles (e.g., male leaders) leads people to associate group stereotypes with the occupational role (Koenig and Eagly, 2014). Yet, it is important to note that role congruity can occur on two levels: first, behaviors and attributes ascribed to the gender and the leader role can coincide. For instance, competence is seen as necessary for leadership and more likely ascribed to men (Stereotype Content Model). Second, the higher positional status (e.g., leader) can match the higher gender status as in the case of men (Expectations State Theory).
Regarding gender and leadership, the male leaders’ social roles align with their workplace leadership roles in both cases, while those of women do not (Schein et al., 1996; Schein, 2001; Koenig et al., 2011). This ‘think manager, think male’ phenomenon (TMTM; Schein, 2001) has been reported in many studies from various countries (Willemsen, 2002; Garcia-Retamero and López-Zafra, 2006; Ryan et al., 2011).
With respect to constructive leadership behaviors, there seems to be a preference for the stereotype content aspect of the Role Congruity Theory (e.g., Eagly and Carli, 2003; Sczesny, 2003; Heilman et al., 2004). Because of stereotype incongruence, women face barriers, since they are not seen as competent enough or are considered too communal for leadership (Heilman and Haynes, 2005). However, this focus on stereotype incongruence neither takes into account potential changes of stereotypes (Duehr and Bono, 2006; López-Zafra and García-Retamero, 2012), nor does it explain several mixed findings (Embry et al., 2008; Koenig et al., 2011; Paustian-Underdahl et al., 2014).
For instance, contrary to the TMTM phenomenon, women show transformational leadership behaviors more often and these behaviors are perceived as being more stereotypical of female leaders (Eagly et al., 2003; Eagly, 2007; Stempel et al., 2015). Nevertheless, a study by Mohr and Wolfram (2008) demonstrated that leaders’ considerate verbal behaviors impact subordinates’ health more when enacted by male leaders, as opposed to female leaders. These findings suggest that even when women show desirable leadership behaviors, they might still not be as influential as male leaders. Here, the Expectations State Theory argues that the established social role of the leader is associated with men, and therefore their leadership behaviors – desirable or not – are more weighted and influential than those of female leaders (Embry et al., 2008; Ridgeway, 2011).
Theoretical Perspectives on Gender and Destructive Leadership
Few studies have investigated the role of gender in the context of less constructive, or destructive, leadership (Eagly et al., 2003; Lee and Brotheridge, 2011; Wang et al., 2013). When the Role Congruity Theory is applied to desirable leadership behavior, a differentiation between stereotype content (Stereotype Content Model) and social roles (Expectations State Theory) is usually not required, because men benefit from the congruence of the higher-status leader role as well as associated gender stereotypes. Nevertheless, blurring the two aspects might not only account for mixed results (Mohr and Wolfram, 2008; Wolfram and Mohr, 2010), but also makes it difficult to derive predictions about role congruity, with respect to abusive supervision. Depending on the weighting of the aspects of the Stereotype Content Model and the Expectations State Theory, the Role Congruity Theory allows for opposing predictions regarding the moderating effect of the leaders gender on subordinates’ health. Since there is, to our knowledge, no clear empirical evidence on the subject, in the context of abusive supervision, the following two sections provide argumentation for two competing hypotheses.
According to the Stereotype Content Model, abusive supervision behavior might damage the competence, as well as the warmth ratings attached to the leader stereotype. It remains unclear, however, how reactions to the stereotype of a ‘bad male leader,’ as opposed to a ‘bad female leader,’ would differ, since both would score low on warmth and competence. We can only make predictions regarding the consequences of abusive supervision for subordinate health, when taking the Role Congruity Theory’s perspective on stereotype content into account. Assuming that assertive and aggressive behavior is more in line with the male gender stereotype (Schein, 2001), abusive supervision by a male leader would speak for role congruence. This overlap between the male gender and the leader role might make the behavior more predictable and acceptable and therefore, may cause less harm to subordinates health. Accordingly, for abusive female leaders, the violation of communal gender stereotypes would cause more negative judgments, due to their incongruence, and subordinates might feel stressed, because the behavior was less expected and predictable. Thus, we propose that the perceived behavioral incongruence for female leaders strengthens the relation between abusive supervision and health outcomes:
H3: A leader’s gender moderates the relationship between abusive supervision and (a) subordinates’ emotional exhaustion and (b) somatic stress such that the relationship is stronger for female than for male leaders.
