- 1Department of Neuromedicine and Movement Science (INB), Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
- 2Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway
Physical inactivity, low physical fitness, and perceived stress during adolescence are presumed to be risk factors for various disorders and subjective health complaints. On the other hand, physical activity and physical fitness, as well as mindfulness qualities, are regarded as prerequisites for health and well-being in children and adolescent, possibly by moderating the negative effects of stress and protecting against stress-related health complaints. Previous studies have suggested gender differences in the relationship between physical activity/physical fitness and psychological variables. The main objective in this study was to evaluate how physical fitness, along with mindfulness qualities (MAAS-A), pain, and BMI, relate to stress (PSQ) in adolescents. Secondary objectives were to explore the relationship between physical fitness, mindfulness (MAAS-A), and BMI more explicitly in the study sample, as well as to evaluate possible gender differences. The cross-sectional sample included 102 Norwegian pupils in 10th grade (15 or 16 years). Study measurements were four items from the Test of Physical Fitness (TPF), the Norwegian version of the four-factor Perceived Stress Questionnaire (PSQ), the Norwegian version of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale-Adolescent (MAAS-A), and BMI (recorded in terms of self-reported height and weight). Additionally, pain was measured in terms of localization, number of pain sites, duration, and intensity (Visual analogue scale; VAS). According to the regression analyses, physical fitness could not explain any variation in stress among the adolescents. Nevertheless, there were some negative associations between one stress factor (lack of joy) and components of physical fitness at a group level, possibly influenced by conditions not measured in this study. As opposed to physical fitness, mindfulness qualities, and to some degree gender, seemed to explain variation in stress among the adolescents. None of the physical fitness components were associated to mindfulness (MAAS-A), but some components seemed negatively related to BMI, particularly among the males. Among the females, higher physical fitness (in terms of endurance) seemed related to reduced number of pain sites. Of note, the cross-sectional design did not allow us to determine any causal direction among the variables.
Introduction
Physical inactivity, low physical fitness, and perceived stress during childhood and adolescence are presumed to be risk factors for various chronic conditions including cardiovascular diseases (Petersen et al., 2012; Bergh et al., 2015), depressive symptoms (Motl et al., 2004; Brunet et al., 2013), pain and subjective health complaints (Sundblad et al., 2008). In turn, physical fitness and physical activity are presumed to moderate the negative effects of stress and protect against stress-related health complaints in children and adolescents (Haugland et al., 2003; Hegberg and Tone, 2015; Gerber et al., 2017a). Gerber et al. (2017a) recently revealed that children (6- to 8-year-old) with higher physical fitness levels, in terms of cardiorespiratory fitness (measured by 20 m shuttle run test), experienced higher levels of psychological well-being relative to their less fit peers when exposed to elevated stress levels (parental questionnaire), and higher levels of physical wellbeing when stress levels were low. Furthermore, children with higher physical activity, as reported by their parents, demonstrated better overall health-related quality of life (parental questionnaire), but only if they experienced low stress (Gerber et al., 2017a). Recently, Cowley et al. (2017) confirmed an inverse relationship between total self-perceived stress and physical activity during spare time in adolescents.
Although strongly related, apparently (Caspersen et al., 1985; Tittlbach et al., 2017), physical activity and physical fitness are terms that describe different concepts, and which are defined and distinguished for health-related research: Physical activity is any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that results in energy expenditure. Physical fitness is a set of attributes that people have or achieve, and the health-related components of physical fitness are (a) cardiorespiratory endurance, (b) muscular endurance, (c) muscular strength, (d) body composition, and (e) flexibility (Caspersen et al., 1985). Of note, physical activity during childhood and adolescence seems only weakly related to cardiorespiratory fitness (a) in terms of peak oxygen uptake (Armstrong, 2013). According to a transactional model of stress, stress is experienced when a person perceives that the demands overload or exceed the personal and social resources the individual is able to mobilize (Lazarus and Folkman, 1984).
In particular, cardiorespiratory fitness is proposed to protect against health-threatening reactions to acute psychosocial stress (Wyss et al., 2016; Gerber et al., 2017a), and stress-related symptoms of burnout and depression (Gerber et al., 2013a,b; Elliot et al., 2015). Recently, authors have recommended vigorous physical activity in adolescents to prevent burnout symptoms in this group, based on the demonstrated negative correlations between these variables (Gerber et al., 2013b; Elliot et al., 2015). Cardiorespiratory fitness is also proposed to protect against overweight and adiposity in children, adolescents, and young adults (Gerber et al., 2017b; Hingorjo et al., 2017). Gerber et al. (2017b) found that children (7–8 years) experiencing elevated school-related stress had lower body mass index (BMI), body fat, and waist circumferences if they had high physical fitness (measured by 20 m shuttle-run test) and physical activity levels.
In general, physical activity and physical fitness are considered beneficial in adolescents, resulting in consensus statements and activity guidelines for this group (Hallal et al., 2006; Tremblay et al., 2011). Several of the health beneficial effects, with respect to various subjective health complaints including pain and psychological symptoms, apparently relate to reduced stress or stress resilience (Haugland et al., 2003; Sundblad et al., 2008; Moljord et al., 2014; Bergh et al., 2015; Hegberg and Tone, 2015; Gerber et al., 2017a). Mindfulness qualities also seem related to reduced stress in adolescents (Bluth et al., 2015; Galla, 2016). Moreover, studies have shown that physically active lifestyles are related to better mindfulness skills (Kangasniemi et al., 2014), and that mindfulness possibly mediates the beneficial (stress resilient) effects of physical fitness (Demarzo et al., 2014). Mindfulness is typically described as a form of nonjudgmental, nonreactive attention to present moment experiences, both internal and external (Brown and Ryan, 2003; Kabat-Zinn, 2003).
