- Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada
Extensive work has demonstrated the benefits of bilingualism on executive functioning (EF) across the lifespan. Concurrently, other research has shown that EF is related to emotion regulation (ER), an ability that is integral to healthy socio-emotional development. However, no research to date has investigated whether bilingualism-related advantages in EF can also be found in emotional contexts. The current study examined the performance of 93 children who were 9-years old, about half of whom were bilingual, on the Emotional Face N-Back Task, an ER task used to assess the interference effect of emotional processing on working memory. Bilingual children were more accurate than monolingual children in both 1-back and 2-back conditions but were significantly slower than monolingual children on the 2-back condition. There were significant effects of emotional valence on reaction time, but these did not differ across language groups. These results confirm previous research showing better EF performance by bilinguals, but no differences in ER were found between language groups. Findings are discussed in the context of our current understanding of the ER literature with potential implications for previously unexplored differences between monolingual and bilingual children.
Introduction
Flexible and effective emotion regulation (ER) is critical for healthy psychosocial adjustment throughout development (Cole and Deater-Deckert, 2009; Eisenberg et al., 2010). The inability to properly regulate emotions, or emotional dysregulation, has been shown to underlie a range of maladaptive outcomes including aggressive behavior problems (e.g., Stieben et al., 2007; Lewis et al., 2008; Holley et al., 2017) and academic underachievement (e.g., Hawkins et al., 1999; Gumora and Arsenio, 2002; Djambazova-Popordanoska, 2016) in children. Although the past several decades have seen a steady increase in research investigating the consequences of maladaptive ER, factors that promote the development of adaptive ER remain poorly understood. However, there is evidence that ER is highly interrelated with executive functioning (EF; e.g., for a review, see Zelazo and Cunningham, 2007; Calkins and Marcovitch, 2010), and that individual differences in EF are predictive of ER abilities (e.g., Kieras et al., 2005; McDermott et al., 2009; for a review, see Schmeichel and Tang, 2015). As such, individual factors that enhance EF may be expected to promote the development of adaptive ER abilities.
Although seemingly distinct from ER, bilingualism, or proficiency in a second language (L2) is associated with advantages on a variety of EF tasks (see Adesope et al., 2010, for a meta-analysis; for a review, see Barac et al., 2014). Some EF tasks administered in research on bilingualism have also been used in ER research to assess cognitive control as it interacts with emotional processing (e.g., Bell and Wolfe, 2004; for a review, see Cole et al., 2004). Examining the interrelation between cognitive and emotional processing (Bell and Wolfe, 2004) may be key to explaining how proficiency in an L2 may also promote ER. The current study investigates whether bilingualism supports more adaptive ER strategies in school-aged children than is found for monolingual children.
Literature on ER has attracted significant attention due to its association with a variety of important developmental outcomes (for review, see Gross, 2002; Eisenberg et al., 2010). Although some controversy continues to exist over its constituents, Calkins and Hill (2007) view ER as the range of conscious and unconscious behaviors, skills, and strategies that change one’s emotional experience and expression either in an automatic or effortful way. Most definitions of ER also recognize interacting emotional and cognitive processes as integral to ER (for review, see Calkins and Marcovitch, 2010). These cognitive operations, including working memory, fall under the umbrella term of EF (Miyake et al., 2000; for a review, see Zelazo and Carlson, 2012). Working memory has been defined as a “cognitive system in which memory and attention interact to produce complex cognition” (Shipstead et al., 2015) and is a pivotal component of the EF system (Miyake et al., 2000). Working memory not only requires memory updating and retention but also relies on attentional control, which can vary between task conditions and challenge the EF system to different degrees (Miyake et al., 2000). Within ER contexts, EF interacts with emotional processing to modify appraisals, feelings, and behaviors in response to emotional experiences (for review, see Zelazo and Cunningham, 2007; Calkins and Marcovitch, 2010). Optimal ER development thus depends on the acquisition of cognitive skills such as working memory that allow the child to focus on task-relevant information with minimal interference from distracting and non-goal-oriented cues.
Research on emotion and working memory has primarily focused on adult clinical populations, but other research has investigated the emotion–cognition interaction in both clinical and non-clinical samples across development (Bradley et al., 1999; Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Ladouceur et al., 2009). One of the most popular paradigms used to assess working memory is the n-back task (for a review, see Owen et al., 2005; Meule, 2017). In this task, participants are asked to recall whether the location or identity of a target on the screen (e.g., the letter M) matches the location or identity of a stimulus presented n trials previously; memory load increases as n increases. Emotional faces (angry, sad, fearful, happy, neutral/calm) are often adopted as distracting emotional cues.
Studies investigating the impact of emotional valence on working memory report mixed findings. This is likely due to the extensive variability in the populations studied (typically vs. atypically developing, and different age groups), making it difficult to draw parallels between findings and particularly challenging to make predictions for typically developing children. Some authors find only significant slowing in response to negative emotional cues relative to positive or neutral ones (healthy adult sample: Kensinger and Corkin, 2003; anxious sample of 8- to 30-year-olds: Ladouceur et al., 2009), whereas others report no reaction time (RT) differences by emotion type and instead report impaired accuracy on trials with negative compared to neutral distractors (adult controls and ADHD participants: Marx et al., 2011). Others have reported varying speed-accuracy trade-offs by emotion type, including higher accuracy but slower RTs for negative compared to neutral stimuli in a non-verbal working memory task with a sample of schizophrenic participants (Becerril and Barch, 2011). This finding supports previous work showing that in emotionally dysregulated populations, aversive stimuli generate a significantly larger burden on the cognitive system than positive ones, depleting resources available for working memory (Bishop et al., 2004; Hare et al., 2005). For example, emotionally dysregulated, clinically depressed patients report an inability to disengage from pervasive negative thoughts (for a review, see Gotlib and Joormann, 2010), resulting in memory challenges as well as difficulty with planning and concentration (Paelecke-Habermann et al., 2005; Rose and Ebmeier, 2006).
