- 1Werklund School of Education – Counselling Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada
- 2Clinical Psychology, Centro Arboretum, San Salvador, El Salvador
- 3Department of Personality, Assessment and Psychological Treatment, Universitat de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
- 4Centro Aida, Santiago, Chile
- 5Institut de Trastorns Alimentaris, Barcelona, Spain
The purpose of this study was to explore the content of personal constructs in people diagnosed with bulimia nervosa (BN). We expected to find differences in the predominant content of the construct systems between women with and without BN. We analyzed the constructs elicited using the repertory grid technique from 120 women aged between 18 to 45 years, divided into two groups: a clinical group of women diagnosed with bulimia (n = 62) and a control group of university students (n = 58). The constructs were categorized using the Classification System for Personal Constructs (CSPC), composed of six themes which are broken down into 45 categories. For this study, a new area called “Physical” was included, and it consists of three categories. The results indicated that women diagnosed with bulimia used significantly more constructs related to the body, while the control group used more constructs from the personal area. In addition, the congruent constructs from the clinical sample were predominantly moral, or related to values and interests, while discrepant constructs were personal and physical. The findings provide evidence for the clinical use of the CSPC as an instrument for exploring the content of personal meaning systems. Understanding the patient’s personal constructions about herself and others is useful for treatment. Moreover, it is important for clinicians to explore the content of constructs related to symptomatic areas, which could be hindering change, and focus on them to facilitate improvement.
Introduction
As reflected in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013), diagnostic criteria for the eating disorders (ED) include the undue influence of body shape and weight on self-evaluation. Weight-based self-evaluation is a core feature of psychopathology common to Anorexia Nervosa (AN), Bulimia Nervosa (BN), Binge Eating Disorder (BED) and other cases of EDs (e.g., Fairburn et al., 2003; Fairburn, 2008; Grilo et al., 2013). Furthermore, this evaluation of the self, based on weight and shape, seems to be resistant to treatment and it is considered a maintaining factor and a predictor of relapse (Grilo et al., 2013; Trottier et al., 2013). In fact, evaluating self and self-worth mostly –or even exclusively- based on body features, combined with a perfectionistic ideal of shape and weight, is a vulnerability factor for low self-esteem (Lampard and Sharbanee, 2015).
The value and adequacy attributed to the self, usually referred to as self-esteem, is one of the most widely studied factors relating to EDs from different theoretical perspectives (e.g., Garner and Bemis, 1982; Button et al., 1996; Fairburn et al., 2003; Jacobi et al., 2004; Welch and Ghaderi, 2013), though not an exclusive factor for these disorders. More than 15 years ago, Bruch (1962, 1978) emphasized the feeling of inadequacy as an underlying psychological characteristic of patients with EDs, indicating lack of a clear and cohesive sense of self. More specifically, it has been remarked in the clinical and research literature that patients with BN lack a clearly defined sense of self and, therefore, they use their physical bodies as a means of self-definition and regulation (e.g., Schupak-Neuberg and Nemeroff, 1993). Eating related symptoms (such as restrained eating, binge eating, or purging) could be understood as attempts to improve the sense of self-worth and/or ease the negative emotions related to a negative self-esteem (Fairburn, 2008).
Even though a negative self-concept and impoverished identity have been widely related to EDs in general, and to BN in particular (Stein and Corte, 2007), previous research has operationalized these aspects of self-construing in different ways. Watson and Watts (2001) emphasized that the sense of self-worth is based on the similarity (or discrepancy) between the perception of the current self and the ideal self, in terms of those attributes deemed to be important for the person. This approach supports the use of theories that promote systematic assessment of the personal (and subjective) views of patients.
This present study is part of the Multicentre Dilemma Project1, a program of research focused on the role of cognitive conflicts in health, which is inspired by Kelly’s Personal Construct Theory (PCT; see Walker and Winter, 2007). PCT is grounded in a constructivist epistemology (Neimeyer and Mahoney, 1995; Botella and Feixas, 1998; Feixas and Villegas, 2000; Winter and Reed, 2015), according to which reality can only be accessed through interpretative dimensions erected by the subject. Kelly (1955/1991) suggested that human beings live and understand experience like scientists who build [informal] hypotheses to interpret, organize, and anticipate their experiences. Self, others, and reality are construed using a set of distinctions, known as personal constructs, created uniquely and personally from previous experience. They are usually represented as bipolar dimensions of meaning (such as “weak-strong” or “fat-thin”). By doing this, an interdependent, hierarchical, and complex meaning network is built, by which a person can understand and anticipate experience. In this network, some lower level constructs could be directly related to others of higher significance for the individual.