Hence, it might be reasonable to assume that physical fitness will relate negatively to perceived stress and pain and positively to mindfulness in adolescents. However, this is not previously investigated in Norwegian adolescents. Previous studies have demonstrated that resilience, which seems relevant for the positive effects of physical activity and physical fitness in adolescents, associates negatively with perceived stress (Montero-Marin et al., 2014) and positively with mindfulness qualities (Montero-Marin et al., 2015). Furthermore, previous studies have suggested plausible gender differences in the relationship between physical activity/physical fitness and psychological variables (Moksnes et al., 2014; Moljord et al., 2014), which are relevant to investigate further.
The main objective in this study was to evaluate how physical fitness, along with mindfulness qualities, pain, and BMI, relate to perceived stress in adolescents. Secondary objectives were to explore the relationship between physical fitness, mindfulness, and BMI more explicitly in the study sample, as well as to evaluate possible gender differences.
Materials and Methods
Design of the Study
This was a cross-sectional study.
Sample and Settings
The sample included 102 Norwegian pupils in 10th grade (15 or 16 years), recruited from a public school in the Trondheim municipality. The participating school was considered to be an “average” school regarding sociocultural- and economic conditions (Ownership Unit in Trondheim Commune, 2012).
Sample Size
Sample size calculation (power analyses) showed that 90 participants were required to have 84% power to significantly detect a correlation (r) of 0.30 in the population at the 5% level (standard error 0.10, 95 % CI: 0.12–0.46). This number was increased to 110 to allow for withdrawals. The total number that completed both questionnaires and physical fitness test were N = 102, giving an overall response rate of 92.7%. 62 (60.8%) females and 40 (39.2%) males were included. The data was collected autumn 2015.
Variables and Instruments
The Perceived Stress Questionnaire (PSQ)
The recent form of the 30-item PSQ, which is used in this work, refer to the period of the last 4 weeks and can be answered with a 4-point rating scale (1 = almost never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often and 4 usually; Levenstein et al., 1993; Fliege et al., 2005; Kocalevent et al., 2007). Higher scores indicate more severe perceived stress. The resulting PSQ total score is linearly transformed between zero and one; PSQ = (raw value – 30)/90 (Levenstein et al., 1993). The cut-off score for moderate to severe stress is set to ≥0.45, based on the mean PSQ scores in the present study samples, which corresponds with previous findings and practice (Kocalevent et al., 2007, 2011). Commonly used cut-off levels of stress with respect to the PSQ are low <0.33; medium ≥0.33 <0.45; moderate ≥0.45 <0.60; severe ≥0.60. Scale instructions are “For each sentence, circle the number that describe how often it applies to you in general, during the last month. Work quickly, without bothering to check your answers, and be careful to describe how it applies to you in general.” The PSQ permits the subjective experience of perceived stressful situations and stress reactions to be assessed, emphasizing cognitive perceptions more than emotional states or specific life events, and is considered a reliable and valid instrument for recording perceived stress in the context of a transactional view of stress (Kocalevent et al., 2007). It contains both positively and negatively formulated items in order to reduce acquiescent bias. Examples of positively formulated items are “You feel rested,” “You are full of energy,” and “You enjoy yourself.” Examples of negatively formulated items are “You feel that too many demands are being made on you,” “You have too many things to do,” and “You have many worries.” Each item is answered using a four-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 1 (“almost never”) to 4 (“almost always”). The translation process and psychometric evaluation of the Norwegian version of the PSQ are explained in detail in a separate paper, providing support for a four-factor model [“worries,” “tension,” (lack of) “joy,” “demands” (Fliege et al., 2005)], of the PSQ in Norwegian adolescents (Østerås et al., in review).
Physical Fitness
To measure physical, four items from the Test of Physical Fitness (TPF) (Fjørtoft et al., 2011) were used for testing strength, speed, agility/plyometrics, and endurance, primarily. The four test items are as follows:
1. Pushing a medicine ball (2 kg) with two hands as far as possible. The starting position is with the feet parallel to each other and shoulder width apart, with the ball held against the chest. The test item score (better of two attempts) is the distance achieved (measured in meters).
2. Running 20 m as quickly as possible. The participant starts in a standing position. At signal, the participant runs as fast as possible toward the finish line. The test item score is the time in seconds needed to run the 20 m.
3. Standing broad jump. The participant starts with the feet parallel and a shoulder width apart behind a starting line. At a signal, the participant swings his or her arms backward and forward and jumps with both feet simultaneously as far forward as possible. The test item score (the better of two attempts) is the distance (in centimeters) between the starting line and the landing position.
4. Reduced Cooper Test. The participant runs or walks around a marked rectangle measuring 9 × 18 m (the size of a volleyball field) for 6 min. Running and walking are allowed. The test item score is the distance covered (in m) in 6 min.
All test item scores were transformed into standardized score (z-scores) from the mean of the whole sample (n = 102). The score of test item 2 (running 20 m as quickly as possible), which measured the time needed to accomplish the test item, was beforehand converted to 1/score, such that higher scores always indicated better performance than lower scores. The test items scores represent subcategories or “sub-variables” of physical fitness, involving different components of physical fitness. Higher z-scores indicate better performance on the tasks. A total test score was calculated to express the adolescent's overall performance on the four physical fitness measures, as a “sum-variable” of total physical fitness. The total test score for each participant was defined as the average z-score on all test items successfully performed by that individual. Fjørtoft et al. (2011) have previously evaluated the TPF in Norwegian children, finding high internal consistency and convergent construct validity as well as fair to good test-retest reliability for individual test items scores and total score. Items from the TPF are also previously applied in adolescents (Gísladóttir et al., 2013).
The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale for Adolescents (MAAS-A)
The MAAS-A includes 14 items, where the responses are made on a 6-point scale, where higher scores reflect more mindfulness-capacity, regarding the addressed quality (Brown et al., 2011; Quaglia et al., 2016). The test-authors have recently specified that the MAAS or MAAS-A focus on a quality of attentiveness involved in mindfulness (Quaglia et al., 2016). Scale instructions were “Below is a collection of statements about your everyday experience. Using the 1–6 scale below, please indicate how frequently or infrequently you currently have each experience. Please answer according to what really reflects your experience rather than what you think your experience should be.” The accompanying 6-point scale was 1 (almost always), 2 (very frequently), 3 (somewhat frequently), 4 (somewhat infrequently), 5 (very infrequently), 6 (almost never). The total sum score of the MAAS-A is the computed mean of the items scores. The authors of the MAAS-A granted us permission for translation and back translation of the MAAS-A, and authorized our final version. The translation process of the instrument into Norwegian is explained in detail in another paper (Østerås et al., in review), wherein the instrument also was evaluated in relation to stress (PSQ). Another recent study has also provided support for the one-factor model of the Norwegian MAAS-A (short-form), with respect to model-based reliability, measurement invariance and concurrent validity (Smith et al., 2017).