Similar tendencies have been reported among clinically anxious and depressed children, showing differences in performance compared to healthy controls. Ladouceur et al. (2005) administered the Emotional N-back Task to a sample of 75 children (8–16 years of age) categorized into one of four groups: children who met criteria for an anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, comorbid anxiety and depression, or were identified as a normal control group. In this version of the task, the distracting emotional stimuli were neutral, negative, and positive images in the background of the to-be-remembered letters. Their results showed that children with major depressive disorder and those with comorbid anxiety and depression had significantly longer RTs on the negative condition than on the neutral condition, whereas children in the normal control group had significantly longer RTs on the positive condition than on the neutral condition. Ladouceur et al. (2009) took a developmental approach to demonstrate ER changes with age within a sample of 8- to 30-year-old participants with low versus high levels of trait anxiety. The authors used the emotional face N-back task with 0-back and 2-back memory load conditions and three emotional face distractor types (neutral, fearful, and happy) as well as a control condition with shapes. Their findings revealed that individuals high in trait anxiety had slower RTs on the fearful 2-back memory-load condition than on the happy and neutral trials, but that the effect was greatest in younger participants. Conversely, individuals low in trait anxiety did not reveal any emotion effects, either in RT or accuracy rates. Taken together, these findings highlight that there are differences between how children and adults process distracting emotional information and that we continue to find inconsistent results when investigating interacting cognitive and emotional processing in typically developing children.
From a separate area of research, the cognitive benefit for bilingual individuals has been identified as enhanced performance on tasks requiring non-verbal EF (for a review, see Bialystok, 2017). Improvement in EF is believed to develop as a result of the well-documented coactivation of both languages within the bilingual brain, even when only one language is in use (e.g., Beauvillain and Grainger, 1987; Colomé, 2001; for a review, see Kroll et al., 2014). The practice of attending to one cue (one language) during interference from another “trains” the EF network (for a review, see Bialystok, 2015), becoming more effective throughout life and thereby extending the practice of verbal cognitive control to the non-verbal EF network (Green, 1998; Abutalebi et al., 2008; Luk et al., 2010). Evidence for the enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals comes from research using a variety of cognitive tasks with infants (Kovacs and Mehler, 2009), toddlers (Poulin-Dubois et al., 2011), young children (e.g., see Adesope et al., 2010, for a meta-analysis), and young adults (e.g., Costa et al., 2009). Recent findings have emerged that do not support these results with young adults (e.g., Paap and Greenberg, 2013) possibly due to differences in populations, criteria for bilingualism, or the nature of the experimental tasks used to assess cognitive ability (for a review, see Antoniou, 2019). However, the majority of the research points to bilingual benefits across a variety of cognitive control operations, especially where conflict conditions pose additional attentional demands on the EF system.
Given the central importance of working memory to EF, some bilingualism research has investigated differences in verbal and non-verbal working memory in children and adults, with mixed findings. Morales et al. (2013) conducted two studies that assessed working memory in 5-year-old (Study 1) and 5- to 7-year-old (Study 2) monolingual and bilingual children. In Study 1, the authors found that differences in performance between the groups emerged only on the most challenging condition of a Simon-type task, with bilingual children showing an EF advantage when a high level of conflict was present. In the second study, where children were required to recall the positions of frogs presented either simultaneously (easy) or sequentially (hard) within a 3 × 3 grid (Frog Matrices task), bilingual children had better accuracy on the more challenging sequential condition. Blom et al. (2014) investigated both visuospatial (Dot Matrix and Odd-One-Out tasks) and verbal working memory (Forward and Backward Digit Recall) performance in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children and Dutch monolingual controls from low socioeconomic backgrounds. Although no difference was found between the two language groups in 5-year-old children, by 6 years of age bilingual children showed overall benefits on the Dot Matrix task and the Backward Digit Recall task, both of which pose additional demand for EF over the other two tasks. While some have failed to reproduce this effect using simpler working memory measures (Engel de Abreu, 2011), bilingual children show advantages over their monolingual peers on conditions of heightened conflict.
In summary, several lines of evidence depict bilingual advantages in EF on tasks where successful performance depends on the ability to resolve conflict from competing cues and ignore interfering information or to maintain task rules in working memory. Ultimately, by monitoring language choice among competing linguistic systems, bilinguals must learn to more effectively regulate attention to distracting information, resulting in an EF system that is better equipped to support processes of working memory.
What are the implications of bilingualism for ER? Importantly, emotional and cognitive processes are highly interactive and integral to ER in that effective ER in emotional contexts depends on the EF system to process relevant information without being impaired by interfering emotional cues (Gray et al., 2002). Concurrently, literature on bilingualism shows evidence of strengthened cognitive control in dual-language users, resulting in greater selective attention to relevant information and reduced interference from distracting cues (Bialystok, 2015). It is thus reasonable to hypothesize that bilingualism may promote the development of more adaptive ER by strengthening the cognitive control system and all its constituents, including working memory. Furthermore, if bilingualism contributes to the development of self-regulatory abilities in emotionally challenging contexts at earlier stages of development than is found for monolinguals, then these enhanced abilities may also have implications for children’s psychosocial outcomes. However, research assessing ER differences between monolingual and bilingual children in this manner, and more comprehensively using standardized ER tasks, is largely lacking, and a direct evaluation of ER differences between these groups has not been undertaken.
The present study aimed to investigate the effect of bilingualism on cognitive and emotional processing that is integral to ER. The Emotional Face N-Back Task, an emotionally based EF task of working memory with three emotion conditions (angry, happy, and neutral), was used to examine differences in ER in school-aged monolingual and bilingual children. The overarching hypothesis was that bilingual children would demonstrate an overall advantage in working memory. Given that an EF advantage was expected in the bilingual group, and that the ability to modulate attention toward or away from emotionally salient information is a marker of ER and associated with EF, we anticipated finding evidence for ER benefits for bilinguals. Although the findings in this area with healthy children are mixed, it was predicted that the ER benefits for bilinguals would be most salient on the particularly challenging angry emotion trials, with the highest EF demands. This is the first study to compare these processes in healthy young children and evaluate the influence of bilingualism on ER. The novelty of this research will contribute to our understanding of differences in emotional processing between groups of children with different language experiences, over and beyond the known advantage of cognitive control in bilingual children.
