Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 26 October 2017
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Self-Regulation in Education and Health View all 23 articles

The Adolescent's Competency for Interacting with Alcohol as a Determinant of Intake: The Role of Self-Regulation

  • 1School of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychology, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales y Humanidades, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Santiago de Chile, Chile
  • 3School of Psychology, Fundación Universitaria Konrad Lorenz, Bogotá, Colombia
  • 4Department of Psychology, Universita Europea di Rome, Rome, Italy

The competency for interacting with alcohol is a highly useful Educational Psychology model for preventing and for understanding the different behavioral levels of this interaction. Knowledge of facts, concepts and principles about alcohol use, self-regulated behavior, and attitudes toward alcohol are predictive of adequate interaction with alcohol. The objective of this study was to empirically evaluate this postulated relationship. A total of 328 Spanish adolescents participated, between the ages of 12 and 17. All were enrolled in 1st–4th year of compulsory secondary education, in the context of the ALADO Program for prevention of alcohol intake in adolescents. An ex post facto design was used, with inferential analyses and SEM analyses. Results show an interdependence relationship, with significant structural prediction between the behavioral levels defined and the level of alcohol intake, with principles, self-regulating control and attitudes carrying more weight. Analyses are presented, as are implications for psychoeducational intervention using preventive programs based on this competency model.

Introduction

Alcohol abuse in adolescents is an old problem (Cortés et al., 2007; Farke and Anderson, 2007; Castellanos-Ryan et al., 2013; Chassin, 2015). The problem stems in part from the firmly entrenched role of alcohol in Western culture. Whether we like it or not, alcohol use is a culturally institutionalized habit in adult social relations; the adolescent simply imitates this. In the period of transition of childhood to adulthood, alcohol use with one's friends is part of the initiation rite in our culture (Ballester et al., 2000; Espada et al., 2008). Social pressure in the case of alcohol is very strong in all spheres of the community, in day-to-day relationships around town, and even in work relationships (Dodge et al., 2009; Zaldívar et al., 2011; Trucco et al., 2014).

In Spain, according to the Ministry of Health and Consumption (Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo, 2005), adolescents beginning at puberty are more willing to engage in risk behaviors such as alcohol use, and at the same time, their relationship to the school context and even their academic achievement are more likely to decline (Bermúdez et al., 2009). Later, a study carried out by the Spanish Observatory on Drugs and Drug Addiction (Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad, 2016; Observatorio Español sobre Drogas, 2017) stated that during 2014, 78.9% of secondary school students between the ages of 14 and 18 years had consumed alcohol regularly during the past month, and initial alcohol intake was established at an average age of 13.8 years. Similarly, the percentage of students who had experienced acute alcohol intoxication in the past month was 33.1%, and binge drinking was practiced by 47.3%.

There is consensus that no one personality type predisposes toward alcoholism, but there are certain important characteristics common to all adolescents who abuse alcoholic drinks, namely, extraversion, nervousness and lack of control. Many studies that have addressed the topic of personality and addiction conclude that some of these characteristics are linked to addictive behavior (Fantín, 2006, 2007; Calvete and Estévez, 2009). Behavior patterns from childhood tend to continue into adolescence; if these habits are inadequate, fewer healthy behaviors and more risk behaviors are produced during adolescence and beyond. Likewise, as age increases, poorer personal adjustment is observed. Stress is a risk factor associated with drug use, and the idiosyncrasies of adolescence involve an increase in stress, something that many studies have agreed on (Windie and Windie, 1996; Nadal, 2008).

There is a great deal of current research on this topic, given that consumption of alcoholic beverages has become popular among adolescents and the age of initiation has gone down (Plan nacional sobre Drogas, 2003). Moreover, when addressing this topic, all related factors must be taken into account (Senra, 2003). The effects of alcohol and drug use during early and mid-adolescence are a real cause for concern, and are related to health problems (Bento et al., 2013), problems at school (Ekberg et al., 2016) mental disorders (Borges et al., 2017), unprotected sex (Boyer et al., 2017), and delinquency (Doherty et al., 2008). As in most Western countries, alcohol use among adolescents in Spain is very high. All this data is evidence of the need to take preventive measures in the spheres of family, school and society (Villatoro et al., 2002).

Limitations of the Classic Models for Preventing Alcohol Intake

Taking the assumptions of a medical approach to primary prevention, traditional strategies for preventing alcohol intake have focused mainly on equipping adolescents with information (Ballesteros et al., 2003). From the bio-psycho-social approach, prior evidence has pointed out the limitations of such an approach, as well as the need to use comprehensive training programs that include self-improvement skills and social skills (Marlatt and Witkiewitz, 2002; De Ridder and de Wit, 2006; Lemstra et al., 2010). Some recent studies from experimental and clinical approaches have reported improvement effects in the adolescents' psychological variables, such as self-esteem and self-efficacy (Alexander and Anderson, 2017), internalizing personality variables (anxiety) and externalizing variables (behavior problems) (Scalco et al., 2014; O'Leary-Barrett et al., 2016; Wills et al., 2016). The role of social identity has also been analyzed, through online contexts (Pegg et al., 2017), and universal programs for preventing alcohol intake in adolescents have been applied (Teesson et al., 2017). Some improvement is seen in all cases, but without conclusive results.

An interactive approach from Educational Psychology, the recent Theory of Self- vs. Externally-Regulated Learning (SRL vs ERL) (de la Fuente, 2015, 2017), has focused on the nature of the adolescent (Oliva et al., 2002; Oliva, 2007), and his/her environment–whether regulatory, a-regulatory or dysregulatory. Regulation is assumed to be an essential procedural component of the competency for managing alcohol intake and other chronic health concerns (Clark et al., 2001; Hull and Slone, 2004; Duell et al., 2016). Thus, the worst scenario for primary prevention would consist of an adolescent with a low level of self-regulation, and a profile of dysregulatory behavior (negative proactivity toward alcohol intake) in interaction with a dysregulatory context (actively encouraging consumption), thereby establishing a high probability of consumption and nonadherence to preventive treatments. In accordance with this theory, the competency model incorporates different behavioral levels in a wholistic fashion, and serves as a model of an Educational Psychology intervention, being distinct from other approaches, as has been illustrated in the case of academic stress (de la Fuente, 2015; de la Fuente et al., 2017).

The Interacting with Alcohol Competency, for Preventing Early Alcohol Intake

An individual may be considered competent when, in a given context, he or she is able to effectively and efficiently solve the problems that arise (Roe, 2002, 2003). From an Educational Psychology point of view, other authors have established that being competent means having the knowledge, skills and attitudes that allow a person to make an appropriate response to a particular, real, problematic situation (de la Fuente et al., 2005). This gives rise to a three-fold division into subcompetencies that are necessary in order to be competent in any field of knowledge: conceptual (knowing), procedural (being able to, knowing how) and attitudinal (wanting to, being). In the specific case of educational prevention of alcohol intake, this approach means delimiting the level of subcompetencies that adolescents possess at each level.

