Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 17 January 2017
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Parenthood from Biology to Relation. Prevention, Assessment and Interventions for Developmental and Clinical Issues View all 32 articles

Infant Massage and Quality of Early Mother–Infant Interactions: Are There Associations with Maternal Psychological Wellbeing, Marital Quality, and Social Support?

  • Department of Developmental and Social Psychology, University of Padua, Padua, Italy

Infant massage programs have proved to be effective in enhancing post-natal development of highly risk infants, such as preterm newborns and drug or HIV exposed children. Less studies have focused on the role of infant massage in supporting the co-construction of early adult–child relationships. In line with this lack of literature, the present paper reports on a pilot study aimed at investigating longitudinally the quality of mother–child interactions, with specific reference to emotional availability (EA), in a group of mother–child pairs involved in infant massage classes. Moreover, associations between mother–child EA, maternal wellbeing, marital adjustment, and social support were also investigated, with the hypothesis to find a link between low maternal distress, high couple satisfaction and high perceived support and interactions of better quality in the dyads. The study involved 20 mothers and their children, aged between 2 and 7 months, who participated to infant massage classes. The assessment took place at three stages: at the beginning of massage course, at the end of it and at 1-month follow-up. At the first stage of assessment self-report questionnaires were administered to examine the presence of maternal psychiatric symptoms (SCL-90-R), perceived social support (MSPSS), and marital adjustment (Dyadic Adjustment Scale); dyadic interactions were observed and rated with the Emotional Availability Scales (Biringen, 2008) at each stage of data collection. The results showed a significant improvement in the quality of mother–child interactions, between the first and the last evaluation, parallel to the unfolding of the massage program, highlighting a general increase in maternal and child’s EA. The presence of maternal psychological distress resulted associated with less optimal mother–child emotional exchanges, while the hypothesis regarding couple satisfaction and social support influence were not confirmed. These preliminary results, if replicated, seem to sustain the usefulness of infant massage and the importance of focusing on early mother–infant interactions.

Theoretical Background

Infant Massage

The practice of infant massage represents a simple but effective way to enhance and strengthen healthy social and emotional relationships between adults and children in early infancy; this is true both from a relational and a practical point of view. On the one hand, in fact, massaging a baby requires and intensifies a series of multimodal and interactive competencies (such as emotional expression, eye-contact, physical touch, vocal communication, and turn-taking) that usually characterize adult–child daily repertoires; in this sense, it represents a privileged moment to cultivate and strengthen the relationship. On the other hand, instead, it is cost-saving and no contraindications were reported up to now; thus, it can be used frequently and without risk, accompanying and supporting a process existing per se, which is the one of adult–infant bonding.

Together with the practices of breastfeeding, baby carrying, and co-sleeping, infant massage is part of a wider “caretaking-package” which involves a set of behaviors necessary to satisfy the child’s needs for contact, holding, communication, and feeding; these needs are simple and primitive but often unrecognized (Balsamo, 2007). A particular feature of these behaviors is that they specifically require tactile interactions with the infant and that they are able to convey a series of somatosensory messages about feelings, pressure, temperature, softness, or pain (Underdown, 2009). As already shown by the research on sensorial deprivation in early infancy, these experiences are fundamental for physical growth, neurological development and for the construction of healthy affective relationships (Robertson and Bowlby, 1952; Harlow and Zimmermann, 1958; Montagu, 1971; Blackwell, 2000; Kim et al., 2003).

From a neural-developmental point of view, touch is one of the first sensorial systems to be activated in the fetus during pregnancy, becoming, thus, one of the primary means of communication with the surrounding environment (Relier, 1994; Hernandez-Reif et al., 2007). After delivery, touch becomes an important channel of communication during mother–infant interactions (De Chateau, 1976; Tronick, 1995; Stack, 2004; Moszkowski and Stack, 2007; Jean and Stack, 2009; Stack et al., unpublished). It occurs more than the 55% of time and communicates security and tenderness, reducing infants’ distress and promoting emotional regulation (Stack and Muir, 1990, 1992; Weiss et al., 2000; Jean et al., 2004; Jean and Stack, 2009). Empirical studies have widely stressed that sensitive caregiving allows the baby not to feel overwhelmed, moderating or accelerating his/her emotions and intervening at a neuro-physiological level on the reactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system (Tronick, 1989; Nelson and Bosquet, 2000; Beebe and Lachmann, 2002). In this sense, early experiences of touch represent one of the means through which the adult works as an external regulator for the baby, helping the co-regulation of his/her behavioral and emotional states and consequently preventing him/her from experiencing developmental difficulties, such as sleep and feeding problems (De Chateau, 1976; Koniak-Griffin and Ludington-Hoe, 1988; Brazelton, 1990; Underdown, 2009).

