Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 11 July 2018
Sec. Personality and Social Psychology

Commentary: Forgetting the best when predicting the worst: preliminary observations on neural circuit function in adolescent social anxiety

  • 1Laboratorio de Neurobiología de la Memoria, Departamento de Fisiología y Biología Molecular y Celular, Instituto de Fisiología, Biología Molecular y Neurociencias-CONICET, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 3Laboratorio de Neurofarmacología de los Procesos de Memoria, Cátedra de Farmacología, Facultad de Farmacia y Bioquímica, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Buenos Aires, Argentina

A commentary on
Forgetting the best when predicting the worst: preliminary observations on neural circuit function in adolescent social anxiety

by Jarcho, J. M., Romer, A. L., Shechner, T., Galvan, A., Guyer, A. E, Leibenluft, E., et al. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. (2015). 13:21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.03.002

Let's imagine someone who is about to perform a presentation at work. As he enters the meeting, he feels self-conscious that he has drawn attention to himself. He thinks “this could be a disaster, I must do it perfectly.” His heart-rate speeds up and he begins to sweat. However, he manages to finish the presentation successfully and he receives positive comments from his colleagues. The following day, he recalls: “the presentation went terrible.”

The aforementioned situation is not unusual. Social anxiety (SA) is one of the most prevalent forms of anxiety (Costello et al., 2005). Individuals with SA fear negative evaluation and persistently avoid social situation (Stein and Stein, 2008; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This causes them marked disability such as; experience difficulty communicating, eating and talking in public, and negatively impacts their social functioning (Liebowitz, 1987; Hazen and Stein, 1995). SA typically begins before the end of adolescence, when increased complexity and salience of peer relationships requires novel forms of social learning (Brown and Larson, 2009; Crone and Dahl, 2012). Importantly, when untreated, this disorder tend to be the most persistent of anxiety disorders (Stein and Stein, 2008). Thus, one of the most challenging endeavors is to understand the mechanisms involved in SA maintenance. Several lines of research highlighted the alteration of different cognitive and learning processes (Foa et al., 1996; Clark and Beck, 2011). According to one popular model, proposed by Clark and Wells (1995), SA persists due to a shift of the attention focus to internal cues, the use of internal cues to interpret how one appears to others and the use of safety behaviors/avoidance. Other models highlighted the role of negative self-images and aversive memories, probably rooted in early experiences during development (Hirsch et al., 2003; Moscovitch et al., 2011).

Prediction error (PE) is defined as a mismatch or an incongruence between predicted and occurred events and it has been proposed as the driving force of learning (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972; Fernández et al., 2016). Recently, it has been proposed that an altered PE signaling during adolescence may contribute to some of the mechanisms that help to maintain SA (Pfeifer et al., 2013; Nelson et al., 2014). Furthermore, dysfunctional PE processing could lead to deficient recall of positive social experiences, which in turn could promote negative social expectations and interpretation biases, frequently observed in SA (Clark and McManus, 2002). Thus, it is of interest to address the implication of alterations in PE signaling during “post-mortem” processing in the maintenance of mental disorders.

A recent study by Jarcho and colleagues (Jarcho et al., 2015) analyzed the relationship between PE and social learning, comparing socially anxious adolescents and non-anxious adolescents, as well as adults. Using a social learning task (“Chatroom Task” Guyer et al., 2009), participants are led to believe they would chat online with a peer predicted, then received, social feedback from high and low-value peers. Later, participants recall the social feedback they received from each peer. Neural correlates of social evaluation were assessed by fMRI scanning, comparing engagement to expected and unexpected positive and negative feedback (PE). Results showed that for socially anxious adolescents, but not adults or controls of either age group, there was an impairment in memory for social feedback. That is, when socially anxious adolescents predicted that someone would not chat with them, but the feedback contradicted this prediction (i.e., positive PE), there is an impaired recall of this event. The authors reported that this memory impairment was correlated with a negative fronto-striatal functional connectivity, suggesting a dysregulated PE signaling in socially anxious adolescents. These results point to a deficit in memory updating, as these participants were not able to change their negative expectations regarding social feedback when the outcome was better than expected. We suggest that these findings could also be analyzed and/or interpreted from the memory reconsolidation perspective, providing complementary tools to address the mechanisms of socially anxiety maintenance.