Pain
Pain was measured by questions about localization (pain site), duration, and intensity. Pain site (0–6) was divided into six main categories, corresponding to the questionnaire in the Young-HUNT Study 2008 (Hoftun et al., 2011; Skrove et al., 2015); head, neck, shoulder, back, arm, lower extremity, with an open line for additionally sites. Three or more (≤3) pain sites were termed multisite pain. Pain duration (1–5) was divided into five categories; 0–2 weeks, 2–4 weeks, 1–2 months, 2–3 months, 3 months, or more. Pain intensity (Visual analog scale VAS: 0–10) was measured using a VAS-line (Price et al., 1983). The participants were instructed to mark on the VAS-line (10 cm) to illustrate their average pain during the last week, with 0 indicating “no pain” and 10 “worst pain imaginable.” VAS is described and applied as an acceptable measurement of average pain during the last week (Breivik et al., 2008; Bäckryd et al., 2016). VAS is demonstrated to be reliable as a measure of pain intensity in children (Bailey et al., 2012) and is used to detect differences in pain between groups of adolescents (Sugiura et al., 2015). The questionnaire also asked if former injuries, diseases or disorders might be related to the current pain experiences.
Body Mass Index
BMI was also recorded, in terms of self-reported height and weight, as this health variable is regarded relevant in relation to the other outcome measures (Sethi et al., 2011; Silva et al., 2014; Loucks et al., 2015; Gerber et al., 2017b).
Variables, instruments, and measurements are presented in Table 1.
Ethics and Procedures
The Regional Committee for Medical Research Ethics in Trondheim approved the data collection processes and the entire study, and the study was in line with the Declaration of Helsinki (The World Medical Association (WMA), 2013). The purpose of the study, the outcome measures and the procedures were explained and in consensus with the participating school (principal and teachers). The adolescents and the parents received an information letter that briefly explained the purpose of the study. In all stages of the data collection, it was emphasized that participation was voluntary, anonymous, and confidential, and that the participants were free to withdraw from the study at any point without giving a reason. Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Students already 16 years were responding on their own behalf. Students not yet turned 16 at the time of data collection received a written consent from their parents. Participating in the study was not presumed to affect the students in any negative way.
The questions are addressing everyday experiences/situations. The physical fitness test contains well-known motor fitness tasks, commonly applied in school gymnastics. The assessment of the physical fitness took place in a sport hall during school hours, in line with previously applied protocols (Haga et al., 2015). Assistants trained in the test protocols tested all the participants individually. Each test item was explained and demonstrated before the participants started. The participants were given verbal encouragement and support throughout the testing procedure. If the participants made a procedural error, instructions and demonstrations were repeated, and the participants made a new attempt. The participants wore clothing suitable for physical activity and sport shoes during both tests. Identification numbers were used for the participants in the study to maintain data confidentiality. The participants accomplished the test items individually in groups, performing different tasks simultaneously. Thus, no one was competing against each other, and no one (of the participants) was watching others completing the test. Test results were filled into a form by the administrator; on page 1 (behind a neutral front page) of a questionnaire folder, and immediately after handed to the participant for completion of the questionnaire on page 2 and 3 in the questionnaire folder. After answering the questionnaire in a silent and sheltered place, the participant enclosed the questionnaire folder in an envelope. The administrator collected the concealed envelopes.
Statistical Analysis
The statistical analyses were conducted in IBM SPSS 24. Preliminary analyses were performed to check for univariate and multivariate normality. Descriptive analyses of the outcome variables were calculated, also separately for gender. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient (rs) was used to assess the associations between the continuous variables (due to lack of normality of pain variables), also with a split file command with respect to gender. Linear regression analyses evaluated the probability that physical fitness, mindfulness, and pain (duration) in addition to gender and BMI could explain variation in stress (lack of joy), with the latter variable applied as the dependent variable. The linear regression model was developed backward, in order to minimize suppressor effects and to reduce the risk of making a Type II error (Field, 2013, pp. 323–324).
Results
Table 2 summarizes the descriptive analyses of dichotomous variables, overall and separately for gender. 52.5% of the males and 45.2% of the females reported pain (Table 2). The prevalence of head pain/headache was highest among the females (Table 2). A similar portion of females and males related their pain to former injuries (25.8% of the females and 27.5% of the males) or to a known disease or disorder (9.7% of the females and 7.5% of the males). The prevalence of moderate to severe stress (PSQ ≥ 0.45) seemed slightly higher among the females, i.e., 27.4% of the females against 20% of the males reported PSQ ≥ 0.45. Descriptive analyses of the continuous variables are presented in means and standard deviation (SD) in Table 3, both overall and separately for gender. Higher scores on the physical fitness measures (except from sprint), indicate higher physical fitness. Higher scores on the PSQ, including factors of PSQ, indicate higher stress. Higher scores on the MAAS-A reflect more mindfulness-capacity with respect to the addressed mindfulness-quality. The pain variables (particularly pain intensity and pain sites) were not normally distributed; hence, pain descriptions in the subsamples of females and males reporting pain are also included in Table 3.
Spearman's rank correlation coefficient (rs) was used to assess the associations between the outcome variables, since not all (pain) variables were normally distributed (Table 3). Tables 4, 5 summarize the results of the correlation analyses with respect to stress and physical fitness, respectively, both overall and separately for gender.
Table 4. Associations (rs) between, stress, physical fitness, pain, mindfulness and BMI, overall, and separately for gender.