1) Regarding the conceptual subcompetency: adolescents' knowledge and their perception of the risk involved with alcohol is different from that of adults, and this in itself is a factor that predisposes toward some dangerous or inadequate behavior. All epidemiological studies agree that adolescent alcohol use in our day is abusive, and the authors conclude that this comes from adolescents' not understanding the real risks. For this reason it is very important to collect their opinions and what they conceive to be facts, concepts and principles in regard to alcohol. According to earlier research studies, adolescents' knowledge and principles center on reasons to justify the use of alcohol and the consequences of alcohol use (Arias et al., 2007).

2) Regarding the procedural subcompetency: personal self-regulation is an essential meta-skill for managing behavior. de la Fuente et al. (2009) have established the hypothesis of lack of personal self-regulation as a variable that determines alcohol intake in adolescents. The structural factors of personality and self-regulation, among others, are related to prosociality. Difficulties in exercising self-control may be risk factors for drug use (Barry et al., 2007; Calvete, 2008; O'Connor and Colder, 2015). Cognitive schemas with insufficient self-control are associated with drug use, and this cognitive style, where limits and tolerance to frustration are lacking, is quite common in our present-day environment (Urra, 2006). Adolescents who do not have adequate self-regulation do not usually plan their behavior, they have no fixed goals, nor do they monitor to what extent their behavior would move them toward such goals. Rather, they act impulsively, with disturbing results in both the academic and personal/social spheres (Eisenberg et al., 1996; King et al., 2013). A large number of studies on the importance of self-regulation have focused on addictive disorders linked to gambling and substance abuse (Madden et al., 1997; Hull and Slone, 2004), especially to alcohol use (Brown et al., 1999; Carey et al., 2004; Pearson et al., 2013). These effects are especially relevant in adolescence and youth—two stages of development that are characterized by a search for personal identity, moving away from the family context and connecting to one's peer group. Consequently, self-regulation might act as an indicator of adolescents' resilience in situations of greater psychosocial risk (Artuch-Garde et al., 2017).

3) Regarding the attitudinal subcompetency. Attitudes, values and habits toward alcohol intake have been shown to be powerful predictors of adolescents' alcohol intake behavior. Evidence has shown significant relationships between attitudes and values toward alcohol (Moral et al., 2004; Moreno, 2006). Especially important for their value in predicting behavior are alcohol intake habits (Mora-Ríos and Natera, 2006; Rodrigo et al., 2006). See Chart 1.

CHART 1
www.frontiersin.org

Chart 1. Multidimensional nature of the The Interacting with Alcohol Competency model (ALADO Program; de la Fuente et al., 2012).

Adjustment in Interacting with Alcohol

The concept of adjustment in interacting with alcohol is understood to be modulated by age, context and culture. In adolescence, this adjustment can be conceived as the behavior of delaying contact with alcohol in a proactive regulatory manner (de la Fuente, 2017), that is, voluntary nonconsumption of alcohol and substituting it with other more adaptive behaviors, such as sports, leisure activities or having fun without recurring to harmful substances. In other words, adopting the absence of substance use in order to gain a state of bio-psycho-social wellbeing (Becoña, 2007a,b).

Objectives and Hypotheses

This research study has several motivations. On one hand, prior research on alcohol prevention has addressed many variables from a partial standpoint, seeking to establish probabilistic relationships between isolated variables and alcohol use in adolescents. However, an Educational Psychology approach, adopting the conceptions of competency and subcompetency, would seek to integrate the different variables into a more powerful schema. Based on the foregoing, then, the objectives and hypotheses of this study were as follows:

1. To determine any interdependence relationships between the low-medium-high level of each subcompetency with respect to the other subcompetencies, and to adjustment in interacting with alcohol. Consequently, Hypothesis 1 established that the low-medium-high level of conceptual knowledge (facts, concepts and principles, especially the latter), procedural knowledge (self-regulation, with self-control having greater weight) and attitudinal knowledge (values about alcohol intake) will be mutually determined. Finally, they will determine the level of adjustment in interacting with alcohol.

2. To establish any structural predictive relationships between the different subcompetencies. In this case, Hypothesis 2 established that adjustment in interacting with alcohol (nonconsumption) will be determined by conceptual type factors (more principles than facts/concepts), procedural type (self-regulation, especially level of control) and attitudinal type, although the latter are mediated by procedural variables.

Methods

Participants

The population under study were students from public secondary schools in a southern province of Andalusia (Spain). In order to include different types of schools in this investigation, schools were selected from three types of urban areas: (1) center of town, from a medium-high social stratum (n = 106), (2) surrounding neighborhoods, from a medium-low social stratum (n = 101) and (3) outlying, marginalized population areas with compensatory education, from a low social stratum (n = 121). The homeroom teachers from every group participated voluntarily in the experiment, having been invited by the School Psychology adviser of the local Teacher Development Center. Participating students were between the ages of 12 and 17 years [12 (n = 51), 13 (n = 80), 14 (n = 97), 15 (n = 55), 16 (n = 35), and 17 (n = 10)] and were enrolled in compulsory secondary education (grades 7–10) at one of three public secondary schools. This age range was selected for methodological purposes, making it possible to form two groups, the 12- to 14-year-olds (n = 207), corresponding to the first stage of adolescence (puberty, or early adolescence), and the 15- to 17-year-olds (n = 121), corresponding to the second stage of adolescence (adolescence per se). The final sample size from which all measurements were taken contained 328 subjects. Of these, 178 were male (54.3% of the sample) and 150 were female (45.7% of the sample). The mean age of the sample was 13.82 years, with a standard deviation de 1.19.

Instruments

Conceptual Subcompetency

The scale Evaluación de los Hechos, Conceptos y Principios sobre el Alcohol, EHCP [Assessment of facts, concepts and principles about alcohol, AFCP] was used (Cubero and Sánchez, 2009a). The scale is composed of 38 items concerning the effects of alcohol use; psychometric analyses of this scale show reliability (alpha = 0.827) and consistent construct validity, with three factors: knowledge of facts, concepts, and principles concerning alcohol. Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) showed KMO = 0.801; Bartlett's Sphericity Test (df = 703) = 2767.595; p < 0.001. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) showed adequate indicators for the Default model: χ2 = 1612.957, Degrees of freedom (779–117): 662, p < 0.001; all the variances are significant for p < 0.001; NFI = 0.865; RFI = 0.848; IFI = 0.914; TLI = 0.902; CFI = 0.913; RMSEA = 0.025; HOELTER model = 1095 (p < 0.05),1138 (p < 0.01). See Figure 1 and Appendix A.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of EHCP scale. H, Facts; C, Concepts; P, Principles.

Procedural Subcompetency

The SRQ, Self-Regulation Questionnaire (Brown et al., 1999) was used, in its 21-item abbreviated Spanish version, SRQ-21 (de la Fuente, 2010). Its reliability (alpha = 0.826) and validity values are consistent, with two dimensions, planning and action control. Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) showed an index KMO = 0.985; Bartlett's Sphericity Test (df = 210) = 3603.882; p < 0.001. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) showed adequate indicators for the Default model: Chi-square = 408.448, Degrees of freedom (252–64):188, p < 0.001; all the variances are significant for p < 0.001; NFI = 0.894; RFI = 0.870; IFI = 0.940; TLI = 0.925; CFI = 0.9393; RMSEA = 0.022. However, this structure does not concur with others found in other samples (Artuch-Garde et al., 2017; Pichardo et al., in review). See Figure 2 and Appendix A.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of the SRQ-21 scale.