Emphasizing the Expectations State Theory aspect of the Role Congruity Theory, allows for differing predictions regarding the consequences of abusive supervision on subordinate health. According to the Expectations State Theory, men are the higher-status group and are considered more legitimate and influential in the leadership role. Consequently, their actions carry considerably more weight, making them more essential in the eyes of their subordinates, and thus might have a stronger impact on their health (Embry et al., 2008; Mohr and Wolfram, 2008; Ridgeway, 2011). Accordingly, for female leaders, the violation of role norms would make them appear even less congruent and legitimate for a leadership position; therefore, subordinates would ascribe female leaders lower levels of power to influence their well-being. Even though these assumptions are contrary to those outlined in the previous section, this reasoning is equally in line with the Role Congruity Theory, since there is congruence between the leader and the gender role for men but not for women (Schein, 2001). Thus, we propose that the perceived status incongruence for female leaders, buffers the relationship between abusive supervision and health outcomes:
H4: The leaders’ gender moderates the relationship between abusive supervision and (a) the subordinates’ emotional exhaustion and (b) somatic stress such that the relationship is stronger for male than for female leaders.
Materials and Methods
Participants and Procedure
The sample for this study is part of an international research project on leadership and well-being (Rigotti et al., 2014). The survey was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Federation of German Psychologists Association’s Code of Ethics. We conducted the study with the permission of the workers’ council of the participating organization, and we informed the participants about the objectives and procedures. Participation in the study was voluntary, and confidential handling of the data was assured. Thus, the procedures of the survey are in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical review and approval were not required for this study as per the institutional and national requirements.
A precondition for participation in the study was that the participants have frequent contact (usually on a daily, but at least weekly, basis) with their immediate supervisors. In Germany, 2,567 subordinates from nine different companies (banks, medium-sized enterprises, public sector) were invited to take part in the first data collection in 2012, and 1,594 individuals participated, resulting in a response rate of 62.1%. Participants rated their direct supervisors on abusive supervision. Six months later, 2,093 subordinates were invited and 1,120 responded (response rate of 53.5%), rating their well-being in terms of emotional exhaustion and somatic symptoms. The survey followed a two-wave multilevel design. We identified subordinates by an individual code and matched them to their respective leaders by a specific team code. Participants were provided with the option to complete the questionnaire online, via a link in an email, or a paper-pencil version, with the majority (97.9%) opting for the online choice. Drop-out analysis on all employed variables, only revealed that participants who dropped out of the sample at T2 were more likely to be male (χ2 = 8.41, p = 0.005) and have a permanent contract (χ2 = 6.05, p = 0.018); however, we excluded employees with temporary employment contracts (n = 38) as they might hold different expectations of leadership (cf. De Cuyper et al., 2008). Missing values further reduced the sample for analysis. We obtained a final sample of 663 subordinates who rated the behavior of their 158 immediate supervisors. Thus, on average, four team-members responded per team.
In the final two-wave sample, 62.9% of the leaders were female and 37.1% were male. The participants were, on average, 41.4 years old (SD = 9.48), and 80.8% of the respondents were female. On average, subordinates had worked for their current leaders for about 5 years (M = 4.87, SD = 4.04) and for their employers for approximately 15 years (M = 15.30, SD = 8.91). Participants worked an average of 38.5 (SD = 5.24) hours per week.
Measures
In order to reduce same method bias, abusive supervision was measured at the first point of data collection, whereas the dependent variables of emotional exhaustion and somatic stress were measured with a time lag of 6 months.
Abusive Supervision
We assessed abusive supervision with a five-item scale, developed by Mitchell and Ambrose (2007). The subordinates indicated, on a seven-point Likert-type scale, how much they agreed with statements such as “My boss puts me down in front of others” (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree; α = 0.93). Due to the limitations of Cronbach’s alpha (α = 0.93, McNeish, 2017), we also calculated the omega total coefficient (ω = 0.89). Factor loadings and residual variances were obtained from a multilevel confirmatory factor analysis.
Emotional Exhaustion
We evaluated emotional exhaustion using the three most prototypical items (De Cuyper et al., 2012) from the Maslach Burnout Inventory (Maslach et al., 1996). Participants were asked to indicate on a seven-point scale (0 = never to 6 = every day) how often a certain feeling occurred. A sample item is “I feel emotionally drained from my work.” The alpha value and total omega for the scale were both 0.85.
Somatic Stress
We applied a four-item measure from the COPSOQ II by Pejtersen et al. (2010) to capture symptoms of somatic stress. Participants were asked to indicate how often they felt “stomach aches,” “headaches,” “palpitations,” or “tension in various muscles” during the last 4 weeks (1 = not at all to 5 = all the time). The period of 4 weeks took into account that psychophysiological responses are more likely to become consolidated over a longer period of stressful events. According to Levenson (2011, p. 603) “for symptoms in general, 75% of the patients presenting to primary care improve within 2–4 weeks (Kroenke, 2003). Thus, it makes sense to rely on an initial 4–6-weeks wait to clarify whether the symptoms will persist” (Kroenke and Jackson, 1998). Because symptoms cannot be expected to occur simultaneously, a medium high α of 0.65 and an omega of 0.62 are acceptable for this index scale.
Gender
The gender of the participants and their respective supervisors was coded 0 for female and 1 for male. The supervisors reported their own gender, and we were also able to fill in missing values with information provided by the participating companies.