Table 5. Associations (rs) between physical fitness, pain, mindfulness and BMI, overall, and separately for gender.
Table 4 shows significant (p < 0.001) and strong negative associations between stress and mindfulness, both overall and separately for gender. Among the females, demands appeared to be strongest related (rs = −0.57) to mindfulness, while tension (rs = −0.51) appeared strongest related to mindfulness in males.
Joy (lack of) seemed negatively associated (p < 0.05) to physical fitness in terms of pushing a medicine ball (strength) and reduced Cooper test (endurance) in the overall sample (Table 4). Also in the overall sample, worries seemed positively related (p < 0.05) to BMI.
Among the females, tension seemed positively associated (p < 0.05) to pain intensity (measured by VAS). Among the males in the study sample, several of the associations appeared reversed: Both total stress (total PSQ) and tension seemed positively associated (p < 0.05) to physical fitness in terms of pushing a medicine ball (strength). Additionally, joy (lack of) appeared negatively associated to pain sites and pain duration, while total stress (total PSQ), worries and joy (lack of) seemed negatively associated to pain intensity (VAS) (Table 4).
Table 5 presents more of the physical fitness associations. In the overall sample, physical fitness, measured by running fast 20 m, was negatively associated (p < 0.05) with BMI. Among the females, physical fitness, measured by the reduced Cooper test (endurance), was negatively associated (p < 0.005) to number of pain sites. Among the males, physical fitness in terms of running 20 m, standing broad jump and reduced Cooper, were negatively associated (p < 0.05) with BMI. There was no associations between physical fitness and mindfulness qualities measured by the MAAS-A (Table 5).
The linear regression analysis with stress (lack of joy) as dependent variable, revealed that mindfulness measured by the MAAS-A (β = −0.40, p < 0.001) and gender (PSQ, β = −0.33, p < 0.05) explained some of the variation in the stress factor (Table 6). Physical fitness (total test score), pain duration and BMI did not explain any variation in the stress factor, according to the regression analyses. The regression model explained 26.3 % of the variation in the stress factor (lack of joy). The R2 (0.263) and significant F-value (5.36, p < 0.001) supported acceptable fit of the model. The adjusted R2 (0.214) indicated very sparse loss of power. A Durbin-Watson test value close to two (1.70) indicated independence of the residuals.
Table 6. Summary of the linear regression analysis for variables predicting joy (lack of joy, as factor of stress [PSQ]).
Discussion
In this study, physical fitness along with mindfulness qualities, pain and BMI were evaluated in relation to stress in adolescents. Other physical fitness-associations were also explored in addition to gender differences. According to the main findings, physical fitness cannot explain any variation in stress among the adolescents (Table 6). Nevertheless, there were some negative associations between one stress factor (lack of joy) and components of physical fitness (strength and endurance) at a group level (Table 5), possibly influenced by conditions not measured in this study. As opposed to physical fitness, mindfulness qualities seemed to explain variation in stress among the adolescents (Table 6). Physical fitness seemed entirely unrelated to mindfulness (MAAS-A) but somewhat (negatively) related to BMI, particularly among the males (Table 5). Among the females, higher physical fitness (in terms of endurance) seemed related to reduced number of pain sites (Table 5).
Regression analyses revealed that physical fitness did not explain any of the variation in stress (lack of joy) (Table 6). Hence, the results do not support that physical fitness protects against stress in adolescents. Rather, the results might correspond with the findings by Klaperski et al. (2013, 2014). They revealed unexpected psychological and physiological stress responses to exercises, including higher mood decrease in young women (Klaperski et al., 2013). Mindfulness qualities (MAAS-A), on the contrary, seemed to explain some of the variation in stress (Table 6). This is in compliance with previous findings, showing improved mindfulness qualities to be accompanied by reduced stress in adolescents (Bluth et al., 2015; Galla, 2016). Furthermore, physical fitness seemed unrelated to mindfulness qualities (MAAS-A) among the adolescents (Table 5). In sum, this might imply that the stress-preventive effect of physical fitness rely on mediating factors/variables such as mindfulness qualities, which is also previously suggested (Demarzo et al., 2014), or variables not measured in this study such as coping strategies.
The weak association (p < 0.05) between higher physical fitness (strength and endurance) and lower stress (increased joy) at a group level (Table 5) might be influenced by conditions not measured in this study. The finding might also be comparable to the results in Moljord et al. (2011). They found that adolescents who reported physical activity-participation two to three times per week or more scored significantly lower on stress (Norwegian version of the Adolescent Stress Questionnaire) and higher on happiness (Fordyce Happiness Scale) than those who participated in physical activity 1 day per week or less. However, they found no significant difference on stress and happiness between those being physically active two or three times a week and those being active almost every day. This might correspond to the findings by Lutz et al. (2010), revealing a positive relationship between stress and exercise frequency, intensity, and duration in students exercising three times a week or more (for 20 min or more each time and for more than 6 months). Put together, previous and present findings suggest that the health beneficial effects of physical activity and physical fitness probably relay on several factors. Among others, activity dose (volume, frequency, intensity) and the factors that initiates/regulates the activity probably influence the outcome (Moljord et al., 2011, 2014; Sibley et al., 2013; Gerber et al., 2015). Sibley et al. (2013) showed that higher intrinsic motivation (more intrinsic motives and intrinsic forms of motivation) predicted healthier levels of physical fitness in students in contrast to stronger introjected regulation and appearance motives, which predicted worse fitness.
At a group level (overall sample), there seemed to be a weak association (p < 0.05) between higher stress in terms of worries and higher BMI (Table 4). This might be considered in compliance with previous studies (Sethi et al., 2011; Gerber et al., 2017b), finding positive associations between stress and BMI. However, this was not evident in the analyses separately for gender. Moreover, higher physical fitness seemed associated (p < 0.05) with lower BMI, both in the overall sample and among the males (Table 5). The negative physical fitness-BMI association might be comparable to the findings by Gerber et al. (2017b). They found that children (7–8 years) experiencing elevated school-related stress had lower body mass index, body fat, and waist circumferences if they had high physical fitness (measured by 20 m shuttle-run test) and physical activity levels. Besides, the physical fitness tasks which were associated with reduced BMI in this study, i.e., running 20 m, standing broad jump, and reduced Cooper (Table 5), commonly involve cardiorespiratory training/fitness which associate with lower BMI in adolescents and young adults (Hingorjo et al., 2017). However, the negative physical fitness-BMI association was not possible to identify among the females (Table 5).
