Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 18 October 2018
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Phantom Sensation and Pain: Underlying Mechanisms and Innovative Treatments View all 14 articles

Commentary: Mechanical Pain Thresholds and the Rubber Hand Illusion

  • School of Psychology, University of East London, London, United Kingdom

A Commentary on
Mechanical Pain Thresholds and the Rubber Hand Illusion

by Bauer, A., Hagenburger, J., Plank, T., Busch, V., and Greenlee, M. W. (2018). Front. Psychol. 9:712. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00712

Investigations into the relationship between bodily illusions and pain perception are representing a relatively modern trend in cognitive science. Recently, Bauer et al. (2018) published a work with the aim to determine if the vision of a potentially painful stimulus threatening the rubber hand can modify the mechanical pain threshold (MPT). They state that MPT remains relatively stable during the induction of the rubber hand illusion (RHI), yet it can be significantly decreased by the vision of an artificial threat to the RH. The purpose of the present commentary is to provide alternative explanations to Bauer's results, which have not been discussed in their article. This process would help promote additional reflection on this topic and hopefully foster further advances in this field.

The analgesic effects linked to the vision of the own body (“visual analgesia”) were initially described by Longo et al. (2009). Although there is not full consensus (Mohan et al., 2012; Torta et al., 2015), such effect has been consistently reported by many other studies (see Martini, 2016 for a review. For a neurophysiological explanation to this phenomenon please see the review written by Haggard et al., 2013). Discussing their main finding and supported by control experiments, Bauer and colleagues argue that their results stand in contrast to Longo's “visual analgesia” and they suggest that this apparent discrepancy might be due to the different material used in their experiments. I agree with the authors on the fact that the vision of a threatening stimulus can increase pain sensation (Arntz and Claassens, 2004; Höfle et al., 2012; Martini et al., 2013). While Bauer and colleagues threatened the rubber hand with a knife, clear threatening stimuli were not used in Longo's et al. study. However, there might be something else. In a recent virtual hand illusion (VHI) study, Nierula et al. (2017) set out to verify whether the distance between the real and the fake limb, typically present in RHI studies, could dampen visual analgesia. What they found was a significant decrease in heat pain thresholds when the virtual hand was far from the real hand compared to when they were perfectly co-located. So, visual analgesia is hindered if the real and the fake hand are not in the same place. The lack of analgesic effect due to the vision of “one' s own” body in Bauer's study could be then due to the distance (20 cm) between the real and the rubber hand. If this is true it might explain why, during the vision of the rubber hand being simply touched by the knife handle, there was no analgesic effect revealed by a higher MPT linked to the vision of “one' s own” body. Additionally, given the type of visual stimuli (knife point = threat vs. knife handle = no-threat) and the paradigm (RHI) used in their study, I think they should have discussed their findings also in the light of the latest findings on skin conductance response (SCR). Indeed, recent evidence point at an increase in the arousal response during the vision of stimuli approaching the owned rubber hand, regardless of the affective valence of the stimulus (Ma and Hommel, 2013; Johnson et al., 2016). So, the choice of a knife handle as a control stimulus could not be entirely appropriate.

In their third control condition Bauer and colleagues asked their participants to close their eyes before the measurement of MPT, so they did not see any stimuli approaching the RH. During this condition a modulation of proprioceptive drift was reported and a high level of ownership was found, but no pain modulation was documented. The authors thus state that “the induction of the RHI alone did not change the MPT values significantly” and that this would be in contrast with Martini et al. (2014). However, in the mentioned study all conditions envisaged constant visual feedback (i.e., no eyes closed) and the main finding was interpreted in favor of the transfer of the visual analgesia to virtual bodies, never mentioning a possible analgesic effect of the VHI “alone.” What precisely is this effect they refer to has to be clarified. Maybe the authors refer to another possible analgesic effect related to “disownership” of the real hand, which they state it did not take place. Unfortunately the phenomenon of disownership, likely overlapping the “loss of own hand” phenomenon (Longo et al., 2008), has not been directly measured by the authors. A future investigation specifically targeting the real contribution of the “disownership” phenomenon in pain studies with bodily illusion is therefore needed.

Another point worth discussing might be the type of pain chosen to measure the participants' pain threshold: the majority of studies about visual analgesia during RHI/VHI paradigms made use of thermal or electrical stimuli. In Bauer's experiment mechanical stimuli were chosen. The authors explain their preference stating that “MPT is assumed to be closer to clinical pain than thresholds measured with thermal stimuli,” but unfortunately no explanations nor any references were provided to support their assertion. Mechanical, electrical and heat pain threshold have been shown to have some level of independence and can react differently to different modulators (for ex. Tong et al., 2007; Okkerse et al., 2017). Furthermore, drawing on previous neurophysiological studies reporting a differential contribution of myelinated A-δ and unmyelienated C-fibers in different types of pain, Lötsch et al. (2016) have shown how electrical, thermal, and pinprick mechanical stimuli belong to three separate clusters of pain measures, and these stimuli seem to be processed differently in the brain (Murrell et al., 2007). Thus, the choice of the type of pain to gauge, as well as of other components of the experimental design (for ex. the choice of the control conditions), can make the difference in this type of experiments (Martini et al., 2015).