Reconsolidation is the mechanism that allows consolidated memories to be updated (Dudai, 2012; Fernández et al., 2016). Thus, our brain is able to update its stored representations in content, strength, and/or expectations by this process (Lee, 2009). However, certain memory features such as the age and strength constrain memory reconsolidation (Fernández et al., 2016). Interestingly, only when there is a mismatch between what is expected and what actually occurs (PE), a reactivated memory enters in a transient labile state (destabilization) followed by its restabilization in order to persist (reconsolidation) (Dudai, 2012; Exton-McGuinness et al., 2014; Fernández et al., 2016; Beckers and Kindt, 2017). Recently, Fernández et al. (2017) postulated a theoretical framework for how anxiety disorders are maintained through impaired memory updating due to a dysfunctional PE minimization strategy. We suggest that this framework could be useful to discuss the results of Jarcho and coworkers (Jarcho et al., 2015). Specifically, why people suffering from SA cannot modify their negative predictions in the light of disconfirmatory evidence (i.e., receiving positive feedback)? Why the positive feedback is not even recalled? In this context, the repeated violation of expectations (PE) should destabilize and re-stabilize memory (update prior predictions) with new safety information (Salkovskis, 1991). However, none of this occurs and dysfunctional memories are maintained or strengthened. What is inside the core of anxiety that prevents memory updating? In highly anxious individuals, when strong and precise memories encounter a PE, the destabilization phase of reconsolidation begins. During the restabilization phase, the error generated, that would otherwise force memory content updating (schema re-organization), is affected. Moreover, PE minimization is accomplished by assimilation (Fernández et al., 2017; Gilboa and Marlatte, 2017) in accordance to prior belief facilitated by the altered cognitive and attentional processes such as those proposed by Clark and Wells (1995). During the “post-mortem” processing, the prediction generated by a strong memory enters in a self-confirmatory vicious cycle (Clark and Beck, 2011) leading to a “blindness” to incongruent information (Fernández et al., 2017). Hence, strong top-down modulation affects experience and in consequence the original prediction and the strong memory persist. In a sense, this strong memory acts as boundary condition for the reconsolidation process. One could speculate that this deficit could be mediated by the negative fronto-striatal functional connectivity and probably the basolateral-amygdala. Albeit Jarcho et al. (Nelson et al., 2014) did not report amygdala activation, this structure plays a key role in aversive memory-updating, and it is known to be hyperactive in anxiety disorders and particularly in social preference processing in SA (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013; Blair et al., 2016).

Currently there is a strong evidence supporting the role of cognitive process, attentional shifting and safety seeking behaviors, in SA maintenance (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). Here we highlighted the findings of Jarcho et. al and proposed an underlying mechanism (memory reconsolidation) for SA persistence. Altered memory reconsolidation could prevent the incorporation of incongruent information (memory updating), and perpetuate the dysfunctional memory.

Author Contributions

Conceived and wrote the paper: RF, MP, MB, and LK.

Funding

The funders played no role preparation of the manuscript or the decision to publish it.xs

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

PubMed Abstract

Beckers, T., and Kindt, M. (2017). Memory reconsolidation interference as an emerging treatment for emotional disorders: strengths, limitations, challenges, and opportunities. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 13, 99–121. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, K. S., Otero, M., Teng, C., Geraci, M., Lewis, E., Hollon, N., et al. (2016). Learning from other people's fear: amygdala-based social reference learning in social anxiety disorder. Psychol. Med. 46, 2943–2953. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001537

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, B. B., and Larson, J. (2009). “Chapter 2: Peer relationships in adolescence” in Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, Vol. 2, eds R. M. Lerner and L. Steinberg (John Wiley & Sons, Inc.).

Clark, D. A., and Beck, A. T. (2011). Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders: Science and Practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Clark, D. M., and McManus, F. (2002). Information processing in social phobia. Biol. Psychiatry 51, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01296-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, D. M., and Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. Soc. Phobia Diagn. Assess. Treat. 41, 00022–00023.

