Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 December 2018
Sec. Movement Science

Motivational Regulations Across the Stages of Change for Exercise in the General Population of Monterrey (Mexico)

  • 1Faculty of Sports Organization, Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, San Nicolás de los Garza, Mexico
  • 2Faculty of Psychology, Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain
  • 3Faculty of Sports, Universidad Autónoma de Baja California, Mexicali, Mexico
  • 4Faculty of Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León, San Nicolás de los Garza, Mexico

Few studies have examined exercise adherence in the Mexican population using self-determination theory proposals and the stages of change model. The objectives of this study were:(a) to translate and adapt the Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire-3 (BREQ-3) to Mexican Spanish and examine its internal consistency and factorial structure (six dimensions); and (b) to analyse variations in behavioral regulations using the stages of change model. This study included 530 participants between 11 and 76 years old who lived in the metropolitan area of the city of Monterrey, Nuevo Leon, Mexico. The Mexican version of the BREQ-3 presented an acceptable six-factor model that agrees with the theory and has good internal consistency. Results showed that the less self-determined regulations (i.e., external and amotivation) predominated in the first stages of change (i.e., pre-contemplation and contemplation) and decreased in the last stages (i.e., action and maintenance); by contrast, the more self-determined regulations (i.e., intrinsic, integrated, and identified) predominated in the last stages (i.e., action and maintenance) and were lower in the first stages (i.e., pre-contemplation and contemplation). Linking these two theoretical constructs contributes to understanding physical exercise adherence.

Introduction

Being physically inactive is a major health risk factor that has been well-documented in the literature. Despite the health benefits associated with physical activity, in the Mexican context almost three quarters of people over 19 years old are overweight or obese (National Institute of Public Health, 2016). Although many people know the benefits of being physically active, not everyone starts or continues an exercise program (Berger et al., 2002), and so more research is needed to better understand physical exercise adherence. Previous research has shown that the motivation underlying exercise behavior could partly explain why some individuals initiate or drop-out from exercise programs (Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006).

Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2002) is one of the most widely studied theoretical constructs in recent years in different contexts (Vallerand et al., 2008), including physical exercise contexts. This theory suggests that individuals can be involved in an activity for different reasons or motives, and these reasons can be detected as motivational regulations that are more or less self-determined (autonomous). Deci and Ryan (1985) proposed that six different forms of regulation can be situated along a self-determination continuum, with behavior ranging from high levels of autonomy (intrinsic motivation), to medium levels (extrinsic motivation), and reaching low levels (amotivation). Specifically, these behavioral regulations are labeled intrinsic regulation, integrated regulation, identified regulation, introjected regulation, external regulation, and amotivation.

Intrinsic motivation is present when an individual performs an activity merely for the pleasure of experiencing it, without expecting a reward or trying to avoid punishment. On the other end of the continuum lies amotivation, where the person does not have any motivation to engage in an activity (Deci and Ryan, 2000). Individuals who are not motivated to practice sports do not seek social, affective, or material objectives; instead, they experience negative sensations such as incompetence, apathy, and even depression (Vallerand, 2001). Extrinsic motivation is found between the extremes of intrinsic motivation and amotivation, and it refers to performing an activity with the aim of obtaining a separable result (Ryan and Deci, 2000).

Within SDT, a mini-theory called organismic integration theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985) was introduced to explain the different forms of extrinsic motivation and the contextual factors that promote internalization and the integration of behavioral regulation. This theory proposes an order or sequence for the degree of autonomy with which extrinsic motivation is regulated. The sequence consists of integrated, identified, introjected, and external regulation. External regulation is the least autonomous type of extrinsic motivation, and it includes the classic case of motivation to obtain rewards or avoid punishment. Introjected regulation is somewhat more internalized and based on behaviors carried out to avoid guilt, anxiety, and shame, or to improve the ego, feelings of value, or pride. Identified regulation involves awarding a conscious value to a behavior in such a way that the action is accepted when it is personally important (Ryan and Deci, 2000). Lastly, integrated regulation represents the most autonomous form of extrinsic motivation (Ryan and Deci, 2000). Integration occurs when regulations have been identified, evaluated, and assimilated.

One of the instruments most frequently used to measure the different forms of behavioral regulation in exercise contexts is the Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire (BREQ; Mullan et al., 1997). The BREQ was composed of 15 items distributed in four factors: external regulation (four items), introjected regulation (three items), identified regulation (four items), and intrinsic motivation (four items). The authors did not include the integrated regulation subscale, and the items that tapped amotivation were dropped due to high levels of skewness. The results of confirmatory factorial analyses have supported the factorial validity of the instrument in different areas (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Ingledew et al., 2004; Landry and Solmon, 2004), highlighting the behavior of university students who practice a sport (Wilson et al., 2002).

