Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 March 2018
Sec. Evolutionary Psychology

Association Between Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes: Cross-Cultural Findings

\r\nEva Landov,*Eva Landová1,2*Natavan BakhshaliyevaNatavan Bakhshaliyeva3Markta Janovcov,Markéta Janovcová1,2&#x;rka Pel&#x;kov,Šárka Peléšková1,2Mesma SuleymanovaMesma Suleymanova4Jakub Polk,Jakub Polák2,5Akif GulievAkif Guliev3Daniel Frynta,*Daniel Frynta1,2*
  • 1Department of Zoology, Faculty of Science, Charles University, Prague, Czechia
  • 2Applied Neurosciences and Brain Imaging, National Institute of Mental Health, Klecany, Czechia
  • 3Biology Faculty, Baku State University, Baku, Azerbaijan
  • 4Natural Historical Museum Named After Gasanbey Zardabi, Baku, Azerbaijan
  • 5Department of Psychology, Faculty of Arts, Charles University, Prague, Czechia

According to the fear module theory, humans are evolutionarily predisposed to perceive snakes as prioritized stimuli and exhibit a fast emotional and behavioral response toward them. In Europe, highly dangerous snake species are distributed almost exclusively in the Mediterranean and Caspian areas. While the risk of a snakebite is relatively low in Central Europe, Azerbaijan, on the other hand, has a high occurrence of the deadly venomous Levant viper (Macrovipera lebetina). We hypothesize that co-habitation with this dangerous snake has shaped the way in which humans evaluate snake species resembling it. For that purpose, we asked respondents from the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan to rank photographs depicting 36 snake species according to perceived fear and beauty. The results revealed a high cross-cultural agreement in both evaluations (fear r2 = 0.683, p < 0.0001; beauty: r2 = 0.816, p < 0.0001). Snakes species eliciting higher fear tend to be also perceived as more beautiful, yet people are able to clearly distinguish between these two dimensions. Deadly venomous snakes representing a serious risk are perceived as highly fearful. This is especially true for the vipers and allies (pit vipers) possessing a characteristic body shape with a distinct triangular head and thick body, which was found as the most fear evoking by respondents from both countries. Although the attitude toward snakes is more negative among the respondents from Azerbaijan, their fear evaluation is similar to the Czechs. For instance, despite co-habitation with the Levant viper, it was not rated by the Azerbaijanis as more fearful than other dangerous snakes. In conclusion, agreement in the evaluation of snake fear and beauty is cross-culturally high and relative fear attributed to selected snake species is not directly explainable by the current environmental and cultural differences. This may provide some support for the evolutionary hypothesis of preparedness to fear snakes.

Introduction

Detection and an appropriate reaction to dangerous animals and other life threatening stimuli were necessary for human survival (Barkow, 1992; New et al., 2007). Even in contemporary humans, animal species receive considerably more attention over other stimuli (Altman et al., 2016; Calvillo and Hawkins, 2016; but see Hagen and Laeng, 2016). For example, people are able to rapidly detect various potentially dangerous animals eliciting fear (Tipples et al., 2002; Yorzinski et al., 2014). Öhman and Mineka (2001) proposed the existence of an evolutionary fear module, a complex system consisting of neural, psychophysiological, and behavioral reactions to potentially life-threatening stimuli, particularly those evolutionary relevant. However, all stimuli evoking emotions, in general, attract attention more than neutral stimuli (Vuilleumier, 2005).

Of all the dangerous animals that human ancestors frequently encountered and feared in their environment, snakes have been used as an example of a prototypical stimulus activating the hardwired neural circuitry of fear (Öhman and Mineka, 2003) and their role as a significant selection pressure seems plausible. Throughout the course of evolutionary history venomous snakes presented a real threat (Öhman and Mineka, 2001; Öhman et al., 2001; Isbell, 2006; LoBue and DeLoache, 2010; LoBue and Rakison, 2013) requiring a rapid detection. This drove human ancestors to evolve attentional bias to snakes and similar survival endangering stimuli (Lipp and Waters, 2007). This has been demonstrated in visual search tasks when both adults, as well as 3-year-old children, visually detected snakes more rapidly than other kinds of stimuli (LoBue and DeLoache, 2011). Interestingly, reactions to snake stimuli are even faster than to spiders (Soares et al., 2009), which are both common phobic objects (Davey, 1994). It is the snake body shape (LoBue and DeLoache, 2011) and bright colors (Hayakawa et al., 2011; Landová et al., 2012) together with other typical snake characteristics (LoBue, 2014; Janovcová, 2015) that enhance the fast and accurate visual detection (LoBue et al., 2014). High attentional bias combined with innate (Weiss et al., 2015) or learned fear of snakes (Mineka et al., 1980; Cook et al., 1985; Cook and Mineka, 1989) in many primates including humans allow for quick associations between snake cues in the environment and an appropriate behavioral reaction to the imminent threat (Van Le et al., 2013; LoBue, 2014).

Tierney and Connolly (2013) note in their comprehensive review that, due to methodological reasons, ontogenetic and comparative studies of snake fear are not conclusive and fully comparable. Thus, we are not certain if snake fear has an evolutionary basis only. However, Van Le et al. (2013) reported the existence of neurons in the medial and dorsolateral pulvinar of macaques that responded faster and stronger to snake stimuli than to monkey faces and other objects. These results further support the evidence that primates (Van Le et al., 2013, 2014) as well as humans (Van Strien et al., 2014; Almeida et al., 2015) possess a neurobiological substrate for a rapid detection of snakes as threatening visual stimuli. They also corroborate the evolutionary perspective that snakes have shaped the evolution of the visual system in the primate lineage (Isbell, 2006). The question remains, however, to what extent these evolutionary based attentional biases coincide with the current level of danger represented by snakes in specific geographical regions and how these both factors influence emotions evoked by snakes.

Some colubroid snakes in Africa and Asia have evolved a potent venom delivery system (Vidal and Hedges, 2002); the highly venomous snakes include viperids and elapids (Fry et al., 2006). Interestingly, this innovation coincided with the appearance of rodents and primates (reviewed in Isbell, 2006). For example, it is now agreed that viperids and anthropoid primates evolved together in the Old World and thus share a long history of co-evolution (Keogh, 1998; Beard, 2002; Miller et al., 2005). The major threat pertaining to snakes is currently the risk of envenoming especially in some regions like South and Southeast Asia and Sub-Saharan Africa (Swaroop and Grab, 1954; Chippaux, 1998). Nowadays, venomous snakebites are still an important cause of human injuries and even deaths in some countries. A global estimate for the occurrence of envenomings per year is 421,000–1,841,000 cases resulting in 20,000–90,000 deaths (Kasturiratne et al., 2008). However, the statistical estimates are based on records reported by doctors or data about compensation for the farming community (various accidents including snakebites). Even if all these factors are taken into account, the data might still be underestimated for developing countries or for regions where no exact information about snakebites epidemiology and subsequent injuries is available. Besides that, constrictor non-venomous snakes were for a long time significant predators of primates and human ancestors and even now may represent a serious danger to humans. It has been shown on Agta Negritos, a preliterate society of hunter-gatherers in the Philippines, that 26% of adult males survived predation attempts by large constrictors, specifically the reticulated python (Headland and Greene, 2011).

The danger of a particular snake species for humans varies with the efficiency of venom and its delivery system, the snake's size, aggressiveness, and the probability of encounter with humans. In North, Central, and Western Europe, only small venomous species of vipers can be found, such as the common European adder (Vipera berus, Figure 1A). More dangerous is the nose-horned viper (Vipera ammodytes), but its distribution is mainly in South-Eastern Europe. Envenoming by the common adder results in tachycardia, dizziness, hypotension, shock, and gastrointestinal symptoms, coagulopathy and neutrophil leucocytosis (Malina et al., 2008). However, in the last 20 years, there has be no reported death caused by this snake in the Czech Republic (Valenta, 2008). Contrastingly, highly dangerous snake species of Europe are distributed almost exclusively at its eastern border, in the Caspian region (Chippaux, 2012). The most important snake species causing the most of envenomings and subsequent deaths in Azerbaijan is the Levant viper (Macrovipera lebetina, Figure 1B). Bites of the Levant viper are far more dangerous compared to the common adder and provoke serious symptoms such as oedema, hypotension shock, hemorrhage, tissue necrosis, and melanoderma (Göçmen et al., 2006). In Azerbaijan, 106 cases of bites and one death caused by this species were reported since April to October in 2016 (Bakhshaliyeva, unpublished data); for more information about snakes in this region (based on Bannikov et al., 1977; Coborn, 1991; Schultz, 1996; Amr et al., 1997; Khan, 2002; Spawls et al., 2002; Marais, 2004; El Din, 2006; Egan, 2007; Valakos et al., 2008; Stojanov et al., 2011; Wallach et al., 2014) and their dangerousness (based on Brown, 1973; Weiser et al., 1984; Spawls et al., 1995; Mallow et al., 2003; Abdel-Aal and Abdel-Baset, 2010; Weinstein et al., 2011; Nalbantsoy et al., 2012, 2013; Hossie et al., 2013; Yousefkhani et al., 2014; Samy et al., 2015; Steinhoff, 2017) see Supplementary Tables 1,2.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The most dangerous species of venomous snakes in each country: (A) the common European adder (V. berus) from the Czech Republic, original photo Pavel Kirillov, standardized version; (B) the Levant viper (M. lebetina) from Azerbaijan, original photo Omid Mozaffari, standardized version. We predict that these snakes should elicit the highest fear in the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan, respectively.