Attitudinal Subcompetency

The scale for Evaluación de las Actitudes ante el alcohol, EAA [Assessment of Attitudes toward Alcohol, AAA] was used (Cubero and Sánchez, 2009b). A total of eight items assess attitudes and values toward alcohol (Alpha = 0.845). Exploratory Factor Analysis (EFA) showed KMO = 0.859; Bartlett's Sphericity Test (df = 28) = 529.335, p < 0.001. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) showed adequate indicators for the Default model: Chi-square = 58.574, Degrees of freedom (44–24): 20, p < 0.001; all the variances are significant for p < 0.001; NFI = 0.903; RFI = 0.826; IFI = 0.34; TLI = 0.8; CFI = 0.2; RMSEA = 0.0; HOELTER model = 1349. See Figure 3 and Appendix A.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of Attitudes scale.

Adjusted Behavior in Interacting with Alcohol

We used the Escala de Ajuste en la interacción con el alcohol [Scale of Adjustment in interacting with alcohol], which contains four items (Alpha = 0.915). This scale belongs to the Inventario de Evaluación de conocimientos, actitudes e interacción con el alcohol (Cubero and Sánchez, 2009c) (Inventory for Assessment of knowledge, attitudes and interaction with alcohol). See Appendix A.

Procedure

The Alado Program is an online program (de la Fuente et al., 2012), designed for adolescents, to work on the three levels of subcompetencies previously established in the model. The program provides assessment and intervention in different matters of learning. It may be used by students, teachers and parents, but in this case it was applied only to students. The Teacher's Guide and Instructions for Use have been published.

Data collection instruments were applied over the course of the school year 2009-10, within the framework of the Alado Project of Excellence (2007–2010), through an online utility created for this purpose (www.alado.es). Specifically, these data correspond to the project baseline, having been collected in September-October, 2008. Previously, cooperation had been requested from the Teacher Development Center, from the students' parents and from the School Board, for their participation. The project was approved by the University Bioethics Commission (University of Almería). The students participated voluntarily. The parents were informed in writing. As the participants in the Project were minors, both the parents and school administrators gave written informed consent for the study. All data was collected in accordance with the principles of the Psychologist's Deontological Code and the Spanish Data Protection Act.

Design and Data Analyses

For this set of data, obtained at the program baseline, an ex-post facto methodological design was used. Relationships were measured just as they occurred in the natural environment, without any treatment, using inferential and structural analyses based on the Presage-Process-Product Model (Biggs, 2001), and applied to educational problem areas in students in compulsory secondary education. The variables selected were as follows:

1) Presage variables, manipulated by selection: age and year in school,

2) Process variables, referring to intake prevention subcompetencies,

1. Conceptual subcompetency: Knowledge (facts, concepts and principles) related to alcohol intake.

2. Procedural subcompetency: Personal self-regulation skills, referring to planning and behavioral action control.

3. Attitudinal subcompetency: Attitudes and values toward alcohol intake.

3) Product variables, referring to interaction with alcohol,

4. Adequate behavior in interaction with alcohol: no contact.

Inferential statistical analyses (multivariate analysis, ANOVAs) were carried out using SPSS (v. 23.0) for Windows. For levels of the independent variable self-regulation, cluster analysis was used, obtaining three levels: low, medium and high. AMOS (v. 23.0) for Windows was used for the structural validity analysis of each inventory and for constructing the structural prediction model. To interpret the CFA and SEM model fit, we focused on the comparative fit index (CFI) and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). CFI values equal to or more than 0.90 and 0.95 respectively were taken to indicate acceptable and close fit to the data (McDonald and Marsh, 1990). RMSEA values equal to or below 0.05 and 0.08 were taken to indicate close and acceptable levels of fit, respectively (Jöreskog and Sörbom, 1993). Keith (2006) proposed the following educational research benchmarks for direct effects in the form of beta coefficients: less than 0.05 is considered too small to be meaningful, above 0.05 is small but meaningful, above 0.10 is moderate, and above 0.25 is large. For indirect effects, we use Kenny's (2012) definition of an indirect effect as the product of two effects; using Keith's benchmarks above, we propose an educationally meaningful, small indirect effect = 0.003, moderate = 0.01, and large = 0.06.

Results

Interdependence Relationships

Effect of the Level of Conceptual Competency (Facts, Concepts, Principles) on the Remaining Variables: KNOWING

A main effect appeared, statistically significant, of the IV level of conceptual competence or knowledge [F(8, 210) = 2.451 (Pillai), p < 0.01, n2 = 0.085, power = 0.897]. Partial effects were shown in attitude [F(2, 110) = 5.974, p < 0.01, n2 = 0.100, power = 0.872; post = 3 > 2,1, p < 0.01] and in adjustment in alcohol consumption [F(2, 2110) = 3.003, p < 0.05, n2 = 0.050, power = 0.576]. No significant effect appeared for the variables planning and control (components of self-regulation). See Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Direct values (means and sd) of the IV low-medium-high level of conceptual competency on the remaining variables.

Effect of the Level of Procedural Competency (Self-Regulation) on the Remaining Variables: KNOWING HOW

A statistically significant main effect appeared for the IV level of self-regulation [F(10, 208) = 3.236 (Pillai), p < 0.001, n2 = 0.135, power = 0.987]. Partial effects were a significant effect of level of self-regulation on knowledge of facts [F(2, 107) = 3.316, p < 0.05, n2 = 0.050, power = 0.617; post = 3 < 1, p < 0.05], on knowledge of concepts [F(2, 107) = 3.160, p < 0.05, n2 = −056, power = 0.595; post = 3 < 1, p < 0.05], on knowledge of principles [F(2, 107) = 4.162, p < 0.01, n2 = 0.072, power = 0.723; post = 3 > 1, p < 0.05] and on attitudes [F(2, 107) = 10.825, p < 0.001, n2 = 0.168, power = 0.989; post = 3 > 1, p < 0.05]. Finally, a significant effect appeared in the variable adjustment in alcohol use [F(2, 107) = 3.360, p < 0.05, n2 = 0.086, power = 0.689; post = 3 > 1, p < 0.05]. See Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Direct values (means and sd) of the low-medium-high level of procedural competency (self-regulation) on the remaining variables.

Effect of the Level of Attitudinal Competency (Attitudes) on the Remaining Variables: WANTING

A main effect appeared, statistically significant, in the IV attitude level [F(12, 206) = 4.378 (Pillai), p < 0.001, n2 = 0.203, power = 1.00]. Partial effects showed a significant effect of the level of attitude toward alcohol on knowledge of facts [F(2, 107) = 5.858, p < 0.01, n2 = 0.099, power = 0.865; post = 1,2 > 3, p < 0.05], on knowledge of concepts [F(2, 107) = 3.419, p < 0.05, n2 = 0.060, power = 0.631; post = 1 > 3 p < 0.05], and on knowledge of principles [F(2, 107) = 11.644, p < 0.001, n2 = 0.179, power = 0.993; post = 3,2 > 1, p < 0.01]. Also, the level of attitude toward alcohol appeared with a significant partial effect on the degree of self-regulatory control [F(2, 107) = 8.835, p < 0.001, n2 = 0.142, power = 0.968; post = 3,2 > 1, p < 0.001]. Finally, a significant effect appeared in the variable adjustment in alcohol use, but without clear directionality [F(2, 107) = 2.285, p < 0.06, n2 = 0.050, power = 0.543]. See Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Direct values (means and sd) of the low-medium-high level of attitudinal competency on the remaining variables.