Later, Markland and Tobin (2004) reincorporated the amotivation items in a second version of the instrument, called the BREQ-2. This version contains 18 items divided into five dimensions: amotivation (four items), external regulation (four items), introjected regulation (three items), identified regulation (three items), and intrinsic motivation (four items). The authors did not include the integrated regulation subscale because they found that it was not possible to empirically distinguish between integrated and identified regulation, and, due to an error, they omitted one identified regulation item. Previous research has supported the BREQ-2's factor structure and subscale reliability in different languages (e.g., Moreno et al., 2007; Moustaka et al., 2010; Costa et al., 2018).

To address the limitations of the BREQ-2, an integration subscale and a new additional introjected item were included to produce the BREQ-3 (Markland and Tobin, 2004; Wilson et al., 2006). This new version contains 23 items divided into six subscales: amotivation (four items), external regulation (four items), introjected regulation (four items), identified regulation (three items), integrated regulation (four items), and intrinsic motivation (four items). The BREQ-3 obtained acceptable fit indexes and adequate reliability in different languages (Wilson et al., 2006; González-Cutre et al., 2010; Guedes and Sofiati, 2015; Cid et al., 2018; Costa et al., 2018). Furthermore, several researchers have provided additional support for the BREQ-3 demonstrating that scores on the questionnaire discriminated among male and female groups (e.g., González-Cutre et al., 2010; Guedes and Sofiati, 2015; Su et al., 2015; Cid et al., 2018).

The different forms of motivational regulation directly predict a wide variety of cognitive, affective, and behavioral outcomes (Vallerand, 1997). Specifically, self-determined regulations (i.e., intrinsic and identified regulations) are related to more adaptive outcomes, compared to less self-determined regulations (i.e., introjected and external regulation) or amotivation (e.g., Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006; Lewis and Sutton, 2011).

Several studies have examined adherence to exercise through the union of two theoretical constructs, the self-determination continuum and the stages of behavioral change proposed by the transtheoretical model (Prochaska and Di Clemente, 1982). This model supports the perspective that physical exercise is a dynamic behavior, and that people pass through five stages of behaviors in their attempts to change their sedentary lifestyle into one that is physically more active. These stages include the pre-contemplation stage (a stage where the subject is physically inactive and has no intention of changing), the contemplation stage (the subject is inactive but has the intention to change), the preparation stage (the subject is active but does not carry out the recommendations for healthy practice), the action stage (the subject is active and complies with the recommendations for healthy practice, but has not completed six full months of regular practice), and the maintenance stage (stage where the person has performed healthy physical activity for more than 6 months). Individuals can move through the different stages (in both directions) as they perceive success in changing or maintaining their lifestyles (Prochaska et al., 1992b).

Mullan and Markland (1997) explored the relationship between behavioral regulations of exercise behavior and stages of change for exercise. They found that people in the early stages of change were less self-determined in their behavioral regulation than those in the later stages. Similar results were found by Daley and Duda (2006), who showed that participants in the early stages were less self-determined in the regulation of their exercise than those in the later stages of change. Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis (2006) found that exercisers in the maintenance stage of change displayed significantly more self-determined motivation to exercise than those in the preparation and action stages. Permanence in a specific stage has repercussions in terms of the type of intervention that might be most effective for each case.

Previous studies have found that the stages of change with the highest prevalence are the maintenance and preparation stages (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Marshall and Biddle, 2001; Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006). Studies on the Mexican population that have analyzed this prevalence differ from the aforementioned findings because most of them agree that contemplation is the most prevalent stage, suggesting cross-cultural differences (Zamarripa et al., 2018). Thus, interest has been shown in examining the applicability of psychological theories in different countries and cultures (e.g., Duda and Hayashi, 1998; Gangyan and Hing-chu, 2007).

From a theoretical perspective, it is necessary to have measures with rigorous psychometric properties within and across countries that can facilitate cross-cultural comparative research and help to understand cultural variations in the motivation to exercise in the specific Mexican context and around the world. Moreover, examining gender invariance will provide further support for testing motivational regulations by gender. For example, previous studies have reported gender differences in levels of motivational regulations (e.g., Daley and Duda, 2006; Johnson et al., 2011; Su et al., 2015), while in other studies no gender differences were observed (e.g., Lutz et al., 2003; Shen et al., 2008). To clarify these inconsistencies, Guérin et al. (2012) conducted a meta-analysis with 27 studies to examine gender-based mean differences in motivational regulations for exercise. Results were in line with findings demonstrating no gender differences on motivational regulations. However, not all studies looked at gender invariance of the instruments used for the study. So, it is important to analyse measurement invariance for further research to clarify the gender differences in motivational regulations. In addition, from a practical perspective, these instruments will be useful in evaluations prior to any type of intervention designed to promote physical exercise because they can identify the person's motivational level and his/her predisposition to change. This information would make it possible to direct the intervention in a more suitable way, in order to reduce the high rates of physical inactivity and obesity in the Mexican population (e.g., Zamarripa et al., 2013; Dávila-Torres et al., 2015).