Snakes are an ambivalent stimulus perceived as a source of actual or evolutionary fixed danger that attracts specialized attention and triggers fear. Together with a negative picture caused by the media, this may result in negative attitudes toward snakes in both adults (Pandey et al., 2016) and children (Yorek, 2009; Ballouard et al., 2013), including snake killing (Pandey et al., 2016). It has been demonstrated that more negative attitudes toward snakes may be associated with a higher risk of snakebite in the specific region. In South-Eastern Australia, for example, where most of the snakes are highly venomous, one-third of encountered snakes are killed by local people (Whitaker and Shine, 2000). However, especially children attitudes toward snakes vary in different countries and may be modified by targeted educational programs (Ballouard et al., 2012, 2013). On the other hand, snakes are seen as positive divinities in some countries, a source of meat, or a part of traditional medicine (Alves et al., 2012).

In contrast to the cross-cultural variation in negative attitude toward snakes (Ballouard et al., 2013), evidence shows that humans express a universal, cross-cultural agreement in how they evaluate beauty of some snake species (Frynta et al., 2011) which might be closely connected with positive emotions such as joy (see a meta-analysis by Kühn and Gallinat, 2012). Although perceived beauty and fear of snakes may correlate (Janovcová, 2015), these two form distinct dimensions so beauty of a snake cannot fully explain the level of fear it evokes, as we found in king snakes (Landová et al., 2012).

Here we hypothesize, that despite the evolutionary preparedness model, the risk of a snakebite in a given locality is an important predictive factor modulating negative or positive evaluation of snakes. People living in a region with a low abundance of dangerous snakes might have less negative attitudes toward them and experience fear of snakes differently than people living in a country where venomous snakes are more numerous. Therefore, the relationship between the perceived danger, emotions underlying the evaluation of fear, and attitudes toward snake species in different countries warrant future research. To study this effect, we recruited participants from two countries with a considerably different risk of envenoming. Only five species of snakes live in the Czech Republic, four of them are non-venomous and only one presents a potential though moderate danger to humans. Contrary to that, 32 species of snakes live in Azerbaijan and some of those are deadly venomous.

The aims of this study are as follows:

1. As the risk of a bite by venomous snakes is considerably lower in the Czech Republic compared to that in Azerbaijan (the area with a high occurrence of the deadly venomous Levant viper), we expect that the fear response will also be qualitatively different in these two countries. We hypothesize that co-habitation with the highly dangerous viper in Azerbaijan shaped human fear evaluation of snake stimuli resembling it.

2. Furthermore, some animals may trigger more than one emotion at a time, e.g., fear linked to a potential threat and pleasure associated with the perception of animal beauty (Landová et al., 2012). Both fear and perceived beauty could influence the way we view animals, and therefore, we have decided to include the evaluation of snake beauty as a control task to fear rankings. This would allow us to disentangle these two judgements and identify whether snake fear and beauty evaluations form two separate dimensions or are in any way interlinked through a general categorization process as we have previously found in the king snakes (Landová et al., 2012). Contrary to fear, we expect a high cross-cultural agreement in snake beauty evaluation, similarly to already reported results on boid snakes (Marešová et al., 2009a; Frynta et al., 2011).

3. An important question that remains unstudied is what accounts for human evaluation of specific snake species as fear or beautiful evoking. Here we suggest that specific features of snakes' appearance lead to their evaluation as “dangerous” and more fear evoking while other features are linked to the positive evaluation of snake beauty. In the study reported here, we examined morphological (body form), color, and other visual features of snake photo stimuli that account for specific fear and beauty ranking.

4. Evaluation of animals by humans might be influenced by sociodemographic factors such as formal education and gender or previous personal experience. In this study, we could analyze the effect of gender and type of education (biological vs. non-biological) on human fear and beauty evaluation of snakes.

5. Finally, as the risk of a venomous bite influences attitudes toward snakes in some countries (Ballouard et al., 2013; Pandey et al., 2016), we also asked our respondents in the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan how much they liked snakes and if they have experienced snake killing. If the attitude was similarly negative in both countries, then we could assume that humans possess an innate “fear module” influencing their mental representation of snakes as dangerous animals, even though some of them (especially the Czech respondents) have never risked a venomous snakebite in their life.

Materials and Methods

Participants

In total, 104 individuals (50 men and 54 women) from the Czech Republic (CZ) and 100 individuals (38 men and 62 women) from Azerbaijan (AZ) were included in the study. We recruited the Czech respondents among undergraduate students from Charles University and University of South Bohemia and their families. Similarly, the Azerbaijani participants were recruited among the undergraduate students and staff of Baku State University together with their families. This has been driven by the need for a homogenous and comparable sample of respondents in both countries that would be attentive enough to complete the task (see the Discussion for more details).

The mean age of Czech respondents was 30.00 ± 1.67 and 30.46 ± 1.45 years, for men and women, respectively. Similarly, the mean age of men and women respondents from Azerbaijan was 31.16 ± 2.46 and 20.79 ± 1.67, respectively. More than a half of the respondents completed a bachelor degree in biology (55 Czechs and 78 Azerbaijanis) or another university degree, only 12 Azerbaijani and 28 Czech individuals had a secondary high school education. All subjects gave their written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and then filled a questionnaire written in their native language (Czech or Azeri) asking for their gender, age, family membership, attitude toward snakes, and experience with them (see Supplementary Table 3 for an English translation of the form and more details below). The study has been approved by the research ethics committee of Charles University and National Institute of Mental Health.

Attitude Toward Snakes

Based on the most frequent spontaneous responses given in our previous research in the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan, attitude toward snakes was measured using a 7-point Likert scale from 1 (“I like snakes, I would like to breed them at home”) through 4 (neutral) up to 7 (“I hate snakes/I fear them). We also asked the respondents whether they have ever encountered a real snake and if so, to give more details (“How and where have you met snakes?”). We also asked if they have ever killed a snake or seen someone else killing a snake. The responses were coded as either 1 = Yes or 0 = No.

Stimuli

We examined a list of extant snake species inhabiting Europe, the Mediterranean and Caspian area (Uetz et al., 2015). For this study, we selected 36 species covering the main morphological diversity and all major phylogenetic lineages. The included species belong to 6 families and 29 genera (Pyron et al., 2013; Uetz et al., 2015; for more information about the tested snakes, see Supplementary Table 1). To test perceived fear and beauty, we developed a standardized set of 36 photographs depicting these species, each representing a typical adult individual in a resting position. In addition, we included a control photograph of the Egyptian cobra (Naja haje) in a striking posture for a comparison with the resting position picture. The photos were adapted from books and internet sources (for sources and other information, see Supplementary Table 4). We digitally set the pictures of snake bodies on a white background, resized them to a comparable size (regardless of their real size) using GIMP2.8.16 (GNU Image Manipulation Program. Spencer Kimball, Peter Mattis et al., 1995 – 2015), and printed them in a 10 × 15 cm format. It has been previously demonstrated that photos may reliably substitute live snakes (Landová et al., 2012).

Stimuli Ranking According to Fear and Beauty

The respondents were instructed to sort the photographs of live snakes so that the “most beautiful” species would be on the top of the pack, the second “most beautiful” under, etc., until the last selected species at the bottom of the pack. After a short break (~5 min) they were asked to sort the photographs again, this time according to perceived fear. No time limit was set for performing both tasks. Each photograph's rank in the pack according to fear and beauty provided by the respondents was further examined. The ranks were square-root arcsin-transformed to increase the importance of the distribution tails and to improve normality. Mean values of the transformed ranks were used further as a scale for perceived fear and beauty of snakes (the higher the value the lower the perceived fear or beauty). These fear and beauty scales were calculated separately for the Azerbaijani and Czech respondents and further correlated to assess similarities in the fear and beauty evaluations of snakes in the two countries.

Extraction of Morphological and Color Characteristics

We measured morphological characteristics of the depicted species, specifically the body length, head width and length, tail width, neck width, and the eye diameter, using the software Image Tool 3.1 (Wilcox et al., 2002). All the values were measured in millimeters and log-transformed for further analyses. Color properties of the stimuli, namely the proportional ratio of chromatic and achromatic colors, the mean and standard deviation of lightness and saturation and the complexity of lightness pattern, were extracted using the software Barvocuc (for a more detailed description of the software see Lišková and Frynta, 2013; Lišková et al., 2015; Rádlová et al., 2016). The hue values (defined in the HSL color space) for chromatic colors were pre-defined using the following angles: red (<330°; 17°), orange/brown (<17°; 39°), yellow (<39°; 67°), green (<67°; 180°), blue (<180°; 260°), purple (260°; 310°), and pink (<310°; 330°). Values for achromatic colors (with values of 0-1 in the HSL color space) corresponded to the following setting: white (L > 0.80), black (L < 0.28), and gray (S < 0.10). The pattern complexity was computed in Barvocuc using the Sobel operator (Sobel, 1978); minimum 0.2013, e.g., a smooth snake as the European blind snake (Xerotyphlops vermicularis), maximum 0.8452, e.g., a snake with a complex pattern with pronounced scales, such as the common European adder or the African puff viper (Bitis arietans). For the graphical output from the Barvocuc program, see Supplementary Figure 1. To improve normality, we square-root arcsin-transformed the values of color representations and pattern complexity (both expressed as a proportion of the area covered by the animal in the picture) for further analyses. Furthermore, we extracted the area covered by the animal in the picture (the number of non-transparent pixels transformed to square millimeters and square rooted).