There are several techniques to explore someone’s personal constructs (e.g., self-characterization, laddering), but the most widely used is the Repertory Grid Technique (RGT), devised to reveal the dimensions and structure of the personal construct system (e.g., Fransella et al., 2004; Saúl et al., 2012). This semi-structured interview aims to identify the constructs by which a person idiosyncratically gives sense and meaning to experiences and organizes his or her world.
Consistent with the constructivist epistemology, RGT is useful for the assessment of perceived self-adequacy, based on those characteristics that are relevant to the person, and not in relation to items derived from theoretical constructs, or to the general desirability of certain features. RGT measures self-discrepancies based on Euclidean distances between current self and ideal self. Besides this general index of self-adequacy, it also provides information about self-discrepancies in each one of the constructs, taken individually. A construct is considered congruent if the position of current self and ideal self are similar, indicating that the person is, in that respect, the way he/she would like to be. On the other hand, if the current self is rated at one pole of the construct and the ideal self is positioned on the opposite pole; the construct is considered discrepant, indicating that this is an aspect in which the person wants to change.
The RGT has been applied to study a broad variety of areas of clinical (e.g., depression, schizophrenia, agoraphobia, self-injury) and health (irritable bowel syndrome, chronic pain, fibromyalgia, cancer) psychology. EDs have also been studied by PCT researchers using the RGT (e.g., Fransella and Crisp, 1970, 1979; Neimeyer and Khouzam, 1985; Button, 1993; Izu, 2007; Feixas et al., 2010; Dada et al., 2012). These studies showed that structural relationships among constructs and cognitive conflicts assessed with the RGT are useful for understanding the construing process of patients with EDs.
Within the Multicentre Dilemma Project, previous studies have focused on cognitive conflicts in EDs, with particular attention to Implicative Dilemmas (ID). An ID can be understood as a strong relationship with a construct based on which the person wishes to change (discrepant construct) and a construct on which changing is not desirable (congruent construct). Due to this association, change in the desired direction on the discrepant construct implies change in the undesired direction on the congruent construct (Feixas and Saúl, 2004). Previous studies found that IDs were more frequent in the grids of ED patients compared to women from non-clinical samples (Izu, 2007; Feixas et al., 2010; Dada et al., 2012; Dada, 2014). Nevertheless, the content of constructs involved in those dilemmas has received less attention.
Even though there is a considerable amount of research on the structural characteristics of construing, studies on the content of personal constructs is scarce. Landfield (1971) proposed a general classification system to categorize personal constructs. This system, however, had several methodological disadvantages such as not being exhaustive (Feixas, 1988). Feixas et al. (2002) presented the Classification System for Personal Constructs (CSPC) comprising 45 exclusive and comprehensive categories. This system is structured according to homogeneous levels of abstraction, and is based solely on the content of the constructs.
The CSPC has been used in previous research to analyze the patterns of construct content related to different clinical samples, such as patients with depression (Montesano et al., 2009; Feixas et al., 2014), women with fibromyalgia (Compañ et al., 2011) and victims of sexual abuse (Harter et al., 2004). To the extent of our knowledge, the content of personal constructs from BN patients has not been studied yet.
The purpose of this research is to analyze the cognitive content of women diagnosed with BN, and to compare it to that of a non-clinical sample in order to advance knowledge of their personal construct systems. Also, the content of the constructs involved in self-construction and in cognitive conflicts in patients with BN will be specifically examined.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The clinical group was comprised of 62 women recruited in two cohorts, from two previous studies (Izu, 2007; Feixas et al., 2010). All patients were recruited within the first month of treatment at the Institut de Trastorns Alimentaris (Barcelona, Spain), a treatment facility specializing in eating disorders, either in full or partial hospitalization. They fulfilled diagnostic criteria for BN at admission for treatment, and when assessed for this study. Age of patients ranged between 18–45 years (M = 25.08; SD = 6.16).
