- 1College of Healthcare Sciences, James Cook University, Townsville, QLD, Australia
- 2Faculty of Health Science, Curtin University, Perth, WA, Australia
- 3Leiden University Medical Centre, Leiden, Netherlands
- 4Faculty of Health Science, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia
- 5School of Allied Health, Australian Catholic University, Sydney, NSW, Australia
Introduction: Standardized assessments are widely used by speech pathologists in clinical and research settings to evaluate the language abilities of school-aged children and inform decisions about diagnosis, eligibility for services and intervention. Given the significance of these decisions, it is important that assessments have sound psychometric properties.
Objective: The aim of this systematic review was to examine the psychometric quality of currently available comprehensive language assessments for school-aged children and identify assessments with the best evidence for use.
Methods: Using the PRISMA framework as a guideline, a search of five databases and a review of websites and textbooks was undertaken to identify language assessments and published material on the reliability and validity of these assessments. The methodological quality of selected studies was evaluated using the COSMIN taxonomy and checklist.
Results: Fifteen assessments were evaluated. For most assessments evidence of hypothesis testing (convergent and discriminant validity) was identified; with a smaller number of assessments having some evidence of reliability and content validity. No assessments presented with evidence of structural validity, internal consistency or error measurement. Overall, all assessments were identified as having limitations with regards to evidence of psychometric quality.
Conclusions: Further research is required to provide good evidence of psychometric quality for currently available language assessments. Of the assessments evaluated, the Assessment of Literacy and Language, the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-5th Edition, the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool: 2nd Edition and the Preschool Language Scales-5th Edition presented with most evidence and are thus recommended for use.
Introduction
Language impairment refers to difficulties in the ability to comprehend or produce spoken language relative to age expectations (Paul and Norbury, 2012a). Specific language impairment is defined when the language impairment is not explained by intellectual, developmental or sensory impairments1 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; World Health Organisation, 2015). Specific Language Impairment is estimated to affect 2–10% of school-aged children with variation occurring due to using different diagnostic criteria (Dockrell and Lindsay, 1998; Law et al., 2000; Lindsay et al., 2010). While there is active debate over terminology and definitions surrounding this condition (Ebbels, 2014), according to Bishop (2011), these children present with “unexplained language problems” that require appropriate diagnosis and treatment because of their increased risk of long-term literacy difficulties (Catts et al., 2008; Fraser and Conti-Ramsden, 2008), social-emotional difficulties (Conti-Ramsden and Botting, 2004; McCormack et al., 2011; Yew and O'Kearney, 2013) and poorer academic outcomes (Dockrell and Lindsay, 1998; Conti-Ramsden et al., 2009; Harrison et al., 2009).
Language assessments are used for a range of purposes. These include: initial screening, diagnosis of impairment, identifying focus areas for intervention, decision-making about service delivery, outcome measurement, epidemiological purposes and other research pursuits that investigate underlying cognitive skills or neurobiology (Tomblin et al., 1996; Shipley and McAfee, 2009; Paul and Norbury, 2012b). Whilst few formal guidelines exist, current literature identifies that speech pathologists should use a range of assessment approaches when making judgments about the spoken language abilities of school-aged children, such as: standardized assessment, language-sampling, evaluation of response-to-intervention, dynamic assessment, curriculum-based assessment and caregiver and teacher reports (Reed, 2005; Bishop and McDonald, 2009; Caesar and Kohler, 2009; Friberg, 2010; Hoffman et al., 2011; Haynes and Pindzola, 2012; Paul and Norbury, 2012c; Eadie et al., 2014). Nonetheless, standardized assessments are a widely used component of the assessment process (Hoffman et al., 2011; Spaulding et al., 2012; Betz et al., 2013), particularly for determining if an individual meets diagnostic criteria for Language Impairment (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; World Health Organisation, 2015) and determining eligibility for services (Reed, 2005; Spaulding et al., 2006; Wiig, 2010). Standardized assessments are also designed to be easily reproducible and consistent, and as a result are also widely used in research (Tomblin et al., 1996; Betz et al., 2013).
Language assessments used in clinical practice and research applications must have evidence of sound psychometric properties (Andersson, 2005; Terwee et al., 2012; Betz et al., 2013; Dockrell and Marshall, 2015). Psychometric properties include the overarching concepts of validity, reliability and responsiveness (Mokkink et al., 2010c). This data is typically established by the developers of assessments and are often reported in the administration manuals for individual assessments (Hoffman et al., 2011). When data on psychometric properties is lacking, concerns may arise with the use of assessment results to inform important clinical decisions and the accuracy of reported outcome data in research (Friberg, 2010).
Previous studies have identified limitations with regards to the psychometric properties of spoken language assessments for school-aged children (McCauley and Swisher, 1984; Plante and Vance, 1994; Andersson, 2005; Spaulding et al., 2006; Friberg, 2010). An earlier study published in 1984 (McCauley and Swisher, 1984) examined the manuals of 30 speech and language assessments for children in relation to ten psychometric criteria. These criteria were selected by the authors and included description and size of normative sample, selection of items, normative data provided, concurrent and predictive validity, reliability and description of test administration. The appraisal indicated that only 20% of the 30 examined assessments met half of the criteria with the most assessments meeting only two of the ten criteria. A decade later this information was updated through another study (Plante and Vance, 1994) examining the manuals of pre-school language assessments using the same ten criteria. In this later study, 38% of the 21 examined assessments met half the criteria with most assessments meeting four of the ten criteria.
More recently, literature has focussed on diagnostic accuracy (sensitivity and specificity). Although this information is often lacking in child language assessments, some authors have suggested that diagnostic accuracy should be a primary consideration in the selection of diagnostic language assessments, and have applied the rationale of examining diagnostic accuracy first when evaluating assessments (Friberg, 2010). A study published in 2006 (Spaulding et al., 2006) examined the diagnostic accuracy of 43 language assessments for school-aged children. The authors reported that 33 assessment manuals contained information to calculate mean differences between children with and without language impairment. While nine assessments included sensitivity and specificity data in the manual, only five of these assessments were determined by the authors as having an acceptable level of sensitivity and specificity (80% or higher). In another study published in 2010 (Friberg, 2010), an unspecified number of assessment manuals were examined with nine assessments identified as having an acceptable level of sensitivity and specificity. These nine assessments were then evaluated using 11 criteria based on a modification of the ten criteria used in earlier studies (McCauley and Swisher, 1984; Plante and Vance, 1994). No assessments were found to meet all 11 of the psychometric criteria, however all assessments met 8–10 criteria. The findings from these studies suggest that, while the psychometric quality of assessments appears to have improved over the last 30 years, assessments of children's language may still require further development to improve their psychometric quality.
No previous reviews investigating the psychometric properties of language assessments for children were systematic in identifying assessments for review or included studies published outside of assessment manuals. This is important for two reasons, first, to ensure that all assessments are identified, and second, to ensure that all the available evidence for assessments, including evidence of psychometric properties that was published in peer reviewed journals, is considered when making overall judgments. Previous reviews have also lacked a method of evaluating the methodological quality of the studies selected for review. When evaluating psychometric properties, it is important to consider not only outcomes from studies, but also the methodological quality of studies. If the methodological quality of studies is not sound, then outcomes of studies cannot be viewed as providing psychometric evidence (Terwee et al., 2012). In addition, many of the assessments reviewed in previous studies have since been superseded by newer editions. Older editions are often not printed once new editions are released; therefore, an updated review is needed to examine the evidence for assessments that are currently available to speech-pathologists.
In the time since previous reviews of child language assessments were conducted, research has also advanced considerably in the field of psychometric evaluation (Polit, 2015; Mokkink et al., 2016). In 2010, the Consensus Based Standards for the Selection of Health Status Measurement Instruments (COSMIN) taxonomy (http://www.cosmin.nl) was developed through a Delphi study including fifty-seven international experts from disciplines including psychometrics, epidemiology and clinimetrics (Mokkink et al., 2010b,c). COSMIN aims to improve the selection of health-related measurement instruments by clinicians and researchers through the provision of evidence-based tools for use when appraising studies examining psychometric quality (Mokkink et al., 2016). This includes provision of a checklist (http://www.cosmin.nl/COSMIN%20checklist.html) for rating the methodological quality of studies examining psychometric properties (Terwee et al., 2012). The COSMIN taxonomy and checklist has been utilized in a large number systematic reviews (http://www.cosmin.nl/images/upload/files/Systematic%20reviews%20using%20COSMIN.pdf); however, has not yet been applied in the evaluation of the methodological quality of children's language assessments.
