Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 02 August 2017
Sec. Developmental Psychology

An Interactive View on the Development of Deictic Pointing in Infancy

  • 1Faculty of Arts and Humanities, Paderborn University, Paderborn, Germany
  • 2Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, TU Dortmund University, Dortmund, Germany

In this review, we will focus on the development of deictic pointing gestures. We propose that they are based on infants’ sensitivities to human motion. Since both conventionalized gestures and bodily movements can be interpreted as communicative, of special interest to us is how pointing gestures are employed within early social interactions. We push forward the idea of a conventionalization process taking place when the interaction partners guide infants’ participation toward joint goals. On their way to deploy pointing gestures and thus to successfully influence the partner for a specific purpose, infants need also to disengage from their own object perception or manipulation. In addition, infants accompany their gestures increasingly with verbal utterances—this form of communication is multimodal and offers the possibility to combine modalities for the purpose of expressing more complex utterances. The multimodal behavior will be picked up by caregivers and extended into linguistically more complex forms. Because of this emerging relationship to language and its social use, gestural behavior in early infancy is a powerful predictor for later language development.

Introduction

In our review, we argue that the development of infants’ communicative gestures is driven by infants’ perceptual sensitivities toward human motion, couplings between the vocal and motor systems and infants’ interactive experience. To demonstrate such complexity of the driving force, we need to broaden the concept of gestures. By definition, gestures are behaviors communicating intentions. However, the period of infancy is one of asymmetric communication between less and more competent partners. The infant, as the less competent partner, relies on resources in the physical and social environment that shape the meaning of communication. Hence, to be interpreted as communicative, infants’ gestures do not need to be intentional. They need to be, however, part of a social engagement toward a joint goal. For example, Reddy et al. (2013) demonstrated that 3-month-olds adjusted their posture in anticipation of being picked up when an adult was approaching. In our view, such adjustments as gaze direction, turning the head or lifting the arms within social interactions (Reddy et al., 2013) are gestures communicating the recognition of (and contribution to) the joint action of picking up. From our perspective, a still unsolved question in research on gesture development is how infants are guided to use more conventionalized communicative means that serve interactional purposes. In our review, we will focus on a gesture that is considered a milestone in communication development, namely pointing, review the production and comprehension development, and address its possible routes of conventionalization.

Development of Pointing

“A canonical pointing gesture is considered to be an extended index-finger with the remaining fingers curled and the arm extended” (Carpendale and Carpendale, 2010, p. 112) (Figure 1B). In contrast to iconic gestures that evoke a referent without its presence, the function of pointing gestures is to draw attention to present referents. Basically, the semantic process requires the observer to “relate the gesture to the situation” (Marcos et al., 2003, p. 217). However, 12-month-olds also use pointing to refer to objects that are not visible but had been present in the past (Bohn et al., 2015). Thus, the reference of deictic gestures does not have to be limited to the ongoing situation.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Different forms of pointing (from the study by Lüke et al., 2017). (A) Pointing with the whole hand: The child is referring to an entity by raising and extending her or his arm. (B) Pointing with the index-finger: The child is referring to an entity by raising and extending her or his arm and the index-finger toward a specific location.

Comprehension

In early infancy, the ability to follow pointing gestures seems to be based on the combination of a child’s ability to disengage from the salient stimulus (i.e., the pointing hand), and sensitivities to human motion. As to the disengagement from the salient stimulus, Amano et al. (2004) showed that although 3- and 4-month-old infants needed both eye gaze and pointing to follow an experimenter’s cues, the hand movement alone sufficed when presented by their mothers. This suggests that the familiarity of the interaction partner influences early perception processes. With respect to the sensitivities to human motion, Rohlfing et al. (2012) demonstrated that 6-month-olds can follow a dynamic pointing gestures, but not static ones: After a pointing hand was presented on the screen, it disappeared, thus facilitating infants’ attentional disengagement before they were expected to follow the direction of the point. Bertenthal et al. (2014) extended this finding by validating this effect even in comparison to an arrow in 4- and 6-month-old infants.