Therefore, the aim of this study was twofold: (1) to translate the original English version of the BREQ-3 (Markland and Tobin, 2004; Wilson et al., 2006) into Mexican Spanish and examine its factor structure (six dimensions), reliability, and discriminant validity. Given that comparisons across genders are meaningful only when gender invariance has been empirically demonstrated, multi-group confirmatory factor analyses will be performed to test invariance by gender; and (2) to analyse variations in the six regulations across the stages of change in a sample of individuals who live in the metropolitan area of the city of Monterrey, Nuevo Leon, Mexico. Based on the theoretical propositions (SDT and stages of change model) and previous work (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006), it is hypothesized that participants who report more self-determined exercise regulations will indicate being in later stages of change than those who endorse less self-determined regulations, which would indicate being in the earlier stages of change. Due to the prevailing obesity in Mexico, it is expected that the majority of the participants will be in the stages of pre-contemplation, contemplation, and preparation; that is, subjects will be inactive or active, but they will not comply with the recommendations for healthy practice.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were 530 individuals (40.2% men and 51.8% women; Mage = 32.22; SD = 15.27; range = 11 – 76 years; median = 30.00 years), who lived in the metropolitan area of Monterrey, Nuevo Leon (Mexico) and were recruited via convenience sampling. Most of the sample were < 30 years old (49.8%), followed by 30 to 44 years (24.2%), 45 to 59 years (18.4%), and 60 years or older (7.6%).

Instruments

The BREQ-3 is a 23-item questionnaire divided into six subscales: intrinsic regulation (four items; e.g., “I exercise because it's fun”), integrated regulation (four items; e.g., “I exercise because it is consistent with my life goals”), identified regulation (three items; e.g., “I value the benefits of exercise”), introjected regulation (four items; “I feel guilty when I don't exercise”), external regulation (four items; e.g., “…because other people say I should”), and amotivation (four items; e.g., I don't see why I should have to exercise”). The questionnaire begins with the stem “I do physical exercise…” Responses are given on a 5-point scale ranging from 0 (definitely not true) to 4 (definitely true). Previous research has confirmed the reliability of this instrument (Wilson et al., 2006; González-Cutre et al., 2010).

To find out what stage of change the person was in, we used a Mexican-Spanish version (Zamarripa et al., 2018) of the stages of change questionnaire for physical activity by Marcus and Forsyth (2009). First, we asked the participants to read the following heading: “physical activity or exercise includes activities such as walking quickly, running, riding a bicycle, swimming, or any other activity in which exercise is at least as intense as these activities”. Then we asked them to indicate yes or no in response to the following statements: (1) I am currently physically active; and (2) I intend to be more physically active in the next 6 months. Participants who answered yes to question (1) did not answer question (2) and went on to read the following heading: “For activity to be regular, you must do a total of 30 min or more per day at least 5 days a week. For example, you could walk once for 30 min or do three 10-min sessions, for a daily total of 30 min.” Afterwards, we asked the participant to answer yes or no to the following statements: (3) I currently do regular physical activity; and (4) I have been doing regular physical activity for the past 6 months. The participants were placed in one of the five stages according to the algorithm presented in Table 1. Previous research has confirmed the reliability of this instrument (Marcus and Forsyth, 2009; Zamarripa et al., 2018). Furthermore, in an independent evaluation (Zamarripa et al., 2018), the criterion validity of the measure was supported through its relationship with self-reported physical activity in a Mexican sample (p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Algorithm for the categorization and distribution of the sample in stages of change.

Procedure

The instrument was administered through personal interviews, with previous consensus, by trained interviewers in the home of the interviewee, who was randomly selected. The interviewer recorded the interviewee's responses. All the participants were informed of the study's objective and voluntary nature. They were told that their answers and data management would be completely confidential, and that there were no right or wrong answers. They were also asked to respond with sincerity and honesty.

The English version of the BREQ-3 was translated into Mexican Spanish using the translation-back translation procedure (Hambleton and Kanjee, 1995). The translation was carried out by a professional translation service contracted by the study investigators. A group of six experts was formed, consisting of three specialists with doctorates who work in the area of physical activity and sport psychology, two specialists with sufficient experience in the validation of psychological instruments, and a translator specialized in the area of physical activity and sports, in order to discuss the discrepancies in the translation and obtain the first version of the instrument in Mexican Spanish. This version of the questionnaire was again translated into English by a different professional translation service, and the two versions of the instrument were compared: the original and the translation. The differences in the versions were again analyzed, and necessary changes were made to facilitate the understanding of the items, thus achieving the final version of each of the scales. Table 2 shows the items that make up each of the six subscales.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire (BREQ-3) items.

All the subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The present research was conducted in accordance with international ethical guidelines, which are consistent with American Psychological Association (APA) guidelines. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Universidad Autonoma de Nuevo Leon (Mexico) ethics review committee (No. 16CI19039021).