Controls
In testing our hypotheses, we used the following controls for subordinates: age, gender and tenure with the leader. Age and gender were included as a control variable in the analysis because they were shown to be relevant for well-being and health at work (Mauno et al., 2013), as well as for attitudes toward leaders (Wolfram et al., 2007). Age (years) was reported by the participants at the initial assessment, as was gender. Because effects of leadership are likely to depend on time spent working with the leader, we asked “How long have you been working under the supervision of this leader?” We measured this time period in years.
Results
Table 1 shows descriptive statistics and correlations of our study variables.
Since the data structure was nested (individuals within teams), multivariate variance analyses, using a team as a factor, were conducted. Results indicated a substantial variation across teams in exhaustion [F(158, 549) = 1.62, p < 0.001] and somatic stress [F(156, 532) = 1.50, p < 0.001]. We analyzed the null-model for all variables in order to estimate the ICCs. The ICC for the dependent variables exhaustion was ICC(1) = 10.2% and for somatic stress ICC(1) = 7.0%. These analyses confirmed that there was a considerable proportion of variance due to team membership in the dependent variables. Because we did not aggregate data, we deemed these results to indicate that a multilevel approach was appropriate (Bliese, 2000).
In the subsequent models, the dichotomous variables, gender of the subordinate (level 1) and gender of the leader (level 2), were included uncentered (Dawson, 2014). Abusive supervision, as well as the control variables of age and tenure with the leader (level 1), were entered grand-mean centered (Aguinis et al., 2013). Following suggestions from Aguinis et al. (2013), we adopted a stepwise procedure. We first tested main effect models comparing a random intercept model with a random slope model (including random slopes for abusive supervision). As the random slope model did not show a significantly better fit (Δ-2LL = 0.67, Δdf = 4, p = 0.48, one-tailed), the cross-level interaction model was tested with fixed slopes for abusive supervision.
Hypotheses H1 and H2: Abusive Supervision and Health
Consistent with predictions, and as displayed in Table 2, the models including the main effects only show a significant relationship between abusive supervision and emotional exhaustion (ϒmodel1 = 0.46, p < 0.001) and somatic stress (ϒmodel1 = 0.11, p < 0.01).
Hypotheses H3 and H4: Gender of the Leader as Moderator
As predicted by H3 and H4 (see Table 2), the gender of the leader was found to be a significant moderator for emotional exhaustion (ϒmodel3 = 0.36, p = 0.026) and somatic stress (ϒmodel3 = 0.16, p = 0.039). With respect to the competing hypotheses, results indicated a stronger relationship for abusive male leaders than for abusive female leaders, as proposed in H4a and H4b (Figures 1, 2).
Simple slope analyses for the relationship between abusive supervision and emotional exhaustion showed significant slopes for female leaders (gradient 0.35, p < 0.001) and male leaders (gradient = 0.71, p < 0.001). For somatic stress, only the slope for male leaders was significant (gradient = 0.22, p = 0.013), indicating a positive relationship between abusive male supervision and somatic stress. No significant relationship emerged for female leaders (gradient 0.06, p = 0.146).
Discussion
The current study investigated the role of gender in abusive leadership and the health of subordinates. As a first step, we showed that abusive supervision is linked to higher emotional exhaustion and more somatic stress. In a second step, the focus was on the role of the leaders’ gender. Even though participants did not report gender differences in the perception of abusive supervision, leader gender moderated the relationship between abusive supervision and health. The study demonstrated that the same behavior enacted by male and female leaders was associated with different health outcomes among their subordinates. Abusive male and female supervisors had subordinates reporting increased emotional exhaustion, but this relationship was considerably stronger for male leaders. In cases of somatic stress, the differences for female and male leaders were even stronger, since only abusive male supervisors had a significant relationship to somatic stress.
With two differentially focused hypotheses, the study explored whether status (Expectations State Theory, Ridgeway, 2011) or stereotype (in-)congruence (Social Content Model, Fiske et al., 2002) were critical in making predictions about abusive supervision and health. The findings offer a fresh look on the theoretical underpinnings of the Role Congruity Theory, revealing that it appears necessary to differentiate between the two aspects, particularly concerning destructive leadership. The current results support the notion that status congruence (male leader prototype) outweighs stereotypical behaviors (e.g., violating communal/agentic norms). Male leaders – even when exhibiting abusive supervision – seem to hold greater influence over their subordinates’ health.