The COSMIN taxonomy describes nine measurement properties relating to domains of reliability, validity and responsiveness. Table 1 provides an overview and definition of all the COSMIN domains and measurement properties (Mokkink et al., 2010c). As the terminology in COSMIN is not always consistent with terms used throughout literature (Terwee et al., 2015), examples of terms that may be used across different studies is also given in this Table.
Table 1. COSMIN domains, psychometric properties, aspects of psychometric properties and similar terms based on Mokkink et al. (2010c).
Study Aim
The aim of this study was to systematically examine and appraise the psychometric quality of diagnostic spoken language assessments for school-aged children using the COSMIN checklist (Mokkink et al., 2010b,c). Specifically, this study aimed to collect information on the overall psychometric quality of assessments and identify assessments with the best evidence of psychometric quality.
Methods
Selection Criteria
Assessments selected for inclusion in the review were standardized norm-referenced spoken language assessments from any English-speaking country with normative data for use with mono-lingual English-speaking children aged 4–12 years. Only the most recent editions of assessments were included. Initial search results indicated 76 assessments meeting this criterion. As it was not possible to review such a large number of assessments, further exclusion criteria were applied. Assessments were excluded if they were not published within the last 20 years. It is recognized that norm-referenced assessments should only be used with children whose demographics are represented within the normative sample (Friberg, 2010; Paul and Norbury, 2012b; Hegde and Pomaville, 2013); therefore the use of assessments normed on populations from several decades ago may be questionable with current populations. Screening assessments were excluded as they are designed to identify individuals who are at risk or may require further diagnostic assessment (Reed, 2005; Paul and Norbury, 2012b) and thus have a different purpose to diagnostic assessments. Similarly, assessments of academic achievement were also excluded, as although they may assess language ability, this occurs as part of a broad purpose of assessing literacy skills for academic success (Wiig, 2010).
For diagnosis of Specific Language Impairment using standardized testing, previous research has recommended the use of composite scores that include measures of both comprehension and production of spoken language across three domains: word (semantics), sentence (morphology and syntax) and text (discourse) (Tomblin et al., 1996; Gillam et al., 2013). While phonology and pragmatics may also be assessed, these areas are not typically considered part of the diagnostic criteria for identifying Specific Language Impairment (Tomblin et al., 1996). While some evidence suggests that children's language skills may not be contrastive across modalities of comprehension and production (Tomblin and Zhang, 2006; Leonard, 2009), current literature conceptualizes language in this way (Wiig, 2010; World Health Organisation, 2015). A recent survey of SLP's in the United States also identified that “comprehensive” language assessments that assess multiple language areas are used more frequently than assessments that assess a single domain or modality (Betz et al., 2013). As comprehensive assessments provide a broad picture of a child's language strengths and weaknesses, these assessments are often selected first, with further examination of specific domains or modalities conducted if necessary (Betz et al., 2013; Dockrell and Marshall, 2015).
Given the support in literature for the use of comprehensive assessments in diagnostics and the wide use of these assessments by speech pathologists, it was identified that a review of comprehensive language assessments for school-aged children is of particular clinical importance. Therefore, assessments were included in this study if they were the latest edition of a language assessment with normative data for monolingual English speaking children aged 4–12 years; were published within the last 20 years; were primarily designed as a diagnostic assessment; and were designed to assess language skills across at least two of the following three domains of spoken language: word (semantics), sentence (syntax/morphology) and text (discourse).
Sources of Information
The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines were developed through consensus of an international group to support high quality reporting of the methodology of systematic reviews (Moher et al., 2009) and were thus used to guide this review. Language assessments were identified through database searches and through comprehensively searching publisher websites, speech pathology websites and textbooks. A flowchart outlining sources of information is contained in Figure 1.
Database searches of PubMed, CINAHL, PsycINFO, and Embase were conducted between February and March 2014. Database searches were conducted with subject headings or mesh terms to identify relevant articles up until the search date. Free text word searches were also conducted for the last year up until the search date to identify recently published articles not categorized in subject headings. The search strategies are described in Table 2.
Assessments were also identified from searches of websites and textbooks. Speech pathology association websites from English speaking countries were searched and one website, the American Speech and Hearing Association, was identified as having an online directory of assessments. The website for this directory was identified as being no longer available as of 30/01/16. Publisher websites were identified by conducting Google searches with search terms related to language assessment and publishing and by searching the publisher sites from assessments already identified. These search terms are listed in Table 2. From these methods, a total of 43 publisher websites were identified and searched. Textbooks were identified from Google searches related to language assessment and the contents of recently published books searched. Three recently published textbooks (Kaderavek, 2011; Paul and Norbury, 2012b; Hegde and Pomaville, 2013) were identified as having lists of language assessments, which were then searched for assessments not already identified.
Published articles relating to psychometric properties of selected assessments were identified through additional database searches conducted between December 2014 and January 2015 using PubMed, CINAHL, Embase, PsycINFO, and HaPI. Searches were conducted using full names of assessments as well as acronyms; and limited to articles written in English and published in or after the year the assessment was published. Articles were included in the psychometric evaluation if they related to one of the selected assessments, contained information on reliability and validity and included children speaking English as a first language in the study. Google Scholar, OpenGrey (http://www.opengrey.eu) and PsycEXTRA® (http://www.apa.org/pubs/databases/psycextra/) were also searched for gray literature. Search terms are contained in Table 2.
All retrieved articles were reviewed for inclusion by two reviewers independently using selection criteria, with differences in opinion settled by group discussion to reach consensus. All appropriate articles up until the search dates were included.
Study Selection
Across all searches, a total of 1,395 records were retrieved from databases and other sources. The abstracts for these records were reviewed and 1,145 records were excluded as they were not related to language assessment for mono-lingual English-speaking children aged 4–12 years. The full text versions of remaining records were then reviewed and 225 records were excluded as they did not provide information on the 15 selected assessments, did not contain information on the reliability and validity of selected assessments, did not examine the study population, or were unpublished or unable to be located. Records were also excluded if they were not an original source of information on the reliability and validity of selected assessments. For example, articles reviewing results from an earlier study or reviewing information from an assessment manual were not included if they did not contain new data from earlier studies. A total of 22 records were identified for inclusion, including 15 assessment manuals and 7 articles. Figure 1 represents the assessment and article selection process using a PRISMA flowchart.
Data Collection Process and Data Synthesis
Studies selected for inclusion in the review were rated on methodological quality using COSMIN with the outcome from studies then rated against criteria based on Terwee et al. (2007) and Schellingerhout et al. (2011). Studies for each measurement property for each assessment were then combined to give an overall evidence rating for each assessment using criteria based on Schellingerhout et al. (2011). This methodology is similar to methodology used in previous systematic reviews examining the other health related measurement instruments (Schellingerhout et al., 2011; Uijen et al., 2012; Vrijman et al., 2012).
The four point COSMIN checklist (http://www.cosmin.nl/images/upload/files/COSMIN%20checklist%20with%204-point%20scale%2022%20juni%202011.pdf) was used for rating methodology (Terwee et al., 2012). This checklist provides a system for rating each of the nine COSMIN measurement properties (internal consistency, reliability, measurement error, content validity, structural validity, hypothesis testing, cross-cultural validity, criterion validity and responsiveness). Interpretability can also be measured but is not considered a psychometric property (Mokkink et al., 2009). Each COSMIN measurement property is assessed on 5–18 items that rate the standard of methodological quality using an “excellent,” “good,” “fair,” or “poor” rating scale (Terwee et al., 2012). Items vary depending on the property being rated; however, most properties include ratings for reporting and handling of missing information, sample size, design flaws and type of statistical analysis. There are also property specific items; for example, time interval, patient stability and similarities in testing conditions are rated for test-retest reliability studies.