As a final point, given the high inter-subject variability and the complexity of the “embodiment” phenomenon (Longo et al., 2008), it might be always worthy reporting qualitative data too. What is a praxis for clinical research with patients could be extended to healthy participants as well, to boost interpretability of data and comparability among studies.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Arntz, A., and Claassens, L. (2004). The meaning of pain influences its experienced intensity. Pain 109, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2003.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauer, A., Hagenburger, J., Plank, T., Busch, V., and Greenlee, M. W. (2018). Mechanical pain thresholds and the rubber hand illusion. Front. Psychol. 9:712. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haggard, P., Iannetti, G. D., and Longo, M. R. (2013). Spatial sensory organization and body representation in pain perception. Curr. Biol. 23, R164–R176. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Höfle, M., Hauck, M., Engel, A. K., and Senkowski, D. (2012). Viewing a needle pricking a hand that you perceive as yours enhances unpleasantness of pain. Pain 153, 1074–1081. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, M. I., Smith, E., Yellow, S., and Mulvey, M. R. (2016). A preliminary investigation into psychophysiological effects of threatening a perceptually embodied rubber hand in healthy human participants. Scand. J. Pain 11, 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Betti, V., Aglioti, S. M., and Haggard, P. (2009). Visually induced analgesia: seeing the body reduces pain. J. Neurosci. 29, 12125–12130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3072-09.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M. P., Tsakiris, M., and Haggard, P. (2008). What is embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition 107, 978–998. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lötsch, J., Dimova, V., Ultsch, A., Lieb, I., Zimmermann, M., Geisslinger, G., et al. (2016). A small yet comprehensive subset of human experimental pain models emerging from correlation analysis with a clinical quantitative sensory testing protocol in healthy subjects. Eur. J. Pain 20, 777–789. doi: 10.1002/ejp.803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, K., and Hommel, B. (2013). The virtual-hand illusion: effects of impact and threat on perceived ownership and affective resonance. Front. Psychol. 4:604. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martini, M. (2016). Real, rubber or virtual: the vision of “one' s own” body as a means for pain modulation. A narrative review. Conscious. Cogn. 43, 143–151. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martini, M., Perez-Marcos, D., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2013). What Color is My Arm? Changes in skin color of an embodied virtual arm modulates pain threshold. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:438. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martini, M., Perez-Marcos, D., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2014). Modulation of pain threshold by virtual body ownership. Eur. J. Pain. 18, 1040–1048. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.00451.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martini, M., Perez-Marcos, D., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2015). Author's reply to the commentary by Gilpin et al. Eur. J. Pain 19, 143–144. doi: 10.1002/ejp.606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohan, R., Jensen, K. B., Petkova, V. I., Dey, A., Barnsley, N., Ingvar, M., et al. (2012). No pain relief with the rubber hand illusion. PLoS ONE 7:e52400. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murrell, J. C., Mitchinson, S. L., Waters, D., and Johnson, C. B. (2007). Comparative effect of thermal, mechanical, and electrical noxious stimuli on the electroencephalogram of the rat. Br. J. Anaesth. 98, 366–371. doi: 10.1093/bja/ael377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nierula, B., Martini, M., Matamala-Gomez, M., Slater, M., and Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2017). Seeing an embodied virtual hand is analgesic contingent on colocation. J. Pain 18, 645–655. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2017.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okkerse, P., van Amerongen, G., de Kam, M. L., Stevens, J., Butt, R. P., Gurrell, R., et al. (2017). The use of a battery of pain models to detect analgesic properties of compounds: a two-part four-way crossover study. Br. J. Clin. Pharmacol. 83, 976–990. doi: 10.1111/bcp.13183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, K. C., Lo, S. K., and Cheing, G. L. (2007). Alternating frequencies of transcutaneous electric nerve stimulation: does it produce greater analgesic effects on mechanical and thermal pain thresholds? Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 88, 1344–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torta, D. M., Legrain, V., and Mouraux, A. (2015). Looking at the hand modulates the brain responses to nociceptive and non-nociceptive somatosensory stimuli but does not necessarily modulate their perception. Psychophysiology 52, 1010–1018. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: pain, body ownership, rubber hand illusion, virtual hand illusion, embodiment

Citation: Martini M (2018) Commentary: Mechanical Pain Thresholds and the Rubber Hand Illusion. Front. Psychol. 9:1715. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01715

Received: 19 July 2018; Accepted: 24 August 2018;
Published: 18 October 2018.

Edited by:

Jack Tsao, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, United States

Reviewed by:

Mark W. Greenlee, University of Regensburg, Germany

Copyright © 2018 Martini. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Matteo Martini, m.martini@uel.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.