Among the females in this study, higher stress in terms of tension seemed associated with more pain in terms of higher pain intensity (Table 4). This corresponds to previous findings in Norwegian adolescents (Østerås et al., 2015, 2016). However, this finding was not evident in the overall sample, nor among the males, possibly due to the non-normal distribution of pain variables in the sample (Table 3). Also among the females, there appeared a negative physical fitness-pain association (p < 0.05) (Table 5). More specific, increased endurance (higher scores on the reduced Cooper test) seemed related to reduced number of pain sites (Table 5). At first, this might resemble activity-induced pain modulation or hypoalgesia (Naugle et al., 2013; Jones et al., 2014; Saanijoki et al., 2017). However, the study design does not allow such an evaluation. It is also likely that the females with fewer pain sites just performed better at the test-day. There were no other associations between physical fitness and pain in this study (Table 5). This is in contrast to previous studies, suggesting pain-moderating effects of physical activity and physical fitness in children and adolescents (Haugland et al., 2003; Sundblad et al., 2008; Gerber et al., 2017a). However, these prior results might have been mediated by reduced stress (Haugland et al., 2003; Sundblad et al., 2008; Gerber et al., 2017a).
The demonstrated gender differences in this study, with respect to stress (Table 3) and its' relation to other health variables (Tables 4, 5), might be considered in compliance with previous studies. Several authors have suggested gender differences in stress appraisals, reactions and responses, both on a behavioral and a neurobiological level (Wang et al., 2007; Mayor, 2015; Marrocco and McEwen, 2016; McEwen and Milner, 2017). Furthermore, in the study by Moljord et al. (2014) in Norwegian adolescents (13–18 years old), physical activity associated with psychological symptoms in the females but not in the males. In the present study also, some of the associations seemed reversed among the males. For instance, higher physical fitness (in terms of strength) seemed associated (p < 0.05) with higher stress among the males, in terms of total PSQ and tension (Table 4). This positive physical fitness-stress association might be comparable with the findings by Lutz et al. (2010) and Gerber et al. (2015). Gerber et al. (2015) found that exercise involvement was associated with increased stress in adolescents with high exercise self-regulation. The adolescents who accepted to participate in this study, which involved willingness to perform a physical fitness test, probably were relatively high in exercise self-regulation. However, this was not measured. Furthermore, among the males in the study sample there appeared negative associations between some of the stress factors and the pain variables (Table 4). This is in contrast to previous findings in Norwegian adolescents (Østerås et al., 2015, 2016). Probably, the findings in this study sample were influenced by the non-normal distribution of pain variables (Table 3). However, the findings might also imply that perceived stress associate differently with pain in a sample of fit adolescent males (see the “Strengths and limitations”-paragraph) than in the general adolescent population.
The lack of support in this study concerning the presumed stress and pain preventing effect of physical fitness in adolescents, might also be understood in light of modern societal trends and cultures. For instance, the promotion of exercise and physical fitness for appearance-motivated reasons are common in our modern society, extensively conveyed by “fitspiration” websites, which often reinforce an over-valuation of physical appearance, eating concerns, and excessive exercise (Boepple et al., 2016). This might contribute to more unhealthy physical fitness trends and habits, probably with less beneficial health outcomes as a result (Sibley et al., 2013; Cunningham et al., 2016). A recent population-based longitudinal study from Sweden revealed a shift in the covariates of perceived health, from physical fitness (cardiorespiratory) and chronic illness in 1990 to age, BMI, and educational level in 2015, i.e., a diminished impact of physical fitness on perceived health (Olsson et al., 2017).
Strengths and Limitations
One strength of this study is the inclusion of an objective measure of physical fitness in adolescents. This prevents self-reporting bias. Previous studies with similar focus in Norwegian adolescents have been based on self-reported physical activity (Haugland et al., 2003; Moljord et al., 2011, 2014). Self-reported physical activity and objectively assessed physical activity/physical fitness have previously demonstrated to be differently and even inversely related to self-rated health (Hamer and Stamatakis, 2010; Lindwall et al., 2012). Another strength of the study is a recent validation of the included stress-instrument (PSQ) in Norwegian adolescents, providing support for the four-factor model and measurement invariance across gender (Østerås et al., in review).
However, we also acknowledge several limitations. Neither the cross-sectional design nor the correlation analyses are entitled to determine any causal relationships between the variables. Furthermore, the inclusion of a physical fitness test might have caused a selection bias. Adolescents already confident and comfortable in physical performances possibly appreciated the invitation and easily accepted to participate in the study. Less fit adolescents possibly declined to participate at a higher rate than their more fit peers. Adolescents perceiving pain and health complaints probably also declined more frequently that their pain-free (or approximately pain-free) peers. The sudden withdrawals at the day of testing due to pain and other subjective health complaints (voluntarily reported), might indicate such dropout mechanisms. The non-normally distribution of the pain-variables might also be an indicator of this. However, this was not evaluated in more detail. Furthermore, the self-reported height and weight, from which the BMI-values were calculated, were possibly not entirely reliable. In order to explain stress, several other factors and variables are also relevant, such as adverse childhood experiences, coping strategies, and catastrophizing. However, those fall beyond the defined scope of this study. Finally, the sample size was not very large (N = 102), and the findings probably needs to be confirmed in further studies.