The fact that participants rated the actual behaviors of their immediate supervisors, might explain this stronger emphasis on role norms. Stereotypes are usually applied in situations where people lack information about the target (Heilman and Eagly, 2008; Rosette and Tost, 2010). Thus, when confronted with abusive supervisors, people seem to rely more on role than on stereotype content information (Koenig and Eagly, 2014). This explanation would be in line with empirical findings regarding constructive leadership, showing that differences between actual female and male leaders are often considered small and context dependent (Vecchio, 2002; Paustian-Underdahl et al., 2014). The fact that similar behavior of female and male leaders still produces different effects on subordinate outcomes, has been demonstrated in various studies (e.g., Heilman and Haynes, 2005; Wolfram and Mohr, 2010; Kulich et al., 2011). Therefore, it seems necessary to take the status aspects of Role Congruity Theory into account when making predictions about the role of gender in abusive supervision, because it seems that people still ascribe male leaders more influence and legitimacy than female leaders.
It is important to note that the outcome variable in this study is the health of the subordinate. For other outcome variables like performance or sympathy, stereotype content congruence might be more pronounced. Here, subordinates might judge female leaders more harshly for violating their role as well as gender expectations. However, the power to influence seems to be associated with the leader role, which is more readily ascribed to men (Kulich et al., 2011).
Another condition may be the cultural setting. According to Hofstede et al. (2010), Germany scores 66 on a scale from 1 to 100 on masculinity and is, therefore, considered a country with masculine norms when compared to other countries. Leaders are expected to be decisive and assertive. These rather agentic role expectations point to a strong male-leader prototype, which might enhance the status aspect of the leader role, irrespective of the actual leadership behaviors.
Furthermore, since abusive supervision depends, by definition, on the perceptions of the subordinates, researchers should take attributional processes into consideration (Martinko et al., 2012; Burton et al., 2014). In a recent study, Schyns and colleagues (2018) highlight the importance of perceptions and attributions regarding abusive supervision. But to date, we have no knowledge of studies dealing with gendered attributions of abusive supervision, even though several researchers acknowledge that gender (Hoel et al., 2004; Burton et al., 2014; Salin, 2015), as well as status and power differences, might be of importance (Escartín et al., 2011; Samnani et al., 2013).
Overall, this study offers two theoretical perspectives on the Role Congruity Theory in the context of abusive supervision and gender. Differentiating between the social role itself and associated gender stereotypes, may help to explain some of the mixed results in studies on gender and leadership. Nevertheless, additional research is necessary to clarify whether the theoretical assumptions made in this study, hold empirically across differing contexts.
Limitations, Strengths and Directions for Future Research
Since this study addresses leader gender as a moderator, the predominance of female participants in our sample was somewhat problematic. Effects for male leaders were significant, despite them being the minority in the sample. Nevertheless, as it has been shown that perceived gender roles differ across branches and teams (Embry et al., 2008; Koenig et al., 2011; Korek et al., 2014; Paustian-Underdahl et al., 2014), future studies should take the organizational context into account.
Additionally, the floor effects of the abusive supervision variable might be problematic for the robustness of the statistical analysis. Abusive supervision seems to be a low-frequency phenomenon that nevertheless has a strong effect on health and well-being outcomes. Another potential limitation to consider, is the time frame used in this study. For separation of the independent and dependent variables, a time lag of 6 months was chosen in order to avoid common method bias, however, little is known about the time it takes for the detrimental effects of abusive supervision to appear or how long it takes for health outcomes to manifest.
Furthermore, because we expected that perceptions would play a significant role in investigating the proposed research question (Schyns et al., 2018), all independent and dependent variables were rated from the perspective of the subordinates. Brees et al. (2016) showed that subordinates’ characteristics were an important factor when it came to the perception of abusive supervision. Our study controlled for gender of the subordinate, and the nested data structure was taken into account, ensuring that every subordinate rated his or her immediate team leader.
As the findings suggest, it is important to consider leader gender as a moderator for abusive supervision and its effects on employee health. Even if the results reported here can help to explain the mixed results of previous studies, additional theoretical and empirical work is necessary to investigate the assumptions about the two different key aspects of the Role Congruity Theory. It needs to be clarified how distinct social role status is from stereotype content, how they interrelate and under what conditions one or the other might reveal its strength. For example, researchers could focus on changes in gender role norms over time. As mentioned with respect to stereotype content, agentic behaviors are shown to be increasingly associated with women (Diekman and Eagly, 2000; Duehr and Bono, 2006; Embry et al., 2008). Thus, it would be interesting to investigate how these changing gender stereotypes might affect the status perceptions of social roles.
The current study focused on leadership and negative indicators of health. Prospective studies should also take positive health and well-being indicators, such as general health or job satisfaction, into account. Since there is very little research on gender as a moderator in the relationship between abusive leadership and various organizational outcomes, it would also be interesting to extend this research question to other outcomes, such as performance, rewards or attitudes.
Practical Implications
Regardless of gender, it seems important to identify leaders who exert abusive supervision in order to prevent considerable damage to subordinates’ health. For this reason, organizations should review their current policies and personnel procedures to recognize factors (Tepper et al., 2011; Neves, 2014) that contribute to the occurrence of abusive supervision and offer adequate programs to train supervisors (Gonzalez-Morales et al., 2016).