Materials and Methods
Participants
One hundred and two children between 8- and 11-years old were recruited from four elementary schools. Based on caregiver reports of the child’s language background, an aggregate score was created to classify children as monolingual or bilingual. Nine children were removed from the study due to behavioral concerns that prevented them from completing the tasks. Complete data for analysis were available for 93 children, 48 monolinguals (M age = 9.3 years, SD = 0.6; 18 boys) and 45 bilinguals (M age = 9.4 years, SD = 0.5; 20 boys). The majority of children were born in Canada (78.5%), with 10 children born in the Philippines (10.8%) and the remainder being born in 10 different countries. Children in the bilingual group proficiently spoke a non-English language at home: Portuguese (n = 15), Philippine dialect (n = 12; Tagalog, Vasayan, or Ilonggo), Italian (n = 5), Spanish (n = 6), or seven other different languages (n = 7). School instruction was in English for all children.
Procedure
Approval to test in the schools was obtained from the University Ethics Committee and from the school board’s ethics committee. The principal and teachers at each school agreed to have researchers introduce the study tasks to the children within their own classrooms. A packet of questionnaires was sent home with each child so that interested parents could complete the parental informed consent, the Language and Social Background Questionnaire (LSBQ), the Strengths and Weaknesses of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms and Normal Behavior Scale (SWAN), and the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC). Before working with a child, qualified research assistants ensured that the complete packet had been returned to the teacher. Teachers were also asked to complete the ERC for each child that returned the packet of questionnaires.
Each child who returned a completed packet to their school was withdrawn from their classroom for approximately 45 min to complete the testing session. The procedure was explained to the child, and verbal assent was obtained prior to testing. During the session, each participant completed the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT), a standardized test of English proficiency, the Raven Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven), assessing spatial reasoning, and the Emotional Face N-Back Task (Figure 1), a task of ER. Upon completion of the n-back task, children were asked to subjectively rate the expression of a subset of angry, happy, and neutral faces that they had seen during the task to assess whether all children perceived the faces similarly. Throughout the session, children received stickers for completing each task. Research assistants made ongoing notes during the session to identify children whose behavior (talking, singing, refusal to continue, excessive fidgeting or movement, etc.) interfered with their ability to complete the tasks; these children were later removed from the study (n = 9). Each child was thanked for their participation and awarded a personalized certificate to recognize their effort before being walked back to their classroom.
FIGURE 1. Emotional Face N-Back Task (adapted from Ladouceur et al., 2009). Children see one letter at a time presented on the screen and are asked to indicate whether the letter is the same as the letter they saw one screen back (1-back) or two screens back (2-back), creating two memory conditions. A face displaying an angry, happy, or neutral expression is shown on both sides of the letter.
Questionnaires and Tasks
Language and Social Background Questionnaire (LSBQ; Anderson et al., 2018)
The LSBQ is completed by parents/guardians and contains questions pertaining to the child’s age, sex, handedness, time spent using video/computer games, and language fluency and use in different contexts. Parental education is indicated and used as a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES). SES was assessed as the average of mother’s and father’s education, using a 5-point scale with 1 indicating no high school diploma, 3 indicating some college or college diploma, and 5 indicating graduate or professional degree.
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn and Dunn, 1997)
The PPVT is a standardized measure of receptive vocabulary. Children hear a word and are required to point to which one of four pictures corresponds with that word. Testing proceeds until the child makes eight errors within a block of 12 words. The PPVT normally takes 15–20 min to complete. Scores are standardized based on the participant’s age (μ = 100, SD = 15). The PPVT has a high reliability (>0.90) across a variety of measures (i.e., internal consistency, split-half, test–retest) and a 0.91 correlation with the Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children’s measure of verbal ability (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991).
Raven Standard Progressive Matrices (Raven Test; Raven et al., 1996)
The Raven test is a standardized test of non-verbal spatial reasoning. Children view test figures and chose which item from a set of six options provides the best completion. The task normally takes 10–15 min to complete with children. Results are converted to standardized scores based on the participant’s age (μ = 100, SD = 15). The predictive validity of the Raven test is around 0.70, whereas test–retest reliability and internal consistency coefficients range between 0.80 and 0.93 (Raven et al., 1996).
Strengths and Weaknesses of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms and Normal Behavior Scale (SWAN; Swanson et al., 2001)
The SWAN questionnaire is completed by a child’s parent or guardian and teacher. The SWAN includes 18 items that are associated with the characteristic symptoms assessed for a diagnosis of ADHD as described in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). These include nine symptoms related to inattention (e.g., “Stays focused on tasks and activities”), six symptoms related to hyperactivity (e.g., “Can sit without constant fidgeting or squirming”), and three items related to impulsivity (e.g., “Easily waits turn, such as standing in line-ups”). Each item is positively worded and was modified slightly from the original test to improve ease of reading and decrease word difficulty for parents/guardians who may struggle with understanding English (e.g., “Sustains attention on tasks or play activities” was changed to “Stays focused on tasks and activities”). A guardian and teacher rated the child on each item using a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “Far below average” (1) to “Far above average” (4). Higher scores are indicative of better attentional abilities, lower hyperactivity, and lower impulsivity. The SWAN has excellent internal consistency and reliability (Young et al., 2009; Lakes et al., 2012).
Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields and Cicchetti, 1997)
The ERC is a 24-item measure intended to assess the frequency of children’s displays of affective behaviors. Parents/caregivers and teachers rate the frequency of the behavior using a 4-point scale. The raw scores generate two subscales: (1) ER, which assesses socially appropriate emotional responses and empathy, and (2) lability-negativity, which assess arousal more broadly, focusing on anger, dysregulation, and mood lability. High internal consistency has been shown for both the lability-negativity and ER subscales, with Cronbach’s alphas of 0.96 and 0.83, respectively (Shields and Cicchetti, 1997).