Structural Prediction Relationships

The results of structural analysis or pathway analysis (SEM) showed an acceptable model of relationships between variables (model 3). The relationship parameters of both models are set out below. See Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Structural Models or pathway analysis (SEM).

Standardized Direct Effects

This predictive linear model establishes that latent variable Knowledge of facts (F) was a significant positive predictor (0.63) of the latent variable Knowledge of concepts (C), and this variable is positively predicted (0.63) for latent variable knowledge of principles (P). In addition, the facts (F) variable (0.63) and the principles variable (P) are positively predicted (0.28) of latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT). Complementarily, the latent variable planning of behavior (PL) was a significant predictor (0.36) of the latent variable attitudes (A). At the same time, self-control of behavior (CONTR) was a positively predictor (0.18) of latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT). Complementarily, the latent variable attitudes (A) predicted a positive and significant relationship (0.20) with the latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT). All the variances of errors were significant (p < 0.001). Table 5 shows the direct effects of the variables inherent in the model.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardized Direct Effects (Default model).

Standardized Indirect Effects

The model also contributed the existence of multiple indirect predictions among the variables. This predictive linear model establishes that latent variable knowledge of facts (F) was a positive significant predictor (0.440) of the latent variable knowledge of principles (P), and a positive predictor (0.124) of latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT). The latent variable knowledge of concepts was a positive predictor (0.192) of the latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT). The latent variable knowledge of principles was a positive predictor (0.072) of the latent variable interaction with alcohol (INT).

The latent variable knowledge of facts (F) was another indirect, positive predictor of items of C, P, and INT; the latent variable knowledge of concepts (C) was an indirect, positive predictor of items of C, P, and INT; the latent variable knowledge of principles (P) was indirect, positive predictor of items of INT.

The procedural and latent variable planning of behavior (P) was an indirect, positive predictor of items of attitudes (A) and interaction with alcohol (INT); the procedural and latent variable self-control of behavior (CONTR) was an indirect, positive predictor of items of interaction with alcohol (INT).

Complementarily, the attitudinal and latent variable attitudes toward alcohol (A) was an indirect, positive predictor of items of interaction with alcohol (INT). See Table 6.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Standardized Indirect Effects (Default model).

Graphic representation of the structural model

The final model is graphically represented in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Graphic representation of the structural model. F, Facts; C, Concepts; P, Principles; PLAN: Planification; CONTR: Control; A: Attitudes: INTERACC: interaction with alcohol.

Discussion and Conclusions

The first objective of this study was to determine any interdependence relationships between the low-medium-high level of each subcompetency with respect to the other subcompetencies, and to adjustment in interacting with alcohol. Thus, Hypothesis 1 established that the low-medium-high level of conceptual knowledge (facts, concepts and especially principles), procedural knowledge (self-regulation, with self-control having greater weight) and attitudinal knowledge (values about alcohol intake) would be mutually determined, and that finally, these levels would determine the level of adjustment in interacting with alcohol.

This hypothesis was partially confirmed. Results provided evidence that the level of conceptual competence (facts, concepts, principles) determines the level of attitudes toward alcohol and the degree of adjustment in interacting with alcohol, that the level of procedural competence (self-regulation) determines the level of the principles and attitudes, and that the level of attitudinal competency (attitudes) differentially and inversely affects the level of knowledge about alcohol (facts and concepts), while directly affecting the degree of principles and of self-control. These results offer support for the competency model, by showing how each type of learning affects the others. One especially interesting result was that concept learning affected the degree of attitude acquisition, but not the degree of acquisition of self-control. However, the level of self-regulation did affect the learning of principles and attitudes toward alcohol. This represents more evidence on the importance of this behavioral level, corroborating that attitudes also affect the acquisition of principles and the degree of self-control. The second part of the hypothesis was also partially validated, showing that only the level of conceptual competence (principles) was accompanied by a higher level of adjustment. This result reinforces the idea that each variable analyzed, on its own, has a limited effect in determining the level of adjustment.

The second objective was to establish any structural prediction relationships between the different subcompetencies, with Hypothesis 2 affirming that adjustment in interacting with alcohol (nonconsumption) would be determined both by conceptual type factors (more principles than facts/concepts), procedural type (self-regulation, especially level of control) and attitudinal type, although the latter are mediated by the procedural variables. In this case, the hypothesis was consistently fulfilled, with the appearance of a significant SEM model that showed different direct effects. Knowledge of facts affects concepts and these in turn affect the principles elaborated; knowledge of facts and of principles are the variables that most affect adjustment in interacting with alcohol. In addition, self-regulation affects adequate interaction via attitudes, and directly through self-control. Finally, attitudes also affect adjustment. However, the indirect effects are what has shown that all the levels of competencies—conceptual (facts, concepts and principles), procedural (planning and self-control) and attitudinal (attitudes)—are joint determinants in the degree of adjustment for nonconsumption of alcohol. These results are consistent with the prior evidence that has shown in part the importance of each of these variables (Moral and Ovejero, 2005; Moral et al., 2006).

From a behavioral point of view, both hypotheses show that different acquisitions are required in order to become competent in alcohol intake: (1) the acquisition of knowing (cognitive level): to have assimilated the information about alcohol and its effects on the immature adolescent brain (Spear, 2015); (2) the acquisition of wanting to not drink (motivational-affective level), of attitudes and values (Foxcroft et al., 2015; O'Connor and Colder, 2015); (3) the acquisition of knowing how (behavioral level): to have skills and meta-skills of self-regulated behavior (O'Connor and Colder, 2015; Yoon et al., 2015). This three-fold acquisition of integrated behavior, on the part of adolescents, having three different levels of complexity, has usually been addressed only partially and separately, from a psychosocial intervention approach.

From a complementary perspective, this investigation also makes a significant methodological contribution, with respect to the type of analyses carried out. Going beyond the usual descriptive or classic association strategies, a two-fold strategy was applied in order to validate the causal relations consistently, using inferential analysis and structural path analysis. The inferential analysis has shown clear interdependence relationships between the levels of the variables. Path analysis, based on the previous inferential analyses, has corroborated the multidirectionality of the proposed causations, producing an empirical model of this competency consistent with the one proposed, and offering a structural foundation for future interventions (MacCallum and Austin, 2000; Asparouhov and Muthén, 2014).