Different methods for scoring the COSMIN 4-point checklist are employed in studies examining the methodology of psychometric studies. One suggested method is a “worst rating counts” system, where each measurement property is given the score of the item with the lowest rating (Terwee et al., 2012). The advantage of this method over other methods, such as giving a “mean score” for each measurement property, is that serious flaws cannot be compensated for by higher scores on other items (Terwee et al., 2012). However, the “worst rating counts” system is severe as an assessment needs only one “poor” rating to be “poor” for a given measurement property and must receive all “excellent” scores to be rated “excellent” for a measurement property. Previous studies (Speyer et al., 2014) have also identified that this method lacks the ability to distinguish “better” assessments when all reviewed assessments have limitations leading to poor ratings on some items.
In this current study, the scores for each item were “averaged” to give an overall rating for each measurement property. This provides information on the methodological quality in general for studies that were rated. In the scoring process, the appropriate measurement properties were identified and a rated on the relevant items. The options for “excellent,” “good,” “fair,” and “poor” on the 4-point checklist were ranked numerically, with “excellent” being the highest score and “poor” being the lowest score. As the current version of the COSMIN 4 point scale was designed for a “worst rating counts” method, some items do not have options for “fair” or “poor.” Therefore, this was adjusted for in the percentage calculation so that the lowest possible option for each item was considered a 0 score. As each measurement property has a different number of items or may have items that are not applicable to a particular study, the number of items rated may differ across measurement properties or across studies. Therefore, overall scores for each measurement property rated from each study were calculated as a percentage of points received compared to total possible points that a study could have received for that measurement property. The resulting percentages for each measurement property were then classified according to quartile, that is: “Poor” = 0–25%, “Fair” = 25.1–50%, “Good” = 50.1–75%, and “Excellent” = 75.1–100% (Cordier et al., 2015). Where a measurement property was rated “excellent” or “good” overall but had a “poor” score at item level for important aspects such as sample size or statistical analysis, this was noted so that both quantitative scores depicting overall quality and descriptive information about specific methodological concerns may be considered when viewing results.
The findings from studies with “fair” or higher COSMIN ratings were subsequently appraised using criteria based on Terwee et al. (2007) and Schellingerhout et al. (2011). These criteria are described in Table 3. Because the COSMIN ratings were averaged to give a rating of overall quality and Table 3 rates studies against specific methodological criteria, it is possible for studies with good COSMIN ratings in to be rated as indeterminate from Table 3.
Table 3. Criteria for measuring quality of findings for studies examining measurement properties based on Terwee et al. (2007) and Schellingerhout et al. (2011).
Overall evidence ratings for each measurement property for each assessment were then determined by considering available evidence from all the studies. These ratings were assigned based on quality of methodology available studies (as rated using COSMIN) and the quality of the findings from the studies (as defined in Table 3). This rating scale was based on criteria used by Schellingerhout et al. (2011) and is outlined in Table 4.
Table 4. Level of evidence for psychometric quality for each measurement property based on Schellingerhout et al. (2011).
To limit the size of this review, selected assessments were not appraised on the measurement property of responsiveness, as that would have significantly increased the size of the review. Interpretability is not considered a psychometric property and was also not reviewed. However, given the clinical importance of responsiveness and interpretability, it is recommended that these properties be a target for future research. Cross-cultural validity applies when an assessment has been translated or adapted from another language. As all the assessments reviewed in this study were originally published in English, cross-cultural validity was not rated. However, it is acknowledged that the use of English language assessments with the different dialects and cultural groups that exist across the broad range of English speaking countries is an area that requires future investigation. Criterion validity was also not evaluated in this study as this measurement property refers to a comparison of an assessment to a diagnostic “gold-standard” (Mokkink et al., 2010a). Consultation with experts and reference to current literature (Tomblin et al., 1996; Dollaghan and Horner, 2011; Betz et al., 2013) did not identify a “gold-standard” or an industry recognized “reference standard” for diagnosis of language impairment, therefore all studies comparing one assessment to another assessment were considered convergent validity and rated as hypothesis testing according to COSMIN.
Diagnostic accuracy, which includes sensitivity and specificity and positive predictive power calculations, is an area that does not clearly fall into a COSMIN measurement property. However, current literature identifies this as being an important consideration for child language assessment (Spaulding et al., 2006; Friberg, 2010). In this review, data from studies examining diagnostic accuracy was collated in a Table 9 to allow for this information be considered alongside information on COSMIN measurement properties. It should be noted that these studies were not rated for methodological quality, as the COSMIN checklist was not identified as providing an appropriate rating scale for these types of studies. However, descriptive information on the methodological quality of these studies was commented upon in the results section.
Where several studies examining one measurement property were included in a manual, one rating was provided based on information from the study with the best methodology. For example, if a manual included internal consistency studies using different populations then a rating for internal consistency was given based on the study with the most comprehensive or largest sample size. The exception was for reliability, where test-retest and inter-rater reliability were rated separately and hypothesis testing where convergent validity and discriminant validity were rated separately. In most cases, these different reliability and hypothesis testing studies were conducted using different sample sizes and different statistical analyses. As it was considered that manuals that include both these studies for each measurement property are providing evidence across different aspects of the measurement property, it was decided that counting these as different studies would allow this to be reflected in final data.
Some assessments also included studies for hypothesis testing examining gender, age and socio-cultural differences. Whilst this information contributes important information on an assessment's usefulness, we identified convergent validity and discriminant validity as key aspects for the measurement property of hypothesis testing and thus only included these studies in this review.
Risk of Bias
All possible items for each assessment were rated from all identified publications. Where an examination of a particular measurement property was not reported in a publication or not reported with enough detail to be rated, this was rated as “not reported” (NR). Two raters were involved in appraising publications. To ensure consistency, both raters involved in the study trained as part of a group prior to rating the publications for this study. The first rater rated all publications with a random sample of 40% of publications also rated independently by a second rater. Inter-rater reliability between the two raters was calculated and determined to be adequate (weighted Kappa = 0.891; SEM = 0.020; 95% confidence interval = 0.851–0.931). Any differences in opinion were discussed and the first rater then appraised the remaining 60% of articles applying rating judgments agreed upon after consensus discussions.
Results
Assessments Selected for Review
A total of 22 publications were identified for inclusion in this review. These included 15 assessment manuals and seven journal articles relating to a total of 15 different assessments. From the 22 publications, 129 eligible studies were identified, including three studies that provided information on more than one of the 15 selected assessments. Eight of these 129 studies reported on diagnostic accuracy and were included in the review, but were not rated using COSMIN, leaving 121 articles to be rated for methodological quality. Of the 15 selected assessments, six were designed for children younger than 8 years and included the following assessments: Assessment of Literacy and Language (ALL; nine studies), Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals: Preschool-2nd Edition (CELF:P-2; 14 studies), Reynell Developmental Language Scales-4th Edition (NRDLS; six studies), Preschool Language Scales-5th Edition (PLS-5; nine studies), Test of Early Language Development-3rd Edition (TELD-3; nine studies) and Test of Language Development-Primary: 4th Edition (TOLD-P:4; nine studies). The Test of Language Development-Intermediate: 4th Edition (TOLD-I:4; nine studies) is designed for children older than 8 years. The remaining eight assessments covered most of the 4–12 primary school age range selected for this study and included the following assessments: Assessment of Comprehension and Expression (ACE 6-11; seven studies), Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language (CASL; 12 studies), Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-5th Edition (CELF-5; nine studies), Diagnostic Evaluation of Language Variance-Norm Referenced (DELV-NR; ten studies), Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities-3rd Edition (ITPA-3; eight studies), Listening Comprehension Test-2nd Edition (LCT-2; seven studies), Oral and Written Language Scales-2nd Edition (OWLS-2; eight studies) and Woodcock Johnson 4th Edition Oral Language (WJIVOL; six studies). These 15 selected assessments are summarized in Table 5 with regards to author, publication date and language area assessed.
During the selection process, 61 assessments were excluded as not meeting the study criteria. These assessments are summarized in Table 6 with regards to author, publication date, language area assessed and reason for exclusion.