Emotional Face N-Back Task (N-back; Adapted From Ladouceur et al., 2009)
The emotional variant of the n-back paradigm is designed to examine the interference effect of emotional information on working memory performance. The task consisted of two memory conditions (1-back and 2-back), with blocked emotional (angry, happy) and neutral conditions, for each level of difficulty. Letters were presented in the middle of the screen and two of the same emotional faces were presented simultaneously on both sides of the letter to act as the emotional distractors (see Figure 1). In the 1-back condition, participants were asked if the letter was the same as the letter on the previous trial (target, “yes”) or not (non-target, “no”). In the 2-back condition, participants decided whether the current letter matched the trial that was presented two trials previously (target) or not (non-target). Responses were made using two mice, one assigned to each response, with the dominant hand assigned to target trials and the non-dominant hand to non-target trials. Angry, happy, and neutral faces were taken from the NimStim set available at www.macbrain.org (Tottenham et al., 2009), and modified so that only an oval-shaped face was visible, without hair or a neck. Each emotion block was made up of 15 target (“yes”) and 25 non-target (“no”) trials. The task took approximately 15 min to complete.
Affect Rating
After completing the n-back task children were presented with the angry, happy, and neutral expressions of three NimStim actors whose faces they had seen during the task. The NimStim actors were two females and one male, all demographically diverse. Children chose one adjective to describe the expression on each face without being told whether the face was meant to portray a happy, angry, or neutral expression. The purpose was to assess whether there were differences in how monolingual and bilingual children perceive emotional expressions. The top three descriptive words used to identify each emotional face were compared between the two language groups. The findings were also used to determine whether the child descriptions found in the current study replicated previous findings from the child literature depicting neutral faces as more aversive to children than happy faces.
Results
The background measures for age, SES (parental education), vocabulary knowledge (PPVT), and nonverbal cognitive functioning (Raven test), are reported in Table 1. One-way ANOVAs for language group showed no differences between children in the two groups on any of these measures (all ps > 0.14). Mean scores on the subscales of the SWAN (attention, hyperactivity, impulsivity) and the ERC (ER, negativity/lability) are reported in Table 2 for teacher and parent/guardian reports. One-way ANOVAs for language group showed no differences between the teacher ratings for children in the two groups on any of the subscales (all ps > 0.31), but parent ratings revealed that monolingual and bilingual children were rated similarly on hyperactivity (p = 0.76), impulsivity (p = 0.99), ER (p = 0.63), and negativity/lability (p = 0.41), but differently on attention, F (1,90) = 4.53, p = 0.04, = 0.05, with bilingual children (3.1) being rated as more attentive than monolingual children (2.8).
TABLE 2. Mean score and standard deviation for reports made by teachers and parents on children’s behavior by language group.
The outcomes for accuracy and RT on the Emotional Face N-back task are reported in Table 3 and Figure 2, respectively. Accuracy on correct target trials was analyzed using a three-way ANOVA for n-back condition (1-back, 2-back), emotion (angry, happy, neutral), and language group (monolingual, bilingual). The analysis revealed a main effect of condition, F (1,91) = 239.97, p < 0.001, = 0.73, with children scoring higher on the 1-back (75.78%) than on the more challenging 2-back (52.70%) condition, and a main effect of language group, F (1,91) = 9.71, p < 0.01, = 0.08, with bilingual children (67.83%) outperforming monolingual children (60.6%). There was no main effect of emotion, p = 0.26, and no interactions, all Fs < 1.37, ps > 0.26.
TABLE 3. Mean score and standard deviation for accuracy on the Emotional Face N-back Task by language group.
FIGURE 2. Mean reaction times (and standard errors) on the Emotional Face N-back Task by condition (1-back vs. 2-back), emotion (angry, happy, neutral), and language group (monolingual vs. bilingual).
Accuracy on nontarget trials was also investigated to determine whether the higher accuracy scores on target trials for the bilingual group reflected a response bias to say “yes” (i.e., identify more trials as target trials). Nontarget trials were analyzed using a three-way ANOVA for condition, emotion, and language group. The results revealed only a main effect of n-back condition, F (1,91) = 27.81, p < 0.001, = 0.22, with children correctly identifying more nontarget trials (“no” response) on the 1-back (87.48%) than on the 2-back (79.04%), as expected. There was no main effect of language group (p = 0.64) or emotion (p = 0.16), or any interactions, all Fs < 0.60, ps > 0.35.
Reaction time data for the Emotional Face N-back task were analyzed the same way as accuracy data, using a three-way ANOVA for n-back condition, emotion, and language group on target trials (see Figure 2). There was a main effect for condition, F (1,91) = 21.86, p < 0.001, = 0.18, with children performing slower on the more challenging 2-back (958 ms) than the 1-back (868 ms). A main effect of emotion, F (1,186) = 13.03, p < 0.001, = 0.13, revealed that RTs were significantly slower on the neutral trials (949 ms) than on angry (902 ms), p < 0.001, or happy (886), p < 0.001, trials, with no difference between the latter two, p = 0.57. Furthermore, a two-way interaction of n-back condition and emotion, F (2,182) = 16.71, p < 0.001, = 0.08, revealed that the effect of emotion was present on the 1-back condition, F (2,182) = 39.09, p < 0.001, = 0.10, but not on the 2-back condition, p = 0.89. Finally, a main effect of language group, F (1,91) = 5.46, p = 0.02, = 0.07, revealed that bilingual children (956 ms) were significantly slower than their monolingual peers (870 ms), but a two-way interaction of condition and language group, F (1,91) = 9.03, p < 0.01, = 0.08, restricted this difference to the 2-back condition F (1,91) = 9.10, p < 0.01, = 0.10, with no difference between groups in the 1-back condition, p = 0.23.
A correlation was computed between accuracy and RT for each condition to determine whether there were speed-accuracy trade-offs. There were no significant correlations in the 1-back condition, r (93) = 0.19, p = 0.16, but the relation was significant in the 2-back condition, r (93) = 0.34, p = 0.001. Given that bilingual children performed significantly slower than monolingual children on the 2-back, a correlation was run separately by language group to determine whether the speed-accuracy trade-off in the 2-back was driven by the bilingual group. The correlation revealed that the speed-accuracy trade-off was significant for the bilingual children, r (45) = 0.34, p = 0.02, but only marginal for monolingual children, r (48) = 0.24, p = 0.09. However, the Fisher r-to-z transformation revealed that the difference between the two correlations was not significant, p = 0.61.