Implications for Educators and Programs

This empirical evidence suggests different implications for educators who wish to make students competent, by working in primary prevention: (1) The concept of competence is multidimensional (Gagné, 2013); therefore, teaching and learning should work on all the levels of learning involved. (2) While information about alcohol is important–especially information that helps students construct an integrated, explanatory model of the effects of alcohol–excess information and inadequate modeling at a young age can be counterpreventive, with a dysregulatory effect. One notable effect was the inverse predictive relationship between adolescents' level of self-regulation and their degree of knowledge about alcohol. From the assumptions of the SRL vs. ERL Theory, this could be interpreted as an effect of a regulatory vs. dysregulatory context. In other words, adolescents who are growing up in a regulatory educational context (urban school and parents with secondary education) are trained to a higher level of self-regulation and they are protected from environments with excessive information about alcohol. The opposite would occur in dysregulatory educational contexts (marginalized school with compensatory education), where adolescents are allowed to have more direct contact with alcohol consumption, and early alcohol intake, or dysregulation, is encouraged. These results are consistent with the effect of the educational context at school, as seen in a previous dissertation report (Marcos, 2013), but research should continue to investigate this in more depth. (3) In addition, this evidence reaffirms the role and importance of planning for adolescents to learn self-regulation and self-discipline (Duckworth et al., 2011) – in association with proactive attitudes of substance rejection, since such attitudes are decisive to whether self-regulation and self-discipline will be practiced. Therefore, from the educational point of view of primary prevention, the experiences, information, modeling and attitudes offered by the educational context (family, school, peers) are determinants in encouraging regulation, nonregulation or dysregulation of the adolescent's alcohol intake (de la Fuente, 2017).

These results corroborate the idea that educational programs focusing on the primary prevention of alcohol intake should rest on two basic pillars: (1) an eminently informational thrust, for learning facts and concepts about the problem (also containing a developmental component); and (2) the more developmental thrust, referring to learning behavioral principles, behavior management skills or self-regulation, and the motivational or attitudinal aspect. Conceptual, procedural and competence would be impregnated with a holistic model of education, approaching each level of competency as different and needing to be approached differently. This question therefore deserves some profound analysis. In the first place, one must reinforce the importance of principles in alcohol use—norms, standards and goals for behavior (Pons et al., 1995). Without behavioral principles, we cannot determine the direction of self-regulation. It is therefore unadvisable to move toward preventive programs that have a large amount of merely conceptual information; rather, principles of behavior should be emphasized, as representations of reality and of oneself, especially in the stage of adolescence and pre-adolescence. In second place, training in self-regulatory behaviors—as a procedural meta-skill—is a must in educational programs for primary prevention of alcohol intake, given that this behavioral level has a clear effect on adjustment, as has been demonstrated in prior evidence from secondary and tertiary therapeutic programs (Fernández et al., 2009). Finally, there is no substitute for educational treatment of the attitudinal level. Attitudes must involve a desire for healthful behavior (Fergus and Zimmerman, 2005; García del Castillo and Días, 2007).

Limitations and Prospects

This study is not free of limitations. First, the effect of gender and of year in school were not analyzed, in an effort to streamline the design and show the relationships presented here, given that these effects were reported in an earlier research report (Marcos, 2013). However, the most important limitation is the absence of any analysis of the students' contextual variables (Smith et al., 2013) as is explained in the recent Theory of Self- vs. External-Regulation™ (de la Fuente, 2015, 2017). Without an interactive view of this problem, we obtain only a partial, personalist view. Future research should take into account the context, be it regulatory, nonregulatory or dysregulatory, in order to consider its likely impact in interaction with each adolescent's competency level (Leal, 2004; Márquez, 2006).

In the future, it would be desirable to perform research that incorporates the effect of other variables that recent research has identified as important in managing stress—an important aspect in adolescence, with all its personal, academic and social changes (González et al., 2002; Gómez-Fraguela et al., 2006). Likewise, longitudinal studies are needed, for prevention and for assessments (Arco and Fernández, 2002; Oliva et al., 2008). This report comes alongside many others that are presently being carried out with regard to adolescence (Casas, 2000; Ruiz-Aranda et al., 2006). Similarly, self-regulation is also the object of much current interest, as a psychological variable that is inherent to personal development competencies in subjects. Results obtained here indicate that it is worthwhile to continue down this road, given the power of this variable, in order that it may be given priority in planning future interventions (de la Fuente et al., 2009; Artuch-Garde et al., 2017).

Author Contributions

JdlF: Research design, data analysis and results writing; IC: Coordinator of the R & D Project and the Program; MS: Research of the R & D Project and the Program; FP: Program implementation and data collection; AG: Manuscript Review and Writing; JF: Writing and review in English.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

ALADO Project (2008-2010). Neurobiological risk factors and alcohol use in adolescents: educating for prevention. A pilot experience in results transfer to an at-risk population. Reference: P06-cts-01350. Junta de Andalucía, Andalusian (Spain). Ministerio de Economia y Competitividad Grant PSI2015-64460-R (Spain).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01800/full#supplementary-material

References

Alexander, J., and Anderson, K. G. (2017). Association between drink refusal self-efficacy, self-esteem, and drinking reduction efforts in a brief prevention program for adolescents. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 237A–237A.

Arco, J. L., and Fernández, A. (2002). Por qué los programas de prevención no previenen. [Why prevention programs do not prevent.] Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 2, 209–226.

Google Scholar

Arias, B., Morentin, R., Ovejero, A., and Calleja, M. F. (2007). Elaboración de un instrumento para evaluar las creencias y percepciones de riesgo de los adolescentes sobre el uso del alcohol. [Development of an instrument to assess adolescents' beliefs and perceptions of risk about alcohol use.] Revista espa-ola de Drogodependencias, 32, 559–582.

Artuch-Garde, R., González-Torres, M. C., de la Fuente, J., Vera, M. M., Fernández-Cabezas, M., and López-García, M. (2017). Relationship between resilience and self-regulation: a study of spanish youth at risk of social exclusion. Front. Psychol. 8:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asparouhov, T., and Muthén, B. (2014). Multiple-group factor analysis alignment. Struct. Equat. Model. 21, 495–508. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.919210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballester, R., Gil, M. D., and Guirado, M. C. (2000). Comportamientos y actitudes relacionadas con el consumo de alcohol en adolescentes de 15 a 17 a-os. [Behaviors and attitudes related to alcohol use in adolescents from 15-17 years of age.] Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 26, 855–895.

Google Scholar

Ballesteros, J., Ariño, J., González-Pinto, A., and Querejeta, I. (2003). Eficacia del consejo médico para la reducción del consumo excesivo de alcohol. Metaanálisis de estudios espa-oles en atención primaria. [Effectiveness of medical advice for reducing excessive alcohol use. Meta-analysis of Spanish studies on primary care.] Gaceta Sanitaria 17, 116–122. doi: 10.1016/S0213-9111(03)71708-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, C. T., Grafeman, S. J., Adler, K. K., and Pickard, J. D. (2007). The relations among narcissism, self-esteem, and delinquency in a sample of at-risk adolescents. J. Adolesc. 30, 933–942. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2006.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becoña, E. (2007a). Resiliencia y consumo de drogas: una revisión. [Resilience and drug use: a review.] Revista de Socidrogalcohol 19, 89–101.

Becoña, E. (2007b). Bases psicológicas de la prevención del consumo de drogas. [Psychological bases for the prevention of drug use.] Papeles Psicól. 28, 11–20.

Bento, M. M. C., Ferreira, C. M. C. B., Chaves, J. C., and Duarte, C. (2013). Conhecimento Sobre Sida e Sexualidade dos Adolescents [Knowledge of AIDS and Sexuality in Adolescents]. Instituto Politénico de Viseu. Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/10400.19/1976

Bermúdez, M., de la, P., Teva, I., and Buela-Casal, G. (2009). Influencia de variables sociodemográficas sobre los estilos de afrontamiento, el estrés social y la búsqueda de sensaciones sexuales en adolescentes. [Influence of sociodemographic variables on coping styles, social stress and the search for sexual thrills in adolescents.] Psicothema 21, 220–226.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (2001). Teaching for Quality Learning at University, 3rd Edn. Buckingham: Open University Press.