The seven identified articles were sourced from database searches and gray literature. These included studies investigating structural and convergent validity (hypothesis testing) of the CASL (Reichow et al., 2008; Hoffman et al., 2011), convergent validity (hypothesis testing) using the CELF-P:2 and the DELV-NR (Pesco and O'Neill, 2012), convergent validity (hypothesis testing) of the CELF-P:2 (Kaminski et al., 2014), convergent validity (hypothesis testing) of the TELD-3 (Spaulding, 2012), diagnostic accuracy of the CELF-P (Eadie et al., 2014), and internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the CASL pragmatic judgment subtest (McKown et al., 2013). All articles appeared to be have been published by authors independent of the developers of the assessments. The seven included articles are described in Table 7.
The assessment manuals for all the selected assessments were not available through open sources and were only accessible by purchasing the assessment. Only three published articles by authors of assessments were identified. One of these contained information on the development, standardization and psychometric properties of the NRDLS (Letts et al., 2014). This study was not included in this review as it was published after the assessment manual and contained no new information. Similarly, another article by the developers of the NRLDS (Letts et al., 2013) examined the relationship between the NRDLS scores and economic status. This study was also reported in the manual and was not included. One other study by Seymour and Zurer-Pearson (2004) described the rationale and proposed structure for the DELV-NR assessment; however, this study was also not included as it did not contain information on the psychometric properties of the final version of the assessment.
Psychometric Evaluation
The results of the COSMIN ratings of the psychometric quality of the 15 assessments are listed in Table 8. Thirteen of the 15 assessment manuals included studies on the six COSMIN measurement properties evaluated in this review. One assessment (NRDLS) presented no examination of structural validity and another assessment (WJIVOL) did not have a reliability study using the subtests that primarily contribute to overall composite language scores. Manuals that contained studies with more than one reliability study i.e., inter-rater or test-retest reliability were given a rating for each type of reliability. Similarly, manuals with more than one study of hypothesis testing i.e., convergent or discriminant validity were given more than one ratings for hypothesis testing. This is noted in Table 7 with two ratings for reliability and hypothesis testing where multiple studies were identified.
Table 8. Ratings of methodological quality and study outcome of reliability and validity studies for selected assessments.
Ratings for each measurement property are shown as percentage of total points available and classified according to quartile in which percentage falls: Excellent (Excell) = 100–75.1, Good = 75–50.1, Fair = 50–25.1, and Poor = 25–0. The rating of measurement properties based on percentages of all items allows for the overall quality of a study be considered, however it also means that it was possible for studies to be rated “excellent” or “good” overall when individual items may have been rated “poor” for methodology. The footnotes in Table 8 indicate where studies were rated “excellent,” “good,” or “fair” overall, but were identified as having a “poor” rating for important items, such as: uni-dimensionality of the scale not checked prior to internal consistency calculation; sample size not stated or small; type of statistical analysis used unclear or inappropriate statistical analysis according to COSMIN; error measurement calculated using Cronbach's Alpha or split-half reliability method; time interval between assessment administrations not deemed appropriate; internal consistency calculated using split-half reliability; or correlations between subtests reported for structural validity rather than factor analysis.
Studies with COSMIN ratings of “fair” or higher were then rated on the evidence provided in the study outcome for each measurement property using the criteria as summarized in Table 3. These results are reported in Table 8 underneath the methodological rating for each assessment. As COSMIN ratings represent the overall methodological quality of assessments and outcome ratings rate studies against specific methodological criteria, it is possible for studies with good COSMIN ratings to be rated as indeterminate for study outcome due to the presence of specific but significant flaws.
The overall rating given after considering the methodological quality and outcome of all available studies (Table 8) is provided in Table 9.
Table 9. Level of evidence for each assessment based on Schellingerhout et al. (2011).
For seven assessments, studies examining diagnostic accuracy were identified. This information came from the respective manuals and one article. Data on sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive power and negative predictive power for these seven assessments are presented in Table 10. With regards to the assessments reviewed in this study, sensitivity and specificity indicates the percentage of children with language impairment identified by the assessment as having language impairment (sensitivity) and the percentage of children with no language impairment identified as having no language impairment (specificity). Higher values indicate higher diagnostic accuracy, with literature suggesting that values between 90 and 100% (0.90–1.00) indicate “good” accuracy and values between 80 and 89% (0.80–0.89) indicate “fair” accuracy (Plante and Vance, 1994; Greenslade et al., 2009). Predictive power indicates how precise an assessment is in predicting children with language impairment (Positive Predictive Power or PPP) and children without language impairment (Negative Predictive Power or NPP) for different cut-off scores against a pre-determined prevalence base rate. Higher predictive values indicate better precision in predictive power.
It should be noted that whilst these results from diagnostic accuracy studies are reported without being rated for methodological quality, significant methodological concerns were noted and are reported in the discussion section of this study.
Discussion
Methodological Quality of Studies
In this study, a total of 121 studies across all six measurement properties were rated for methodological quality. Of these, 5 were rated as “excellent” for overall methodological quality, 55 rated as “good,” 56 rated as “fair,” and 5 rated as “poor.” However, whilst almost half (n = 60) of all studies rated as “good” or better overall, only one quarter (n = 29) of all studies had sufficient methodological quality to meet the criteria in Table 3 based on a revision of criteria proposed by Terwee et al. (2007) and Schellingerhout et al. (2011). Therefore, over half of the studies with generally good design were identified as having specific weaknesses which ultimately compromised the usefulness of findings. Methodological flaws in studies examining psychometric quality of language assessments have also been noted in other literature (LEADERS, 2014, 2015). Therefore, there is a great need for improvements in the design and reporting of studies examining psychometric quality of language assessments for children. Clinicians and researchers also need to be critical of methodology when viewing the results of studies examining reliability and validity of assessments.
Overall, across all measurement properties, reporting on missing data was insufficient, with few studies providing information on the percentage of missing items or a clear description of how missing data was handled. Bias may be introduced if missing data is not determined as being random (Bennett, 2011); therefore, this information is important when reporting on the methodology of studies examining psychometric quality.
A lack of clarity in reporting of statistical analysis was also noted, with a number of assessment manuals not clearly reporting the statistics used. For example, studies used terms such as “correlation” or “coefficient” without specifying the statistical procedure used in calculations. Where factor analysis or intra-class correlations were applied in structural validity or reliability studies, few studies reported details such as the rotational method or formula used. Lack of clear reporting creates difficulty for independent reviewers and clinicians to appraise and compare the quality of evidence presented in studies.
COSMIN ratings for internal consistency were rated between “excellent” and “fair” with most rated as “good.” However, only two thirds of the reviewed assessments used the statistical analysis required for evidence of internal consistency according to Terwee et al. (2007) and Schellingerhout et al. (2011); that is, Cronbach's Alpha or Kuder-Richardson Formula–20. The remaining assessments (CASL, CELF-5, OWLS-II, PLS-5, and WJIVOL) used a split-half reliability method. Of the ten studies that utilized Cronbach alpha, five studies did not have uni-dimensionality of the scale confirmed through factor analysis and the remaining five did not have an adequate sample size. For internal consistency results to have interpretable meaning, the scale needs to be identified as being uni-dimensional (Terwee et al., 2012).
With regards to reliability most assessments rated in the range of “good” or “fair.” Three assessments (ACE6-11, CASL, and NRDLS) reported test-retest reliability but did not examine inter-rater reliability. One assessment (WJIVOL) did not present with any reliability studies for the subtests that contribute to composite scores that target oral language. All other assessments included examinations of both test-retest and inter-rater reliability within the manuals. Two assessments (OWLS-II and TELD-3) were designed with alternate record forms and, although not included in this review, it was noted that these assessments also reported on the parallel-forms reliability. However, only two assessments (CELF-5 and OWLS-II) used the statistical analysis identified as optimal in Table 3, intra-class correlation or weighted kappa; and were thus the only two studies identified as having evidence of reliability.