The control group was formed of 58 women, either undergraduate or graduate students from the University of Barcelona. During the assessment interview, the questions from the section on Eating Disorders of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Diagnosis (First et al., 1996) were used to screen participants in the control group. The age of participants composing the control group ranged between 18 and 45 years (M = 25.14; SD = 5.6) in age.
Instruments and Measures
Repertory Grid Technique
This is a semi-structured interview, originally proposed by Kelly (1955/1991). It can be adapted for the purposes of each particular research or clinical interest (Fransella et al., 2004; Feixas and Cornejo, 2002), resulting in a variety of formats. The format used in this study is designed to explore the participants’ constructs about their interpersonal world. Initially, the interviewer elicited elements for inclusion in the grid, which included the self now, the “ideal self” (i.e., “How I would like to be”), and significant others identified by the participant (e.g., parents, siblings, friends, current and/or former partner, and a persona non grata). These elements were contrasted in dyads, eliciting constructs describing similarities and differences between elements. An opposite or “construct pole” was also described for each similarity or difference. Elicitation continued till the person was unable to generate new constructs. The mean number of constructs elicited for the grids of the student group was 32.04, (SD = 9.12) and did not differ significantly [t(119) = 0.50, p = 0.620] from that of the BN group (32.81, SD = 7.78).
Following elicitation of constructs, the interviewer asked the participant to rate each element on each of his or her constructs. For these ratings, a 7-point Likert scale was used, ranging from “very much like the construct’s left pole” to “very much like the construct’s right pole.” These ratings provided a data matrix for each participant, with columns representing important people (including “current self” and “ideal self”) in the interpersonal world of the interviewee, and rows representing her construct dimensions.
The grids were analyzed using the GRIDCOR 4.0 software (Feixas and Cornejo, 2002) to identify congruent constructs, discrepant constructs, and constructs involved in IDs.
A congruent construct is a construct in which the self and the ideal self are rated on the same pole, and the difference between the rating for these elements is lower or equal to one point. This similarity between ratings indicates a characteristic on which the respondent sees her actual self akin to how she would ideally like to be.
On the contrary, a discrepant construct indicates dissatisfaction, revealed by a discrepancy between the respondent’s self-representation and what she considers ideal. It is a construct on which the person wishes to change. This is evidenced by a difference of 4 or more points between the ratings given to the self and the ideal self, which are therefore positioned at opposite poles of the construct.
Implicative Dilemmas were identified using the GRIDCOR 4.0 Software, based on the correlations between congruent constructs and discrepant constructs (Feixas et al., 2000; Feixas and Saúl, 2004).
Classification System for Personal Constructs
This is a coding system developed by Feixas et al. (2002) designed to classify the content of personal constructs derived from the RGT and other constructivist assessment procedures. This qualitative analysis by categories is useful to explore the presence or absence of a variety of aspects of content in the personal construct systems of grid respondents.
The CSPC is composed of 45 exclusive and mutually exclusive categories classified into six thematic areas. These areas are hierarchically ordered to increase the reliability of the system by eliminating potential overlap among them: whenever a construct fits within two or more areas, it is coded in the area of the higher level. Categories belonging to the same area are considered as hierarchically equivalent.
The CSPC focuses on what Feixas (1988) regards as value constructs, which include the meanings or attributions about one’s and others’ psychological traits. Other characteristics such as physical attributes were excluded, because they were considered superficial or irrelevant distinctions. However, for the purpose of this study another area was added to the system, in order to classify those contents related to the construction of the body. This new area is called “Physical,” and it is composed of three categories: body appearance, physical malaise, and attitude toward the body. We provided examples of the constructs that could be codified in each one of the categories included in this area (see Table 1), using the same format as in all 45 categories from the CSPC.
TABLE 1. Categories included in the “Physical Area” included for the CSPC for the purposes of this study.
Previous studies (Feixas et al., 2002; Montesano et al., 2009; Compañ et al., 2011; Feixas et al., 2014) have confirmed a high level of reliability of the CSPC. In effect, inter-rater agreement between judges encoding the same constructs separately and after discussing discrepancies in codings showed indices between 0.90 and 0.95 for both areas and categories. This level of reliability is quite satisfactory, more characterized of standardized scales in psychological testing than of a coding system.