Borges, G., Benjeta, C., Orozco, R., Medina-Mora, M. E., and Menendez, D. (2017). Alcohol, cannabis and other drugs and subsequent suicide ideation and attempt among young Mexicans. J. Psychiatr. Res. 91, 74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyer, C. B., Grenberg, L., Chutuape, K., Walker, B., Monte, D., Kirk, J., et al. (2017). Exchange of sex for drugs or money in adolescents and young adults: an examination of sociodemographic factors, HIV-related risk, and community context. J. Commun. Health 42:90. doi: 10.1007/s10900-016-0234-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. M., Miller, W. R., and Lawendowski, L. A. (1999). “The self-regulation questionnaire,” in Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book, Vol. 17, eds L. Vandecreek and T. L. Jackson (Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press), 281–293.

Google Scholar

Calvete, E. (2008). Justification of violence and grandiosity schemas as predictors of antisocial behavior in adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 37, 108–1095 doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9229-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvete, E., and Estévez, A. (2009). Consumo de drogas en adolescentes: el papel del estrés, la impulsividad y los esquemas relacionados con la falta de límites. [Drug use in adolescents: the role of stress, impulsivity and schemas related to lack of limits.] Adicciones 21, 49–56. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, K. B., Neal, D. J., and Collins, S. E. (2004). A psychometric analysis of the self-regulation questionnaire. Addict. Behav. 29, 253–260. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casas, F. (2000). La adolescencia: retos para la investigación y para la sociedad europea de cara al siglo XXI. [Adolescence: challenges for research and for European society in the 21st century.] Anuario de Psicol. 31, 5–14.

Google Scholar

Castellanos-Ryan, N., Parent, S., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., and Séguin, J. R. (2013). Pubertal development, personality, and substance use: a 10-year longitudinal study from childhood to adolescence. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 122, 782–796. doi: 10.1037/a0033133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chassin, L. (2015). “Self-regulation and adolescent substance use,” in Self-Regulation in Adolescence, eds G. Oettingen and P. M. Gollwitzer (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 266–275.

Google Scholar

Clark, N. M., Gong, M., and Kaciroti, N. (2001). A Model of self-regulation for control of chronic disease. Health Educ. Behav. 28:769. doi: 10.1177/109019810102800608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortés, M. T., Espejo, B., and Jiménez, J. A. (2007). Características que definen el fenómeno del botellón en universitarios y adolescentes. [Characteristics that define the phenomenon of the mass drinking party (botellón) in university students and adolescents.] Revista de Socidrogalcohol 19, 357–372. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.295

CrossRef Full Text

Cubero, I., and Sánchez, M. C. (2009a). Evaluación de Los Hechos, Conceptos y Principios Sobre el Alcohol, EHCP [Assessment of Facts, Concepts and Principles About Alcohol, AFCP]. Almería: University of Almería.

Cubero, I., and Sánchez, M. C. (2009b). Escala Para la Evaluación de las Actitudes Ante el Alcohol, EAA [Scale for Assessment of Attitudes Toward Alcohol, AAA]. Almería: University of Almería.

Cubero, I., and Sánchez, M. C. (2009c). Escala de Ajuste en la Interacción Con el Alcohol [Scale of Adjustment in Interacting with Alcohol]. Almería: University of Almería

de la Fuente, J. (2010). Cuestionario de Autorregulación, CAR-21 Items. Versión castellana [Spanish version of the Self-Regulation Questionnaire, SRQ-21 items.] Almería: Universidad de Almería.

de la Fuente, J. (2015). Competence of Learning, Study and Performance under Stress (CLSPS) Model. Self-help Guide for University Students, Graduates and Professional Examination Candidates. Almería: Education & Psychology I+D+I, e-Publishing Series I+D+I.

de la Fuente, J. (2017). The theory of self- vs. externally-regulated learningtm: fundamentals, evidence, and applicabilty. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Fernández-Cabezas, M., Cambil, M., Vera, M. M., González-Torres, M. C., and Artuch-Garde, R. (2017). Linear relationship between resilience, learning approaches, and coping strategies to predict achievement in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:1039. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Justicia, F., Casanova, P. F., and Trianes, M. V. (2005). Perceptions about the construction of academic and professional competencies in psychologists. Electr. J. Res. Educat. Psychol. 3, 3–34.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta, F. J., and Sánchez, M. D. (2009). Autorregulación personal y percepción de los comportamientos escolares desadaptativos. [Personal self-regulation and perception of maladaptive school behaviors.] Psicothema, 21, 548–554.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta, Sánchez-Roda, M. D., Cubero, I., and Sánchez-Amate, M. C. (2012). Programa ALADO. Guía de Intervención. [Program ALADO. Intervention Guide Almería]. Education and Psychology I+D+i, e-Publishing Series I+D+i. Available online at: http://www.mitienda.investigacion-psicopedagogica.org/english/seccion.php?idsec=15&idcat=9

De Ridder, D., and de Wit, J. (eds.) (2006). Self-Regulation in Health Behavior. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Dodge, K. A., Malone, P. S., Lansford, J. E., Miller, S., Pettit, G. S., and Bates, J. E. (2009). A dynamic cascade model of the development of substance-use onset. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 74. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2009.00528.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doherty, E. E., Green, K. M., and Ensminger, M. E. (2008). Investigating the long-term influence of adolescent delinquency on drug use initiation. Drug Alcohol Depend. 93, 72–84. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation strategies improve self-discipline in adolescents: benefits of mental contrasting and implementation intentions. Educ. Psychol. 31, 17–26. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2010.506003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duell, N., Steinberg, L., Chein, J., Al-Hassan, S. M., Bacchini, D., Lei, C., et al. (2016). Interaction of reward seeking and self-regulation in the prediction of risk taking: a cross-national test of the dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1593–1605. doi: 10.1037/dev0000152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M., and Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of children's dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Dev. Psychol. 32, 195–209. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.2.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekberg, M. S., Fonseca, L., Anderberg, M., and Dahlberg, M. (2016). Ungdomar med missbruksproblem och deras uppfattning om skolproblem. [Adolescents with substance abuse problems and their perception of school problems.] Nordic Stud. Educ. 35, 266–278. doi: 10.18261/issn.1891-5949-2016-04-03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Espada, J. P., Pereira, J. R., and García-Fernández, J. M. (2008). Influencia de los modelos sociales en el consumo de alcohol de los adolescentes. [Influence of social models in alcohol use in adolescents.] Psicothema 20, 531–537.

Google Scholar

Fantín, M. B. (2006). Perfil de personalidad y consumo de drogas en adolescentes escolarizados. [Personality profile and drug use in adolescents enrolled in school.] Adicciones 18, 285–292. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fantín, M. B. (2007). Personalidad, variables familiares y biológicas como factores de riesgo en el consumo de sustancias en el adolescente. [Personality, family and biological variables as risk factors for substance use in the adolescent.] Revista de Psicopatología y salud mental del ni-o y del adolescente 53–61.