Statistical Analysis

Before proceeding on to data analysis, the data set was screened for multivariate outliers, and the normality of the distributions was also examined. Missing data were very small (< 0.1%) so we chose not to replace these scores. According to Tabachnick and Fidell (2007), when < 5% of data points are missing from the data set at random, almost any method for handling missing values yields similar results.

BREQ-3 items were subjected to confirmatory factorial analysis (CFA) using LISREL 8.80 (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 2006) to test whether the six-factor structure adequately fit the data collected from a Mexican population. Considering the ordinal nature of the data, the sample size, the number of response categories (k = 5), and the fact that the items were normally distributed (see Table 2), maximum likelihood was used to estimate model parameters, and the polychoric correlation matrix and the asymptotic covariance matrix were used as input for the analyses. We considered CFI, NNFI, and RMSEA to evaluate goodness of fit and the parameter estimates. CFI and NNFI values >0.95 indicate an acceptable fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999). For RMSEA, values less than or equal to.08 are considered satisfactory (Cole and Maxwell, 1985).

A multi-sample CFA was employed to examine whether the BREQ-3 displayed invariance by gender. Incremental fit indices were estimated to compare the alternative models' goodness of fit. A difference of 0.01 or less between values of CFI (Cheung and Rensvold, 2002) and NNFI (Widaman, 1985) reflect practically irrelevant differences between models. Regarding RMSEA, an increase of 0.015 or less between alternative models indicates irrelevant differences (Chen, 2007) and therefore the most parsimonious model should be selected.

Internal consistency was evaluated using Cronbach's (1951) alpha, Composite Reliability (CR), and the Average Variance Extracted (AVE). Alpha and CR values should be above 0.70 to be considered acceptable. AVE values of 0.50 and greater are considered acceptable (Fornell and Larcker, 1981). Convergent validity was analyzed considering that the items load high on their respective constructs, and considering values of AVE ≥0.50. Discriminant validity was assessed using the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criterion, that is, the square root of each construct's AVE should have a greater value than the correlations with other latent constructs. Another measure for discriminant validity has been to examine the correlation between the constructs that should be lower than 0.85 (Kline, 2005).

Variations in the different regulations across the stages of change were analyzed using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) and post hoc Tukey tests. A value of partial eta-squared above 0.06 is considered a moderate effect size, and a value above 0.14 is considered a large effect size (Cohen, 1988). For these analyses, we used the SPSS 21 program.

Results

Confirmatory Factorial Analysis

The proposed factorial structure adequately fitted the data (χ2 (215) = 548.93, p < 0.01; NNFI = 0.980, CFI = 0.983, RMSEA = 0.0545), confirming the validity of the six-factor model. Factor loadings of the model ranged from 0.49 to 0.85 and loaded significantly in their respective constructs (Figure 1) supporting measurement convergent validity. Means, standard deviations, skewness and kurtosis of BREQ-3 items are displayed in Table 2, demonstrating acceptable normality of score distribution.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Standardized parameters (factorial weights, covariance factors, and measurement errors) for the BREQ-3. All factor loadings were significant (p < 0.05).

Measurement Invariance

Table 3 shows that the model was invariant across gender. The CFA results showed that the proposed factorial structure was acceptable for males and females, confirming the validity of the six-factor model in the two groups considered separately (Models M0a and M0b). Model 1 (configural invariance), where no equality constrains were imposed, showed a good fit to the data, and so this model was used as the baseline model. Results of Model 2 (metric invariance), Model 3 (scale invariance), and Model 4 (latent means invariance) showed an acceptable fit for gender invariance. Compared to the baseline model (M1), the increase in NNFI, and CFI did not exceed the criterion value of 0.01, and the increase in RMSEA did not exceed the criterion value of 0.15.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Goodness of fit indices of gender invariance models.

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 shows the distribution of the participants in the different stages of change. The majority of the participants were located in maintenance (31.1%), followed by contemplation (22.6%), pre-contemplation (20.4%), preparation (13%), and action (12.8%). The total sample had higher scores on more autonomous regulations, with identified regulation standing out. By contrast, the regulation with the lowest score was amotivation, followed by external and introjected regulation (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Means, standard deviations, reliability, discriminant validity and bivariate correlations between the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire (BREQ-3) subscales.

Internal Consistency, Convergent and Discriminant Validity

Table 4 shows the reliability results. The alpha reliabilities ranged from 0.68 to 0.84, which is considered adequate. The composite reliabilities ranged from 0.74 to 0.89, which is considered satisfactory. In general, the AVEs of this study were above the recommended threshold of 0.50, ranged from 0.60 to 0.67, except for the value of the introjected regulation dimension, which exhibited a value of 0.42. The weak factorial load of some introjected regulation items results in an AVE below the recommended value of 0.50. These results support adequate convergent validity of the BREQ-3 scales.