Reaction time on nontarget trials was also investigated using a three-way ANOVA for n-back condition, emotion, and language group. There was a main effect of emotion, F (2,182) = 19.29, p < 0.001, = 0.18, with children performing more slowly on neutral trials (969 ms) than on angry (909 ms) or happy (901 ms) trials, all ps < 0.001, with no difference in speed of performance between angry and happy emotions, p = 1.00. A main effect of language group was also found, F (1,91) = 5.30, p = 0.02, = 0.07, in which bilingual children (971 ms) were slower than their monolingual peers (880 ms). An interaction of n-back condition by emotion, F (2,182) = 21.06, p < 0.001, = 0.20, revealed that differences in speed of responding between emotion blocks emerged only on the 1-back version of the task, F (2,182) = 43.13, p < 0.001, = 0.42, where children performed slower on angry (900 ms) than happy (856 ms) trials, slower on neutral (995 ms) than angry trials, and slower on happy than neutral trials, all ps < 0.01; no differences between angry (918 ms), happy (946 ms), and neutral (944 ms) trials were found on the 2-back version of the task, all ps > 0.14. No other main or interaction effects were found, all Fs < 2.5, ps > 0.14.
Children’s affect ratings of nine preselected facial expressions (three per emotional type) used in the task were recorded and evaluated. The three most frequently occurring words used by children in each language group were tabulated by facial expression and emotion (Table 4). These top three words were then inspected to determine whether there were notable differences in the valence of the words within each emotion block and across language groups. The word inspection revealed that children used all positively valenced words (e.g., happy, excited, joyful) to describe the three standardized happy faces, and they rated all standardized angry faces using negatively valenced words (e.g., angry, mad, scary, furious), as expected. The three standardized neutral faces rated by the children generated the greatest amount of variety in valence. Children described neutral faces using words with a positive (i.e., happy), negative (i.e., shocked, sad, scared, serious), and neutral (i.e., normal, bored, no emotion/expression) valence. No outstanding differences were detected between language groups in the words children selected to describe any of the actors’ faces in either emotion block.
TABLE 4. Three most commonly used words by children to describe the emotional expressions of three actors with standardized angry, happy, and neutral affects viewed during the Emotional Face N-back Task by language group.
Discussion
The present study investigated the interrelation between cognitive and emotional processing in typically developing monolingual and bilingual children. Children in the two language groups were similar on age, SES, English proficiency, and nonverbal cognitive functioning. Parents provided information on children’s language background, and both parents and teachers reported on children’s emotional and behavioral functioning. All children were tested using the Emotional Face N-Back Task, an ER task assessing working memory within an emotional context.
Ratings of attention obtained from the SWAN showed that all children were rated similarly by teachers, but bilingual children were rated by parents as significantly more attentive than monolingual children. Although it is common to find discrepancies between informants when gathering ratings on children’s behavior (Gresham et al., 2010; Wray et al., 2013), little is known about characteristics that predict discrepancies in ratings. Nonetheless, parental ratings indicating greater attention in bilingual children bring to light an important consideration, namely the frame of reference of the informant. Teachers have experience with children from different communities and cultures and so have a wide frame of reference for rating children’s performance relative to their same-aged peers. Parents, in contrast, may be limited to observing the children living in their own community or even household. Furthermore, parents of monolingual or bilingual children may have different culture-specific expectations that influence their parenting practices and expectations for normative development. Thus, while it was beyond the scope of this study, future bilingualism researchers may consider gathering information on familial expectations and background as these may be related to differences between language groups in parental ratings of children’s behavior.
The Emotional Face N-back Task was used to investigate differences in ER between monolingual and bilingual children by manipulating working memory load (1-back or 2-back) and emotional distraction (angry, happy, and neutral faces). As expected, all children were more accurate and faster on the easier condition than on the more challenging condition of the task. Also in line with our predictions, bilingual children demonstrated better cognitive performance on both working memory conditions. Specifically, depending on the emotion block and condition, accuracy rates of bilingual children ranged from 6% to 9% higher than those of their monolingual peers. This observed working memory advantage for bilingual children supports research highlighting the cognitive benefits of bilingualism on a variety of EF tasks (Adesope et al., 2010).
The findings also revealed significantly slower RTs on neutral emotion trials than on angry and happy trials for children in both language groups, but only on the 1-back condition. Thus, the easy working memory condition showed differences between the emotional stimuli but the difficult working memory condition did not, presumably because the effort required for the working memory response overwhelmed the more subtle difference between emotion conditions. As in previous research with emotional n-back tasks (e.g., Ladouceur et al., 2005; Cromheeke and Mueller, 2016; Villemonteix et al., 2017), accuracy of responses was not impacted by the emotion condition.
The longer RTs on neutral trials may reflect the challenges children experienced in accurately labeling neutral faces. When asked to generate affect ratings for the angry, happy, and neutral emotional expressions for the subset of stimuli used in the task, all children gave accurate positively valenced ratings to happy faces and negatively valenced ratings to angry faces, but neutral faces elicited variable responses, ranging from positively to negatively valenced descriptions. This difficulty in interpreting neutral faces has been found on previous emotion recognition tasks (Kujawa et al., 2014; see Herba et al., 2006; Thomas et al., 2007, for research on age-related changes in emotion recognition). Consequently, the slow responses to neutral emotion trials may reflect children’s difficulty in interpreting ambiguous neutral face stimuli.