Procedure
This study was carried out with the approval of the Bioethics Commission from the University of Barcelona (Institutional Review Board IRB00003099), with written informed consent from all subjects. Students were recruited from undergraduate and graduate courses at the University. The clinical participants were selected from the registries of the above-mentioned treatment facility, based on the inclusion criteria for the study: women, over 18 years old, diagnosed with BN. A brief interview was conducted, to confirm the diagnosis.
Once identified, each participant was contacted by the researcher, informed about the purposes of the studies, and granted written informed consent.
An interviewer, trained in the use of the RGT, assessed those participants who fulfilled inclusion criteria.
All constructs from the participants’ grids were classified by two independent judges, using the CSPC. To avoid bias, judges were not informed about the study’s hypothesis. Both judges were postgraduate students who received training in the CSPC. Each judge separately analyzed a total of 3,777 constructs extracted from the RGT from all 120 participants. Once all constructs were categorized with the CSPC, both judges compared their classifications for every single construct, discussed their discrepancies and registered final agreement or disagreement. Those constructs on which no agreement was reached between judges were excluded from further analyses. Data were analyzed using the statistical package SPSS v. 20.
Results
Inter-rater agreement was assessed using Cohen’s kappa statistic. Results indicated a very good agreement between judges before discussion (K = 0.813, p < 0.001), and after discussion (K = 0.968, p < 0.001). Therefore, the application of the CSPC can be considered as reliable.
A Chi-square test for independence indicated significant differences between groups regarding the distribution of constructs in the areas of content, χ2(7,n = 3,777) = 72.55, p < 0.001, φ = 0.14. In order to know which areas produced this difference between groups, standardized residuals (SR) were compared to the critical value of ±1.96 (α = 0.05). If the SR are greater than or equal to 1.96, or less than or equal to -1.96, then the observed frequency value is significantly different from the expected frequency (i.e., if the residual value is positive, then the group is over-represented, and if the value is negative, then the group is under-represented). As expected, constructs from the physical area were over-represented in the clinical sample. The non-clinical sample showed more constructs from the personal area than BN patients (see Table 2).
To analyze in more detail the differences between groups, a second analysis was performed to contrast the categories of constructs. Again, statistically significant differences between groups were found, χ2(48, n = 3776) = 137.586, p < 0.001, φ = 0.19.
As expected, constructs from the categories “body appearance” and “attitude toward the body” were overrepresented in BN patients compared to controls. On the other hand, participants of the non-clinical sample used more constructs from the “visceral-rational” category (See Table 3).
TABLE 3. Frequency of constructs and standardized residuals by area and category in BN patients and non-clinical controls.
The differences between congruent and discrepant constructs involved in IDs were analyzed separately for both groups. Significant differences were found both for the non-clinical group, χ2(7,n = 1210) = 36.602, p < 0.001, φ = 0.174 and for the clinical group, χ2(7,n = 1379) = 71.191, p < 0.001, φ = 0.227.
Analyzing the standardized residuals, we found that for the non-clinical group, congruent constructs are predominantly moral; while discrepant constructs are mostly personal and emotional (see Table 4).
TABLE 4. Frequency and standardized residuals of congruent and discrepant constructs from the non-clinical sample.
For BN patients, as shown in Table 5, moral constructs and those regarding values and interests were overrepresented as congruent constructs, while personal and physical constructs were more prominent in discrepant constructs.
TABLE 5. Frequency and standardized residuals of congruent and discrepant constructs from the clinical sample.
Discussion
The findings of this study suggest that the construction pattern, in terms of content, is the difference between patients with BN and women without EDs.
From many theoretical perspectives, it is understood that the excessive focus on the body as the primary source of self-definition may be a compensation, given the lack of other positive attributes perceived by patients with ED (Bruch, 1978; Jacobi et al., 2004; Stein et al., 2013). Despite this relative transtheoretical agreement, there are not many instruments available for systematic exploration of the construction of the self, based on personal meanings. This study showed promising results related to the use of RGT and the CSPC as means to explore the constructions of self and others.
Compared to controls, individuals with BN used fewer constructions from the personal area and many constructs from the physical area, particularly regarding body appearance and attitude toward the body, to construe themselves and others and to anticipate experience. Therefore, it is possible that, due to a lack of constructs to elaborate personal characteristics of self and others, women suffering from BN tend to exacerbate the attention paid to the body, perhaps as an attempt to achieve more predictability and control in social relationships.