The results of discriminant validity using the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criterion indicated a lack of discriminant between six of the 15 inter-construct dimensions (see Table 4), that is, between intrinsic regulation and integrated regulation, between intrinsic regulation and identified regulation, between integrated regulation and identified regulation, between introjected regulation and external regulation, between introjected regulation and amotivation, and between external regulation and amotivation. However, correlations among the subscales conformed to a simplex pattern, with stronger positive correlations between adjacent factors than with factors further away from the diagonal. Specifically, the results showed (Table 4) that the most autonomous regulations (intrinsic, integrated, and identified) had positive, moderate, and significant correlations with each other. In addition, these regulations also correlated positively but slightly with introjected regulation. By contrast, external regulation and amotivation correlated positively with each other and negatively with the more autonomous regulations. Following Kline's (2005) criteria, because inter-factor correlations are below 0.85, factor discrimination can be established among the questionnaire's dimensions. Overall, these results provide evidence of discriminant validity between the subscales of the BREQ.

Multivariate Analysis of Variance

The results of the MANOVA revealed statistically significant differences between the different stages of change and the forms of regulation (Wilks Lambda = 0.563, F = 13.47, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.13). Follow-up univariate analysis indicated significant differences in all the forms of regulation, except introjected regulation (see Table 5). Tukey's post hoc test revealed that participants in the pre-contemplation stage had significantly lower intrinsic regulation than those in all the other stages; by contrast, individuals in the maintenance stage showed significantly higher scores than those in all the other stages. The intrinsic regulation score increased from the pre-contemplation stage to the maintenance stage. With regard to integrated regulation, participants in the pre-contemplation and contemplation stages had lower scores than those in the preparation, action, and maintenance stages. In addition, participants in the maintenance stage had greater integrated regulation than individuals in the other stages. The integrated regulation scores also increased across the stages; however, there were no differences between pre-contemplation and contemplation, or between action and preparation. Regarding identified regulation, Tukey's post hoc test showed that participants in pre-contemplation had lower identified regulation than participants in all the other stages, whereas participants in maintenance presented higher identified regulation than those in all the other stages. As in the previous regulations, identified regulation also increased from the pre-contemplation stage to the maintenance stage; however, the differences between the stages of contemplation, preparation, and action were not significant (see Table 5; Figure 2).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means and standard deviations differences among the stages of change in physical exercise regulation.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences among the stages of change in physical exercise regulation. PC = precontemplation; C = contemplation; P, preparation; A, action; M, maintenance; R, regulation.

Unlike the previous regulations, the external regulation scores showed a decrease across the stages. Tukey's post hoc test revealed significant differences between the stages of pre-contemplation and maintenance, with participants in pre-contemplation showing greater external regulation than those in the maintenance stage. Finally, amotivation also showed significant differences across the different stages of change. Tukey's post hoc test showed that people in the pre-contemplation stage were more amotivated than those in the stages of preparation, action, contemplation, and maintenance (see Table 5; Figure 2).

Discussion

The first objective of this study was to translate the English version of the Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire-3 (BREQ-3), adapt it to Mexican Spanish, and analyse its factorial structure and internal consistency. The findings supported the factorial validity of the questionnaire. The six-factor model tested (intrinsic, integrated, identified, introjected, external, and amotivation) showed adequate fit indexes, and all the items presented factorial saturations above 0.40 in each of their factors; therefore, the full 23-item version was maintained. The internal consistency estimates for the six BREQ-3 subscales were adequate.

As Deci and Ryan (1985) predicted, the self-determination continuum emerged in the present sample. The correlations among the six subscales conformed to a simplex pattern; that is, adjacent factors were found to be more highly and positively correlated than with those at opposite ends of the continuum, thus supporting the construct validity of the BREQ-3. Furthermore, the correlation values between the factors were similar to those reported in previous studies with diverse samples and different language versions (e.g., Wilson et al., 2006; González-Cutre et al., 2010; Guedes and Sofiati, 2015; Cid et al., 2018). It should be noted that the correlations found in the present study ranged from low to moderate, indicating that the different factors correspond to constructs that are related, but independent from each other, offering support for the discriminant validity of the questionnaire. However, when using the Fornell and Larcker (1981) criterion, the results indicated a lack of discriminant especially among the closest regulations, i.e., between intrinsic, identified and integrated regulations; and between introjected regulation, external regulation and amotivation. This is consistent with results obtained in a meta-analysis testing the self-determination simplex pattern (Howard et al., 2017). This meta-analysis demonstrated that regulations can be ordered along the self-determination continuum and that it is possible to use all BREQ-3 regulation subscales individually. However, these authors supported the idea that it would be better to use a single motivation score representing degree of self-determination. According to Howard et al. (2017), future research might focused in how is the best way to operationalize motivational regulations (e.g., individual regulations, index of self-determination, more or less self-determined motivation).