COSMIN ratings for measurement error were rated the lowest of all measurement properties, with no studies rating better than “fair.” All studies were rated “poor” for statistical analysis as reliabilities calculated from split-half or Cronbach alpha were used to calculate standard error of measurement, which does not meet COSMIN's requirement of two administrations for evaluating measurement error (Terwee et al., 2012). Measurement error is the variability of random error that may affect assessment results. This is used to develop confidence intervals for scores and reflects the precision to which assessment scores for individuals can be reported.
Ratings for content validity varied considerably across different assessments. While most assessments mapped content onto modalities of comprehension and production and domains of semantics, syntax/morphology, pragmatics and phonology, different theoretical constructs were used to guide content selection. As no empirical evidence currently exists regarding the modalities or domains of language that should be assessed and the criteria for determining impairment (Tomblin et al., 1996; Tomblin and Zhang, 2006; Van Weerdenburg et al., 2006; Eadie et al., 2014), assessments that rated lower were those that did not: (1) provide a clear definition of theoretical construct, (2) provide a clear rationale for how items were selected for the purpose of the assessment, or (3) have an assessment of content from experts during the development of the assessment. The assessments identified as having evidence of content validity were the ALL, CELF-5, CELF:P-2, and PLS-5.
COSMIN ratings for structural validity studies rated between “good” and “poor.” Of the 15 assessments rated, nine assessments (ALL, CELF-5, CELF-P:2, ITPA-3, CASL, OWLS-II, TOLD-P:4, TOLD-I:4, WJIVOL) had an examination of structural validity using factor analysis which is the statistical method required for evidence of structural validity according to COSMIN and Schellingerhout et al. (2011). However, of these nine assessments, only two (CELF-5 and ITPA-3) were rated as “good” or “excellent” for the sample size used. Sample size for factor analysis depends on the number of items in an assessment. As comprehensive language assessments tend to have a large number of items, many studies did not have sample sizes large enough for an “excellent” factor analysis rating on COSMIN, despite the sample appearing large. No studies reported on the percentage of explained variance in structural validity studies, therefore no studies were rated as having a good level of evidence in this measurement property.
Five assessment manuals (ACE6-11, DELV-NR, LCT-2, PLS-5, and TELD-3) did not report on a structural validity study using factor analysis but reported on correlations between subtests; however, this is not sufficient evidence of structural validity according to COSMIN. One assessment (NRDLS) did not provide any evidence to support structural validity through either factor analysis or an examination of correlations between subtests. Structural validity studies are important to examine the extent to which an assessment reflects the underlying constructs being measured in both the overall score and the subtests.
The majority of studies relating to hypothesis testing rated as “fair” or “good” for overall methodological quality. All 15 assessments reported on a comparison between the performance of children with language impairment and typical children and all, except the LCT-2, provided information on convergent validity with related measures of language. Fourteen studies presented with some level of evidence in this measurement property, with only one study (DELV-NR) lacking in studies with sufficient methodological quality for evidence to be determined. For three assessments (CASL, CELF-P, DELV-NR) convergent validity studies outside of the manuals presented with conflicting results. However, it t should be noted that these assessments were three of the very few assessments for which independent studies were identified. As such, the possibility exists that conflicting evidence may appear for other assessments if independent studies were available.
Studies on diagnostic accuracy were available for half of the selected assessments. This information included studies examining positive predictive power (PPP) using estimates of the percentage of children expected to have language impairment in a sample population and studies examining sensitivity and specificity using another assessment as a criterion. Population estimates were set at 10–20% for an overall child population and 60–90% for a population of children referred to services for assessment. Many studies also included PPP calculations with a base percentage of 50%. Most assessments presented data using a range of different standard deviations as cut-off points (between 1 standard deviation and 2 standard deviations) for identification of impairment. The variation in population estimates and cut-off points may reflect the lack of consistency with criteria for diagnosis of language impairment which is noted in literature (Tomblin et al., 1996; Spaulding et al., 2006; Greenslade et al., 2009).
Diagnostic accuracy studies were not rated for methodological quality; however significant methodological flaws were noted in the reporting of information. The evaluated article (Eadie et al., 2014) reported the sample size and sample selection methods used in the study, however no manuals reported this information. When this information is lacking, it is impossible for speech pathologists to evaluate the quality of study or to determine if the sample population represents the clinical population for which the assessment is to be used (Dollaghan and Horner, 2011). Of the studies reporting on sensitivity and specificity against another criteria for identifying language impairments, only the TOLD-P:4 manual, TOLD-I:4 manual and the article (Eadie et al., 2014) provided any description of the reference measure used and time length between assessment administrations. This lack of reporting is a serious flaw as it does not allow for the impact of potential classification errors by the reference standard to be considered in evaluating the validity of findings (Dollaghan and Horner, 2011; Betz et al., 2013). When the reference standard is not specified it also creates difficulty when attempting to compare findings for different assessments or compare different studies for the same assessment. Therefore, evidence regarding the diagnostic accuracy of currently available language assessments is lacking due to an overall trend with poor methodological quality. Improvements in methodological quality and reporting of studies are needed to provide this evidence and to assist Speech Pathologists in understanding the diagnostic utility of available assessments (Dollaghan and Horner, 2011; LEADERS, 2014, 2015).
An important discovery was that all the studies examined in this review used statistical methods solely from classical test theory (CTT), as opposed to item response theory (IRT). Although some manuals made reference to the use of IRT methods in the initial development of assessment items, no studies reported any details or outcomes for these methods. Whilst COSMIN does not currently indicate a preference between these two methods, IRT methods are increasingly being utilized for the development of assessments within fields such as psychology and have numerous reported advantages over CTT-only methods (Reise et al., 2005; Edelen and Reeve, 2007). Further investigation is needed to examine reasons for the lack of IRT methods in the development of child language assessments.
Comparison between Manuals and Independent Studies
Comparisons between manuals and independent articles are limited to instances where studies with adequate methodology from both a manual and an article are available for a measurement property. These included three instances examining convergent validity of the CASL, CELF:P-2 and DELV-NR (Hoffman et al., 2011; Pesco and O'Neill, 2012; Kaminski et al., 2014). In all three of these examples, the articles were rated as reporting conflicting evidence whilst studies in manuals were rated as having positive evidence. Pesco and O'Neill (2012) examined the ability for DELV-NR and CELF:P-2 scores to be predicted by earlier scores on another assessment, the Language use Inventory (LUI). The study reported correlations above the 0.5 suggested by Schellingerhout et al. (2011) for one of five age groups investigated, although the authors named a significant correlation for three age groups. Kaminski et al. (2014) examined the correlation between CELF-P:2 scores and scores on an assessment called the Preschool Early Literacy Indicators (PELI). In this study, correlations between composite scores were found to be slightly above the level suggested by Schellingerhout et al. (2011) for predictive validity and slightly below for convergent validity. Another study by Hoffman et al. (2011) examined convergent validity between the CASL and the Test of Language Development-Primary: 3rd Edition (TOLD-I:3). This study identified a correlation using Pearson's r above the level described as acceptable by Schellingerhout et al. (2011); however, further analysis using a t-test for significance identified a significant difference between the composite scores from the two assessments. From this, the authors suggested that it may not be accurate to assume that different assessments can be used inter-changeably with the same results.
The correlations reported in the CELF-P:2 manual (Wiig et al., 2004) for convergent validity were higher than the correlations reported in articles, however in the manual, the CELF-P:2 was compared to different versions of itself (CELF-P and CELF-4) and with a similar test published by the same publisher (PLS-4). Therefore, the correlations would be expected to be higher than the correlations reported in the articles where the CELF-P:2 was compared to language assessments with different theoretical backgrounds. The time period between administrations of assessments also differed between studies, which may be a source of difference given the potential for change in status of children over time.
The study by Hoffman et al. (2011) also examined structural validity of the CASL using factor analysis. Although this study was not identified as having adequate methodology due to small sample size, the results are interesting to note because different findings were reported in comparison to the factor analysis reported in the CASL manual (Carrow-Woolfolk, 1999). Hoffman et al. (2011) reported evidence of a single factor model however the manual reported a 3-factor model. However, the 3-factor model was only reported in the manual for children 7 years and older, with a single factor model reported for ages six and below. The sample in the article included 6, 7, and 8 year-olds, therefore encompassing both these age-ranges. Furthermore, the two studies did not administer the same subtests from the CASL and both studies received a “poor” COSMIN rating for sample size. Factor analysis on five subtests of the CASL collectively containing 260 items would require a sample size of over 1,300 for a COSMIN rating higher than “poor,” Both these studies had sample sizes less than 250. Given the shortcomings of these studies, further studies with good methodology are required to provide evidence of structural validity.