Obviously, it takes time and “interactive specialization” (Bertenthal et al., 2014, p. 2043), i.e., experience with pointing as a particular ‘unit’ (cf. Tomasello et al., 2005, p. 679) of behavior occurring recurrently within social interactions, for children to derive meaning from its static presentation. In fact, in natural settings such as labeling routines, caregivers use movements to increase an objects’ salience: In a context of book reading, Rossmanith et al. (2014) found that mothers used dynamic pointing, i.e., they moved their pointing gestures up and down—a behavior that is also known from showing gestures for early verbal children (Gogate et al., 2000; de Villiers Rader and Zukow-Goldring, 2010). Not only dynamic gestures can direct infants’ attentional focus toward specific targets, but also the combinations of gestures and other attentional cues such as gaze. Flom et al. (2004) found that when parents combined communicative means, 9-month-olds followed gaze toward peripheral targets increasingly.

Overall, early comprehension takes place in a rich interactive environment in which infants’ perceptual sensitivities are integrated. These interactive routines help children to learn means that signal the focus of attention (Bruner, 1983). Event-related potential (ERP) data obtained from 8-month-old infants revealed greater neural activation for targets that were congruent with a pointing gesture compared to non-congruent targets, thereby supporting the proposition that infants begin to process the functional aspects of pointing before starting to point themselves (Gredebäck et al., 2010). Gliga and Csibra (2009, p. 352) attribute the functional aspect of pointing to “referential expectation”—a cognitive state that children are likely to develop with increasing experience in interactive routines: By being able to perceive that they are being addressed and to expect a referent to occur in the direction of the point, infants’ early understanding of deictic gestures entails an awareness of mental states in the communicative partner. A study with 14- to 28-month-old children revealed that deictic gestures, i.e., pointing, giving, or holding up, did not just elicit the children’s attention but also elicited responses specific to the type of gesture, thereby suggesting an understanding of the gesture’s function (Morford and Goldin-Meadow, 1992). In experimental studies, for example, infants understood the function of pointing within a hiding game as providing information about hidden objects (Behne et al., 2012).

Whether pointing in input influence infants’ own use of gestures has been addressed in longitudinal observations of typically developing children (Rowe et al., 2008; Matthews et al., 2012) and children with later language delay (Lüke et al., in press). Overall, the amount of parents’ and infants’ pointing seems to be related (Rowe et al., 2008; Matthews et al., 2012).

Production

Infants produce some hand forms resembling pointing gestures very early in their development. Index-finger extensions are visible in infants as young as 3 months (Hannan, 1987; Masataka, 2003). Some authors (Bates et al., 1976; Masataka, 2003; Gómez, 2007) presume that these gesture-like hand forms serve infants’ own attention regulation and are coupled with object exploration. Carpendale and Carpendale (2010) review that one possible route to using pointing is infants’ own directedness toward aspects of the world.

Tomasello et al. (2005) refer to the occurrence of pointing as typical for humans. Indeed, although great apes are reported to point (Leavens et al., 1996), they do not point for each other. In human infants, the most frequently used communicative type are deictic hand gestures (Morford and Goldin-Meadow, 1992), and these emerge between 9 and 12 months. However, they do not emerge from unsuccessful reaching; the two gestures, pointing and reaching, follow different developmental trajectories (Blake et al., 1994). Evidence for different developmental trajectories comes from research investigating the relation to language acquisition: A meta-analysis by Colonnesi et al. (2010) revealed that those children who used more pointing gestures than their peers in their 2nd year of life had better language skills at a later age (medium-to-large combined effect size, r = 0.35). In addition, investigating pointing gestures in contrast to reaching gestures, Esseily et al. (2011) introduced the aspect of handedness as indication of the left cerebral hemisphere being involved in the communication production process (cf. Vauclair and Cochet, 2013)—a region that in most right-handed adults is specialized for language (Knecht et al., 2000); in children, a lack of lateralization is associated with specific language impairment (Bishop, 2014). For crude forms of pointing, Hannan (1987) found more right-handed gesturing compared to pointing with the left hand in 3- to 12-month-olds when observed during toy play with their mothers. For conventionalized forms of pointing, Esseily et al. (2011)’ study revealed that 14-month-old infants who use their right index-finger or arm for pointing had larger receptive and productive vocabularies than non-right-handed pointers, an effect that was not found for the handedness of grasping. The authors conclude that right-handed pointing (but not object manipulation) seems to be related to a more advanced language development (cf. Vauclair and Cochet, 2013).

The appearance of pointing undergoes developmental changes: Within their 2nd year of life, infants point increasingly using their index-finger rather than their whole hand (Lüke et al., in press)—this form seems to be related to other advances in communication such as pointing comprehension (Liszkowski and Tomasello, 2011) or later language (Lüke et al., 2017).