Farke, W., and Anderson, P. (2007). Binge drinking in Europe. Revista de Socidrogalccohol 19, 333–339.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fergus, S., and Zimmerman, M. A. (2005). Adolescent resilience: a framework for understanding, healthy development in the face of risk. Annu. Rev. Public Health 26, 339–419. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.26.021304.144357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernández, B., Jorge, V., and Bejar, E. (2009). Función protectora de las habilidades emocionales en la prevención del consumo de tabaco y alcohol: una propuesta de intervención. [Protective function of emotional skills in preventing tobacco and alcohol use: an intervention proposal.] Psicooncología 6, 243–256.

Foxcroft, D. R., Coombes, L., Wood, S., Allen, D., Almeida-Santimano, N. M., and Moreira, M. T. (2015). Motivational interviewing for the prevention of alcohol misuse in young adults. J. Subst Abuse Treat. 51, 1–18. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD007025.pub4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, R. M. (ed.). (2013). Instructional Technology: Foundations. New York, NY: Routledge.

García del Castillo, J. A., and Días, P. (2007). Análisis relacional entre los factores de protección, resilencia, autorregulación y consumo de drogas. [Relational analysis between the factors of protection, resilience, self-regulation and drug use.] Salud y Drogas 7, 309–332.

Google Scholar

Gómez-Fraguela, J. A., Luengo-Martín, A., Romero-Tri-anes, E., Villar-Torres, P., and Sobral-Fernández, J. (2006). Estrategias de afrontamiento en el inicio de la adolescencia y su relación con el consumo de droga y la conducta problemática. [Coping strategies in early adolescence and their relationship to drug use and problem behavior.] Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 6, 581–597.

Google Scholar

González, R., Montoya, I., Casullo, M. M., and Bernabéu, J. (2002). Relación entre estilos y estrategias de afrontamiento y bienestar psicológicos en adolescentes. [Relationship between coping styles and strategies and psychological wellbeing in adolescents.] Psicothema 14, 363–368.

Google Scholar

Hull, J. G., and Slone, L. B. (2004). “Alcohol and self-regulation,” in Handbook of self-regulation: Research, Theory, and Applications, eds R. F. Baumeister and K. D. Vohs (New York, NY: Guildford Press), 466–491.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Jöreskog, K. G., and Sörbom, D. (1993). LISREL 8: Structural Equation Modeling with the SIMPLIS Command Language. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International.

Google Scholar

Keith, T. Z. (2006). Multiple Regression and Beyond. Boston, MA: Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Kenny, D. (2012). April 3 Effect Size of the Indirect Effect. Available online at: http://davidakenny.net/cm/mediate.htm

King, K. M., Lengua, L. J., and Monahan, K. C. (2013). Individual differences in the development of self-regulation during pre-adolescence: connections to context and adjustment. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 41:57. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9665-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leal, E. R. (2004). Adolescentes y alcohol: la búsqueda de sensaciones en un contexto social y cultural que fomenta el consumo. [Adolescents and alcohol: the search for thrills in a social and cultural context that fosters consumption.] Apuntes de Psicología 22, 403–420.

Google Scholar

Lemstra, M., Bennett, N., Nannapaneni, U., Neudorf, C., Warren, L., Kershaw, T., et al. (2010). A systematic review of school-based marijuana and alcohol prevention programs targeting adolescents aged 10–15. Addict. Res. Theory 18, 84–96. doi: 10.3109/16066350802673224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., and Austin, J. T. (2000). Applications of structural equation modeling in psychological research. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51, 201–226. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madden, G. J., Petry, N. M., Badger, G. J., and Bickel, W. K. (1997). Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control patients: drug and monetary rewards. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 5, 256–262. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcos, E. (2013). Competencia de interacción con el alcohol en adolescents: efectos de una e-Intervención. [Competence of Interaction with Alcohol in Adolescents: Effects of An E-Intervention.] Doctoral Dissertation. Universidad de Almería: Servicio de Publicaciones. Available online at: http://www.diegomarin.net/ual/es/4__tesis-doctorales-edicion-electronica

Marlatt, G. A., and Witkiewitz, K. (2002). Harm reduction approaches to alcohol use: health promotion, prevention, and treatment. Addict. Behav. 27, 867–886. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(02)00294-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Márquez, M. L. (2006). Influencia de la familia y de otros contextos de desarrollo y aprendizaje en los estilos de vida del adolescente. [Influence of the family and other developmental and learning contexts on adolescent lifestyles.] Cultura y Educación 18, 331–333. doi: 10.1174/113564006779172984

CrossRef Full Text

McDonald, R. P., and Marsh, H. W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: noncentrality and goodness of fit. Psychol. Bull. 107, 247–255. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.247

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad. (2016). Informe 2016: Alcohol, tabaco y drogas ilegales en Espa-a. [Report 2016: Alcohol, tobacco and ilegal drugs in Spain.] Madrid: Centro de Publicaciones del Ministerio.

Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo (2005). Informe Annual del Sistema Nacional de Salud [Annual Report of the National Health System]. Available online at: https://www.msssi.gob.es/organizacion/sns/informeAnualSNS/docs/general2005/informeSNS2005ParteGeneralCompleta.pdf

Moral, M. V., and Ovejero, A. (2005). Un programa de intervención psicosocial para la mejora de habilidades sociales de adolescentes consumidores de alcohol y otras sustancias psicoactivas. [A psychosocial intervention program for improving social skills in adolescents who use alcohol and other psychoactive substances.] Apuntes de Psicología 23, 3–26.

Google Scholar

Moral, M. V., Rodríguez, F. J., and Sirvent, C. (2006). Factores relacionados con las actitudes juveniles hacia el consumo de alcohol y otras sustancias psicoactivas. [Factors related to juvenile attitudes toward use of alcohol and other psychoactive substances.] Psicothema 18, 52–58.

Google Scholar

Moral, M. V., Sirvent, C., Ovejero, A., and Rodríguez, F. J. (2004). Comparación de la eficacia preventiva de programas de intervención psicosocial sobre las actitudes hacia el consumo juvenil de sustancias psicoactivas. [Comparison of the preventive effectiveness of psychosocial intervention programs on attitudes toward juvenile consumption of psychoactive substances.] Trastornos Adictivos 6, 248–261. doi: 10.1016/S1575-0973(04)70170-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mora-Ríos, J., and Natera, G. (2006). Expectativas, consumo de alcohol y problemas asociados en estudiantes universitarios de la ciudad de México. [Expectations, alcohol use and associated problems in university students in Mexico City.] Salud Pública de México 43, 89–96. doi: 10.1590/S0036-36342001000200002

CrossRef Full Text

Moreno, J. (2006). Valores, actitudes hacia el alcohol y consumo en adolescentes varones. [Values, attitudes toward alcohol and alcohol use in male adolescents.] Límite. Revista de Filosofía y Psicología 1, 195–211.

Google Scholar

Nadal, R. (2008). La búsqueda de sensaciones y su relación con la vulnerabilidad a la adicción y el estrés. [The search for thrills and its relationship to vulnerability to addiction and stress.] Adicciones 20, 59–72. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.289

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Observatorio Español sobre Drogas (2017). Informe de la Situación y Tendencias de Losproblemas de Drogas en Espa-a [Report on the situation and trends of drug problems in Spain.] Madrid: Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo.