When applied to oneself, moral constructions tend to be congruent, while constructs pertaining to the personal area are mostly discrepant, for both clinical and non-clinical participants. This finding suggests that, independently suffering from BN or not, these women perceived themselves as morally acceptable, and they would like to change some characteristics related to personality, character or way of being. Beside these similarities, we found that controls were unsatisfied with their emotionality while patients with BN wanted to change their physical characteristics and their attitude toward their bodies, and some personal characteristics as well. Moreover, results suggest that, due to IDs, change in those physical and personal constructs, although desirable, is linked to an undesirable change in moral constructs and values. This could contribute to the understanding of the difficulty of change in patients. EDs have been considered in both literature and research as ego-syntonic disorders, in which changing may be both desirable and undesirable, since it could be related to personal values (Serpell et al., 1999; Mulkerrin et al., 2016). An understanding about the relationship between a desired change, and the aspects of the self that are congruent and possibly nuclear, may facilitate treatment. Clinicians could benefit from identifying and elaborating IDs with the patient, maintaining those moral constructs and values that are desirable to the patient, while enhancing change.
There are some limitations that need to be acknowledged and taken into account when considering the findings from this study. Due to the low rates of men affected from eating disorders, only women were included in the study. In addition, all women included in the non-clinical sample were students, and the education level of the clinical sample was not recorded or considered for the analysis. Therefore, even though the results were statistically significant, they should be viewed as preliminary. Although requiring further exploration in a large-scale investigation, the findings of this study suggest that the content of personal constructs is relevant for the understanding of disordered eating. An additional research is needed to explore the construct system and its content in patients with other eating disorder diagnoses.
Considering that a weight-based evaluation of the self could be considered as a maintaining factor for eating disorders and a predictor of relapse (Trottier et al., 2013), psychological treatment should aim to elaborate diversified constructions of the self, which may include more aspects from other areas. The findings from this study suggest that RGT and CSPC can be used by clinicians to explore the content of personal constructs used by patients with BN, assessing those areas in which constructs are over-represented and those in which more elaboration is needed.
It is important for clinicians to explore the content of constructs related to symptomatic areas, which could be hindering change, and focus on them to facilitate improvement. The present study must be considered as a limited first step to identify prototypic types of dilemmas for eating disorders. Therapy manuals or protocols could be designed to work with dilemmas in this specific clinical population, as in the ones proposed for social anxiety (Winter, 1998) and depression (Feixas and Compañ, 2015).
Ethics Statement
The objectives and procedures of this study were discussed with those responsible within the treatment facility in which participants were recruited. Once potential participants were identified, they were informed about the study, emphasizing that their participation was completely voluntary, the assessment would not imply any changes in the treatment, and that they could withdraw from the study asking for the complete deletion of their data at any time before the publication of study. If the participants agreed, they proceeded to sign the informed consent before the interview. Till date, none of the participants has requested the deletion of their data.
Author Contributions
All authors of this study are part of a research team led by GF. This work is not an isolated study, but arises from the continuing work of this team in the field of eating disorders, to answer questions and limitations identified in previous studies (e.g., Feixas et al., 2010; Dada et al., 2012). The authors have worked together throughout the process, from conceptualization and development of the research problem, contact with treatment centers, data collection, analysis, to the drafting, revision and final approval of the article.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Footnotes
References
American Psychiatric Association [APA] (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM 5), 5th. Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
Botella, L., and Feixas, G. (1998). La Teoría de los Constructos Personales: Aplicaciones a la Práctica Psicológica [Personal Construct Theory: Applications to Psychological Practice]. Barcelona: Laertes.
Bruch, H. (1962). Perceptual and conceptual disturbances in anorexia nervosa. Psychosomatic Medicine 14, 187–194. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196203000-00009
Bruch, H. (1978). The Golden Cage. The Enigma of Anorexia Nervosa. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Button, E. J., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Davies, J., and Thompson, M. (1996). A prospective study of self-esteem in the prediction of eating problems in adolescent schoolgirls: questionnaire Findings. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 35, 193–203. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1996.tb01176.x
Compañ, V., Feixas, G., Varlotta, N., Torres, M., Aguilar, A., Dada, G., et al. (2011). Cognitive factors in fibromyalgia: the role of self-concept and identity related conflicts. J. Construct. Psychol. 24, 1–22.