Organizations should raise gender consciousness in order to improve their corporate climate. Implementing a gender-sensitive approach to training programs along with guidelines for leaders which could help to strengthen constructive leadership (Kelloway and Barling, 2010) and make the work of female leaders more visible. Since the results suggest that males are, indeed, perceived as more legitimate in leadership positions, human resource professionals should take this into account during selection processes and performance evaluations. Female leaders need to be rewarded for good and health-oriented leadership performance in the same way their male counterparts are but should likewise be held responsible when showing poor performance.
Conclusion
As the results demonstrate, the gender of the leader is important to consider when studying abusive leadership and its effects on health. We showed that abusive male leaders seem to have a more significant influence on their subordinates’ health than their female colleagues do. Taking a closer look at the theoretical implications of the Role Congruity Theory (Eagly and Karau, 2002), it seems necessary to differentiate between the status of social roles and the stereotype content. Underscoring the status aspect, the results of this study suggest that men are indeed perceived as more legitimate in their leadership positions, even when they exert detrimental leadership behaviors. If and how this gender difference translates into differential consequences in terms of evaluation, career prospects or well-being for female and male leaders, is an unexplored question that opens a broad avenue for future studies.
Ethics Statement
We conducted this study in accordance with the ethical standards of the Institutional and National Guidelines and with the Declaration of the World Medical Association (2013). We informed all participants of the objectives and the procedures prior to initiating the study. In the form of oral presentations, the participants were informed that participation was voluntary, and the data was treated anonymously. We obtained informed consent from all individual participants by virtue of survey completion. Ethical review and approval were not required for this study as per the institutional and national requirements.
Author Contributions
All authors worked together in a joint research project on leadership and health. Discussions and feedback processes happened on a regular basis. CS contributed to the general research question, the theoretical underpinning, the hypotheses and the discussion and wrote major parts of this manuscript. TR contributed to the methodological approach and the presentation of the results and reviewed the other sections and provided feedback.
Funding
The data reported here was collected in the research project Re-Su-Lead (Rewarding and Sustainable Health-Promoting Leadership). This research was supported by the Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin (BAuA: Federal Institute for Occupational Safety and Health) under Grant: F2199.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Aguinis, H., Gottfredson, R. K., and Cuelpepper, S. A. (2013). Best-Practice recommendations for estimating cross-level interaction effects using multilevel modeling. J. Manage. 39, 1490–1528. doi: 10.1037/a0028380
Amanathulla, E. T., and Tinsley, C. H. (2013). Punishing female negotiators for asserting too much…or not enough: exploring why advocacy moderates backlash against assertive female negotiators. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 120, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2012.03.006
Bliese, P. D. (2000). “Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis,” in Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations, eds K. J. Klein and S. W. J. Kozlowski (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 349–381.
Brees, J., Martinko, M., and Harvey, P. (2016). Abusive supervision: subordinate personality or supervisor behavior? J. Manag. Psychol. 31, 405–419. doi: 10.1108/JMP-04-2014-0129
Burton, J. P., Taylor, S. G., and Barber, L. K. (2014). Understanding internal, external, and relational attributions of abusive supervision. J. Organ. Behav. 35, 871–891. doi: 10.1002/job.1939
Chhokar, J. S., Brodbeck, F. C., and House, R. J. (2013). Cultureand Leadership Across the World: The GLOBE Book of in-Depth Studies of 25 Societies. New York, NY: Routledge.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., and Glick, P. (2004). When professionals become mothers, warmth doesn’t cut the ice. J. Soc. Issues 60, 701–718. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-4537.2004.00381.x
Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in management research: What, Why, When, and How?. J. Bus. Psychol. 29, 1–19. doi: 10.1007/s10869-013-9308-7
De Cuyper, N., De Jong, J., De Witte, H., Isaksson, K., Rigotti, T., and Schalk, R. (2008). Literature review of theory and research on the psychological impact of temporary employment: towards a conceptual model. Int. J. Manage. Rev. 10, 25–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2370.2007.00221.x
De Cuyper, N., Mäkikangas, A., Kinnunen, U., Mauno, S., and De Witte, H. (2012). Cross-lagged associations between perceived external employability, job insecurity, and exhaustion: testing gain and loss spirals according to the Conservation of Resources Theory. J. Organ. Behav. 33, 770–788. doi: 10.1002/job.1800
Diekman, A. B., and Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: women and men of the past, present, and future. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1171–1188. doi: 10.1177/0146167200262001
Duehr, E. E., and Bono, J. E. (2006). Men, women and managers: Are stereotypes finally changing? Pers. Psychol. 59, 815–846. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2006.00055.x
Eagly, A. H. (2007). Female leadership advantage and disadvantage: resolving the contradictions. Psychol. Women Q. 31, 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2007.00326.x
Eagly, A. H., and Carli, L. L. (2003). The female leadership advantage: an evaluation of the evidence. Leadersh. Q. 14, 807–834. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2003.09.004
Eagly, A. H., Johannesen-Schmidt, M. C., and Van Engen, M. L. (2003). Transformational, transactional, and laissez-faire leadership styles: a meta-analysis comparing women and men. Psychol. Bull. 129, 569–591. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.569
Eagly, A. H., and Karau, S. J. (2002). Role congruity theory of prejudice toward female leaders. Psychol. Rev. 109, 573–598. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.573
Eagly, A. H., Makhijani, M. G., and Klonsky, B. G. (1992). Gender and the effectiveness of leaders: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 117, 125–145.