The multi-sample CFA supported the measurement invariance of the BREQ-3 across gender groups, suggesting that participants responded in a similar fashion regardless of gender, allowing unbiased comparisons of average scores across male and female groups (Sass, 2011). In sum, this study is in line with previous studies that examined gender differences in motivational regulations (e.g., González-Cutre et al., 2010; Guedes and Sofiati, 2015; Su et al., 2015; Cid et al., 2018) indicating that the BREQ-3 in its Mexican version can be used to examine gender differences in exercise settings. To clarify the gender differences in motivational regulations is important not only for further research but also to identify which strategies are better to maximize people participation in exercise settings.

The second objective of the present study was to analyse the variations in the different regulations across the stages of change. As Daley and Duda (2006) pointed out, the transtheoretical model assumes a quantitative perspective, whereas SDT researchers are more concerned with the quality of motivation. Our results revealed that the quality of the motivation is related to the stages of change. That is, as people's motivation becomes more self-determined, their willingness to engage in physical exercise becomes more consistent. Thus, those located in the first stages of change, that is, where physical exercise is not done and there is no intention to do it, amotivation and external regulation predominated, compared to people who reported practicing physical exercise on a regular basis (action and maintenance). By contrast, intrinsic, integrated, and identified regulations were more predominant in individuals in the stages of action and maintenance, compared to those in the stages of contemplation and pre-contemplation.

All in all, these relationships between the stages of change and the forms of regulation provide evidence of the validity of the BREQ-3 based on its relationship with other variables. Specifically, participants who reported that they had exercised for 6 months or more (maintenance stage) had significantly higher scores on intrinsic regulation than participants who indicated that they did not exercise (precontemplation stage).

The predominance of less self-determined regulations in individuals in the early stages of change, as well as more self-determined regulations in individuals located in later stages, has also been found in previous studies (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Landry and Solmon, 2004; Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006).

Our results confirmed the relation between the quality (motivational regulations) and quantity (stages of change) of the motivation to exercise in the Mexican population. Moreover, they confirm relationships found in other cultures using previous versions of the BREQ (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Landry and Solmon, 2004; Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006).

Although previous studies found a higher prevalence of maintenance and preparation in the stages of change (Mullan and Markland, 1997; Marshall and Biddle, 2001; Daley and Duda, 2006; Thogersen-Ntoumani and Ntoumanis, 2006), and previous studies in the Mexican population found a prevalence of contemplation (Zamarripa et al., 2018), the prevalence in our sample is mainly of the maintenance, contemplation, and pre-contemplation stages. Daley and Duda (2006) combined the contemplation and pre-contemplation stages, arguing that these stages are similar with regard to attitudes and the intention to engage in future practice. With this in mind, our results can be polarized in two groups: a third (31.1%) of our sample that has actually engaged in exercise for more than 6 months (maintenance) and more than a third (20.4 and 22.6%) that is not interested at all in exercise (pre-contemplation and contemplation). These results have implications for interventions designed to keep people in the maintenance stage and help people in other stages to engage in physical activity.

The dynamic nature of stages of changes, that is, the fact that people can change stages as they perceive success or difficulties in making changes in their lifestyle (Prochaska et al., 1992b), offers opportunities for intervention. Our results suggest that the more self-determined a person's motivation is, the more he or she will do healthy exercise. According to Daley and Duda (2006), this has valuable implications for the design of physical activity interventions. Grounded in self-determination theory, providing autonomy support, that is, understanding what people want from physical activity and fostering opportunities for choice, enjoyment, and mastery, will increase the likelihood of people being in the maintenance stage.

Conclusions

The Mexican version of the BREQ-3 is a reliable and valid instrument for studying motivational regulations in exercise contexts, and it can be used in future studies to increase the generation of knowledge and scientific production in this area in Mexico. Its factorial structure coincides with what has been found in previous studies, and it contributes to understanding adherence to and adoption of physical exercise by combining two theoretical proposals: self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2002) and the stages of change from the transtheoretical model (Prochaska and Di Clemente, 1982; Prochaska et al., 1992a).

Author Contributions

JZ and IC conceived the hypothesis of this study. JZ was responsible for ethical approval. RB, MD and OA participated in data collection. JZ and IC analyzed the data and drafted the article. All the authors contributed to data interpretation of the statistical analysis. All the authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Berger, B. G., Pargman, D., and Weinberg, R. S. (2002). Foundations of Exercise Psychology. Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fi t indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, G. W., and Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model 9, 233–255. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem0902_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cid, L., Monteiro, D., Teixeira, D., Teques, P., Alves, S., Moutão, J., et al. (2018). The behavioural regulation in exercise questionnaire (BREQ-3) Portuguese-version: evidence of reliability, validity and invariance across gender. Front. Psychol. 9:1940. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cole, D. A., and Maxwell, S. E. (1985). Multitrait-multimethod comparisons across populations: a confirmatory factor analytic approach. Multivari. Behav. Res. 20:389. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2004_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, R., Probst, M., Bastos, T., Vilhena, E., Seabra, A., and Corredeira, R. (2018). Behavioural regulation in exercise questionnaire in people with schizophrenia: construct validity of the portuguese versions. Disabil. Rehabil. 40, 2577–2584, doi: 10.1080/09638288.2017.1342277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika 16, 297–334. doi: 10.1007/BF02310555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, A. J., and Duda, J. L. (2006). Self-determination, stage of readiness to change for exercise, and frequency of physical activity in young people. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 6, 231–243. doi: 10.1080/17461390601012637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dávila-Torres, J., González-Izquierdo, J. J., and Barrera-Cruz, A. (2015). Panorama de la obesidad en México [Overview of obesity in Mexico]. Rev. Med. Inst. Mex. Seguro. Soc. 53, 241–249.