Statistical Analyses

Agreement among the respondents was quantified by the Kendall's coefficient of concordance (W) as implemented in SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences), version 16.0 (Chicago, IL, USA). Next, the transformed data (square-root arcsin-transformed) were analyzed by the Principal Component Analysis (PCA) to visualize their multivariate structure. We computed two-way, consistency average score Intra-class Correlations (ICC, Hallgren, 2012) to estimate reliabilities of mean transformed ranks (command icc, irr package, in R; R Development Core Team, 2010). The cross-cultural agreement in fear and beauty and a correlation between both evaluations were analyzed by the least square linear regression (Pearson r-squared). To test which respondent's characteristic (sex, age, nationality, attitudes toward snakes and their interaction) influence the evaluation of beauty and fear, we used MANOVA (Multivariate Analysis of Variance); a final reduced model was built from an initial full model by backwards removal of the factors which appeared non-significant.

A different model was used for testing the effect of family membership and respondents' individual characteristics on the attitude itself. We computed a marginal model (command gls as implemented in package nlme in R software), testing the effect of nationality, sex, age, type of education and nationality*sex interaction. We computed one model with and another one without the random effect of family membership. Subsequently, these two versions of the model were compared using ANOVA. We also performed Canonical Discrimination Function Analysis (DFA, forward stepwise model) for the fear and beauty evaluations to identify those species whose evaluations matched/differed significantly. Next, we performed GLMs (Generalized Linear Models) to analyze the effect of traits of the assessed snakes, i.e., morphological and color characteristics (see above) on their beauty and fear evaluation. To calculate correlations between the fear eliciting evaluation and a specific level of threat to humans we used the Spearman's rank correlation coefficient (Spearman R). All those analyses, if not stated otherwise, were performed in Statistica 8.0 (StatSoft Inc., 2010).

Results

Congruence Among Respondents

First, we evaluated agreement among the respondents for each country and evaluation separately. Kendall's coefficient of concordance (W) showed sufficient agreement among the respondents from the Czech Republic for fear (N = 104, W = 0.341, p < 0.0001) and beauty evaluation (N = 104, W = 0.201, p < 0.0001). The concordance coefficients computed for Azerbaijan respondents were also significant (fear: N = 100, W = 0.180, p < 0.0001; beauty: N = 100, W = 0.263, p < 0.0001). When the picture of the Egyptian cobra (N. haje) in the threatening posture was excluded from the datasets, the agreement in fear evaluation remained significant in both Czech (N = 104, W = 0.315, p < 0.0001) and Azerbaijani respondents (N = 100, W = 0.151, p < 0.0001).

In order to estimate reliabilities of mean transformed ranks, we computed average score intra-class correlations (ICC). The resulting population values were high in both Czech [Nstimuli = 37, Nraters = 104, fear: ICC = 0.983, 95% CI = 0.974–0.990, F(36, 3708) = 59.4, p < 0.0001; beauty: ICC = 0.967, 95% CI = 0.950–0.980, F(36, 3708) = 30.3, p < 0.0001] and Azerbaijan datasets [Nstimuli = 37, Nraters = 100, fear: ICC = 0.958, 95% CI = 0.936–0.975, F(36, 3564) = 23.8, p < 0.0001; beauty: ICC = 0.975, 95% CI = 0.962–0.985, F(36, 3564) = 40.2, p < 0.0001].

Respondents' Characteristics Affecting Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes

To test for the effect of respondents' individual characteristics (biology education, age, sex, nationality, and interactions thereof) on the fear and beauty evaluations, we employed MANOVA. For fear, there was no significant effect of biology [F(164, 35) = 1.36, p = 0.1025, power = 0.97], age [F(164, 35) = 0.63, p = 0.9486, power = 0.61], nor sex [F(164, 35) = 0.99, p = 0.4907, power = 0.87], however there was a significant effect of nationality [F(164, 35) = 4.65, p < 0.0001, power = 1.00] and nationality*sex interaction [F(164, 35) = 2.03, p = 0.0017, power = 1.00]. For beauty, we found a significant effect of nationality [F(164, 35) = 4.13, p < 0.0001, power = 1.00], and age [F(164, 35) = 1.68, p = 0.0162, power = 0.99], while biology [F(164, 35) = 1.48, p = 0.0551, power = 0.98], sex [F(164, 35) = 1.13, p = 0.2989, power = 0.92], and nationality*sex interaction [F(164, 35) = 1.49, p = 0.0528, power = 0.98] were not significant. We further tested the effect of family membership as a random factor on fear and beauty scores in the two most prominent species in both countries, i.e., the common European adder (Vipera) and the Levant viper (Macrovipera). Two versions of a marginal model (command gls as implemented in package nlme in R software) with and without the family factor were compared by ANOVA. It appeared that neither fear nor beauty evaluation was effected by the family membership as having included it into the model did not result in a significant improvement (Macrovipera-fear: likelihood ratio = 1.67, p = 0.1961; Vipera-fear: likelihood ratio = 1.21, p = 0.2720; Macrovipera-beauty: likelihood ratio = 0.11, p = 0.7401; Vipera-beauty: likelihood ratio = 0.65, p = 0.4213).

Attitude Toward Snakes and Experience With Killing a Snake in Czech and Azerbaijani Respondents

We tested the effect of individual characteristics (nationality, sex, type of education, and nationality*sex interaction) and family membership on the attitude toward snakes. Using the same type of analysis as mentioned above, i.e., creating two marginal models with and without the family membership as a random factor and comparing them by ANOVA, it was revealed that the model with the family membership was significantly better (likelihood ratio = 8.72, p = 0.0033; AIC of the model with and without the family membership: 816.04 vs. 823.05, respectively). The resulting model further showed a significant effect of nationality [F(1, 199) = 27.44, p < 0.0001] and sex [F(1, 199) = 17.34, p < 0.0001], but not the nationality*sex interaction [F(1, 199) = 0.75, p = 0.3879] nor biology education [F(1, 199) = 3.29, p = 0.0714].

Different attitude toward snakes in both countries could be further demonstrated by responses on the provided measure. In the Czech Republic, 54% of respondents had a positive attitude toward snakes, 20% neutral, and 26% a negative one. In Azerbaijan, only 14% of respondents had a positive attitude toward snakes, 49% neutral, and 37% a negative one. Interestingly, the incidence of snake encounters was comparable in both countries (99% of Czechs and 89% of Azerbaijanis have met a snake, p = 0.0023), even though the number of snake species living in the respective country is strikingly different (see the Introduction).

We also tested the effect of individual characteristics on experience with killing a snake or having seen someone killing one. We computed a GLM model (for binomial data with logit link function) with nationality, sex, nationality*sex interaction, and biological education as fixed factors. The frequency of killing a snake was influenced only by nationality (Z = −3.60, p = 0.0003); more respondents in Azerbaijan than in the Czech Republic have killed a snake or have seen someone killing it (49% vs. 14%). Moreover, people in the Czech Republic reported having this experience mostly as a part of veterinary care or road accident.

When using MANOVA to analyze the additive effect of reported attitude toward snakes and experience with snake killing in both countries on fear evaluation, both the nationality (F = 3.16, p = 0.0001, power = 0.99) and snake killing influenced it significantly (F = 1.62, p = 0.0252, power = 0.99), but no significance was found for the attitude toward snakes (F = 1.00, p = 0.4834, power = 1.00). A similar statistical model for beauty evaluation was influenced only by nationality (F = 2.54, p = 0.0001, power = 0.99), but neither the attitude (F = 1.15, p = 0.0895, power = 1.00) nor snake killing (F = 1.48, p = 0.0562, power = 0.98) were significant.

Are Fear and Beauty Two Separate Dimensions?

In the Czech respondents, perceived fear positively correlated with beauty (r2 = 0.379, p = 0.0001), however this correlation was insignificant in Azerbaijanis (r2 = 0.063, p = 0.1415). Such difference could be explained by the MANOVA results (see section Respondents' Characteristics Affecting Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes above) showing that there was a significant difference in the rankings between men and women, but only in the Azerbaijani sample. Therefore, we analyzed the relationship of perceived fear and beauty for each sex and country separately. In Czech men, the correlation was highly significant and stronger (r2 = 0.424, p < 0.0001) than in Czech women (r2 = 0.299, p = 0.0006). In Azerbaijani men, the correlation was rather weak, yet significant (r2 = 0.170, p = 0.0125). However, in Azerbaijani women, there was no significant correlation (r2 = 0.012, p = 0.5290). These results suggest that the correlation between fear and beauty in Azerbaijani respondents is influenced by the different precision in fear rankings (the highest fear/beauty discrepancy was found in the Azerbaijani women, followed by the Azerbaijani men, Czech women, and Czech men) and the difference in experience with snake killing (see above). Nonetheless, when the data from Azerbaijan were analyzed separately, the sexes correlated significantly in both fear (r2 = 0.746, p < 0.0001) and beauty evaluation (r2 = 0.866, p < 0.0001).