Conclusion
This study evaluated physical fitness, mindfulness (MAAS-A), pain and BMI in relation to stress (PSQ) in Norwegian adolescents. Additional physical fitness associations were also explored as well as gender differences. Analyses revealed that physical fitness did not explain any variation in stress among the adolescents. However, there were some negative associations between one of the stress factors (lack of joy) and physical fitness components at a group level, which might have been influenced by conditions not measured in this study. As opposed to physical fitness, mindfulness qualities (MAAS-A), and to some degree gender, seemed to explain variation in stress among the adolescents. Physical fitness was unrelated to mindfulness (MAAS-A) but seemed somewhat related to BMI, particularly in males. Among the females, higher physical fitness (in terms of endurance) seemed related to fewer pain sites. The cross-sectional design did not allow us to determine any causal direction among the variables. Further studies might be needed to confirm these findings.
Author Contributions
BØ has made substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work, the acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of data for the work. BØ has also drafted the work, revised it critically for important intellectual content, and approved the final version to be published. BØ agrees to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. HS and MH has made contributions to the conception and design of the work, and has contributed in revising it, and given final approval of the version to be published. HS agrees to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Funding
This study was funded by a Ph.D. grant from Sør-Trøndelag University College (now; Norwegian University of Science and Technology). The funder had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Armstrong, N. (2013). Aerobic fitness and physical activity in children. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 25, 548–560. doi: 10.1123/pes.25.4.548
Bäckryd, E., Ghafouri, B., Larsson, B., and Gerdle, B. (2016). Plasma pro-inflammatory markers in chronic neuropathic pain: a multivariate, comparative, cross-sectional pilot study. Scand. J. Pain 10, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2015.06.006
Bailey, B., Gravel, J., and Daoust, R. (2012). Reliability of the visual analog scale in children with acute pain in the emergency department. Pain 153, 839–842. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.01.006
Bergh, C., Udumyan, R., Fall, K., Almroth, H., and Montgomery, S. (2015). Stress resilience and physical fitness in adolescence and risk of coronary heart disease in middle age. Heart 101, 623–629. doi: 10.1136/heartjnl-2014-306703
Bluth, K., Roberson, P. N., and Gaylord, S. A. (2015). A Pilot study of a mindfulness intervention for adolescents and the potential role of self-compassion in reducing stress. Explore 11, 292–295. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2015.04.005
Boepple, L., Ata, R. N., Rum, R., and Thompson, J. K. (2016). Strong is the new skinny: a content analysis of fitspiration websites. Body Image 17, 132–135. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.03.001
Breivik, H., Borchgrevink, P. C., Allen, S. M., Rosseland, L. A., Romundstad, L., Hals, E. K., et al. (2008). Assessment of pain. Br. J. Anaesth. 101, 17–24. doi: 10.1093/bja/aen103
Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 822–848. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Brown, K. W., West, A. M., Loverich, T. M., and Biegel, G. M. (2011). Assessing adolescent mindfulness: validation of an adapted mindful attention awareness scale in adolescent normative and psychiatric populations. Psychol. Assess. 23, 1023–1033. doi: 10.1037/a0021338
Brunet, J., Sabiston, C. M., Chaiton, M., Barnett, T. A., O'Loughlin, E., Low, N. C., et al. (2013). The association between past and current physical activity and depressive symptoms in young adults: a 10-year prospective study. Ann. Epidemiol. 23, 25–30. doi: 10.1016/j.annepidem.2012.10.006
Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., and Christenson, G. M. (1985). Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Rep. 100, 126–131.
Cowley, J., Kiely, J., and Collins, D. (2017). Is there a link between self-perceived stress and physical activity levels in Scottish adolescents? Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health. doi: 10.1515/ijamh-2016-0104. [Epub ahead of print].
Cunningham, H. E., Pearman, S. III., and Brewerton, T. D. (2016). Conceptualizing primary and secondary pathological exercise using available measures of excessive exercise. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49, 778–792. doi: 10.1002/eat.22551
Demarzo, M. M., Montero-Marin, J., Stein, P. K., Cebolla, A., Provinciale, J. G., and García-Campayo, J. (2014). Mindfulness may both moderate and mediate the effect of physical fitness on cardiovascular responses to stress: a speculative hypothesis. Front. Physiol. 5:105. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00105
Elliot, C., Lang, C., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., Pühse, U., and Gerber, M. (2015). The relationship between meeting vigorous physical activity recommendations and burnout symptoms among adolescents: an exploratory study with vocational students. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 37, 180–192. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2014-0199
Field, A. (2013). Discovering Statistics using IBM SPSS Statistics: and Sex and Drugs and Rock ‘n’ Roll, 4th Edn. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE.