O'Connor, R. M., and Colder, C. R. (2015). The prospective joint effects of self-regulation and impulsive processes on early adolescence alcohol use. J. Stud. Alcohol Drugs 76, 884–894. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2015.76.884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Leary-Barrett, M., Castellanos-Ryan, N., Pihl, R. O., and Conrod, P. J. (2016). Mechanisms of personality-targeted intervention effects on adolescent alcohol misuse, internalizing and externalizing symptoms. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 84:438. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliva, A. (2007). Desarrollo cerebral y asunción de riesgos durante la adolescencia. [Brain development and risk-taking in adolescence.] Apuntes de Psicol. 25, 239–254.

Google Scholar

Oliva, A., Parra, A., and Sánchez-Queija, I. (2002). Relaciones con padres e iguales como predictores del ajuste emocional y conductual durante la adolescencia. [Relationships with parents and peers as predictors of emotional and behavioral adjustment during adolescence.] Apuntes de Psicol. 20, 225–242.

Oliva, A., Parra, A., and Sánchez-Queija, I. (2008). Consumo de sustancias durante la adolescencia: trayectorias evolutivas y consecuencias para el ajuste psicológico. [Substance use during adolescence: developmental trajectories and consequences for psychological adjustment.] Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 8, 153–169.

Google Scholar

Pearson, M. R., Kite, B. A., and Henson, J. M. (2013). Predictive effects of good self-control and poor regulation on alcohol-related outcomes: do protective behavioral strategies mediate? Psychol. Addict. Behav. 27, 81–89. doi: 10.1037/a0028818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pegg, K. J., O'Donnell, A. W., Lala, G., and Barber, B. L. (2017). The role of online social identity in the relationship between alcohol-related content on social networking sites and adolescent alcohol use. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0665. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plan nacional sobre Drogas (2003). Encuesta Sobre Drogas a la Población Escolar 2002. [A survey of the school-enrolled population about drugs, 2002.] Madrid: Delegación del Gobierno para el Plan Nacional sobre Drogas.

Pons, J., Molpeceres, M. A., and Beriano, E. (1995). Exploración de las motivaciones asociadas al abuso de alcohol en adolescentes: análisis discriminante en función de los valores. [Exploring the motivations associated with alcohol abuse in adolescents: discriminant analysis as a function of values.] Revista espa-ola de Drogodependencias 20, 197–213.

Rodrigo, M. J., Márquez, M. L., García, M., Medina, A., Martínez, M. A., and Martín, J. C. (2006). La influencia de las características personales y contextuales en los estilos de vida en la adolescencia: aplicaciones para la intervención en contextos de riesgo psicosocial. [The influence of personal and contextual characteristics on adolescent lifestyles.] Anuario de Psicología 37, 259–276.

Roe, R. A. (2002). Competences- A key towards the integration of theory and practice in work psychology. Gedrag en Organisatie 15, 203–224.

Roe, R. A. (2003). ¿Qué hace competente a un psicólogo? [What makes a psychologist competent?] Papeles del Psicólogo 24, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Ruiz-Aranda, D., Fernández-Berrocal, P., Cabello, R., and Extremera, N. (2006). Inteligencia emocional percibida y consumo de tabaco y alcohol en adolescentes. [Perceived emotional intelligence and tobacco and alcohol use in adolescents.] Ansiedad y Estrés 12, 223–230.

Scalco, M. D., Colder, C. R., Hawk, L. W. Jr., Read, J. P., Wieczorek, W. F., and Lengua, L. J. (2014). Internalizing and externalizing problem behavior and early adolescent substance use: a test of a latent variable interaction and conditional indirect effects. Psychol. Addict. Behav. 28, 828–840. doi: 10.1037/a0035805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Senra, M. (2003). Etiología del consumo de alcohol en la adolescencia: análisis de diversos enfoques y teorías [Etiology of alcohol use in adolescence: an analysis of different approaches and theories] Revista Espa-ola de orientación y psicopedagogía 14, 83–99.

Google Scholar

Smith, A. R., Chein, J., and Steinberg, L. (2013). Impact of socio-emotional context, brain development, and pubertal maturation on adolescent risktaking. Horm. Behav. 64, 323–332. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2015). Adolescent alcohol exposure: are there separable vulnerable periods within adolescence? Physiol. Behav. 148, 122–130. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teesson, M., Newton, N., Slade, T., Carragher, N., Barrett, E., Champion, K., et al. (2017). Combined universal and selective prevention for adolescent alcohol use: a cluster randomized controlled trial. Psychol. Med. 47, 1761–1770. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trucco, E. M., Colder, C. R., Wieczorek, W. F., Lengua, L. J., and Hawk, L. W. (2014). Early adolescent alcohol use in context: how neighborhoods, parents, and peers impact youth. Dev. Psychopathol. 26, 425–436. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urra, J. (2006). El arte de educar [The art of educating]. Madrid: La Esfera de los Libros.

Villatoro, J., Medina-Mora, M. E., Rojano, C., Fleiz, C., Bermúdez, P., Castro, P., et al. (2002). ¿Ha cambiado el consumo de drogas de los estudiantes? Resultados de la encuesta de estudiantes. Medición oto-o del 2000. [Has the student's drug use changed? Results of the student survey. Fall 2000 measurement.]. Salud Mental, Vol. 25, 43–54. Available online at: http://new.medigraphic.com/cgi-bin/resumen.cgi?IDARTICULO=17608

Wills, T. A., Simons, J. S., Sussman, S., and Knight, R. (2016). Emotional self-control and dysregulation: a dual-process analysis of pathways to externalizing/internalizing symptomatology and positive well-being in younger adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend. 163, S37–S45. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Windie, M., and Windie, R. C. (1996). Coping strategies, drinking motives, and stressful life events among middle adolescents. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 551–560. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.4.551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, S., Lam, W. W., Sham, J. T., and Lam, T. H. (2015). Learning to drink: how Chinese adolescents make decisions about the consumption (or not) of alcohol. Int. J. Drug Policy 26, 1231–1237. doi: 10.1016/j.drugpo.2015.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zaldívar, F., López-Ríos, F., García-Montse, J. M., and Molina-Moreno, A. (2011). Self-reported consumption of alcohol and other drugs in the university population. Elect. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 9, 113–132.

Keywords: interacting with alcohol competency, self-regulation, attitudes, adolescence, prevention program

Citation: de la Fuente J, Cubero I, Sánchez-Amate MC, Peralta FJ, Garzón A and Fiz Pérez J (2017) The Adolescent's Competency for Interacting with Alcohol as a Determinant of Intake: The Role of Self-Regulation. Front. Psychol. 8:1800. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01800

Received: 08 August 2017; Accepted: 28 September 2017;
Published: 26 October 2017.

Edited by:

Douglas F. Kauffman, Boston University School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Jesús Nicasio García Sánchez, Universidad De León, Spain
Ana Miranda, Universitat de València, Spain

Copyright © 2017 de la Fuente, Cubero, Sánchez-Amate, Peralta, Garzón and Fiz Pérez. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús de la Fuente, amZ1ZW50ZUB1YWwuZXM=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.