Google Scholar

Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., and Angold, A. (2005). The developmental epidemiology of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and comorbidity. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. 14, 631–648. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2005.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crone, E. A., and Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 636. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dudai, Y. (2012). The restless engram: consolidations never end. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 227–247. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Exton-McGuinness, M. T., Patton, R. C., Sacco, L. B., and Lee, J. L. (2014). Reconsolidation of a well-learned instrumental memory. Learn. Mem. 21, 468–477. doi: 10.1101/lm.035543.114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, R. S., Boccia, M. M., and Pedreira, M. E. (2016). The fate of memory: reconsolidation and the case of prediction error. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 423–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, R. S., Pedreira, M. E., and Boccia, M. M. (2017). Does reconsolidation occur in natural settings? Memory reconsolidation and anxiety disorders. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 57, 45–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foa, E. B., Franklin, M. E., Perry, K. J., and Herbert, J. D. (1996). Cognitive biases in generalized social phobia. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105:433. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilboa, A., and Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of schemas and schema-mediated memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 618–631. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grupe, D. W., and Nitschke, J. B. (2013). Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 14, 488–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn3524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guyer, A. E., McClure-Tone, E. B., Shiffrin, N. D., Pine, D. S., and Nelson, E. E. (2009). Probing the neural correlates of anticipated peer evaluation in adolescence. Child Dev. 80, 1000–1015. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01313.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazen, A. L., and Stein, M. B. (1995). Social phobia: prevalence and clinical characteristics. Psychiatr. Ann. 25, 544–549. doi: 10.3928/0048-5713-19950901-08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, C. R., Clark, D. M., Mathews, A., and Williams, R. (2003). Self-images play a causal role in social phobia. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 909–921. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00103-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jarcho, J. M., Romer, A. L., Shechner, T., Galvan, A., Guyer, A. E., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2015). Forgetting the best when predicting the worst: preliminary observations on neural circuit function in adolescent social anxiety. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 13, 21–31. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J. L. (2009). Reconsolidation: maintaining memory relevance. Trends Neurosci. 32, 413–420. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). “Social phobia,” in Anxiety, Vol. 22, ed D. F. Klein (Basel: Karger Publishers), 141–173.

Google Scholar

Moscovitch, D. A., Gavric, D. L., Merrifield, C., Bielak, T., and Moscovitch, M. (2011). Retrieval properties of negative vs. positive mental images and autobiographical memories in social anxiety: outcomes with a new measure. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 505–517. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, E. E., Lau, J. Y., and Jarcho, J. M. (2014). Growing pains and pleasures: how emotional learning guides development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfeifer, J. H., Kahn, L. E., Merchant, J. S., Peake, S. J., Veroude, K., Masten, C. L., et al. (2013). Longitudinal change in the neural bases of adolescent social self-evaluations: effects of age and pubertal development. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 33, 7415–7419. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4074-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rescorla, R. A., and Wagner, A. R. (1972). “A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement,” in Classical Conditioning: Current Research And Theory, eds A. H. Black, and W. F. Prokasy (New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts), 64–99.

Google Scholar

Salkovskis, P. M. (1991). The importance of behaviour in the maintenance of anxiety and panic: a cognitive account. Behav. Psychother. 19, 6–19. doi: 10.1017/S0141347300011472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, M. B., and Stein, D. J. (2008). Social anxiety disorder. Lancet 371, 1115–1125. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60488-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anxiety disorders, memory reconsolidation, prediction error, social anxiety, adolescent

Citation: Fernández RS, Pedreira ME, Boccia MM and Kaczer L (2018) Commentary: Forgetting the best when predicting the worst: preliminary observations on neural circuit function in adolescent social anxiety. Front. Psychol. 9:1088. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01088

Received: 23 March 2018; Accepted: 07 June 2018;
Published: 11 July 2018.

Edited by:

Sarah Whittle, University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Emiliano Merlo, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Argentina

Copyright © 2018 Fernández, Pedreira, Boccia and Kaczer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rodrigo S. Fernández, cm9kcmlnb2Zlcm5hbmRlekBmYm1jLmZjZW4udWJhLmFy

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.