In addition, around 18 months of age, Franco and Gagliano (2001) observed that infants begin to coordinate their gesture and gaze systematically, and to ascertain that the partner is looking at them before pointing. “Gaze alternation […] implies the awareness that others have a visual perspective that may differ from one’s own” (Leavens et al., 1996, p. 351). Termed “visual checking” (Bates et al., 1976), this behavior refers to the infants’ ability to regard the partner in order to communicate successfully. Matthews et al. (2012) even reported of successfully training infants in their ability of gaze checking when pointing. This result speaks to the possibility that pointing develops on the basis of infants’ becoming sensitive to the experience of the social uptake. Indeed, when 17-month old children are encouraged to use pointing in an experimental setting, they will not only point more when interacting with their caregiver, but also use more different words (LeBarton et al., 2015).

Co-Expression: Gesture and Language

Temporal Coordination

A tight temporal action–language synchrony does not only occur in the spontaneous (Gogate et al., 2000) and facilitative input (de Villiers Rader and Zukow-Goldring, 2010) to children but also in children’s early vocal productions suggesting “developmentally prior couplings between the vocal and motor systems” (Masataka, 2003; Iverson, 2010, p. 267). Iverson and Fagan (2004) studied arm gestures in infants as young as 6–9 months and found a temporal coordination of canonical babbling with rhythmic hand movements—an important basis for the integrated speech–gesture system. Even though young children first produce gestures with little speech (Morford and Goldin-Meadow, 1992), infants soon co-express mainly deictic gestures and vocalizations even before they produce their first words (Esteve-Gibert and Prieto, 2014). However, gesture–speech combinations become proportionally more frequent than gesture-only utterances only after the onset of first word production (Esteve-Gibert and Prieto, 2014). These authors also observed that prosodic features of speech (i.e., the accented syllable) and features of the gesture execution – the gesture’s apex (i.e., the moment when the finger is maximally extended) – became temporally more closely related once infants began to produce first words.

Semantic Coordination

When combined with speech, Morford and Goldin-Meadow (1992, p. 560) differentiated between gestures being redundant to the verbal information and gestures adding a “new semantic element to the meaning of the spoken word.” This interplay of verbal and gestural information is typical to any communicative gesture (Goldin-Meadow, 1999). Exhibited in early pointing, it seems to predict aspects of syntactic development (e.g., Iverson and Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Igualada et al., 2015). Iverson and Goldin-Meadow (2005), for example, report that 10- to 14-month-old infants combined deictic gestures with words on average 2 months before producing first two-word combinations.

In input to infants, mainly deictic gestures are used to reinforce verbal utterances (Iverson et al., 1999). In addition, Iverson and Goldin-Meadow (2005) pushed forward the idea of caregivers ‘translating’ the child’s multimodal utterances into two-word utterances, thus, scaffolding the next step of their verbal development (cf. Goldin-Meadow et al., 2007). However, it remains an open question whether caregivers of infants who are later identified as at risk for language delay already differ in their semantic co-expressiveness in early interactions.

Conventionalization

At the beginning, we mentioned the 3-month-olds’ adjustments in posture in anticipation of being picked up as being gestures. However, these gestures are not conventionalized, and although their meaning resides in the movement itself, they might remain without feedback from the interaction partner. For the first and crude forms of pointing, Carpendale and Carpendale (2010) suggest that this action of own directedness toward aspects of the world – although not consciously perceived in interactions with 3- to 12-month-olds (Hannan, 1987) – can be understood and responded to by the caregivers.

Marcos et al. (2003, p. 216) define a communicative behavior as being conventionalized “if the utterances or gestures in it have meanings that are shared by all members of the social group,” such as negation gestures, because they are understandable within a given culture. In infancy, however, most gestures are “partially conventional” (ibid), and even the meaning of pointing gestures is “elaborated in interaction settings that are widespread […] in the culture” (ibid, p. 217). A child is using a gesture in a conventionalized manner when she or he can perform this corresponding movement and does it deliberately (Lock, 1980; Kettner and Carpendale, 2013).