The absence of an emotion effect on the 2-back condition may have been influenced by the difficulty that this task posed for children this age in both language groups. Support for this interpretation comes from the neuroscientific literature; studies using neuroimaging during ER tasks have found that increasing requirements for EF can override emotional effects (Hart et al., 2010; Mueller et al., 2017). For example, Erk et al. (2006) found no effects of emotional cueing on working memory accuracy on their ER task; however, the fMRI results revealed a valence-specific regulation effect on brain regions whereby participants had significantly reduced activity in brain areas responsible for emotional processing during high cognitive effort conditions than during low cognitive effort conditions, and significantly greater recruitment of regions implicated in working memory as the complexity of the task increased. This research supports our behavioral findings in that the effects of emotional context tend to be reduced under conditions of high cognitive effort as participants attempt to meet the demands of increasing task complexity (e.g., on the 2-back version of the task).
In this context, it might therefore be expected that bilingual children would respond differently than monolingual children in the 1-back version of the task, but this did not happen. Overall, the study failed to capture the anticipated differences in ER between the language groups. Instead, the main finding was that bilingual children were significantly slower than their monolingual peers on the 2-back condition of the Emotional Face N-back Task.
In healthy adult populations, researchers find that positive emotional stimuli are generally processed more quickly and automatically than emotionally neutral stimuli (see Pool et al., 2016, for a meta-analysis), as was found in the present study. Research with typically developing children is sparse and has generated mixed results; however, it is generally accepted that children’s ability to modulate attention toward or away from emotionally salient information is a marker of ER that distinguishes healthy from at-risk or atypically developing children (e.g., Shackman et al., 2007; Nuske et al., 2017), and that responses are less consistent than those observed in adults. For example, Mueller et al. (2012) tested healthy versus anxious 12-year-old youth using an antisaccade task with emotional faces and found that healthy youth were more accurate during angry trials and happy trials relative to neutral trials, but revealed no emotion effects in RT. However, typically developing children have also been shown to demonstrate longer RTs on positive emotional conditions than on neutral emotional conditions (Ladouceur et al., 2005). Conversely, studies with anxious children, youth, and adults on ER tasks consistently find a threat bias, also described as biased allocation of attentional resources toward threatening stimuli, which is reflected in longer RTs on trials with aversive stimuli (Ladouceur et al., 2005, 2009; for a review, see Williams et al., 1997). Similar findings have been observed in depressed individuals, whose responses are characterized by impaired disengagement from negative stimuli and deficits in cognitive control while processing negative information (for a review, see Gotlib and Joormann, 2010). Taken together, prolonged engagement with emotionally threatening information is believed to be mediated by deficits in cognitive control. However, our finding that bilingual children exhibited longer RTs, particularly on the 2-back, cannot be explained by this theoretical proposal, because: (1) the slowing on (or lack of quick disengagement from) emotional stimuli for bilinguals was generalized to the whole 2-back condition and was nonspecific to either emotion condition, and (2) bilingual children demonstrated overall enhanced working memory relative to monolingual children, with no cognitive deficits noted across any conditions on the task. As such, it is reasonable to assume that there is a continuum between typical capture of attention by emotional cues and dysregulated or maladaptive emotional processing.
A possible explanation for the longer RTs for bilinguals and lack of ER differences between the language groups may lie in differences in monitoring and cognitive flexibility (or shifting) abilities between the groups (e.g., Bialystok and Viswanathan, 2009; Prior and MacWhinney, 2010). Shifting or cognitive flexibility is a component of EF, and the ability to think flexibly that includes switching strategies or responses as task demands change (Miyake et al., 2000). Vitiello et al. (2011) have linked enhanced cognitive flexibility to school success in children. It is notable that children in both language groups accommodated the difficulty of the 2-back condition by slowing down, but only the bilinguals maintained high accuracy in the difficult condition. Hur et al. (2017) observed that particularly on a more challenging task as the 2-back, “participants’ efforts are generally focused more on performing the task accurately than responding as fast as they can” (p. 4). Many studies show an increase in RT and decrease in accuracy with increasing task difficulty on n-back tasks (for a review, see Meule, 2017). Therefore, the slowing for bilingual children cannot be explained by impaired cognitive processing within emotional contexts, but may reflect normal development in healthy bilingual children who are better able than monolingual children to adjust their behavior to task demands.
In summary, this study demonstrated advantages in working memory for bilingual children compared to monolingual children, consistent with previous research showing EF benefits in bilingual individuals, but no evidence for better ER in bilinguals. ER has not been previously investigated with monolingual and bilingual individuals using a working memory task outside of the linguistic context. Although behavioral responses to negative emotional stimuli have commonly been studied within the context of dysregulation and maladjustment, and responses to positive emotions have been studied within the context of healthy socioemotional development, viewing child behavior through this narrow lens may be an oversimplification of functioning and undermine the importance of individual differences that modulate interacting cognitive and emotional processing. Continued research using ER tasks such as the Emotional Face N-back Task has the potential of advancing our understanding of the developmental mechanisms underlying ER in children, and more specifically in elucidating any differences in emotional processing between children with different language experiences.
Ethics Statement
This research was approved by the Human Participants Review Committee of York University. Parents signed informed consent prior to the experiment and children provided verbal assent before each task.
Author Contributions
MJ conducted the study under the supervision of EB in partial fulfillment of requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.
Funding
The study was funded by Grant R01HD052523 from the U.S. National Institutes of Health and Grant A2559 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to EB.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer LF-P and handling Editor declared their shared affiliation at the time of the review.