Collectively, these findings indicate that further independent studies are required to examine the validity of different comprehensive language assessments for children. Further research is also required to determine if children are categorized similarly across different assessments with regards to diagnosis and severity of language impairment (Hoffman et al., 2011; Spaulding, 2012; Spaulding et al., 2012).
Overall Quality of Language Assessments
It is acknowledged that speech pathologists should consider a range of factors as well as psychometric quality when selecting an assessment for use including the clinical population for which the assessment will be used, the purpose for which the assessment will be used and theoretical construct of the assessment (Bishop and McDonald, 2009). This study examined the reliability and validity of currently available assessments and identified that all assessments present with notable shortcomings when rated against methodological quality (COSMIN) and the criteria of evaluating findings of studies (Table 3). However, considering the data that is available, some assessments have more psychometric evidence to support use as diagnostic assessments. These assessments include: ALL, CELF-5, CELF:P-2, and PLS-5. It is noted that the ALL currently only provides grade level normative data for the United States of America population. The ALL, CELF-5, and PLS-5 were all rated as having “strong” or “moderate” evidence across two or more measurement properties. The CELF:P-2 was identified as having evidence in two measurement properties from the manual, however there was some conflicting information regarding hypothesis testing in independent literature. The ALL, CELF-5, and PLS-5 were not examined in independent literature. The DELV-NR, ITPA-3, LCT-2, TELD-3, and WJIVOL had no more than limited evidence for one measurement property. However, it should be noted that where evidence is reported as lacking, it does not mean that these assessments are not valid or reliable, but rather that further research is required to determine psychometric quality.
Implications
Standardized assessments are frequently used to make important diagnostic and management decisions for children with language impairment in both clinical and research contexts. For accurate diagnosis and provision of effective intervention, it is important that assessments chosen for use have evidence of good psychometric quality (Friberg, 2010). However, a previous study identified that speech pathologists may not be selecting child language assessments based on the psychometric quality reported in assessment manuals (Betz et al., 2013). Therefore emphasis needs to be placed on the selection of assessments that are evidence-based and appropriate to the needs of the client, the speech pathologist and the service delivery context. Speech pathologists also need to advocate for improvements to the quality of both currently used assessments and those developed in the future.
This review also identifies areas in need of further research with regards to individual assessments and development of the field of child language assessment in general. Where an assessment does not present with an “excellent” or “good” level of evidence for all measurement properties, further research is required to determine if this evidence exists. In general, further information is particularly needed to provide evidence of structural validity, measurement error and diagnostic accuracy. The use of IRT methods for statistical analysis of psychometric properties of also identified as an area in need of further exploration within the field of child language assessment.
Very limited evidence of psychometric quality currently exists outside of what is reported in manuals for child language assessments and where evidence does exist, it does not always support information reported in manuals Assessment manuals are produced by developers who have commercial interest in the assessment. Furthermore, the reporting of psychometric quality in manuals is not peer-reviewed and can only be viewed after purchasing. When assessment developers make information on psychometric properties available online or in published peer-reviewed journals, transparency is achieved and clinicians and researchers are able to review psychometric properties prior to purchasing assessments. A need for independent studies is also identified in order to provide additional information to data provided in assessment manuals. When information is able to be collated from a variety of different studies, then the evidence regarding psychometric quality of assessments will become more substantial.
This review identified a number of assessments that currently present with better evidence of psychometric quality than others, although substantially more data is required to show that any assessments have “good” evidence. Until further information becomes available, it is suggested that speech pathologists favor assessments with better evidence when assessing the language abilities of school-aged children, provided that the normative sample is appropriate for the population in which the assessment is to be used. However, given that all assessments have limitations, speech pathologists should avoid relying on the results of a single assessment. Standardized assessment results should be supplemented with information from other assessment approaches (e.g., response to intervention, curriculum-based assessment, language sampling, dynamic assessment) when making judgments regarding diagnosis and intervention needs (Hoffman et al., 2011; Eadie et al., 2014). In addition, as it is possible that differences in underlying constructs between assessments contributes to differences in diagnostic abilities of assessments (Hoffman et al., 2011), it is important for speech pathologists to consider theoretical construct when choosing standardized assessments for use or when comparing results between different assessments.
Limitations
Due to a need to restrict size, responsiveness was not investigated in this review. It was, however, noted that no assessment manuals reported on responsiveness studies. These studies have a longitudinal design with multiple administrations of the assessment across time to measure sensitivity to change in a person's status. Evidence of responsiveness is particularly important when assessments are to be used for measuring intervention outcomes or monitoring stability over time (Eadie et al., 2014; Polit, 2015). Therefore, further research is recommended to investigate the evidence for using comprehensive language assessments for these purposes. Further investigation is also needed to compare assessments across different English speaking countries and cultural groups.
This review was confined to school-age language assessments that cover both the production and comprehension of spoken language. While this reflects current literature and clinical practice (Tomblin et al., 1996; Wiig, 2010), there may be clinical applications for assessments specific to one modality, for example when assessing language abilities of children who are non-verbal or have unintelligible speech. Assessments targeting single aspects of language such as semantics or syntax were also not included in this study, however, these may be used by Speech Pathologists (Betz et al., 2013), therefore an examination of psychometric quality of these assessments is recommended.
There is a need for future research to examine the psychometric quality of assessments for children who are bi-lingual or speaking English as a second language (Gillam et al., 2013). An examination of standardized written language assessments is also needed as there is a strong overlap between spoken and written language impairment in school-aged children (Bishop and Snowling, 2004; Snowling and Hulme, 2012). In addition, there is also a need for investigation into assessments that target activity and participation levels of the World Health Organization's International Classification of Functioning and Disability—Child and Youth (McLeod and Threats, 2008; Roulstone et al., 2012).
Conclusion
This systematic review examines the psychometric quality of 15 currently available standardized spoken language assessments for children aged 4–12 years. Overall, limitations were noted with the methodology of studies reporting on psychometric quality, indicating a great need for improvements in the design and reporting of studies examining psychometric quality of both existing assessments and those that are developed in the future. As information on psychometric properties is primarily provided by assessment developers in manuals, further research is also recommended to provide independent evidence for psychometric quality. Whilst all assessments were identified as having notable limitations, four assessments: ALL, CELF-5, CELF:P-2, and PLS-5 were identified as currently having better evidence of reliability and validity. These four assessments are suggested for diagnostic use, provided they suit the purpose of the assessment process and are appropriate for the population being assessed. Emphasis on the psychometric quality of assessments is important for speech pathologists to make evidence-based decisions about the assessments they select when assessing the language abilities of school-aged children.
Author Contributions
DD, RS, NM, WP, and RC all contributed to the conceptual content of the manuscript. DD and YC contributed to data collection and analysis.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
1. ^Recent international consensus has replaced the term Specific Language Impairment with Developmental Language Disorder (Bishop et al., 2017).
References
Adams, C., Cooke, R., Crutchley, A., Hesketh, A., and Reeves, D. (2001). Assessment of Comprehension and Expression 6-11. London: GL Assessment.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Andersson, L. (2005). Determining the adequacy of tests of children's language. Commun. Disord. Q. 26:207. doi: 10.1177/15257401050260040301
Bennett, D. A. (2011). How can I deal with missing data in my study? Aust. N. Z. J. Public Health 25, 464–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842X.2001.tb00294.x
Betz, S. K., Eickhoff, J. K., and Sullivan, S. F. (2013). Factors influencing the selection of standardized tests for the diagnosis of Specific Language Impairment. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 44, 133–143. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2012/12-0093)
Bishop, D. V. M. (2011). Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101
Bishop, D. V. M., and McDonald, D. (2009). Identifying language impairment in children: combining language test scores with parental report. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 44, 600–615. doi: 10.1080/13682820802259662
Bishop, D. V. M., and Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific langauge impairment: same or different? Psychol. Bull. 130, 858–886. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.858
Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., and Greenhalgh, T. (2017). Phase 2 of CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatr. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12721. [Epub ahead of print].