Joint Goals

We think that the deliberate control over the gestural form is learnt by getting the action reflected back by the caregiver in a particular frame (Heller and Rohlfing, 2017). As parts of continuous behaviors produced within a joint goal-directed act, pointing gestures allow “the use of social agents as a means to achieve some end” (Leavens et al., 1996, p. 2). The social environment, first eager to adopt bodily movements as gestures (Reddy et al., 2013), increasingly shapes communication toward more specific means: A micro-analysis of caregiver-child interaction during early book reading demonstrates that 10-month-olds are assisted in using their pointing gestures for referential purposes; other gestures like using the nose to point to an object in the picture will not be ratified by caregivers (Heller and Rohlfing, 2017). This process of conventionalization (Bruner, 1983; Marcos et al., 2003) requires the child’s attention to be shaped toward interactive goals and for the child to show this kind of attentional management by decoupling his/her own object manipulation from monitoring the interaction partner’s object activity (De Barbaro et al., 2015).

Motives of Pointing

Bates et al. (1976, p. 51) describe early communicative signals as being goal-oriented that becomes visible by “consummatory behavior confirming that the child did indeed have that goal in mind.” They identify two major motives: The goal of early imperatives is to obtain an object or event, whereas the goal of declaratives is the interaction with an adult by using objects. In a series of experiments (Table 1), researchers have confirmed the usefulness of differentiating between declarative expressive and declarative informative gestures: When infants point they take “cumulative history” (Flom et al., 2004, p. 192) with an adult into account, e.g., they pointed more to an object that the experimenter was looking for, and had not witnessed falling down or being hidden (Liszkowski et al., 2006). Also, when an experimenter repeatedly had not shown interest in a referent infants pointed at, they will point less (Liszkowski et al., 2007). Hence, already 12-month-olds construe their pointing as a unit in joint behaviors by monitoring an adult’s knowledge and interest.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Different motives of pointing gestures.

Joint Attention and Individual Differences

Closely related to the development of gestures and linguistic skills is the ability to share attention which is often operationalized as the use of gestures. Although children can also gain word knowledge through other routines such as hiding games, most research has studied joint attention (JA) routines, in which caregivers and infants coordinate their attention for a purpose such as labeling a referent. Mundy et al. (2007) operationalized infants’ ability to engage in JA as (a) responding to and (b) initiating cues such as gaze and pointing in interaction to guide the partner’s attention, as well as (c) responding to and (d) initiating behavior regulation including a give gesture. The authors found that responding to JA at 9 months and initiating JA at 18 months significantly correlated with language development at 24 months. Beuker et al. (2013) studied the developmental trajectory of the different skills in JA and language once a month from 8 to 24 months of age. For the sample of 23 children, an order of acquisition could be identified according to which the different JA skills and referential language emerged. However, this order of acquisition was true for only about 35% of the individual infants, indicating that development might be more like a matrix of factors mutually influencing each other (Beuker et al., 2013). Lüke et al. (2017) qualified these findings by showing that at 12 months of age, only index-finger pointing – and not whole-hand pointing (Figure 1) – predicted language skills at 24 months such as vocabulary size, morphological skills, and syntactic skills, which were assessed using standardized language measures. In this study, index-finger pointing at 12 months explained 17–21% of the variance of different linguistic skills at 24 months. However, no differences in pointing behavior could be found between caregivers of typical versus later-language-delayed children (Lüke et al., in press).

Looking at these results, it seems necessary to further explore the process of conventionalization manifested in the hand form and how children and their caregivers differ in their way of introducing, using, and cultivating conventionalized means of communication.

Conclusion

In our review, we attempted to capture the complexity of the driving force for infants’ pointing: For pointing comprehension, infants’ can build on sensitivities toward human motion. For pointing production, the early linkage of the vocal and motor systems (Iverson, 2010) yields a tight temporal coordination between canonical babbling and hand movements. Further in the development, even before infants speak their first words, they co-express their deictic gestures and vocalizations. Such early coupling attests to the integrated speech–gesture system. Furthermore, we have argued for caregivers responding to infants’ gestures once they can perform the appropriate movement. When experienced repeatedly, it becomes a means toward a joint goal in a process of conventionalization. If this argument is correct, then future training studies will find that particular goal-oriented activities can reinforce children’s pointing behavior. For further studies on the relationship of pointing and language development, it is necessary to analyze gesture development in language delayed children to reveal whether the vocal-motor coupling might be of different nature and whether it is taken up differently by the caregivers.

Author Contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work took place within the project GestEnTalker that is funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (RO 2443/3-1 and LU 2116/1-2). We are grateful for the constructive input we received from the reviewers who had commented on an earlier version of this paper.