Embry, A., Padgett, M. Y., and Caldwell, C. B. (2008). Can leaders step outside of the gender box? An examination of leadership and gender role stereotypes. J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 15, 30–45. doi: 10.1177/1548051808318412
Escartín, J., Salin, D., and Rodríguez-Carballeira, A. (2011). Conceptualizations of workplace bullying. J. Pers. Psychol. 10, 157–165.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 878–902. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878
Garcia-Retamero, R., and López-Zafra, E. (2006). Prejudice against women in male-congenial environments: perceptions of gender role congruity in leadership. Sex Roles 55, 51–61. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9068-1
Gonzalez-Morales, M. G., Kernan, M. C., Becker, T. E., and Eisenberger, R. (2016). Defeating abusive supervision: training supervisors to support subordinates. J. Occup. Health Psychol. doi: 10.1037/ocp0000061 [Epub ahead of Print].
Heilman, M. E., and Eagly, A. H. (2008). Gender stereotypes are alive, well, and busy producing workplace discrimination. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 1, 393–398. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2008.00072.x
Heilman, M. E., and Haynes, M. C. (2005). No credit where credit is due: attributional rationalization of women’s success in male-female teams. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 905–916. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.5.905
Heilman, M. E., Wallen, A. S., Fuchs, D., and Tamkins, M. M. (2004). Penalties for success: reactions to women who succeed at male gender- typed tasks. J. Appl. Psychol. 89, 416–427. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.89.3.416
Hoel, H., Faragher, B., and Cooper, C. L. (2004). Bullying is detrimental to health, but all bullying behaviours are not necessarily equally damaging. Br. J. Guid. Couns. 32, 367–387. doi: 10.1080/03069880410001723594
Hofstede, G., Hofstede, G. J., and Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind, 3rd edn, New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Holstad, T. J., Rigotti, T., and Otto, K. (2013). Procedural fairness as a mediator between transformational leadership and followers strain: a multilevel study. Z. Arbeits Organisationspsychol. 57, 163–176. doi: 10.1026/0932-4089/a000120
Johnson, S. K., Murphy, S. E., Zewdie, S., and Reichard, R. J. (2008). The strong, sensitive type: effects of gender stereotypes and leadership prototypes on the evaluation of male and female leaders. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 106, 39–60. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.12.002
Kelloway, E. K., and Barling, J. (2010). Leadership development as an intervention in occupational health psychology. Work Stress 24, 260–279. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2010.518441
Koenig, A. M., and Eagly, A. H. (2014). Evidence for the social role theory of stereotype content: observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 107, 371–392. doi: 10.1037/a0037215
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., and Ristikari, T. (2011). Are leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research paradigms. Psychol. Bull. 137, 616–642. doi: 10.1037/a0023557
Korek, S., Sobiraj, S., Weseler, S., Rigotti, T., and Mohr, G. (2014). The gender role self-concept of men in female-dominated occupations: Does it depend on how they see their jobs? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 44, 241–254. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12235
Kroenke, K. (2003). Patients presenting with somatic complaints: epidemiology, psychiatric co-morbidity and management. Int. J. Methods Pychiatr. Res. 12, 34–43. doi: 10.1002/mpr.140
Kroenke, K., and Jackson, J. L. (1998). Outcome in general medical patients presenting with common symptoms: a prospective study with a 2-week and a 3-month follow-up. Fam. Pract. 15, 398–403. doi: 10.1093/fampra/15.5.398
Kulich, C., Ryan, M. K., and Haslam, A. (2007). Where is the romance for women leaders? The effects of gender on leadership attributions and performance-based pay. Appl. Psychol. 56, 582–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.2007.00305.x
Kulich, C., Trojanowski, G., Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., and Renneboog, L. D. R. (2011). Who gets the carrot and who gets the stick? Evidence of gender disparities in executive remuneration. Strateg. Manage. J. 32, 301–321. doi: 10.1002/smj.878
Kuoppala, J., Lamminpää, A., Liira, J., and Vainio, H. (2008). Leadership, job well-being, and health effects—A systematic review and a meta-analysis. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 50, 904–915. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31817e918d
Lee, R. T., and Brotheridge, C. M. (2011). Sex and position status differences in workplace aggression. J. Manage. Psychol. 26, 403–418. doi: 10.1108/02683941111139010
Levenson, J. L. (2011). The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Psychosomatic Medicine: Psychiatric care of the Medically ill. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Pub.