References
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Zimine, I., Pegna, A. J., Seghier, M. L., Lee-Jahnke, H., et al. (2008). Language control and lexical competition in bilinguals: an event-related FMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1496–1505. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm182
Adesope, O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., and Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and Meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Rev. Educ. Res. 80, 207–245. doi: 10.3102/0034654310368803
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: Author. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Anderson, J. A. E., Mak, L., Chahi, A. K., and Bialystok, E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behav. Res. Methods 1– 14, 250–263. doi: 10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9
Antoniou, M. (2019). The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annu. Rev. Linguist. 5, 1–21. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics011718-011820
Barac, R., Bialystok, E., Castro, D. C., and Sanchez, M. (2014). The cognitive development of young dual language learners: a critical review. Early Childhood Res. Q. 29, 699–714. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.02.003
Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburgh, M. J., and van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Bull. 133, 1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1
Beauvillain, C., and Grainger, J. (1987). Accessing interlexical homographs: some limitations of a language-selective access. J. Mem. Lang. 26, 658–672. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(87)90108-2
Becerril, K., and Barch, D. (2011). Influence of emotional processing on working memory in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. 37, 1027–1038. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq009
Bell, M. A., and Wolfe, C. D. (2004). Emotion and cognition: an intricately bound developmental process. Child Dev. 75, 366–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00679.x
Bialystok, E. (2015). Bilingualism and the development of executive function: the role of attention. Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 117–121. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12116
Bialystok, E. (2017). The bilingual adaptation: how minds accommodate experience. Psychol. Bull. 143, 233–262. doi: 10.1037/bul0000099
Bialystok, E., and Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112, 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014
Bishop, S., Duncan, J., Brett, M., and Lawrence, A. D. (2004). Prefrontal cortical function and anxiety: controlling attention to threat-related stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 7, 184–188. doi: 10.1038/nn1173
Blom, E., Kuntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., and Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: working memory in bilingual turkish-dutch children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 128, 105–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007
Bradley, B. P., Mogg, K., White, J., Groom, C., and de Bono, J. (1999). Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 38, 267–278. doi: 10.1348/014466599162845
Calkins, S. D., and Hill, A. (2007). “Caregiver influences on emerging emotion regulation: Biological and environmental transactions in early development,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation, ed. J. Gross (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 229–248.
Calkins, S. D., and Marcovitch, S. (2010). “Emotion regulation and executive functioning in early development: Integrated mechanisms of control supporting adaptive functioning,” in Child Development: At the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition, eds S. D. Calkins and M. A. Bell (Washington, DC: APA Press), 37–58.
Cole, P. M., and Deater-Deckert, K. (2009). Emotion regulation, risk, and psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 50, 1327–1330. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02180.x
Cole, P. M., Martin, S., and Dennis, T. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Dev. 75, 317–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x
Colomé, À (2001). Lexical activation in bilinguals’ speech production: language-specific or language-independent? J. Mem. Lang. 45, 721–736. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2793
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., and Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 113, 135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
Cromheeke, S., and Mueller, S. C. (2016). The power of a smile: stronger working memory effects for happy faces in adolescents compared to adults. Cogn. Emot. 30, 288–301. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.997196
Djambazova-Popordanoska, S. (2016). Implications of emotion regulation on young children’s emotional wellbeing and educational achievement. Educ. Rev. 68, 497–515. doi: 10.1080/00131911.2016.1144559
Dunn, L. M., and Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., and Eggum, N. D. (2010). Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to children’s maladjustment. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 495–525. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131208
Engel de Abreu, P. M. (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: is there a bilingual effect? Memory 19, 529–537. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.590504
Erk, S., Abler, B., and Walter, H. (2006). Cognitive modulation of emotion anticipation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 1227–1236. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04976.x
Gotlib, I. H., and Joormann, J. (2010). Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 6, 285–312. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305
Gray, J. R., Braver, T. S., and Raichle, M. E. (2002). Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 4115–4120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062381899
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism 1, 67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133
Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., Cook, C. R., Vance, M. J., and Kettler, R. (2010). Cross-informant agreement for social skill and problem behavior ratings: an investigation of the Social Skills Improvement System – Rating Scales. Psychol. Assess. 22, 157–166. doi: 10.1037/a0018124
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology 39, 281–291. doi: 10.1017/S0048577201393198
Gumora, G., and Arsenio, W. F. (2002). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and school performance in middle school children. J. Sch. Psychol. 40, 395–413. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(02)00108-5
Hare, T. A., Tottenham, N., Davidson, M. C., Glover, G. H., and Casey, B. J. (2005). Contributions of amygdala and striatal activity in emotion regulation. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 624–632. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.038
Hart, S. J., Green, S. R., Casp, M., and Belger, A. (2010). Emotional priming effects during Stroop task performance. Neuroimage 49, 2662–2670. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.076
Hawkins, J., Catalano, R., Kosterman, R., Abbott, R., and Hill, K. (1999). Preventing adolescent health-risk behaviors by strengthening protection during childhood. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 153, 226–234. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.153.3.226
Herba, C. M., Landau, S., Russell, T., Ecker, C., and Phillips, M. L. (2006). The development of emotion processing in children: effects of age, emotion, and intensity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1098–1106. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01652.x
Holley, S. R., Ewing, S. T., Stiver, J. T., and Bloch, L. (2017). The relationship between emotion regulation, executive functioning, and aggressive behaviors. J. Interpers. Violence 32, 1692–1707. doi: 10.1177/0886260515592619
Hur, J., Iordan, A. D., Dolcos, F., and Berenbaum, H. (2017). Emotional influences on perception and working memory. Cogn. Emot. 31, 1294–1302. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1213703
Kensinger, E. A., and Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion 3, 378–393. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.4.378
Kieras, J. E., Tobin, R. M., Graziano, W. G., and Rothbart, M. K. (2005). You can’t always get what you want: effortful control and children’s responses to undesirable gifts. Psychol. Sci. 16, 391–396. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01546.x
Kovacs, A. M., and Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 6556–6560. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811323106
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., and Hoshino, N. (2014). Two languages in mind: bilingualism as a tool to investigate language, cognition, and the brain. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 159–163. doi: 10.1177/0963721414528511