References

Amano, S., Kezuka, E., and Yamamoto, A. (2004). Infant shifting attention from an adult’s face to an adult’s hand: a precursor of joint attention. Infant Behav. Dev. 27, 64–80. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.06.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baldwin, D. A., and Moses, L. J. (1996). The ontogeny of social information gathering. Child Dev. 67, 1915–1939. doi: 10.2307/1131601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, E., Camaioni, L., and Volterra, V. (1976). “Sensorimotor performatives,” in Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics, ed. E. Bates (New York, NY: Academic Press), 49–71.

Google Scholar

Begus, K., and Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Dev. Sci. 15, 611–617. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01160.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., and Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve–month–olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 359–375. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bertenthal, B. I., Boyer, T. W., and Harding, S. (2014). When do infants begin to follow a point? Dev. Psychol. 50, 2036–2048. doi: 10.1037/a0037152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beuker, K. T., Rommelse, N. N., Donders, R., and Buitelaar, J. K. (2013). Development of early communication skills in the first two years of life. Infant Behav. Dev. 36, 71–83. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M. (2014). Uncommon Understanding. Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children. London: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Blake, J., O’Rourke, P., and Borzellino, G. (1994). Form and function in the development of pointing and reaching gestures. Infant Behav. Dev. 17, 195–203. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bohn, M., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2015). Communication about absent entities in great apes and human infants. Cognition 145, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s Talk: Learning to Use Language, 1st Edn. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., and Colonnesi, C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy 5, 291–308. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0503_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carpendale, J. I., and Carpendale, A. B. (2010). The development of pointing: from personal directedness to interpersonal direction. Hum. Dev. 53, 110–126. doi: 10.1159/000315168

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., and Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Dev. Rev. 30, 352–366. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Barbaro, K., Johnson, C. M., Forster, D., and Deák, G. O. (2015). Sensorimotor decoupling contributes to triadic attention: a longitudinal investigation of mother–infant–object interactions. Child Dev. 87, 494–512. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Villiers Rader, N., and Zukow-Goldring, P. (2010). How the hands control attention during early word learning. Gesture 10, 202–221.

Google Scholar

Esseily, R., Jacquet, A.-Y., and Fagard, J. (2011). Handedness for grasping objects and pointing and the development of language in 14-month-old infants. Laterality 16, 565–585. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2010.499911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteve-Gibert, N., and Prieto, P. (2014). Infants temporally coordinate gesture-speech combinations before they produce their first words. Speech Commun. 57, 301–316. doi: 10.1016/j.specom.2013.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flom, R., Deák, G. O., Phill, C. G., and Pick, A. D. (2004). Nine-month-olds’ shared visual attention as a function of gesture and object location. Infant Behav. Dev. 27, 181–194. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franco, F., and Gagliano, A. (2001). Toddlers’ pointing when joint attention is obstructed. First Lang. 21, 289–321. doi: 10.1177/014272370102106305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gliga, T., and Csibra, G. (2009). One-year-old infants appreciate the referential nature of deictic gestures and words. Psychol. Sci. 20, 347–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02295.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., and Watson, J. D. (2000). A study of multimodal motherese: the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Dev. 71, 878–894. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin-Meadow, S. (1999). The role of gesture in communication and thinking. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 419–429. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01397-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E., and Iverson, J. M. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Dev. Sci. 10, 778–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00636.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gómez, J.-C. (2007). Pointing behaviors in apes and human infants: a balanced interpretation. Child Dev. 78, 729–734. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01027.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gredebäck, G., Melinder, A., and Daum, M. (2010). The development and neural basis of pointing comprehension. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 441–450. doi: 10.1080/17470910903523327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hannan, T. E. (1987). A cross-sequential assessment of the occurrences of pointing in 3- to 12-month-old human infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 10, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(87)90003-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heller, V., and Rohlfing, K. J. (2017). Reference as an interactive achievement: sequential and longitudinal analyses of labeling interactions ins shared book reading and free play. Front. Psychol. 8:139. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igualada, A., Bosch, L., and Prieto, P. (2015). Language development at 18 months is related to multimodal communicative strategies at 12 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 39, 42–52. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M. (2010). Multimodality in infancy: vocal-motor and speech-gesture coordinations in typical and atypical development. Enfance 3, 257–274. doi: 10.4074/S0013754510003046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., Longobardi, E., and Caselli, M. C. (1999). Gesturing in mother-child interactions. Cogn. Dev. 14, 57–75. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(99)80018-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., and Fagan, M. K. (2004). Infant vocal–motor coordination: precursor to the gesture–speech system? Child Dev. 75, 1053–1066. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00725.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iverson, J. M., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychol. Sci. 16, 367–371. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kettner, V. A., and Carpendale, J. I. M. (2013). Developing gestures for no and yes: head shaking and nodding in infancy. Gesture 13, 193–209. doi: 10.1075/gest.13.2.04ket