Fjørtoft, I., Pedersen, A. V., Sigmundsson, H., and Vereijken, B. (2011). Measuring physical fitness in children who are 5 to 12 years old with a test battery that is functional and easy to administer. Phys. Ther. 91, 1087–1095. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20090350
Fliege, H., Rose, M., Arck, P., Walter, O. B., Kocalevent, R. D., Weber, C., et al. (2005). The Perceived Stress Questionnaire (PSQ) reconsidered: validation and reference valuesfrom different clinical and healthy adult samples. Psychosom. Med. 67, 78–88. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000151491.80178.78
Galla, B. M. (2016). Within-person changes in mindfulness and self-compassion predict enhanced emotional well-being in healthy, but stressed adolescents. J. Adolesc. 49, 204–217. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.03.016
Gerber, M., Endes, K., Brand, S., Herrmann, C., Colledge, F., Donath, L., et al. (2017a). In 6- to 8-year-old children, cardiorespiratory fitness moderates the relationship between severity of life events and health-related quality of life. Qual. Life Res. 26, 695–706. doi: 10.1007/s11136-016-1472-6
Gerber, M., Endes, K., Herrmann, C., Colledge, F., Brand, S., Donath, L., et al. (2017b). Fitness, stress, and body composition in primary schoolchildren. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 49, 581–587. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001123
Gerber, M., Lang, C., Feldmeth, A. K., Elliot, C., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E., et al. (2013a). Burnout and mental health in swiss vocational students: the moderating role of physical activity. J. Res. Adolesc. 25, 63–74. doi: 10.1111/jora.12097
Gerber, M., Lindwall, M., Brand, S., Lang, C., Elliot, C., and Pühse, U. (2015). Longitudinal relationships between perceived stress, exercise self-regulation and exercise involvement among physically active adolescents. J. Sports Sci. 33, 369–380. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2014.946072
Gerber, M., Lindwall, M., Lindegård, A., Börjesson, M., and Jonsdottir, I. H. (2013b). Cardiorespiratory fitness protects against stress-related symptoms of burnout and depression. Patient Educ. Couns. 93, 146–152. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2013.03.021
Gísladóttir, Þ*., Haga, M., and Sigmundsson, H. (2013). Physical fitness measures among adolescents with high and low motor competence. SAGE Open 3, 1–8. doi: 10.1177/2158244013500282
Haga, M., Gísladóttír, T., and Sigmundsson, H. (2015). The relationship between motor competence and physical fitness is weaker in the 15–16 Yr. adolescent age group than in younger age groups (4–5 Yr. and 11–12 Yr.). Percept. Mot. Skills 121, 900–912. doi: 10.2466/10.PMS.121c24x2
Hallal, P. C., Victora, C. G., Azevedo, M. R., and Wells, J. C. (2006). Adolescent physical activity and health: a systematic review. Sports Med. 36, 1019–1030. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200636120-00003
Hamer, M., and Stamatakis, E. (2010). Objectively assessed physical activity, fitness and subjective wellbeing. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 3, 67–71. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2010.09.001
Haugland, S., Wold, B., and Torsheim, T. (2003). Relieving the pressure? The role of physical activity in the relationship between school-related stress and adolescent health complaints. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 74, 127–135. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609074
Hegberg, N. J., and Tone, E. B. (2015). Physical activity and stress resilience: considering those at-risk for developing mental health problems. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 8, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.10.001
Hingorjo, M. R., Zehra, S., Hasan, Z., and Qureshi, M. A. (2017). Cardiorespiratory fitness and its association with adiposity indices in young adults. Pak. J. Med. Sci. 33, 659–664. doi: 10.12669/pjms.333.12294
Hoftun, G. B., Romundstad, P. R., and Rygg, M. (2011). Factors associated with adolescent chronic non-specific pain, chronic multisite pain, and chronic pain with high disability: the Young-HUNT Study 2008. Pain 152, 2259–2266. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.05.007
Jones, M. D., Booth, J., Taylor, J. L., and Barry, B. K. (2014). Aerobic training increases pain tolerance in healthy individuals. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 46, 1640–1647. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000273
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clin. Psychol. 10, 144–156. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpg016
Kangasniemi, A., Lappalainen, R., Kankaanpää, A., and Tammelin, T. (2014). Mindfulness skills, psychological flexibility, and psychological symptoms among physically less active and active adults. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 7, 121–127. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.06.005
Klaperski, S., von Dawans, B., Heinrichs, M., and Fuchs, R. (2013). Does the level of physical exercise affect physiological and psychological responses to psychosocial stress in women? Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 266–274. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.11.003
Klaperski, S., von Dawans, B., Heinrichs, M., and Fuchs, R. (2014). Effects of a 12-week endurance training program on the physiological response to psychosocial stress in men: a randomized controlled trial. J. Behav. Med. 37, 1118–1133. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9562-9
Kocalevent, R. D., Hinz, A., Brähler, E., and Klapp, B. F. (2011). Determinants of fatigue and stress. BMC Res. Notes 4:238. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-4-238
Kocalevent, R. D., Levenstein, S., Fliege, H., Schmid, G., Hinz, A., Brähler, E., et al. (2007). Contribution to the construct validity of the perceived stress questionnaire from apopulation-based survey. J. Psychosom. Res. 63, 71–81.
Levenstein, S., Prantera, C., Varvo, V., Scribano, M. L., Berto, E., Luzi, C., et al. (1993). Development of the perceived stress questionnaire: a new tool for psychosomatic research. J. Psychosom. Res. 37, 19–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.02.010
Lindwall, M., Ljung, T., Hadzibajramovic, E., and Jonsdottir, I. (2012). Self-reported physical activity and aerobic fitness are differently related to mental health. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 5, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2011.12.003
Loucks, E., Britton, W., Howe, C., Gutman, R., Gilman, S., Brewer, J., et al. (2015). Associations of dispositional mindfulness with obesity and central adiposity: the New England family study. Int. J. Behav. Med. 23, 224–233. doi: 10.1007/s12529-015-9513-z
Lutz, R. S., Stults-Kolehmainen, M. A., and Bartholomew, J. B. (2010). Exercise caution when stressed: stages of change and the stress–exercise participation relationship. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 11, 560–567. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2010.06.005
Marrocco, J., and McEwen, B. S. (2016). Sex in the brain: hormones and sex differences. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 18, 373–383.
Mayor, E. (2015). Gender roles and traits in stress and health. Front. Psychol. 6:779. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00779
McEwen, B. S., and Milner, T. A. (2017). Understanding the broad influence of sex hormones and sex differences in the brain. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 24–39. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23809
Moksnes, U. K., Espnes, G. A., and Haugan, G. (2014). Stress, sense of coherence and emotional symptoms in adolescents. Psychol. Health 29, 32–49. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2013.822868
Moljord, I. E., Moksnes, U. K., Eriksen, L., and Espnes, G. A. (2011). Stress and happiness among adolescents with varying frequency of physical activity. Percept. Mot. Skills 113, 631–646. doi: 10.2466/02.06.10.13.PMS.113.5.631-646
Moljord, I. E. O., Moksnes, U. K., Espnes, G. A., and Hjemdal, O. (2014). Physical activity, resilience, and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 7, 79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2014.04.001
Montero-Marin, J., Piva Demarzo, M. M., Pereira, J. P., Olea, M., and García-Campayo, J. (2014). Reassessment of the psychometric characteristics and factor structure of the'Perceived Stress Questionnaire' (PSQ): analysis in a sample of dental students. PLoS ONE 9:e87071. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0087071
Montero-Marin, J., Tops, M., Manzanera, R., Piva Demarzo, M. M., Álvarez de Mon, M., and García-Campayo, J. (2015). Mindfulness, resilience, and burnout subtypes in primary care physicians: the possible mediating role of positive and negative affect. Front. Psychol. 6:1895. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01895
Motl, R. W., Birnbaum, A. S., Kubik, M. Y., and Dishman, R. K. (2004). Naturally occurring changes in physical activity are inversely related to depressive symptoms during early adolescence. Psychosom. Med. 66, 336–342.