Infant touching and infant massage are also part of a “high-” or “proximal-contact” model of caretaking (Stork, 1986; Balsamo, 2002), that promotes intense physical and psychological contact between mother and child, with the aim to hold the child and to protect him/her from dangers. From this point of view, and given the relational and developmental influences of early experiences of touch, infant massage appears a useful technique to support parenting by promoting sensitivity and enhancing the construction of healthy affective bonds between adults and children (Bozza, unpublished).

Research on infant massage as intervention has been applied especially to preterm infants. In this particular population the exposure to infant massage sessions resulted in an improvement in several health indexes, such as weight-gain, increases in length, head circumference, bone density, and body temperature (Scafidi et al., 1986; Scafidi et al., 1990; Kuhn et al., 1991; Wheeden et al., 1993; Moyer Mileur et al., 1995; Jinon, 1996; Dieter et al., 2001, 2003; Ke et al., 2001; Duan et al., 2002; Ferber et al., 2002; Liu, 2005; Lu et al., 2005; Na et al., 2005; Diego et al., 2008).

Moreover, massage therapy resulted also capable of modifying the distribution of sleep/awake states, favoring longer periods of active alertness and reducing excitability (Scafidi et al., 1986, 1990; Field et al., 1987, 2004; Wheeden et al., 1993; Dieter et al., 2003).

As far as it concerns other groups at high-risk, infant massage sessions have worked successfully on HIV- and drug-exposed newborns, leading to fewer medical complications, less irritability and increased weight, and to an improvement in the performance on social and emotional scales (Scafidi et al., 1990; Wheeden et al., 1993; Scafidi and Field, 1996; Dieter et al., 2001).

Although most non-at-risk infants will receive adequate sensitive handling, the administration of infant massage with regular frequency can represent an useful way to support parenting and to promote caregivers’ sensitive touch in early infancy (Underdown, 2009). Some authors emphasized the bidirectional effects of giving and receiving massage, supporting the use of the technique as a safe and cost-effective intervention for adult–child relationships (Feijò et al., 2006). Firstly, in fact, maternal resources can be enhanced by massage lessons through the promotion of a better knowledge of the infant’s needs and characteristics; secondly, the child’s features can be better modulated thanks to a more satisfying contact with the caregiver (Oswalt et al., 2009).

Massages can be given to children on a daily basis and they are economically saving when parents are enrolled as therapists (Field, 2002). The massage of infants promotes a general sense of wellbeing in the adult, helping the parent to feel close to the baby, and less fearful of touching and handling him/her. Moreover mothers who massaged their infants, both preterm and full term, reported less anxiety, less depressed mood, and improved mother–child interactions (Onozawa et al., 2001; Ferber et al., 2005; Feijò et al., 2006). Teaching parents to massage their infants, especially in risk conditions, often lowers anxiety levels related to the feelings of helplessness (Field, 2002). As far as it concerns the child, infant massage reduces and balances cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine hormones which control stress levels (Acolet et al., 1993; Field et al., 1996). As a consequence, this practice could be considered as a facilitator of the normal development of these catecholamines that characterize early stages of life (Kuhn et al., 1991).

Promoting infant massage lessons may represent an ideal way to support parenting and to support early emotional and social relationships between adults and children, useful both for high- and low-risk groups (Underdown, 2009). This technique offers a perspective which is simple but very useful, since it combines a behavioral intervention (i.e., the physical manipulation of the baby) with a profoundly emotional dimension (i.e., the close psychological and emotional proximity that massage enhances). Moreover, participating to massage classes constitutes an opportunity to meet other adults that are living the same experience, to share and compare different points of view, to create new bonds (Adamson, 1996).

Parenting and Early Interactions from a Multi-Determined Point of View

Parenting encompasses a broad range of nurturing and care-taking actions performed by caregivers toward the child. The actual behaviors that parents provide are among the most salient aspects of parenting, since the most of the experience of infants stems directly from interactions with caregivers occurring daily (Bornstein, 2002). Recently, increasing attention has been given to the emotional features that accompany parenting routines; more specifically, an important contribution to the investigation of parenting has been given by the theoretical frame of Emotional Availability (EA), which has pointed out the importance to create an emotional connection and to be able to share a wide range of affects during caretaking behaviors (Biringen, 2008; Biringen and Easterbrooks, 2012). As pointed out above, infant massage can be considered as a component of care-taking behaviors, that offers a valuable interactive context to parents, who can involve in pleasant and rewarding exchanges with their babies. Thanks to Belsky’s work on parenting (1984), nowadays it is well acknowledged that parenting, in all its components, is a multi-determined construct, encompassing both contextual variables and baby’s and parents’ individual characteristics. Empirical evidence attests that some of these factors are of particular relevance, such as the quality of marital relationship, maternal psychological wellbeing, and perceived social support.