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knecht, S., Deppe, M., Dräger, B., Bobe, L., Lohmann, H., Ringelstein, E., et al. (2000). Language lateralization in healthy right-handers. Brain 123, 74–81. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.1.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., and Bard, K. A. (1996). Indexical and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 110, 346–353. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.110.4.346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

LeBarton, E. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., and Raudenbush, S. (2015). Experimentally-induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary. J. Cogn. Dev. 16, 199–220. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.858041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liszkowski, U. (2005). Human twelve-month-olds point cooperatively to share interest with and helpfully provide information for a communicative partner. Gesture 5, 135–154. doi: 10.1075/gest.5.1-2.11lis

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., and Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Dev. Sci. 7, 297–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., and Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. J. Cogn. Dev. 7, 173–187. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., and Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. J. Child Lang. 34, 1–20. doi: 10.1017/S0305000906007689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liszkowski, U., and Tomasello, M. (2011). Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cogn. Dev. 26, 16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lock, A. (1980). The Guided Reinvention of Language. London: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Lüke, C., Grimminger, A., Rohlfing, K. J., Liszkowski, U., and Ritterfeld, U. (2017). In infants’ hands: identification of preverbal infants at risk for primary language delay. Child Dev. 88, 484–492. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüke, C., Ritterfeld, U., Grimminger, A., Liszkowski, U., and Rohlfing, K. J. (in press). Development of pointing gestures in children with typical and delayed language acquisition. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res.

Marcos, H., Ryckebusch, C., and Rabain-Jamin, J. (2003). Adult responses to young children’s communicative gestures: joint achievement of speech acts. First Lang. 23, 213–237. doi: 10.1177/01427237030232004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masataka, N. (2003). The Onset of Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511489754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, D., Behne, T., Lieven, E., and Tomasello, M. (2012). Origins of the human pointing gesture: a training study. Dev. Sci. 15, 817–829. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01181.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morford, M., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (1992). Comprehension and production of gesture in combination with speech in one-word speakers. J. Child Lang. 19, 559–580. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900011569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Van Hecke, A. V., and Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Dev. 78, 938–954. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01042.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddy, V., Markova, G., and Wallot, S. (2013). Anticipatory adjustments to being picked up in infancy. PLoS ONE 8:e65289. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rohlfing, K. J., Longo, M. R., and Bertenthal, B. I. (2012). Dynamic pointing triggers shifts of visual attention in young infants. Dev. Sci. 15, 426–435. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01139.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossmanith, N., Costall, A., Reichelt, A. F., López, B., and Reddy, V. (2014). Jointly structuring triadic spaces of meaning and action: book sharing from 3 months on. Front. Psychol. 5:1390. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan,Ş., and Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Lang. 28, 182–199. doi: 10.1177/0142723707088310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C., and Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Dev. 78, 735–740.

Google Scholar

Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–691. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05000129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vauclair, J., and Cochet, H. (2013). Hand preference for pointing and language development in toddlers. Dev. Psychobiol. 55, 757–765. doi: 10.1002/dev.21073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social learning, gestures, pointing, multimodal interaction, conventionalization

Citation: Rohlfing KJ, Grimminger A and Lüke C (2017) An Interactive View on the Development of Deictic Pointing in Infancy. Front. Psychol. 8:1319. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01319

Received: 06 April 2017; Accepted: 18 July 2017;
Published: 02 August 2017.

Edited by:

Markus Paulus, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany

Reviewed by:

Stuart Ian Hammond, University of Ottawa, Canada
Christina Bergmann, École Normale Supérieure, France

Copyright © 2017 Rohlfing, Grimminger and Lüke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katharina J. Rohlfing, a2F0aGFyaW5hLnJvaGxmaW5nQHVuaS1wYWRlcmJvcm4uZGU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.