Kujawa, A., Dougherty, L., Durbin, C. E., Laptook, R., Torpey, D., and Klein, D. N. (2014). Emotion recognition in preschool children: associations with maternal depression and early parenting. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 159–170. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000928
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., Williamson, D. E., Birmaher, B., Ryan, N. D., and Casey, B. J. (2005). Altered emotional processing in pediatric anxiety, depression, and comorbid anxiety-depression. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 165–177. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1825-z
Ladouceur, C. D., Silk, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Ostapenko, L., Kronhaus, D. M., and Phillips, M. L. (2009). Fearful faces influence attentional control processes in anxious youth and adults. Emotion 9, 855–864. doi: 10.1037/a0017747
Lakes, K. D., Swanson, J. M., and Riggs, M. (2012). The reliability and validity of the English and Spanish Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD and Normal Behavior Rating Scales in a preschool sample: continuum measures of hyperactivity and inattention. J. Atten. Disord. 16, 510–516. doi: 10.1177/1087054711413550
Lewis, M. D., Granic, I., Lamm, C., Zelazo, P. D., Stieben, J., Todd, R. M., et al. (2008). Changes in the neural bases of emotion regulation associated with clinical improvement in children with behavior problems. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 913–939. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000448
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A., Craik, F. I., Grady, C., and Bialystok, E. (2010). Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain Cogn. 74, 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004
Marx, I., Domes, G., Havenstein, C., Berger, C., Sczulze, L., and Herperts, S. C. (2011). Enhanced emotional interference on working memory performance in adults with ADHD. J. Biol. Psychiatry 12, 70–75. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.599213
McDermott, J. M., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Pine, D. S., and Fox, N. A. (2009). A history of childhood behavioral inhibition and enhanced response monitoring in adolescence are linked to clinical anxiety. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 445–448. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.043
Meule, A. (2017). Reporting and interpreting working memory performance in n-back tasks. Front. Psychol. 8:352. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00352
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wagar, T. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
Morales, J., Calvo, A., and Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 187–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
Mueller, S. C., Cromheeke, S., Siugzdaite, R., and Boehler, N. C. (2017). Evidence for the triadic model of adolescent brain development: cognitive load and task-relevance of emotion differentially affect adolescents and adults. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.06.004
Mueller, S. C., Hardin, M. G., Mogg, K., Benson, V., Bradley, B. P., Reinholdt-Dunne, M. L., et al. (2012). The influence of emotional stimuli on attention orienting and inhibitory control in pediatric anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 856–863. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02541.x
Nuske, H. J., Hedley, D., Woollacott, A., Thomson, P., Macari, S., and Dissanayake, C. (2017). Developmental delays in emotion regulation strategies in preschoolers with autism. Autism Res. 10, 1808–1822. doi: 10.1002/aur.1827
Owen, A. M., McMillan, K. M., Laird, A. R., and Bullmore, E. (2005). N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis or normative functional neuroimaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 25, 46–59. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20131
Paap, K. R., and Greenberg, Z. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 66, 232–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
Paelecke-Habermann, Y., Pohl, J., and Leplow, B. (2005). Attention and executive functions in remitted major depressed patients. J. Affect. Disord. 89, 125–135. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.09.006
Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., and Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: a meta-analytic investigation. Psychol. Bull. 142, 79–106. doi: 10.1037/bul0000026
Poulin-Dubois, D., Blaye, A., Coutya, J., and Bialystok, E. (2011). The effects of bilingualism on toddlers’ executive functioning. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 567–579. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.10.009
Prior, A., and MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism 13, 253–262. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990526
Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., and Raven, J. (1996). Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: H. K. Lewis.
Rose, E. J., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2006). Pattern of impaired working memory during major depression. J. Affect. Disord. 90, 149–161. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2005.11.003
Schmeichel, B. J., and Tang, D. C. (2015). Individual differences in executive functioning and their relationship to emotional processes and responses. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 24, 93–98. doi: 10.1177/0963721414555178
Shackman, J. E., Shackman, A. J., and Pollak, S. D. (2007). Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion 7, 838–852. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.838
Shields, A., and Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: the development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Dev. Psychol. 33, 906–916. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906
Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., and Engle, R. W. (2015). Working memory capacity and the scope and control of attention. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 1863–1880. doi: 10.3758/s13414-015-0899-0
Stieben, J., Lewis, M. D., Granic, I., Zelazo, P. D., and Segalowitz, S. (2007). Neurophysiological mechanisms of emotion regulation for subtypes of externalizing children. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 455–480. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070228
Swanson, J., Schuck, S., Mann, M., Carlson, C., Hartman, C., Sergeant, J., et al. (2001). Over-identification of extreme behavior in the evaluation and diagnosis of ADHD/HKD. Available at: http://www.adhd.net/SWAN_Paper.pdf
Thomas, L. A., De Bellis, M. D., Graham, R., and LaBar, K. S. (2007). Development of emotional facial recognition in late childhood and adolescence. Dev. Sci. 10, 547–558. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00614.x
Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Res. 168, 242–249. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
Villemonteix, T., Marx, I., Septier, M., Berger, C., Hacker, T., Bahadori, S., et al. (2017). Attentional control of emotional interference in children with ADHD and typically developing children: an emotional N-back study. Psychiatry Res. 254, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.027
Vitiello, V. E., Greenfield, D. B., Munis, P., and George, J. (2011). Cognitive flexibility, approaches to learning, and academic school readiness in Head Start Preschool Children. Early Educ. Dev. 3, 388–410. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2011.538366
Wechsler, D. (1991). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children, 3rd Edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Williams, J. M., Watts, F. N., MacLeod, C., and Mathews, A. (1997). Cognitive psychology and emotional disorders, 2nd Edn. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons.
Wray, E., Shashi, V., Schoch, K., Curtiss, K., and Hooper, S. R. (2013). Discrepancies in parent and teacher ratings of social-behavioral functioning of children with chromosome 22q11.2 deletion syndrome: implications for assessment. Am. J. Intell. Dev. Disab. 118, 339–352. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-118.5.339
Young, D. J., Levy, F., Neilson, C. M., and Hay, D. A. (2009). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: a Rasch analysis of the SWAN Rating Scale. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 40, 543–559. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0143-z
Zelazo, P. D., and Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: development and plasticity. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 354–360. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x
Keywords: bilingualism, executive control, emotional regulation, working memory, n-back
Citation: Janus M and Bialystok E (2018) Working Memory With Emotional Distraction in Monolingual and Bilingual Children. Front. Psychol. 9:1582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01582
Received: 29 June 2018; Accepted: 08 August 2018;
Published: 28 August 2018.
Edited by:
Roberto Filippi, University College London, United KingdomReviewed by:
Liory Fern-Pollak, University College London, United KingdomJulia Ouzia, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom
Copyright © 2018 Janus and Bialystok. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ellen Bialystok, ellenb@yorku.ca