López-Zafra, E., and García-Retamero, R. (2012). Do gender stereotypes change? The dynamic of gender stereotypes in Spain. J. Gender Stud. 21, 169–183. doi: 10.1080/09589236.2012.661580
Lyons, J. B., and Schneider, T. R. (2009). The effects of leadership style on stress outcomes. Leadersh. Q. 20, 737–748. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0423-z
Martinko, M. J., Harvey, P., Brees, J. R., and Mackey, J. (2013). A review of abusive supervision research. J. Organ. Behav. 34, 120–137. doi: 10.1002/job.1888
Martinko, M. J., Sikora, D., and Harvey, P. (2012). The relationships between attribution styles, LMX, and perceptions of abusive supervision. J. Leadersh. Organ. Stud. 19, 397–406. doi: 10.1177/1548051811435791
Maslach, C., Jackson, S. E., and Leiter, M. P. (1996). Maslach Burnout Inventory Manual, 3rd Edn. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Maslach, C., and Leiter, M. P. (2008). Early predictors of job burnout and engagement. J. Appl. Psychol. 93, 498–512. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.3.498
Mauno, S., Ruokolainen, M., and Kinnunen, U. (2013). Does aging make employees more resilient to job stress? Age as a moderator in the job stressor–well-being relationship in three Finnish occupational samples. Aging Ment. Health 17, 411–422. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.747077
McNeish, D. (2017). Thanks coefficient alpha, we’ll take it from here. Psychol. Methods 23, 412–433. doi: 10.1037/met0000144
Mitchell, M., and Ambrose, M. (2007). Abusive supervision and workplace deviance and the moderating effects of negative reciprocity beliefs. J. Appl. Psychol. 92, 1159–1168. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.92.4.1159
Mohr, G., and Wolfram, H.-J. (2008). Leadership and effectiveness in the context of gender: the role of leaders verbal behaviour. Br. J. Manage. 19, 4–16.
Mullen, J., and Kelloway, K. E. (2011). “Occupational health and safety leadership,” in Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology, 2nd Edn, eds J. C. Quick and L. E. Tetrick (Washington, DC: APA), 357–372.
Neves, P. (2014). Taking it out on the survivors: submissive employees, downsizing, and abusive supervision. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 87, 507–534. doi: 10.1111/joop.12061
Nielsen, K., Randall, R., Yarker, J., and Brenner, S.-O. (2008). The effects of transformational leadership on followers’ perceived work characteristics and psychological well-being: a longitudinal study. Work Stress 22, 16–32. doi: 10.1080/02678370801979430
Nyberg, A., Holmberg, I., Bernin, P., Alderling, M., Åkerblom, S., Widerszal-Bazyl, M., et al. (2011). Destructive managerial leadership and psychological well-being among employees in Swedish, Polish, and Italian hotels. Work 39, 267–281. doi: 10.3233/WOR-2011-1175
Parks-Stamm, E. J., Heilman, M. E., and Hearns, K. A. (2008). Motivated to penalize: women’s strategic rejection of successful women. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 34, 237–247. doi: 10.1177/0146167207310027
Paustian-Underdahl, S. C., Walker, L. S., and Woehr, D. J. (2014). Gender and perceptions of leadership effectiveness: a meta-analysis of contextual moderators. J. Appl. Psychol. 99, 1129–1145. doi: 10.1037/a0036751
Pejtersen, J. H., Søndergård Kristensen, T., Borg, V., and Bjorner, J. B. (2010). The second version of the Copenhagen psychosocial questionnaire. Scand. J. Public Health 38, 8–24. doi: 10.1177/1403494809349858
Ridgeway, C. L. (2001). Gender, status and leadership. J. Soc. Issues 57, 637–655. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00233
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by Gender: How Gender Inequality Persists in the Modern World. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199755776.001.0001
Rigotti, T., Holstad, T., Mohr, G., Stempel, C. R., Hansen, E., Loeb, C., et al. (2014). Rewarding and Sustainable Health-Promoting Leadership. Dortmund: BAuA.