Dada, G. (2014). Los Conflictos Cognitivos y la Construcción del sí Mismo y de los Otros en los Trastornos de la Conducta Alimentaria. Doctoral dissertation, University of Barcelona, Barcelona.
Dada, G., Feixas, G., Compañ, V., and Montesano, A. (2012). Self-construction, cognitive conflicts and disordered eating attitudes in young women. J. Construc. Psychol. 25, 70–89. doi: 10.1080/10720537.2012.629117
Fairburn, C. G. (2008). “Eating disorders: the transdiagnostic view and the cognitive behavioral theory,” in Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders, ed. C. G. Fairburn (New York, NY: Guilford), 7–22. doi: 10.1002/9781444300895.ch41
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., and Shafran, R. (2003). Cognitive behaviour therapy for eating disorders: a “transdiagnostic” theory and treatment. Behav. Res. Ther. 4, 509–528. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00088-8
Feixas, G. (1988). L’anàlisi de les Construccions Personals en Textos de Significació Psicològica. Doctoral dissertation, University of Barcelona, Barcelona.
Feixas, G., and Compañ, V. (2015). Manual de Intervención Centrada en Dilemas Para la Depresión. Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer.
Feixas, G., and Cornejo, J. M. (2002). GRIDCOR v.4.0: Correspondence Analysis for Grid Data [Computer Software and Repertory Grid Manual]. Barcelona: Psi media.
Feixas, G., Geldschalänger, H., Carmona, M., and Garzón, B. (2002). Sistema de categorías de contenido para codificar constructos personales. Rev. Psicol. Gen. Apl. 55, 337–348.
Feixas, G., Montebruno, C., Dada, G., del Castillo, M., and Compañ, V. (2010). Self-construction, cognitive conflicts and polarization in Bulimia Nervosa. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 10, 445–457.
Feixas, G., Montesano, A., Compañ, V., Salla, M., Dada, G., Pucurull, O., et al. (2014). Cognitive conflicts in major depression: between desired change and personal coherence. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 53, 369–385. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12050
Feixas, G., and Saúl, L. A. (2004). The multi-center dilemma project: an investigation on the role of cognitive conflicts in health. Span. J. Psychol. 7, 69–78. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600004765
Feixas, G., Saúl, L. A., and Sánchez Rodríguez, V. (2000). “Detection and analysis of implicative dilemmas: implications for the therapeutic process,” in The Person in Society: Challenges to a Constructivist Theory, ed. J. W. Scheer (Giessen: Psychosozial-Verlag), 391–399.
Feixas, G., and Villegas, M. (2000). Constructivismo y Psicoterapia [Constructivism and Psychotherapy]. Bilbao: Desclée de Brouwer.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. B. (1996). User’s Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Clinical Version. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Fransella, F., Bell, R., and Bannister, D. (2004). A Manual for Repertory Grid Technique, 2nd. Edn. Chichester: Wiley.
Fransella, F., and Crisp, A. H. (1970). Conceptual organisation and weight change. Psychother. Psychosom. 18, 176–185. doi: 10.1159/000286076
Fransella, F., and Crisp, A. H. (1979). Comparison of weight concepts in groups of neurotic, normal and anorexic Females. Br. J. Psychiatry 134, 79–86. doi: 10.1192/bjp.134.1.79
Garner, D. M., and Bemis, K. M. (1982). A cognitive-behavioral approach to anorexia nervosa. Cogn. Ther. Res. 6, 123–150. doi: 10.1007/BF01183887
Grilo, C. M., White, M. A., Gueorguieva, R., Wilson, G. T., and Masheb, R. M. (2013). Predictive significance of the overvaluation of shape/weight in obese patients with binge eating disorder: findings from a randomized controlled trial with 12-month follow-up. Psychol. Med. 43, 1335–1344. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002097
Harter, S., Erbes, C., and Hart, C. (2004). Content analysis of the personal constructs of female sexual abuse survivors elicited through Repertory Grid Technique. J. Constr. Psychol. 17, 27–43. doi: 10.1080/10720530490250679
Izu, S. (2007). Disociación y Factores Cognitivos en la Bulimia Nerviosa. Master’s thesis, University of Barcelona, Barcelona.