Google Scholar

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (1985). Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Human Behavior. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Google Scholar

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behaviour. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (2002). Handbook of Self-Determination Research. New York, NY: The University Rochester Press.

Google Scholar

Duda, J. L., and Hayashi, C. T. (1998). “Measurement issues in cross-cultural research within sport and exercise psychology,” in Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement, eds J. L. Duda (Morgantown, WV: FIT), 471–483.

Fornell, C., and Larcker, D. F. (1981). Structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error: algebra and statistics. J. Mark Res. 382–388. doi: 10.2307/3151312

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gangyan, S., and Hing-chu, L. (2007). “Cross-cultural issues in sport psychology research,” in Social Psychology in Sport, eds S. Jowett and D. Lavallee. (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 279–288.

Google Scholar

González-Cutre, D., Sicilia, A., and Fernández, A. (2010). Hacia una mayor comprensión de la motivación en el ejercicio físico: medición de la regulación integrada en el contexto español [Toward a deeper understanding of motivation towards exercise: measurement of integrated regulation in the Spanish context]. Psicothema 22, 841–847. Available online at: http://www.psicothema.com/psicothema.asp?id=3810

Guedes, D., and Sofiati, S. (2015). Tradução e validação psicométrica do Behavioral Regulation in Exercise Questionnaire para uso em adultos brasileiros [Translation and psicometric validation of the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire for use in brazilian adults]. Rev. Bras. Ativ. Fís. Saúde 20, 397–412. doi: 10.12820/rbafs.v.20n4p397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guérin, E., Bales, E., Sweet, S., and Fortier, M. (2012). A meta-analysis of the influence of gender on self-determination theory's motivational regulations for physical activity. Can Psychol. 53, 291–300. doi: 10.1037/a0030215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hambleton, R. K., and Kanjee, A. (1995). Increasing the validity of cross-cultural assessments: use of improved methods for test adaptations. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess 11, 147–157. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.11.3.147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, J., Gagné, M., and Bureau, J. (2017). Testing a continuum structure of self-determined motivation: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 143, 1346–1377. doi: 10.1037/bul0000125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55.

Google Scholar

Ingledew, D. K., Markland, D., and Sheppard, K. E. (2004). Personality and self-determination of exercise behaviour. Pers. Individ. Dif. 36, 1921–1932. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.08.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, T. G., Prusak, K. A., Pennington, T., and Wilkinson, C. (2011). The effects of the type of skill test, choice, and gender on the situational motivation of physical education students. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 30, 281–295. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.30.3.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jöreskog, K., and Sörbom, D. (2006). LISREL 8.80. Structural Equation Modeling With the Simplis Command Language. Chicago, EU: Scientific Software International.

Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Landry, J. B., and Solmon, M. A. (2004). African american women's self-determination across the stages of change for exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 26, 457–469. doi: 10.1123/jsep.26.3.457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, M., and Sutton, A. (2011). Understanding exercise behavior: Examining the interaction of exercise motivation and personality in predicting exercise frequency. J. Sport Behav. 34, 82–97.

Google Scholar

Lutz, R., Lochbaum, M., and Turnbow, K. (2003). The role of relative autonomy in post-exercise affect responding. J. Sport Behav. 26, 137–154.

Google Scholar

Marcus, B., and Forsyth, L. (2009). Motivating People to Be Physically Active, 2nd Edn. New York, NY: Human Kinetics.

Markland, D., and Tobin, V. (2004). A Modification to the behavioural regulation in exercise questionnaire to include an assessment of amotivation. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 26, 191–196. doi: 10.1123/jsep.26.2.191

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, S., and Biddle, S. (2001). The transtheoretical model of behavior change: a meta-analysis of applications to physical activity and exercise. Ann. Behav. Med. 23, 229–246. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2304_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, J. A., Cervelló, E., and Martínez, A. (2007). Measuring self-determination motivation in a physical fitness setting: validation of the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire-2 (BREQ-2) in a Spanish sample. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 47, 366–374.