Rosette, A. S., and Tost, L. P. (2010). Agentic women and communal leadership: How role prescriptions confer advantage to top women leaders. J. Appl. Psychol. 95, 221–235. doi: 10.1037/a0018204
Ryan, M. K., and Haslam, S. A. (2007). The glass cliff: exploring the dynamics surrounding women’s appointment to precarious leadership positions. Acad. Manage. Rev. 32, 549–572. doi: 10.5465/amr.2007.24351856
Ryan, M. K., Haslam, S. A., Hersby, M. D., and Bongiorno, R. (2011). Think crisis–think female: the glass cliff and contextual variation in the think manager–think male stereotype. J. Appl. Psychol. 96, 470–484. doi: 10.1037/a0022133
Salin, D. (2015). Risk factors of workplace bullying for men and women: the role of the psychosocial and physical work environment. Scand. J. Psychol. 56, 69–77. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12169
Samnani, A.-K., Singh, P., and Ezzedeen, S. (2013). Workplace bullying and employee performance: an attributional model. Organ. Psychol. Rev. 3, 337–359. doi: 10.1177/2041386613475370
Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women’s progress in management. J. Soc. Issues 57, 675–688. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00235
Schein, V. E., Mueller, R., Lituchy, T., and Liu, J. (1996). Think manager - think male: a global phenomenon? J. Organ. Behav. 17, 33–41. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1379(199601)17:1<33::AID-JOB778>3.0.CO;2-F
Schieman, S., and McMullen, T. (2008). Relational demography in the workplace and health: an analysis of gender and the subordinate-superordinate role-set. J. Health Soc. Behav. 49, 286–300. doi: 10.1177/002214650804900304
Schyns, B. (2007). “Implicit theory of leadership,” in Encyclopedia of Industrial and Organizational Psychology, ed. S. G. Rogelberg (Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE), 334–335.
Schyns, B., Felfe, J., and Schilling, J. (2018). Is it me or you? – How reactions to abusive supervision are shaped by leader behavior and follower perceptions. Front. Psychol. 9:1309. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01309
Schyns, B., and Schilling, J. (2013). How bad are the effects of bad leaders? A meta-analysis of destructive leadership and its outcomes. Leadersh. Q. 24, 138–158. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.09.001
Sczesny, S. (2003). A closer look beneath the surface: various facets of the think-manager–think-male stereotype. Sex Roles 49, 353–363. doi: 10.1023/A:1025112204526
Skakon, J., Nielsen, K., Borg, V., and Guzman, J. (2010). Are leaders’ well-being, behaviours and style associated with the affective well-being of their employees? A systematic review of three decades of research. Work Stress 24, 107–139. doi: 10.1080/02678373.2010.495262
Stempel, C. R., Rigotti, T., and Mohr, G. (2015). Think transformational leadership - think female. Leadership 11, 259–280. doi: 10.1177/1742715015590468
Tepper, B. J. (2007). Abusive supervision in work organizations: review, synthesis, and research Agenda. J. Manage. 33, 261–289. doi: 10.1177/0149206307300812
Tepper, B. J., Moss, S. E., and Duffy, K. K. (2011). Predictors of abusive supervision: supervisor perceptions of deep-level dissimilarity, relationship conflict, and subordinate performance. Acad. Manage. J. 54, 279–294. doi: 10.5465/amj.2011.60263085
Tuckey, M. R., Bakker, A. B., and Dollard, M. F. (2012). Empowering leaders optimize working conditions for engagement: a multilevel study. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 17, 15–27. doi: 10.1037/a0025942
Van Dierendonck, D., Haynes, C., Borrill, C., and Stride, C. (2004). Leadership behavior and subordinate wellbeing. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 9, 165–175. doi: 10.1037/1076-8998.9.2.165
Vecchio, R. P. (2002). Leadership and gender advantage. Leadersh. Q. 13, 643–671. doi: 10.1016/S1048-9843(02)00156-X
Wang, A., Chaing, J. T., Tsai, C., Lin, T., and Cheng, B. (2013). Gender makes the difference: the moderating role of leader gender on the relationship between leadership styles and subordinate performance. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 122, 101–113. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.06.001
Willemsen, T. (2002). Gender typing of the successful manager – a stereotype reconsidered. Sex Roles 46, 385–391. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12187
Wolfram, H. J., and Mohr, G. (2010). Gender-typicality of economic sectors and gender-composition of working groups as moderating variables in leadership research. Gender Manage. 25, 320–339. doi: 10.1108/17542411011048182
Wolfram, H. J., Mohr, G., and Schyns, B. (2007). Professional respect for female and male leaders: influential gender-relevant factors. Women Manage. Rev. 22, 19–32. doi: 10.1108/09649420710726201
Wu, T. J., and Hu, C. (2009). Abusive supervision and employee emotional exhaustion: dispositional antecedents and boundaries. Group Organ. Manage. 34, 143–169. doi: 10.1177/1059601108331217
Keywords: gender, abusive supervision, leadership, health, role congruity theory
Citation: Stempel CR and Rigotti T (2018) Leaders’ Gender, Perceived Abusive Supervision and Health. Front. Psychol. 9:2427. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02427
Received: 23 May 2018; Accepted: 19 November 2018;
Published: 05 December 2018.
Edited by:
Badri Bajaj, Jaypee Institute of Information Technology, IndiaReviewed by:
Bertolt Meyer, Technische Universität Chemnitz, GermanyJeffrey Robert Bentley, California State University, Long Beach, United States
Copyright © 2018 Stempel and Rigotti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Christiane R. Stempel, Christiane.Stempel@medicalschool-hamburg.de