Bowers, L., Huisingh, R., and LoGiudice, C. (2006). The Listening Comprehension Test, 2nd Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Caesar, L. G., and Kohler, P. D. (2009). Tools clinicians use: a survey of language assessment procedures used by school-based speech-pathologists. Commun. Disord. Q. 30, 226–236. doi: 10.1177/1525740108326334
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (2011). Oral and Written Language Scales, 2nd Edn. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Psychcorp.
Catts, H. W., Bridges, M. S., Little, T. D., and Tomblin, J. B. (2008). Reading achievement growth in children with language impairments. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 51, 1569–1579. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0259)
Conti-Ramsden, G., and Botting, N. (2004). Social difficulties and victimization in children with SLI at 11 years of age. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 47, 145–161. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2004/013)
Conti-Ramsden, G., Durkin, K., Simkin, Z., and Knox, E. (2009). Specific language impairment and school outcomes: identifying and explaining variability at the end of compulsory education. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 44, 15–35. doi: 10.1080/13682820801921601
Cordier, R., Speyer, R., Chen, Y., Wilkes-Gillan, S., Brown, T., Bourke-Taylor, H., et al. (2015). Evaluating the psychometric quality of social skills measures: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 10:e0132299. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0132299
Dockrell, J., and Lindsay, G. (1998). The ways in which speech and language difficulties impact on children's access to the curriculum. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 14, 117–133. doi: 10.1177/026565909801400201
Dockrell, J. E., and Marshall, C. R. (2015). Measurement issues: assessing language skills in young children. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 20, 116–125. doi: 10.1111/camh.12072
Dollaghan, C. A., and Horner, E. A. (2011). Bilingual language assessment: a meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 1077–1088. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2010/10-0093)
Eadie, P., Nguyen, C., Carlin, J., Bavin, E., Bretherton, L., and Reilly, S. (2014). Stability of language performance at 4 and 5 years: measurement and participant variability. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 215–227. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12065
Ebbels, S. (2014). Introducing the SLI debate. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 377–380. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12119
Edelen, M. O., and Reeve, B. B. (2007). Applying item response theory (IRT) modeling to questionnaire development, evaluation, and refinement. Qual. Life Res. 16, 5–18. doi: 10.1007/s11136-007-9198-0
Edward, S., Letts, C., and Sinka, I. (2011). New Reynell Developmental Language Scales. London: GL Assessment.
Fraser, J., and Conti-Ramsden, G. (2008). Contribution of phonological and broader language skills to literacy. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 43, 552–569. doi: 10.1080/13682820701778069
Friberg, J. C. (2010). Considerations for test selection: how do validity and reliability impact of diagnostic decisions? Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 26, 77–92. doi: 10.1177/0265659009349972
Gillam, R. B., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Bohman, T. M., and Mendez-Perezb, A. (2013). Identification of specific language impairment in bilingual children: assessment in english. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 1813–1823. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0056)
Greenslade, K. J., Plante, E., and Vance, R. (2009). The diagnostic accuracy and construct validity of the structured photographic expressive language test—preschool. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 40, 150–160. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0049)
Hammill, D. D., Mather, R., and Roberts, R. (2001). Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities, 3rd Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Hammill, D. D., and Newcomer, P. L. (2008). Test of Language Development—Primary, 4th Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Harrison, L. J., McLeod, S., Berthelsen, D., and Walker, S. (2009). Literacy, numeracy, and learning in school-aged children identified as having speech and language impairment in early childhood. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 11, 392–403. doi: 10.1080/17549500903093749
Haynes, W. O., and Pindzola, R. H. (2012). Diagnosis and Evaluation in Speech Pathology, 8th Edn. Harlow: Pearson Education Inc.
Hegde, M. N., and Pomaville, F. (2013). Assessment of Communication Disorders in Children, 2 Edn. San Diego, CA: Plural publishing.
Hoffman, L. M., Leob, D. F., Brandel, J., and Gillam, R. B. (2011). Concurrent and construct validity of oral language measures with school-aged children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 20, 1597–1608. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0213)
Hresko, W. P., Reid, D. K., and Hammill, D. D. (1999). Test of Early Language Development, 3rd Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Kaderavek, J. N. (2011). Language Disorders in Children: Fundamental Concepts of Assessment and Intervention. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc.
Kaminski, R. A., Abbott, M., Aguayo, K. B., Liatimer, R., and Good, R. H. (2014). The preschool early literacy indicators: validity and benchmark goals. Top. Early Child. Spec. Educ. 34, 71–82. doi: 10.1177/0271121414527003
Law, J., Boyle, J., Harris, F., Harkness, A., and Nye, C. (2000). Prevalence and natural history of primary speech and language delay: findings from a systematic review of the literature. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 35, 165–188. doi: 10.1080/136828200247133
LEADERS (2014). Test Review: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—5th Edn (CELF-5). Available online at : https://edblogs.columbia.edu/leadersproject/files/2014/02/CELF5-Test-Review-LEADERS-1-29ay7q2.pdf
LEADERS (2015). Test Review: Preschool Langage Scales—5th Edn. (PLS-5). Available online at: https://edblogs.columbia.edu/leadersproject/files/2015/05/PLS5-English-finaldraft-2dgptop.pdf
Leonard, L. B. (2009). Is expressive language disorder an accurate diagnostic category? Am. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 18, 115–123. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2008/08-0064)
Letts, C., Edward, S., Schaefer, B., and Sinka, I. (2014). The new Reynell developmental language scales: descriptive account and illustrative case study. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 30, 103–116. doi: 10.1177/0265659013492784
Letts, C., Edwards, S., Sinka, I., Schaefer, B., and Gibbons, W. (2013). Socio-economic status and language acquisition: children's performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language Scales. Internat. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 48, 131–143. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12004
Lindsay, G., Dockrell, J., Desforges, M., Law, J., and Peacey, N. (2010). Meeting the needs of children and young people with speech, language and communication difficulties. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 45, 448–460. doi: 10.3109/13682820903165693
Lombardino, L. J., Leiberman, R., and Brown, J. C. (2005). Assessment of Literacy and Language. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Psychcorp.
McCauley, R. J., and Swisher, L. (1984). Psychometric reveiw of language and articulation tests for preschool children. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 49, 34–42. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4901.34
McCormack, J., Harrison, L. J., McLeod, S., and McAllister, L. (2011). A nationally representative study of the association between communication impairment at 4–5 years and children's life activities at 7–9 years J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 54, 1328–1348. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0155)
McKown, C., Allen, A. M., Russo-Ponsaran, N. M., and Johnson, J. K. (2013). Direct assessment of children's social-emotional comprehension. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1154–1166. doi: 10.1037/a0033435
McLeod, S., and Threats, T. T. (2008). The ICF-CY and children with communication disabilities. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 10, 92–109. doi: 10.1080/17549500701834690
Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the prisma statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097
Mokkink, L. B., Prinsen, C. A., Bouter, L. M., De Vet, H. C. W., and Terwee, C. B. (2016). The COnsensus-based standards for the selection of health measurement instruments (COSMIN) and how to select an outcome measurement instrument. Braz. J. Phys. Ther. 1413–3555. doi: 10.1590/bjpt-rbf.2014.0143
Mokkink, L. B., Terwee, C. B., Knol, D. L., Stratford, P. W., Alonso, J., Patrick, D. L., et al. (2010a). The COSMIN checklist for evaluating the methodological quality of studies on measurement properties: a clarification of its content. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 10:22. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-10-22
Mokkink, L. B., Terwee, C. B., Patrick, D. L., Alonso, J., Stratford, P. W., Knol, D. L., et al. (2010b). The COSMIN checklist for assessing the methodological quality of studies on measurement properties of health status measurement instruments: an international Delphi study. Qual. Life Res. 19, 539–549. doi: 10.1007/s11136-010-9606-8
Mokkink, L. B., Terwee, C. B., Patrick, D. L., Alonso, J., Stratford, P. W., Knol, D. L., et al. (2010c). The COSMIN study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology and definitions of measurement properties for health-related patient-reported outcomes. Qual. Life Res. 19, 539–549. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.02.006
Mokkink, L. B., Terwee, C. B., Stratford, P. W., Alonso, J., Patrick, D. L., Riphagen, I., et al. (2009). Evaluation of the methodological quality of systematic reviews of health status measurement instruments. Qual. Life Res. 18, 313–333. doi: 10.1007/s11136-009-9451-9
Newcomer, P. L., and Hammill, D. D. (2008). Test of Language Development—Intermediate, 4th Edn. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Paul, R., and Norbury, C. F. (2012a). “Chapter 1: Models of child language disorders,” in Language Disorders from Infancy through Adolescence: Listening, Speaking, Reading, Writting and Communicating, 4th Edn, eds R. Paul and C. F. Norbury (St Louis, MI: Mosby Elsevier), 1–21.