Correlation Between Czechs and Azerbaijanis in Fear and Beauty Evaluation

In section Respondents' Characteristics Affecting Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes, we detected a significant effect of nationality on mean ranks of fear and beauty. Despite that, a comparison of photos ranking in both countries according to a particular evaluation showed a high cross-cultural agreement (Pearson correlation coefficient; fear: r2 = 0.648, p < 0.0001, Figure 2; beauty: r2 = 0.832, p < 0.0001, Supplementary Figure 2). The remaining variability in the data may be explained by the statistical models we have used to analyze the effect of demographic variables and attitude toward snakes. For the complete results of fear and beauty evaluations for particular species, see Supplementary Table 5.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A correlation of fear evaluations from the Czech Republic and Azerbaijan. The graph was calculated from square-root arcsin-transformed data, i.e., the lower the value the more fearful the species is according to the respondents (Pearson correlation coefficient; r2 = 0.683, p < 0.0001). The highlighted evaluation of the Egyptian cobra in two positions illustrates different perception of snakes depending on their posture.

We also performed a canonical discrimination function analysis (DFA) to assess differences/overlaps between the groups of respondents (nationality and gender) in the fear evaluation [Wilks' lambda = 0.36; F(51, 548) = 4.43; p < 0.0001] of selected 17 species, see Figure 3 for details. Even though the analysis was significant, the classification success was rather low (62.25%), which signifies an overlap between the groups. This analysis showed significant differences between the sexes and countries when comparing the Azerbaijani women to the remaining groups (Azerbaijani men: F = 8.95, p = 0.0031; Czech men: F = 33.14, p < 0.0001; Czech women: F = 39.37, p < 0.0001) and the Azerbaijani men to the Czech respondents (Czech men: F = 4.87, p = 0.0284; Czech women: F = 6.70, p = 0.0103). However, there was no significant difference when comparing the Czech men and women (F = 0.14, p = 0.71). A plot of the first two canonical factors showed a considerable overlap of the groups (see Figure 3, loadings are provided in Supplementary Table 6).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The canonical DFA of fear evaluation. This analysis showed significant differences between the sexes and countries when comparing the Azerbaijani women to all other groups and the Azerbaijani men to the Czech respondents. However, a plot of the first two canonical factors showed a considerable overlap of the groups for elicited fear. The number of included species was reduced by the forward stepwise procedure to just 17 out of 36. Three species, the meadow viper (Vipera ursinii; Wilks' lambda = 0.3850, p = 0.0042), the coastal viper (Montivipera xanthina; Wilks' lambda = 0.3975, p = 0.0002), and the Palestine saw-scaled viper (Echis coloratus; Wilks' lambda = 0.3898, p = 0.0014) had the largest Wilks' lambda and therefore, corresponded the best with the discrimination criteria. Thus, only three of the 36 tested snake species are responsible for the detected differences in fear evaluation.

Similarly, an analogical analysis was performed to assess differences/overlaps between the groups of respondents (ethnic and gender) in their evaluation of snake beauty [Wilks' lambda: 0.3390, F(108,495) = 2.00, p < 0.0001, 24 species were included, see Supplementary Figure‘3]. The classification success was again rather low (62.75%) signifying an overlap between the groups. As with the analysis of fear evaluation (see above), there was no difference between the Czech men and women (F = 1.17, p = 0.2749). Contrary to that, the Azerbaijani women were different from all the other groups again (Azerbaijani men: F = 2.58, p = 0.0002; Czech men: F = 3.69, p < 0.0001; Czech women: F = 3.51, p < 0.0001) and the Azerbaijani men differed from the Czech participants (Czech men: F = 3.70, p < 0.0001; Czech women: F = 2.58, p = 0.002). Still, a plot of the first two canonical factors showed a considerable overlap of the groups (see Supplementary Figure 3, loadings are provided in Supplementary Table 7).

Explanation of Perceived Fear and Beauty by Physical Traits of Snake Stimuli

Because of the high correlation of snake rankings according to fear and beauty between the Czech and Azerbaijan respondents (see above), we computed a model explaining the influence of snake morphology and color from a combined data set (CZ and AZ respondents pooled together), but separately for beauty and fear rankings. We computed optimal linear models (LM), which we used to analyze the effect of morphological and other perception characteristics (color, pattern, lightness, saturation) of stimuli on the fear and beauty evaluations.

The optimal model (LM) for the perceived fear explained 83.6% of variability. There was a positive effect of the head width, eye width, tail width, and white color on high fear evaluation. We found a negative effect of the overall body length, photograph lightness and pink color that reduce fear evaluation of snakes (see Table 1). The optimal model (LM) for the perceived beauty explained 80.7% of variability. We found a positive effect of the pattern complexity, amount of black color, and overall lightness of the photograph that all increase the snake beauty evaluation. On the contrary, white and red colors of snakes had a negative effect and decreased the beauty evaluation. Interestingly, none of the morphological characteristics had a significant effect on beauty (see Table 2). Dark, short, thick snakes with a wide head and big eyes elicit the highest fear, while snakes with complex disrupted patterns and bright colors are evaluated as the most beautiful. Thus, different sets of morphological and color characteristics influence the fear and beauty evaluation, but only lightness of the snake picture stimuli influences the fear (dark snakes are the most fear eliciting) as well as beauty (brightly colored snakes are the most beautiful) evaluation.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Final reduced model (LM) describing the effects of snake morphology and color on fear ranking.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Final reduced model (LM) describing the effects of snake morphology and color on beauty ranking.

Explanation of Perceived Fear by the Snake's Real Dangerousness

The presented snake species also differ in their dangerousness for humans. We compared the specific level of threat to humans (the dangerousness category, see the Materials and methods and Supplementary Table 2) with the species' fear evaluation. In both the Czech and Azerbaijani respondents, the perceived fear significantly correlated with the real threat (Spearman correlation coefficient; Czechs: r = 0.716, p < 0.0001; Azerbaijanis: r = 0.725, p < 0.0001), but the perceived beauty did not (Spearman correlation, Czechs: r = 0.227, p = 0.1767; Azerbaijanis: r = 0.160, p = 0.3430).

Discussion

Comparison of Fear and Beauty Evaluations of Snakes Between Czech and Azerbaijanis

In this paper, our main aim was to make cross-cultural comparisons of human fear responses to various venomous and non-venomous snake species common in Europe, Middle East, and North Africa. We hypothesized differences in fear perception of snakes in the Czech Republic compared to Azerbaijan, the two countries with considerably different risks of envenoming. While snakes in the Czech Republic do not present a significant threat, the risk of serious injuries or even deaths caused by snakebites in Azerbaijan is much higher. Contrary to our expectations, in the sample consisted largely of college students, we found a significant cross-cultural agreement in fear evaluation (r2 = 0.65, p < 0.0001). This is in agreement with our previous study demonstrating that despite differences in snake abundance and risk of envenoming people from different countries self-report comparable snake fear (Polák et al., 2016).

To some extent, the Czechs and Azerbaijanis were able to categorize the snake species according to the real threat to humans. Most of the envenomings in Azerbaijan are caused by one particular species, the Levant viper, an aggressive snake that is responsible for many deaths. We predicted that it should be seen by Azerbaijanis as more fearful than other snakes. Despite that, the Levant viper did not figure at the top of fear ranking (14th most fearful out of 36 species). By contrast, only one slightly venomous snake that lives in the Czech Republic, the common European adder, was seen as quite dangerous by the Czech respondents (4th most fearful out of 36 species). Other deadly venomous species that are similar to the Levant viper (see Supplementary Figure 4), and thus present a serious threat, are evaluated by both the Azerbaijani and Czech respondents as highly fear eliciting. Compared to vipers, slender bodied elapids, including the Egyptian cobra in a resting position, were perceived as less fear eliciting. This could mean the fear module responds to a broader and more general category of dangerous, fear eliciting snakes rather than particular species. This is especially important from the ethnobiological and conservational perspective as due to the presence of one dangerous snake species people may incline to killing all other snakes that are similar though less dangerous. From the psychological point of view, it is interesting to find out how broad this “snake” category is (see below and in section Attitude Toward Snakes).

Moreover, when we analyzed the differences in fear evaluation between men and women from both countries in more details, the species causing slight discrepancies are not the most dangerous snakes of Azerbaijan, but other viperids (controls) that live neither in Azerbaijan nor the Czech Republic. Thus, co-habitation with the highly dangerous Levant viper in Azerbaijan did not lead to increased fear elicited by this species. The respondents rather generalized the fear evaluation of the local dangerous model snake to all vipers and pit vipers resembling it. Another possibility is that the characteristic viper-like body shape, i.e., a triangular head, narrow neck, and a thick body, is a prototypical stimulus that humans recognize ancestrally. This is because in Eastern Africa, the place of human origins, our ancestors have probably lived for last two million years together with highly venomous viperid species (Isbell, 2006). Nowadays, this region is inhabited by many deadly venomous species that still cause human deaths like the North-East carpet viper (Echis pyramidum), African puff adder (B. arietans), rhinoceros viper (Bitis nasicornis), rhombic night adder (Causus rhombeatus), or the African hairy bush viper (Atheris hispida) (Spawls et al., 1995). To tackle this question, research must be conducted on other ethnics where meeting a snake is still risky, but viperids do not pose the major threat.