Naugle, K. M., Fillingim, R. B., and Riley, J. L. III. (2013). A meta-analytic review of the hypoalgesic effects of exercise. J. Pain 13, 1139–1150. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2012.09.006
Olsson, S. J. G., Ekblom-Bak, E., Ekblom, B., Kallings, L. V., Ekblom, Ö., and Börjesson, M. (2017). Association of perceived physical health and physical fitness in two Swedish national samples from 1990 and 2015. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports. doi: 10.1111/sms.12943. [Epub ahead of print].
Ownership Unit in Trondheim Commune (2012). Living Conditions 2011. A Rapport on Living Conditions in Trondheim.
Østerås, B., Sigmundsson, H., and Haga, M. (2015). Perceived stress and musculoskeletal pain are prevalent and significantly associated in adolescents: an epidemiological cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 15:1081. doi: 10.1186/s12889-015-2414-x
Østerås, B., Sigmundsson, H., and Haga, M. (2016). Pain is prevalent among adolescents and equally related to stress across genders. Scand. J. Pain 12, 100–107. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2016.05.038
Østerås, B., Sigmundsson, H., and Haga, M. (2017). Psychometric properties of the Perceived StressQuestionnaire (PSQ) in Norwegian adolescents. Front. Psychol.
Petersen, C. B., Grønbæk, M., Helge, J. W., Thygesen, L. C., Schnohr, P., and Tolstrup, J. S. (2012). Changes in physical activity in leisure time and the risk of myocardial infarction, ischemic heart disease, and all-cause mortality. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 27, 91–99. doi: 10.1007/s10654-012-9656-z
Price, D. D., McGrath, P. A., Rafii, A., and Buckingham, B. (1983). The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental pain. Pain 17, 45–56. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(83)90126-4
Quaglia, J. T., Braun, S. E., Freeman, S. P., McDaniel, M. A., and Brown, K. W. (2016). Meta-analytic evidence for effects of mindfulness training on dimensions of self-reported dispositional mindfulness. Psychol. Assess. 28, 803–818. doi: 10.1037/pas0000268
Saanijoki, T., Tuominen, L., Tuulari, J. J., Nummenmaa, L., Arponen, E., Kalliokoski, K., et al. (2017). Opioid release after high-intensity interval training in healthy human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 1–9. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.148. [Epub ahead of print].
Sethi, J., Sandhu, J. S., and Imbanathan, V. (2011). Effect of body mass index on work related musculoskeletal discomfort and occupational stress of computer workers in a developed ergonomic setup. Sports Med. Arthrosc. Rehabil. Ther. Technol. 3:22. doi: 10.1186/1758-2555-3-22
Sibley, B. A., Hancock, L., and Bergman, S. M. (2013). University students exercise behavioral regulation, motives, and physical fitness. Percept. Mot. Skills 116, 322–339. doi: 10.2466/06.10.PMS.116.1.322-339
Silva, M. R. O. G. C. M., Badaró, A. F. V., and Dall'Agnol, M. M. (2014). Low back pain in adolescent and associated factors: a cross sectional study with schoolchildren. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 18, 402–409. doi: 10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0051
Skrove, M., Romundstad, P., and Indredavik, M. (2015). Chronic multisite pain in adolescent girls and boys with emotional and behavioral problems: the Young-HUNT study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 503–515. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0601-4
Smith, O. R., Melkevik, O., Samdal, O., Larsen, T. M., and Haug, E. (2017). Psychometric properties of the five-item version of the Mindful Awareness Attention Scale (MAAS) in Norwegian adolescents. Scand. J. Public Health 45, 373–380. doi: 10.1177/1403494817699321
Sugiura, S., Aoki, Y., Toyooka, T., Shiga, T., Otsuki, K., Aikawa, E., et al. (2015). Characteristics of low back pain in adolescent patients with early-stage spondylolysis evaluatedusing a detailed visual analogue scale. Spine 40, E29–E34. doi: 10.1097/BRS.0000000000000657
Sundblad, G. B., Jansson, A., Saartok, T., Renström, P., and Engström, L. M. (2008). Self-rated pain and perceived health in relation to stress and physical activity amongschool-students: a 3-year follow-up. Pain 136, 239–249. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.032
The World Medical Association (WMA) (2013). Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. 64th WMA General Assembly, Fortaleza.
Tittlbach, S. A., Jekauc, D., Schmidt, S. C. E., Woll, A., and Bös, K. (2017). The relationship between physical activity, fitness, physical complaints and BMI in German adults - results of a longitudinal study. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 17, 1090–1099. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2017.1347963
Tremblay, M. S., Warburton, D. E., Janssen, I., Paterson, D. H., Latimer, A. E., Rhodes, R. E., et al. (2011). New Canadian physical activity guidelines. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. 36, 36–46; 47–58. doi: 10.1139/H11-009
Wang, J., Korczykowski, M., Rao, H., Fan, Y., Pluta, J., Gur, R. C., et al. (2007). Gender difference in neural response to psychological stress. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2, 227–239. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsm018
Keywords: adolescents, perceived stress, physical fitness, mindfulness, pain, BMI
Citation: Østerås B, Sigmundsson H and Haga M (2017) Physical Fitness Levels Do Not Affect Stress Levels in a Sample of Norwegian Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 8:2176. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02176
Received: 04 October 2017; Accepted: 29 November 2017;
Published: 13 December 2017.
Edited by:
Adelaida María A. M. Castro Sánchez, University of Almería, SpainReviewed by:
Phyllis Kravet Stein, Washington University in St. Louis, United StatesBárbara Oliván Blázquez, University of Zaragoza, Spain
Copyright © 2017 Østerås, Sigmundsson and Haga. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Berit Østerås, berit.osteras@ntnu.no