Paul, R., and Norbury, C. F. (2012b). “Chapter 2: Assessment,” in Language Disorders from Infancy through Adolescence: Listening, Speaking, Reading, Writting and Communicating, 4th Edn, eds R. Paul and C. F. Norbury (St Louis, MI: Mosby Elsevier), 22–60.
Paul, R., and Norbury, C. F. (2012c). “Chapter 8: Assessment of developing Language,” in Language Disorders from Infancy through Adolescence: Listening, Speaking, Reading, Writting and Communicating, 4th Edn, eds R. Paul and C. F. Norbury (St Louis, MI: Mosby Elsevier), 280–346.
Pesco, D., and O'Neill, D. K. (2012). Predicting later language outcomes from the Language Use Inventory. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 55, 421–434. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0273)
Plante, E., and Vance, R. (1994). Selection of preschool language tests: a data based approach. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 25, 15–24. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461.2501.15
Polit, D. F. (2015). Assessing measurement in health: beyond reliability and validity. Int. J. Nurs. Stud. 52, 1746–1753. doi: 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2015.07.002
Reed, V. A. (2005). “Assessment,” in An Introduction to Children with Language Disorders, 3nd Edn, ed V. A. Reed (Boston, MA: Pearson Education), 404–442.
Reichow, B., Salamack, S., Paul, R., Volkmar, F. R., and Klin, A. (2008). Pragmatic assessment in autism spectrum disorders: a comparison of a standard measure with parent report. Commun. Disord. Q. 29, 169–176. doi: 10.1177/1525740108318697
Reise, S. P., Ainsworth, A. T., and Haviland, M. G. (2005). Item response theory: fundamentals, applications, and promise in psychological research. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 95–101. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00342.x
Roulstone, S., Coad, J., Ayre, A., Hambly, H., and Lindsay, G. (2012). The Preferred Outcomes of Children with Speech, Language and Communication Needs and their Parents. Available online at: http://dera.ioe.ac.uk/16329/7/DFE-RR247-BCRP12_Redacted.pdf
Schellingerhout, J. M., Heymans, M. W., Verhagen, A. P., de Vet, H. C., Koes, B. W., and Terwee, C. B. (2011). Measurement properties of translated versions of neck-specific questionaires: a systematic review. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 11:87. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-11-87
Seymour, H. N., Roeper, T. W., de Villiers, J., and de Villiers, P. A. (2005). Diagnostic Evaluation of Langauge Variation. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Psychcorp.
Seymour, H. N., and Zurer-Pearson, B. (2004). Steps in designing and implementing an innovative assessment instrument. Semin. Speech Lang. 25, 27–31. doi: 10.1055/s-2004-824823
Shipley, G. S., and McAfee, J. G. (2009). Assessment in Speech-Language Pathology: A Resource Manual, 4th Edn. Clifton Park, NY: Cengage Learning.
Shrank, F. A., Mather, N., and McGrew, K. S. (2014). Woodcock-Johnson IV Tests of Oral language. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside.
Snowling, M. J., and Hulme, C. (2012). annual research reveiw: the nature and classifiation of redaing disorders - acommentary on proposals for DSM-5. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 53, 593–607. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02495.x
Spaulding, T. J. (2012). Comparison of severity ratings on norm-referenced tests for children with specific language impairment. J. Commun. Disord. 45, 59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2011.11.003
Spaulding, T. J., Plante, E., and Farinella, K. A. (2006). Elligibility criteria for language impairment: is the low end of normal always appropriate? Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 37, 61–72. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2006/007)
Spaulding, T. J., Szulga, M. S., and Figueroa, C. (2012). Using norm-referenced tests to determine severity of language impairment in children: disconnect between US policy makers and test developers. Lang. Speech Hear. Serv. Sch. 43, 176–190. doi: 10.1044/0161-1461(2011/10-0103)
Speyer, R., Cordier, R., Kertscher, B., and Heijnen, B. (2014). Psychometric properties of questionnaires on functional health status in oropharyngeal dysphagia: a systematic literature review. Biomed Res. Int. 2014:458678. doi: 10.1155/2014/458678
Terwee, C. B., Bot, S. D. M., de Boer, M. R., van der Windt, D. A. W. M., Knol, D. L., Dekker, J., et al. (2007). Quality criteria were proposed for measurement properties of health status questionnaires. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 60, 34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.03.012
Terwee, C. B., de Vet, H. C. W., and Mokkink, L. B. (2015). Reply to ‘COSMIN for quality rating systematic reviews on psychometric properties'. Phys. Ther. Rev. 20, 266–267. doi: 10.1179/1743288X15Y.0000000021
Terwee, C. B., Mokkink, L. B., Knol, D. L., Ostelo, R. W. J. G., Bouter, L. M., and de Vet, H. C. W. (2012). Rating the methological quality in systematic reviews of studies on measurement properties: a scoring system for the COSMIN checklist. Qual. Life Res. 21, 651–657. doi: 10.1007/s11136-011-9960-1
Tomblin, J. B., Records, N. L., and Zhang, X. (1996). A system for the diagnosis of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 39, 1284–1294. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1284
Tomblin, J. B., and Zhang, X. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 49, 1193–1208. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2006/086)
Uijen, A. A., Heinst, C. W., Schellevis, F. G., van den Bosch, W. J., van de Laar, F. A., Terwee, C. B., et al. (2012). Measurement properties of questionnaires measuring continuity of care: a systematic review. PLoS ONE 7:e42256. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042256
Van Weerdenburg, M., Verhoeven, L., and Van Balkom, H. (2006). Towards a typology of specific language impairment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 176–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01454.x
Vrijman, C., Homan, M. L., Limpens, J., van der Veen, W., Wolkerstorfer, A., Terwee, C. B., et al. (2012). Measurement properties of outcome measures for vitiligo: a systematic review. Arch. Dermatol. 148, 1302–1309. doi: 10.1001/archdermatol.2012.3065
Wiig, E. H. (2010). “How SLD manifests in oral expression,” in Essentials of Specific Learning Disability Identification, eds D. P. Flanagan and V. C. Alfonzo (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons), 106–108.
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W. A., and Semel, E. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool, 2nd Edn. San Antonio, TX: Pearson Psychcorp.
Wiig, E. H., Semel, E., and Secord, W. A. (2013). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 5th Edn. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Psychcorp.
World Health Organisation (2015). International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision, Vol. 10. Geneva: World Health Organisation.
Keywords: language assessment, language impairment, psychometric properties, reliability, validity, Language Disorder
Citation: Denman D, Speyer R, Munro N, Pearce WM, Chen Y-W and Cordier R (2017) Psychometric Properties of Language Assessments for Children Aged 4–12 Years: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 8:1515. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01515
Received: 08 May 2017; Accepted: 21 August 2017;
Published: 07 September 2017.
Edited by:
Sergio Machado, Salgado de Oliveira University, BrazilReviewed by:
Antonio Zuffiano, Liverpool Hope University, United KingdomMarie Arsalidou, National Research University – Higher School of Economics, Russia
Copyright © 2017 Denman, Speyer, Munro, Pearce, Chen and Cordier. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Deborah Denman, ZGVib3JhaC5kZW5tYW5AcG9zdGdyYWQuY3VydGluLmVkdS5hdQ==