The colubroid snakes evolved a highly efficient venom delivery system probably in the Late Cretaceous (around 60 Myr; Vidal and Hedges, 2002), with viperids evolving in Asia in the Early Miocene (around 20 Myr) or even later (Wüster et al., 2008). Molecular data show the diversification of some genera within viperids, e.g., Vipera and Macrovipera, around 10 Myr (Pook et al., 2009). Primates, including human ancestors, have also evolved predominantly in the Old World (switching between Africa and SE Asia), i.e., they continuously occurred in sympatry with the viperids and other venomous snakes. Subsequently, the mankind evolved in Africa from its common ancestor with chimpanzees around 7–13 million years ago (Langergraber et al., 2012). Thus, the last 10 million years our direct ancestors have spent in Africa, initially in tropical forests and later in open savannas (Dawkins, 2005). The repeated expansion of modern humans out of the African continent to Eurasia, where they could encounter the current genera of viperids, is estimated to about 90–100 Kyr to Levant, 60 Kyr to western Asia, and 40 Kyr to Europe (Deshpande et al., 2009; Stewart and Stringer, 2012). Thus, contemporary human populations have been exposed to various local snake faunas representing a different risk of envenoming. It can be expected that the number of deaths/injuries caused annually by snakebites in some countries is not a sufficient selective pressure to create an evolutionary adaptation to recognize and fear specific snake species in a given region (here the Levant viper in Azerbaijan) within such an evolutionary short time scale. A hypothetical innate adaptation of humans to the local snake species would need to evolve during a couple thousands of years following the African migration, which is unlikely. On the other hand, the co-evolution with viperids have been long enough to form a general mental representation of dangerous viperid snakes associated with a high fear response.

We have also tested human perception of snake beauty that, as we expected based on our previous findings and contrary to fear, should demonstrate intercultural similarity. We have already shown that in Papua New Guinea which is inhabited by highly venomous snakes from the Elapidae family (O'Shea, 1996), villagers evaluate the beauty of boid snakes similarly to Czech students (Marešová et al., 2009a); the cross-cultural agreement on beauty ranking was relatively high (r2 = 0.76). This is despite an entirely different cross-cultural background of the Papuans who are exposed to a higher envenoming risk than people in Central Europe. Moreover, the viperids are missing in Papua New Guinea, so the morphotype of local dangerous snakes is different. A broader cross-cultural agreement (r2 ranging from 0.56 up to 0.92) on beauty of these snakes was also found between the Papuans and villagers from Bolivia, Philippines, India (Rajasthan and Dehli), Malawi, and Morocco (Frynta et al., 2011). In this paper, we provide support for such findings showing a comparable or even higher cross-cultural agreement in the snake beauty evaluation (r2 = 0.83, p < 0.0001).

Relationship Between Perceived Fear and Beauty of Snakes

We suppose that asthetic evaluation of snake beauty is closely connected with joy (Kühn and Gallinat, 2012; Frynta et al., 2014) and that during ranking according to fear this emotion is triggered as well. Previously, we have found only a loose correlation between perceived beauty and fear in king snakes when respondents evaluated photographs but not the same live specimens (Landová et al., 2012). King snakes include brightly colored species mimicking deadly venomous coral snakes and the perception of beauty and fear were rather two independent processes in these snakes. Interestingly, in this study, where respondents evaluate a more diverse sample containing snakes from 29 genera and 6 families, we also found that snake species eliciting higher fear tend to be at the same time perceived as more beautiful by all nationality/sex sub groups except Azerbaijani women. However, this correlation between fear and beauty evaluations is lower and varies cross-culturally (Czech men: r2 = 0.42; Czech women: r2 = 0.30; and Azerbaijan men: r2 = 0.17), so people are able to subjectively distinct these two dimensions during ranking of the snake photographs. If we found, on the other hand, a high agreement between the fear and beauty rankings, it would mean that there is a need for simple explicit categorization rules that help respondents deal with both tasks promptly and are not dependent on a particular evaluation.

Features Predicting Evaluation of Snake Species as Fearful or Beautiful

The most fear evoking trait on snakes was a specific body form, a short snake with a wide but distinct neck and big eyes. On the contrary, worm-like snakes from the Typhlopidae family did not elicit fear in neither of the countries (see Figure 2). It has been shown that the typical curvilinear shape enhances quick detection of the snake (LoBue, 2014) even if it is introduced in scenes in gray scale (Hayakawa et al., 2011). Azerbaijani, as well as Czech respondents, perceive as the most fear evoking stimulus the Egyptian cobra in a threatening posture (with the extended neck and in an upright position, see Supplementary Figure 4D). However, the same species in a resting position (control) was according to fear only 22nd out of 36 species in Azerbaijani and 28th out of 36 species in Czech respondents. Thus, the snake's position seems crucial for its fear evaluation. Only this particular species was included in two different positions as the threatening cobras are very well known from the media, while threatening positions of other snakes are not so apparent. However, the snake's position before an attack (S-shaped neck) communicates a willingness to behave defensively (Johnson, 1975) and is worth of interest if people that are unfamiliar with snake behavior are able to recognize it as a signal of danger and perceive consequently more fear.

The high congruence in beauty perception may be partly explained not only by asthetic preferences for colors (Crozier, 1999), symmetry (Enquist and Arak, 1994), and contrast patterns on animals (Marešová et al., 2009b; Lišková et al., 2015), but also by more delicate features like animal body shapes (Lišková and Frynta, 2013; Janovcová, 2015). We have previously found in king snakes that perception of beauty depends on the body weight and presence of red and black colors (Landová et al., 2012). King snakes include a great variety of color forms, some possessing aposematic patterns (red-black-yellow/white stripes) that are highly preferred by respondents. However, in other studies, this color pattern contributes to perceiving the animal as dangerous (Prokop and Fančovičová, 2013). The set of snakes used in this paper does not comprise brightly colored species, thus the pattern complexity, high lightness, and presence of black color (inherently presented in contrast patterns) increase the relative beauty of presented species. Thus, the contrasting pattern on snake body is considered as beautiful similarly to our results in boid snakes (Marešová and Frynta, 2008) or interestingly in pitta birds (Lišková et al., 2015).

Attitude Toward Snakes

According to the contemporary theory in social psychology, learning can account for most of the attitudes we hold. We know that attitudes can be implicitly acquired via classical, operant, or evaluative conditioning. Formation of implicit attitudes employs the processes of both classical and operant conditioning, but social learning is probably of the same importance (Wegener Carlston and Carlston, 2005). Moreover, people can also form attitudes explicitly on the spot with just a little cognitive effort using several heuristics (Schwarz, 2000). It is generally accepted that attitudes form future behavior and results from a meta-analysis by Glasman and Albarracín (2006) indicate that this effect is stronger when attitudes are easy to recall and stable over time.

In our study, the respondents also filled out a questionnaire about their attitude toward snakes. Although most of our subjects were students and employees of biological sciences colleges, this relationship was different in both countries. In the Czech Republic, the proportion of people who like snakes (54%) is similar to those who hate them (46%). However, the majority of the Azerbaijani respondents have either a neutral relationship toward snakes (49%) or hate them (37%). This is an interesting finding regarding the association between biology education and more positive attitudes toward animals (see also section Study Limitations). Despite more biology students being represented in the Azerbaijani sample, their attitudes were more negative than those of Czechs. Thus, their education did not overcome the aversion to snakes. The number of unpleasant encounters should be theoretically an explanation for the different relationship to snakes, as only five species of snakes live in the Czech Republic, whilst about 32 snake species occur in Azerbaijan. Nevertheless, our results show that encounters with snakes are comparably common in both countries (87 and 99%). Different attitudes toward snakes are related to another question; if the respondent has ever killed a snake or has seen someone killing it.

It is known that dangerous animals, especially snakes, are killed for safety reasons in different geographical regions, e.g., in Brazil (Alves et al., 2009, 2012), India (Balakrishnan, 2010), or Nepal (Pandey et al., 2016). In Azerbaijan, a half of the respondents (49%) have seen killing a snake or killed one themselves (Supplementary Figure 5 shows a dead Levant viper killed by villagers in Katex, northern Azerbaijan, found during a zoological field expedition). By contrast, only a few Czechs have ever killed a snake (14%), either as a part of research or involuntarily (driving over a snake with a car). This proportion of reported cases of snake killing by adults is consistent with previously published data on children who intended to kill a snake if they would encounter one (Ballouard et al., 2013). This was surprisingly high among children from Morocco (45%), Portugal (60%), or even Slovakia (90%). However, in other countries this willingness to kill a snake was lower, ranging from 29% in Turkey to 7–13% in six European countries. Moreover, this was influenced by the sex of the respondents and their fear or likability of snakes (Ballouard et al., 2013). As Makashvili et al. (2014) reported, negative attitude to snakes can be overcome with knowledge.

Does Attitude Toward Snakes Explains Beauty or Fear Evaluation?

The attitude toward snakes explains perceived fear of snakes only marginally in the Czech respondents. Only 6% of women and 3% of men have purely negative attitudes to snakes, e.g., dislike and fear them. However, any cross-cultural differences in fear evaluation of snakes are mainly due to the different fear evaluation of snakes in Azerbaijan women. Higher fear response and more negative attitudes toward snakes in women have been reported several times (Arrindell et al., 2003; Prokop et al., 2009). Although we confirmed that Azerbaijani women in our sample had more negative attitudes toward snakes (26% highly dislike snakes) this does not explain their differences in relative fear evaluation. Moreover, ranking according to beauty was fully independent regardless of personal attitudes toward snakes.