Google Scholar

Moustaka, F. C., Vlachopoulos, S. P., Vazou, S., Kaperoni, M., and Markland, D. A. (2010). Initial validity evidence for the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire-2 among greek exercise participants. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess 26, 269–276. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759/a000036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullan, E., and Markland, D. (1997). Variations in self-determination across the stages of change for exercise in adults. Motiv. Emot. 21, 349–362. doi: 10.1023/A:1024436423492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mullan, E., Markland, D., and Ingledew, D. K. (1997). Motivation for exercise: Development of a measure of behavioral regulation. J. Sport Sci. 15, 98–99.

National Institute of Public Health (2016). National Health and Nutrition Survey (ENSANUT). Mexico: National Institute of Public Health. Available online at: http://ghdx.healthdata.org/record/mexico-national-survey-health-and-nutrition-mid-way-2016

Prochaska, J. O., and Di Clemente, C. C. (1982). Transtheoretical therapy: toward a more integrative model of change. Psychotherapy 19, 276–288. doi: 10.1037/h0088437

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., and Norcross, J. C. (1992a). In search of how people change: applications to addictive behaviors. Am. Psychol. 47, 1102–1114. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.47.9.1102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prochaska, J. O., Norcross, J. C., Fowler, J., Follick, M., and Abrams, D. B. (1992b). Attendance and outcomes in a work-site weight control program: processes and stages of change as process and predictor variables. Addict. Behav. 17, 35–45.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R., and Deci, E. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sass, D. A. (2011). Testing measurement invariance and comparing latent factor means within a confirmatory factor analysis framework. J. Psychoeduc. Assess 29, 347–363. doi: 10.1177/0734282911406661

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, B., McCaughtry, N., and Martin, J. (2008). Urban adolescents' exercise intentions and behaviors: an exploratory study of a trans-contextual model. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33, 841–858. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Su, X., McBride, R. E., and Xiang, P. (2015). College students' achievement goal orientation and motivational regulations in physical activity clases: a test of gender invariance. J. Teach Phys. Educ. 34, 2–17. doi: 10.1123/jtpe.2013-0151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multivariate Statistics. New York, NY: Pearson Education.

Google Scholar

Thogersen-Ntoumani, C., and Ntoumanis, N. (2006). The role of self-determined motivation in the understanding of exercise-related behaviours, cognitions and physical self-evaluations. J. Sport Sci. 24, 393–404. doi: 10.1080/02640410500131670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J. (1997). “Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation,” in Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 29, ed M. P. Zanna (San Diego: Academic Press), 271–360

Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J. (2001). “A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport and exercise settings,” in Advances in Motivation in Sport and Exercise (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 321–357.

Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., and Koestner, R. (2008). Reflections on self-determination theory. Can. Psychol. 49, 257–262. doi: 10.1037/a0012804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Widaman, K. F. (1985). Hierarchically nested covariance structure models for multitrait-multimethod data. Appl. Psychol. Meas 9, 1–26.

Google Scholar

Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., and Fraser, S. N. (2002). Examining the psychometric properties of the behavioral regulation in exercise questionnaire. Meas Phys. Educ. Exerc. Sci. 6, 1–21. doi: 10.1207/S15327841MPEE0601_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., Loitz, C. C., and Scime, G. (2006). “It's Who I Am … Really!' The importance of integrated regulation in exercise contexts. J. Appl. Biobehav. Res. 11, 79–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2006.tb00021.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamarripa, J., Ruiz-Juan, F., López-Walle, J. M, and Fernández-Baños, R. (2013). Actividad e inactividad física durante el tiempo libre en la población adulta de Monterrey (Nuevo León, México) [Physical activity and inactivity during leisure time in the adult population of Monterrey (Nuevo Leon, Mexico)]. Retos 24, 91–96. Available online at: https://recyt.fecyt.es/index.php/retos/article/view/34534

Google Scholar

Zamarripa, J., Ruiz-Juan, F., and Ruiz-Risueño Abad, J. (2018). El balance decisional, etapas de cambio y nivel de actividad física en estudiantes de bachillerato [The decisional balance, stages of change and level of physical activity in high school students]. Andamios.

Keywords: motivation, self-regulation, adherence, exercise, BREQ-3, stages of change

Citation: Zamarripa J, Castillo I, Baños R, Delgado M and Álvarez O (2018) Motivational Regulations Across the Stages of Change for Exercise in the General Population of Monterrey (Mexico). Front. Psychol. 9:2368. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02368

Received: 05 July 2018; Accepted: 12 November 2018;
Published: 03 December 2018.

Edited by:

Sergio Machado, Salgado de Oliveira University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Juergen Beckmann, Technische Universität München, Germany
Diogo Monteiro, Escola Superior de Desporto de Rio Maior, Instituto Politécnico Santarém, Portugal

Copyright © 2018 Zamarripa, Castillo, Baños, Delgado and Álvarez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jorge Zamarripa, jorge.zamarriparv@uanl.edu.mx

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.