Jacobi, C., Paul, T., de Zwaan, M., Nutzinger, D., and Dahme, B. (2004). Specificity of self-concept disturbances in eating disorders. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 35, 204–210. doi: 10.1002/eat.10240
Lampard, A. M., and Sharbanee, J. M. (2015). The cognitive-behavioural theory and treatment of bulimia nervosa: an examination of treatment mechanisms and future directions. Austr. Psychol. 50, 6–13. doi: 10.1111/ap.12078
Montesano, A., Feixas, G., and Varlotta, N. (2009). Análisis de contenido de constructos personales en la depresión [Content analysis of personal contructs in depression]. Salud Ment. 32, 371–379.
Mulkerrin, U., Bamford, B., and Serpell, L. (2016). How well does Anorexia Nervosa fit with personal values? An exploratory study. J. Eat. Disord. 4, 20. doi: 10.1186/s40337-016-0109-z
Neimeyer, G. R., and Khouzam, N. (1985). A repertory grid study of restrained eaters. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 58, 365–367. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1985.tb02654.x
Neimeyer, R. A., and Mahoney, M. J. (eds) (1995). Constructivism in Psychotherapy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10170-000
Saúl, L. A., López-González, M. A., Moreno-Pulido, A., Corbella, S., Compañ, V., and Feixas, G. (2012). Bibliometric review of the repertory grid technique: 1998–2007. J. Constr. Psychol. 25, 112–131. doi: 10.1080/10720537.2012.651065
Schupak-Neuberg, E., and Nemeroff, C. J. (1993). Disturbances in identity and self-regulation in bulimia nervosa: implications for a metaphorical perspective of “body as self”. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 13, 335–347. doi: 10.1002/1098-108X(199305)13:4<335::AID-EAT2260130402>3.0.CO;2-M
Serpell, L., Treasure, J., Teasdale, J., and Sullivan, V. (1999). Anorexia nervosa: friend or foe? A qualitative analysis of the themes expressed in letters written by anorexia nervosa patients. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 25, 177–186. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199903)25:2<177::AID-EAT7>3.0.CO;2-D
Stein, K. F., and Corte, C. (2007). Identity impairment and the eating disorders: content and organization of the self-concept in women with anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 15, 58–69. doi: 10.1002/erv.726
Stein, K. F., Corte, C., Chen, D. G. D., Nuliyalu, U., and Wing, J. (2013). A randomized clinical trial of an identity intervention programme for women with eating disorders. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 21, 130–142. doi: 10.1002/erv.2195
Trottier, K., McFarlane, T., and Olmsted, M. P. (2013). A test of the weight – based self – evaluation schema in eating disorders: understanding the link between self – esteem, weight – based self – evaluation, and body dissatisfaction. Cogn. Ther. Res. 37, 122–126. doi: 10.1007/s10608-012-9446-7
Walker, B. M., and Winter, D. A. (2007). The elaboration of personal construct psychology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 453–477. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085535
Watson, N., and Watts, R. (2001). The predictive strength of personal constructs versus conventional constructs: self-image disparity and neuroticism. J. Pers. 69, 121–145. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00138
Welch, E., and Ghaderi, A. (2013). “Eating disorders and self-esteem,” in Girls at Risk, ed. A. K. Andershed (New York, NY: Springer), 35–56.
Winter, D., and Reed, N. (2015). The Wiley Handbook of Personal Construct Psychology. Sussex: Wiley. doi: 10.1002/9781118508275
Keywords: bulimia nervosa, RepGrid, personal construct, content analysis, eating disorders
Citation: Dada G, Izu S, Montebruno C, Grau A and Feixas G (2017) Content Analysis of the Construction of Self and Others in Women with Bulimia Nervosa. Front. Psychol. 8:480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00480
Received: 08 October 2016; Accepted: 14 March 2017;
Published: 06 April 2017.
Edited by:
Alix Timko, University of Pennsylvania, USAReviewed by:
Hemal Shroff, Tata Institute of Social Sciences, IndiaLucy Serpell, University College London, UK
Copyright © 2017 Dada, Izu, Montebruno, Grau and Feixas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Gloria Dada, gloria.dadasanchez@ucalgary.ca