Study Limitations

More than a half of our respondents from both countries were university biology students. One might argue that concerning their type of education and young age they did not constitute a proper representative sample. For example, Schlegel and Rupf (2010) found that natural resource sciences students showed higher levels of affinity to animals. Bjerke and Østdahl (2004) reported a positive correlation between the respondent's educational level in general and positive attitudes toward animals. In this study, this might be true for the attitude toward snakes, however, our results, and previous studies showed that the effect of individual characteristics on the ranking of animal species according to their beauty or perceived fear was usually only marginal (Frynta et al., 2010), even when comparing such different populations as Czech students and villagers in Papua New Guinea (Marešová et al., 2009a). Moreover, Collins (1976) pointed out that obvious differences between the first-year students (represented the most in our study) and biology majors could be found. The students we have recruited were mainly from the first or second year of university. At this stage of curriculum they have still not done any specialized course in herpetology. We also hypothesize that in the case of salient, emotionally charged, and evolutionary relevant stimuli, such as snakes, the stimulus' characteristics may be more important for its evaluation than sociodemographic traits of the respondent. This is consistent with results of Prokop and Tunnicliffe (2008), who found a moderate correlation between attitude toward and knowledge of bats, but not spiders. They suggested that this may be partly influenced by greater fear of spiders compared to fear of bats. To conclude, although we are aware of the limitations and want to avoid over-generalization of our results, we believe that our sample was adequate for the purpose of this study.

Throughout the study, we have found discrepancies between the Azerbaijani women and the rest of the subject groups (Azerbaijani men and Czech men and women) on almost all measures. Based on an evolutionary hypothesis, women have a higher reproductive value compared to men, because they give birth to the progeny. In the evolutionary history of mankind, women were staying around their homes gathering food while men went out hunting. Doing so, women had more chances of encountering snakes lurking in the environment. Having to protect not only themselves, but also their children or even unborn babies, there was a good evolutionary reason for women to develop higher fear of snakes compared to men (Öhman et al., 2012). Even today, women suffer considerably more often from specific animal phobias than men (Fredrikson et al., 1996). In light of the above, the results obtained from the Azerbaijani women make sense. Nevertheless, one has to explain then why no similar gender difference has been found in the Czech Republic. Perhaps, it may be argued that the traditional gender roles associated with differences in the expression of fear, among others, are more pronounced in Azerbaijan, where 98% of the population are Muslims. Contrary to that, the mostly atheist Czech population has gone through sociodemographic changes in the last few decades resulting in equalization of gender roles and stereotypes.

Summary and Conclusion

The major portion of variability in the data explains high similarity in fear 65% (as well as beauty 83%) perception of snakes in the Czech Republic compared to Azerbaijan, the two countries with considerably different risks of envenoming. The rest of the variability reflects the differences that should be accounted to some respondents' individual characteristics (especially the Azerbaijani women being different in fear evaluation of the snake species). Interestingly, biology education of the respondents influenced only the attitude to toward snakes, but not the fear evaluation itself. These results are further demonstrated by the canonical DFA showing the overlap between nationality/sex groups. The differences are caused by different evaluation of only three snake species specific for fear and beauty ranking.

Snake beauty evaluation was a valuable control task, to be sure that respondents rank the pictures according to elicited fear, not only due to applying more general esthetic and categorization rules. We found only a slight correlation between fear and beauty, indicating that only some species were evaluated as both fearful and beautiful at the same time. Further, we detected a different set of attributes behind the evaluation of fear (mainly the snake's morphology) and beauty (mainly colors and pattern) of snakes with only the lightness affecting both. In both countries, the respondents evaluated the Egyptian cobra in a threatening position or the “viper” morphotype as the most fear eliciting and dangerous. However, they did not evaluate the cobra in a resting position nor the particular species that locally represent the major risk of snakebite (the common European adder in Czech Republic and the Levant viper in Azerbaijan) as the most fear eliciting. The generalization concept of viperids as the most fearful snakes in our study may be due to their co-evolutionary history with humans and is responsible for a high cross-cultural agreement. We also found the role of experience (killing a snake or seeing someone killing a snake) on fear evaluation, but not on beauty evaluation of snakes. A general attitude (likability) of snakes was influenced by the family membership, but the influence of attitude or family factor on relative fear or beauty evaluations was not significant. Thus, it seems that the family context, environment where one grows up, may change the way we view snakes, whether we like them or not, but it will not affect the perceived fear or beauty of particular snake species which might be a universally shared pattern.

Despite the differences in the attitude toward snakes between the Azerbaijani and Czech respondents, agreement in the evaluation of snake beauty and fear is cross-culturally high. The “viper” type is generalized as the most fear evoking appearance of a snake in both countries. Thus, the fear response to snakes is not directly explainable by the observed current environmental and cultural differences.

Author Contributions

EL, DF, and AG: conceived and designed the research; NB, MJ, EL, SP, MS, and JP: performed the research; DF and MJ: analyzed the data; EL, DF, MJ, and JP: wrote the paper; EL, DF, MJ, JP, NB, AG, MS, and SP: revised the paper and approved the final version.

Funding

The financial support was given by GAUK n. 1310414 and n. 346315, GAČR n. 17-15991S, personal costs of MJ were partly covered by project n. LO1611 (NPU I) provided by Ministry of Education, Youth and Sports.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are thankful to Barbora Žampachová for her valuable help with statistical analyses, Silvie Rádlová for critical reading of the manuscript, and Lucie Průšová for the stimuli preparation. Last but not least, we would like to thank two reviewers for their insightful and valuable comments which greatly helped improve the text.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00333/full#supplementary-material

References

Abdel-Aal, A., and Abdel-Baset, A. (2010). Venom yield and toxicities of six Egyptian snakes with a description of a procedure for estimating the amount of venom ejected by a single snakebite. Sci. J, King Faisal Univ. 11:1431.

Google Scholar

Alekperov, A. M. (1977). Zemnovodnye i Presmykayushchiesya Azerbaijana. [Amphibians and Reptiles of Azerbaijan.] Baku: Elm Publications. (in Russian).

Almeida, I., Soares, S. C., and Castelo-Branco, M. (2015). The distinct role of the amygdala, superior colliculus and pulvinar in processing of central and peripheral snakes. PLoS ONE 10:e0129949. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0129949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altman, M. N., Khislavsky, A. L., Coverdale, M. E., and Gilger, J. W. (2016). Adaptive attention: how preference for animacy impacts change detection. Evol. Hum. Behav. 37, 303–314. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alves, R. R., Mendonça, L. E., Confessor, M. V., Vieira, W. L., and Lopez, L. C. (2009). Hunting strategies used in the semi-arid region of northeastern Brazil. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 5:12. doi: 10.1186/1746-4269-5-12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alves, R. R. N., Vieira, K. S., Santana, G. G., Vieira, W. L. S., Almeida, W. O., Souto, W. M. S., et al. (2012). A review on human attitudes towards reptiles in Brazil. Environ. Monit. Assess. 184, 6877–6901. doi: 10.1007/s10661-011-2465-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amr, Z. S., Disi, A. M., and Al-Melhim, W. N. (1997). Additions to the knowledge of Müller's Snake, Micrelaps muelleri (Boettger, 1880; Squamata: Serpentes: Colubridae). Herpetozoa 10, 163–168.

Arrindell, W. A., Eisemann, M., Richter, J., Oei, T. P., Caballo, V., E., van der Ende, J., et al. (2003). Phobic anxiety in 11 nations: Part I: Dimensional constancy of the five-factor model. Behav. Res. Ther. 41, 461–479. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00047-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balakrishnan, P. (2010). An education programme and establishment of a citizen scientist network to reduce killing of non-venomous snakes in Malappuram district, Kerala, India. Conserv. Evid. 7, 9–15.

Google Scholar

Ballouard, J. M., Ajtic, R., Balint, H., Brito, J. C., Crnobrnja-Isailovic, J., Desmonts, D., et al. (2013). Schoolchildren and one of the most unpopular animals: are they ready to protect snakes? Anthrozoös 26, 93–109. doi: 10.2752/175303713X13534238631560

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballouard, J. M., Provost, G., Barré, D., and Bonnet, X. (2012). Influence of a field trip on the attitude of schoolchildren toward unpopular organisms: an experience with snakes. J. Herpetol. 46, 423–428. doi: 10.1670/11-118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannikov, A. G., Darevsky, I. S., Ishchenko, V. G., Rustamov, A. K., and Szczerbak, N. N. (1977). Guide to Amphibians and Reptiles of the USSR Fauna. Moscow: Prosveshchenie.

Barkow, J. H. (1992). “Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and social stratification,” in The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture, eds J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, and J. Tooby (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 627–637.

Google Scholar

Beard, K. C. (2002). “Basal anthropoids,” in The Primate Fossil Record, ed W. C. Hartwig (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 133–149.

Google Scholar

Bjerke, T., and Østdahl, T. (2004). Animal-related attitudes and activities in an urban population. Anthrozoös 17, 109–129. doi: 10.2752/089279304786991783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. H. (1973). Toxicology and Pharmacology of Venoms from Poisonous Snakes. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

Google Scholar

Calvillo, D. P., and Hawkins, W. C. (2016). Animate objects are detected more frequently than inanimate objects in inattentional blindness tasks independently of threat. J. Gen. Psychol. 143, 101–115. doi: 10.1080/00221309.2016.1163249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chippaux, J. P. (1998). Snake-bites: appraisal of the global situation. Bull. World Health Organ. 76:515.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chippaux, J. P. (2012). Epidemiology of snakebites in Europe: a systematic review of the literature. Toxicon 59, 86–99. doi: 10.1016/j.toxicon.2011.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coborn, J. (1991). The Atlas of Snakes of the World. Neptune City, NJ: TFH Publications.

Collins, M. A. (1976). Student attitudes towards animals. Am. Biol. Teach. 491–493.

Google Scholar

Cook, M., and Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 98, 448. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.98.4.448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, M., Mineka, S., Wolkenstein, B., and Laitsch, K. (1985). Observational conditioning of snake fear in unrelated rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 94:591. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.94.4.591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crozier, W. R. (1999). The meanings of colour: preferences among hues. Pigment Resin Technol. 28, 6–14. doi: 10.1108/03699429910252315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davey, G. C. (1994). Self-reported fears to common indigenous animals in an adult UK population: the role of disgust sensitivity. Br. J. Psychol. 85, 541–554. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02540.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawkins, R. (2005). The Ancestor's Tale: A Pilgrimage to the Dawn of Life. New York, NY: Mariner Books.

Google Scholar

Deshpande, O., Batzog lou, S., Feldman, M. W., and Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (2009). A serial founder effect model for human settlement out of Africa. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 291–300. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2008.0750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egan, D. (2007). Snakes of Arabia: a Field Guide to the Snakes of the Arabian Peninsula and Its Shores. Dubai: Motivate Publishing Limited.

El Din, S. B. (2006). A Guide to the Reptiles and Amphibians of Egypt. New York, NY: The American University in Cairo Press.

Google Scholar

Enquist, M., and Arak, A. (1994). Symmetry, beauty and evolution. Nature 372, 169–172. doi: 10.1038/372169a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrikson, M., Annas, P., Fischer, H., and Wik, G. (1996). Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behav. Res. Ther. 34, 33–39.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fry, B. G., Vidal, N., Norman, J. A., Vonk, F. J., Scheib, H., Ramjan, S. F. R., et al. (2006). Early evolution of the venom system in lizards and snakes. Nature 439, 584–588. doi: 10.1038/nature04328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frynta, D., Landová, E., and Lišková, S. (2014). “Animal beauty, cross-cultural perceptions,” in Encyclopaedia of Quality of Life and Well-Being Research, ed A. C. Michalos (Dordrecht: Springer), 179–185.

Google Scholar

Frynta, D., Lišková, S., Bültmann, S., and Burda, H. (2010). Being attractive brings advantages: the case of parrot species in captivity. PLoS ONE 5:e12568. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frynta, D., Marešová, J., Reháková-Petru, M., Šklíba, J., Šumbera, R., and Krása, A. (2011). Cross-cultural agreement in perception of animal beauty: boid snakes viewed by people from five continents. Hum. Ecol. 39, 829–834. doi: 10.1007/s10745-011-9447-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasman, L. R., and Albarracín, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: a meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. Psychol. Bull. 132:778. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.778

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Göçmen, B., Arikan, H., Özbel, Y., Mermer, A., and Çiçek, K. (2006). Clinical, physiological and serological observations of a human following a venomous bite by Macrovipera lebetina lebetina (Reptilia: Serpentes). Turkiye Parazitol. Derg. 30, 158–162.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hagen, T., and Laeng, B. (2016). The change detection advantage for animals: an effect of ancestral priorities or progeny of experimental design? Iperception 7, 1–17. doi: 10.1177/2041669516651366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallgren, K. A. (2012). Computing inter-rater reliability for observational data: an overview and tutorial. Tutor. Quant. Methods Psychol. 8:23. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.08.1.p023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayakawa, S., Kawai, N., and Masataka, N. (2011). The influence of color on snake detection in visual search in human children. Sci. Rep. 1:80. doi: 10.1038/srep00080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Headland, T. N., and Greene, H. W. (2011). Hunter–gatherers and other primates as prey, predators, and competitors of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, E1470–E1474. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1115116108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hossie, T. J., Hassall, C., Knee, W., and Sherratt, T. N. (2013). Species with a chemical defence, but not chemical offence, live longer. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 1598–1602. doi: 10.1111/jeb.12143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isbell, L. A. (2006). Snakes as agents of evolutionary change in primate brains. J. Hum. Evol. 51, 1–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2005.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janovcová, M. (2015). Factors Influencing Worldwide zoo Collections of Lizards, Snakes, Turtles and Crocodiles: Effect of Conservation Status, Body Size and Their Attractiveness to Humans. Prague: Faculty of Science, Charles University in Prague.

Google Scholar

Johnson, C. R. (1975). Defensive display behaviour in some Australian and Papuan-New Guinean pygopodid lizards, boid, colubrid and elapid snakes. Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 56, 265–282. doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1975.tb00269.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasturiratne, A., Wickremasinghe, A. R., de Silva, N., Gunawardena, N. K., Pathmeswaran, A., Premaratna, R., et al. (2008). The global burden of snakebite: a literature analysis and modelling based on regional estimates of envenoming and deaths. PLoS Med. 5:e218. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.0050218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keogh, J. S. (1998). Molecular phylogeny of elapid snakes and a consideration of their biogeographic history. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 63, 177–203. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1998.tb01513.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khan, M. S. (2002). Guide to the Snakes of Pakistan. Frankfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira.

Google Scholar

Kühn, S., and Gallinat, J. (2012). The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness. Neuroimage 61, 289–294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landová, E., Marešová, J., Šimková, O., Cikánová, V., and Frynta, D. (2012). Human responses to live snakes and their photographs: evaluation of beauty and fear of the king snakes. J. Environ. Psychol. 32, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2011.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langergraber, K. E., Prüfer, K., Rowney, C., Boesch, C., Crockford, C., Fawcett, K., et al. (2012). Generation times in wild chimpanzees and gorillas suggest earlier divergence times in great ape and human evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 15716–15721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211740109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lipp, O. V., and Waters, A. M. (2007). When danger lurks in the background: attentional capture by animal fear-relevant distractors is specific and selectively enhanced by animal fear. Emotion 7:192. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lišková, S., and Frynta, D. (2013). What determines bird beauty in human eyes? Anthrozoös 26, 27–41. doi: 10.2752/175303713X13534238631399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lišková, S., Landová, E., and Frynta, D. (2015). Human preferences for colorful birds: vivid colors or pattern? Evol. Psychol. 13, 339–359. doi: 10.1177/147470491501300203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V. (2014). Deconstructing the snake: the relative roles of perception, cognition, and emotion on threat detection. Emotion 14:701. doi: 10.1037/a0035898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V., and DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Dev. Sci. 13, 221–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00872.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V., and DeLoache, J. S. (2011). What's so special about slithering serpents? Children and adults rapidly detect snakes based on their simple features. Vis. Cogn. 19, 129–143. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2010.522216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V., Matthews, K., Harvey, T., and Stark, S. L. (2014). What accounts for the rapid detection of threat? Evidence for an advantage in perceptual and behavioral responding from eye movements. Emotion 14:816. doi: 10.1037/a0035869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LoBue, V., and Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most: a developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Dev. Rev. 33, 285–303. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2013.07.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makashvili, M., Kaishauri, N., and Azmaiparashvili, T. (2014). The role of knowledge in overcoming snake fear. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 152, 184–187. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.09.178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malina, T., Krecsak, L., and Warrell, D. A. (2008). Neurotoxicity and hypertension following European adder (Vipera berus berus) bites in Hungary: case report and review. QJM 101, 801–806. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcn079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mallow, D., Ludwig, D., and Nilson, G. (2003). True Vipers: Natural History and Toxinology of Old World Vipers. Malabar, FL: Krieger Publishing Company.

Google Scholar

Marais, J. (2004). A complete guide to the snakes of southern Africa. Cape Town: Struik.

Google Scholar

Marešová, J., and Frynta, D. (2008). Noah's Ark is full of common species attractive to humans: the case of boid snakes in zoos. Ecol. Econ. 64, 554–558. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolecon.2007.03.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marešová, J., Krása, A., and Frynta, D. (2009a). We all appreciate the same animals: cross-cultural comparison of human aesthetic preferences for snake species in Papua New Guinea and Europe. Ethology 115, 297–300. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01620.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marešová, J., Landová, E., and Frynta, D. (2009b). What makes some species of milk snakes more attractive to humans than others?. Theor. Biosci. 128:227. doi: 10.1007/s12064-009-0075-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. R., Gunnell, G. F., and Martin, R. D. (2005). Deep time and the search for anthropoid origins. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 128, 60–95. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.20352

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mineka, S., Keir, R., and Price, V. (1980). Fear of snakes in wild-and laboratory-reared rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Ani. Learn. Behav. 8, 653–663. doi: 10.3758/BF03197783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nalbantsoy, A., Erel, S. B., Köksal, Ç., Göçmen, B., Yildiz, M. Z., and Yavaşoglu, N. Ü. K. (2013). Viper venom induced inflammation with Montivipera xanthina (Gray, 1849) and the anti-snake venom activities of Artemisia absinthium L. in rat. Toxicon 65, 34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.toxicon.2012.12.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nalbantsoy, A., Karabay-Yavasoglu, N. U., Sayim, F., Deliloglu-Gurhan, I., Gocmen, B., Arikan, H., et al. (2012). Determination of in vivo toxicity and in vitro cytotoxicity of venom from the Cypriot blunt-nosed viper Macrovipera lebetina lebetina and antivenom production. J. Venomous Ani. Toxins Include. Trop. Dis. 18, 208–216. doi: 10.1590/S1678-91992012000200011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

New, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (2007). Category-specific attention for animals reflects ancestral priorities, not expertise. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.U.S.A. 104, 16598–16603. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703913104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., Flykt, A., and Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J. Exp. Psychol. 130:466. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychol. Rev. 108:483. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., and Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent snakes as a prototypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 12, 5–9. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A., Soares, S. C., Juth, P., Lindström, B., and Esteves, F. (2012). Evolutionary derived modulations of attention to two common fear stimuli: Serpents and hostile humans. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 17–32. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.629603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Shea, M. (1996). Guide to the Snakes of Papua New Guinea. Pt. Moresby: Independent Publishing.

Pandey, D. B., Pandey, G. S., Evkota, K., and Goode, M. (2016). Public perceptions of snakes and snakebite management: implications for conservation and human health in southern Nepal. J. Ethnobiol. Ethnomed. 12, 1–24. doi: 10.1186/s13002-016-0092-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polák, J., Sedláčková, K., Nácar, D., Landová, E., and Frynta, D. (2016). Fear the serpent: a psychometric study of snake phobia. Psychiatry Res. 242, 163–168. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pook, C. E., Joger, U., Stümpel, N., and Wüster, W. (2009). When continents collide: phylogeny, historical biogeography and systematics of the medically important viper genus Echis (Squamata: Serpentes: Viperidae). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 53, 792–807. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2009.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokop, P., and Fančovičová, J. (2013). Does colour matter? The influence of animal warning coloration on human emotions and willingness to protect them. Ani. Conserv. 16, 458–466. doi: 10.1111/acv.12014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokop, P., Özel, M., and Uşak, M. (2009). Cross-cultural comparison of student attitudes toward snakes. Soc. Anim. 17, 224–240. doi: 10.1163/156853009X445398

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prokop, P., and Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2008). “Disgusting” animals: primary school children's attitudes and myths of bats and spiders. Eur. J. Math. Sci. Technol. Educ. 4, 87–97. doi: 10.12973/ejmste/75309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pyron, R. A., Burbrink, F. T., and Wiens, J. J. (2013). A phylogeny and revised classification of Squamata, including 4161 species of lizards and snakes. BMC Evol. Biol. 13:93. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-13-93

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rádlová, S., Viktorin, P., and Frynta, D. (2016). Barvocuc 2.0, Software for Color Image Analysis. Available online at: http://www.nudz.cz/en/w_group/wg-on-emotions-and-phobias-triggered-by-animal

R Development Core Team (2010). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Austria.

Samy, E. M., Shaaban, E. A., Kenawy, S. A., Galal, M. A., and Salama, W. H. (2015). Evaluation of the effect of gamma rays on Echis Coloratus snake venom through toxicological, immunological and biological studies. Int. J. Sci. Res. Publ. 5, 79–87.

Google Scholar

Schlegel, J., and Rupf, R. (2010). Attitudes towards potential animal flagship species in nature conservation: a survey among students of different educational institutions. J. Nat. Conserv. 18, 278–290. doi: 10.1016/j.jnc.2009.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultz, K. D. (1996). A Monograph of the Colubrid Snakes of the Genus Elaphe, Fitzinger. Havlíčkuv Brod: Koeltz Scientific Books.

Schwarz, N. (2000). AGENDA 2000 - social judgment and attitudes: Warmer, more social, and less conscious. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 30, 149–176. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(200003/04)30:2<149::AID-EJSP998>3.0.CO;2-N

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, S. C., Esteves, F., Lundqvist, D., and Öhman, A. (2009). Some animal specific fears are more specific than others: evidence from attention and emotion measures. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 1032–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sobel, I. (1978). Neighborhood coding of binary images for fast contour following and general binary array processing. Comput. Graph. Image Process. 8, 127–135. doi: 10.1016/S0146-664X(78)80020-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spawls, S., Branch, B., and Branch, W. R. (1995). The Dangerous Snakes of Africa: Natural History, Species Directory, Venoms, and Snakebite. Sanibel, FL: Ralph Curtis Books.

Spawls, S., Howell, K., Drewes, R., and Ashe, J. (2002). A Field Guide to the Reptiles of East Africa. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

StatSoft Inc. (2010). Statistica (data Analysis Software System), Version 9.1. Available online at: www.statsoft.com

Steinhoff, S. (2017). SnakeDatabase. Available online at: http://snakedatabase.org (accessed 25 March 2017)

Stewart, J. R., and Stringer, C. B. (2012). Human evolution out of Africa: the role of refugia and climate change. Science 335, 1317–1321. doi: 10.1126/science.1215627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stojanov, A., Tzankov, N., Naumov, B., and Nöllert, A. (2011). Die Amphibien und Reptilien Bulgariens. [Amphibians and reptiles of Bulgaria]. Franfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira. (In German).

Swaroop, S., and Grab, B. (1954). Snakebite mortality in the world. Bull. World Health Organ. 10:35.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tierney, K. J., and Connolly, M. K. (2013). A review of the evidence for a biological basis for snake fears in humans. Psychol. Rec. 63:919. doi: 10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.4.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tipples, J., Young, A. W., Quinlan, P., Broks, P., and Ellis, A. W. (2002). Searching for threat. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 55, 1007–1026. doi: 10.1080/02724980143000659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uetz, P., Freed, P., and Hošek, J. (2015). The Reptile Database. Available online at: http://www.reptile-database.org (Accessed March 24, 2015).

Valakos, E. D., Pafilis, P., Sotiropoulos, K., Lymberakis, P., Maragou, P., and Foufopoulos, J. (2008). The Amphibians and Reptiles of Greece. Frankfurt am Main: Edition Chimaira.

Valenta, J. (2008). Jedovatí hadi. Intoxikace, Terapie. Praha: Galén Press.

Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Nguyen, M., Hori, E., Maior, R. S., et al. (2013). Pulvinar neurons reveal neurobiological evidence of past selection for rapid detection of snakes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 19000–19005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1312648110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Le, Q., Isbell, L. A., Matsumoto, J., Quang Le, V., Hori, E., Tran, A. H., et al. (2014). Monkey pulvinar neurons fire differentially to snake postures. PLoS ONE 9:e114258. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Strien, J. W., Franken, I. H., and Huijding, J. (2014). Testing the snake-detection hypothesis: larger early posterior negativity in humans to pictures of snakes than to pictures of other reptiles, spiders and slugs. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:691. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00691

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidal, N., and Hedges, S. B. (2002). Higher-level relationships of snakes inferred from four nuclear and mitochondrial genes. C. R. Biol. 325, 977–985. doi: 10.1016/S1631-0691(02)01510-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 585–594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wallach, V., Williams, K. L., and Boundy, J. (2014). Snakes of the World: A Catalogue of Living and Extinct Species. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Wegener, D. T., and Carlston, D. (2005). “Cognitive processes in attitude formation and change,” in The Handbook of Attitudes eds D. Albarracín, B. T. Johnson, and M. P. Zanna (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 493–542.

Google Scholar

Weinstein, S. A., Warrell, D. A., White, J., and Keyler, D. E. (2011). “Venomous“ Bites from Non-Venomous Snakes: A Critical Analysis of Risk and Management of “Colubrid“ Snakebites. London: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Weiser, E., Wollberg, Z., Kochva, E., and Lee, S. Y. (1984). Cardiotoxic effects of the venom of the burrowing asp, Atractaspis engaddensis (Atractaspididae, Ophidia). Toxicon 22, 767–774. doi: 10.1016/0041-0101(84)90159-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, L., Brandl, P., and Frynta, D. (2015). Fear reactions to snakes in naïve mouse lemurs and pig-tailed macaques. Primates 56, 279–284. doi: 10.1007/s10329-015-0473-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitaker, P. B., and Shine, R. (2000). Sources of mortality of large elapid snakes in an agricultural landscape. J. Herpetol. 121–128. doi: 10.2307/1565247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcox, C. D., Dove, S. B., Doss-McDavid, W., and Greer, D. B. (2002). UTHSCSA ImageTool© Ver 3.1. San Antonio: University of Texas Health Science Center.

Wüster, W., Peppin, L., Pook, C. E., and Walker, D. E. (2008). A nesting of vipers: phylogeny and historical biogeography of the Viperidae (Squamata: Serpentes). Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 49, 445–459. doi: 10.1016/j.ympev.2008.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorek, N. (2009). The only good snake is a dead snake: secondary school students' attitudes toward snakes. Biotechnol. Biotechnol. Equip. 23(Suppl. 1), 31–35. doi: 10.1080/13102818.2009.10818358

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yorzinski, J. L., Penkunas, M. J., Platt, M. L., and Coss, R. G. (2014). Dangerous animals capture and maintain attention in humans. Evol. Psychol. 12, 534–548. doi: 10.1177/147470491401200304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yousefkhani, S. S. H., Yousefi, M., Khani, A., and Pouyani, E. R. (2014). Snake fauna of Shirahmad wildlife refuge and Parvand protected area, Khorasan Razavi province, Iran. Herpetol. Notes 7, 75–82.

Google Scholar

Keywords: attitude to snakes, cross-cultural study of emotions, envenoming, fear of snakes, perception of animal beauty, snakebites, viperidae

Citation: Landová E, Bakhshaliyeva N, Janovcová M, Peléšková Š, Suleymanova M, Polák J, Guliev A and Frynta D (2018) Association Between Fear and Beauty Evaluation of Snakes: Cross-Cultural Findings. Front. Psychol. 9:333. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00333

Received: 30 August 2017; Accepted: 27 February 2018;
Published: 16 March 2018.

Edited by:

Danielle Sulikowski, Charles Sturt University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Ferenc Kocsor, University of Pécs, Hungary
Karlijn Massar, Maastricht University, Netherlands

Copyright © 2018 Landová, Bakhshaliyeva, Janovcová, Peléšková, Suleymanova, Polák, Guliev and Frynta. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eva Landová, ZXZhbGFuZG92YUBzZXpuYW0uY3o=
Daniel Frynta, ZnJ5bnRhQGNlbnRydW0uY3o=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.