The transition to parenthood is an event of the family life cycle that asks the couple to face potentially stressful changes and challenges (Belsky and Rovine, 1990; Cowan and Cowan, 2000) both at an inner and behavioral level (Cowan and Hetherington, 2001), in order to meet child’s need and develop parenting competences. In these terms, it is a critical period for marital satisfaction, which goes through a small but reliable decline, persisting at least until the preschool age as reported by several studies on dyadic satisfaction (Belsky et al., 1983; Terry et al., 1991; Favez et al., 2012; Kohn et al., 2012; Trillingsgaard et al., 2014). Women seem to experience a steeper and more precipitous drop in marital quality (Shapiro et al., 2000; Twenge et al., 2003; Bower et al., 2013), especially when they are scarcely satisfied with the division of labor and they manage a greater amount of childcare activities (Belsky, 1986; Gjerdingen and Center, 2005; Dew and Wilcox, 2011; Chong and Mickelson, 2013). However, other studies indicate that mothers and fathers share similar perception of post-birth relationship functioning (Lawrence et al., 2008; Doss et al., 2009; Jackson et al., 2014).

The transition toward parenthood is a very critical and stressful stage that may lead to serious psychological distress symptoms in pregnant women and women that recently gave birth to their offsprings, ranging from 4.8% (Glasheen et al., 2015) to 19% (Czarnocka and Slade, 2000; Saurel-Cubizolles et al., 2007; Gavin et al., 2011). Maternal psychological distress appears to be enduring (Horwitz et al., 2007) and, given that the early postpartum months are especially important for the establishment of a satisfactory dyadic relationship and for infant development (Hay and Kumar, 1995; Murray et al., 2015), it may negatively affect child outcomes (Goodman et al., 2011), mother–infant interactions (Singer et al., 2003), conjugal and family relationships (Whisman, 2001; Sutter-Dallay, 2006). Some studies highlight that marital conflict shows the most significant association with maternal distress in women who have recently given birth (Stemp et al., 1986).

Depression represents one of the most frequent distress conditions in the context of the transition to motherhood; review and meta-analytic studies have demonstrated that depression is linked to a range of adverse behavioral and emotional outcomes for the child, in terms of psychopathology and negative affects and behaviors, especially for younger children (Goodman, 2007; Goodman et al., 2011). Maternal depression is linked with impaired mother–infant interactive patterns since early age (i.e., 4 months) in both partners, especially concerning self-and interactive contingency (Beebe et al., 2012; Murray et al., 2015); depressed mothers are described by Weinberg and Tronick (1997) as intrusive or withdrawn. Notably, improving maternal depression does not imply per se an enhancement of the quality of mother–infant interaction, indicating the importance of targeting not merely mother’s depression and adopting instead a broader approach (Forman et al., 2007). A pilot study (Onozawa et al., 2001) reports that depressed mothers beneficiate from attending a massage class, attenuating depression symptoms and learning to interact better with their babies, leading in turn to an improvement in dyadic interaction (maternal attitudes and behaviors toward the child and infants’ responses), more than mothers in a support group did. Another study (O’Higgins et al., 2008) attests a clinically significant reduction in depression for the majority of mothers, even though scores remained high.

In a wider perspective, parenting and child development inextricably take place in the context of social relationships. Social support can be defined as the amount of advice received and personal needs fulfilled through the presence and interaction with significant others, within or outside the family, such as partners, relatives, or friends (Kaplan et al., 1977). Social support has been widely linked to individual wellbeing and positive mental health at an individual level (Mitchell and Trickett, 1980); in particular, women are more likely to give social support, draw on socially supportive networks in times of stress and, more importantly, benefit from it (Taylor, 2011).

Moreover, according to Belsky’s (1984) model, social support is a key determinant of parenting quality; it plays a role (as mediator, moderator or as a direct influence), in influencing parent–child interactions (Jennings et al., 1991), attitudes about parenting, parenting behaviors and, in turn, child outcomes (Feiring et al., 1987; Melson et al., 1993; Bender and Losel, 1998). Pregnant women that can rely on others for support show better outcomes for stress, depression and anxiety (Glazier et al., 2004). As regards mothers, the advantages of social support, especially if it is provided from one’s own mother, can be detected in the domains mental health, reducing the risk of postpartum depression, and of marital satisfaction (Findler et al., 2008); in fact, social support is important for the couple too, being associated to partners’ cohesion and intimacy (Cutrona, 1996). Nevertheless, support to mothers peaks in the first weeks after childbirth, but it appears to decrease in the following months (Pinelli, 2000; Rowe and Jones, 2010). If social support is typically considered as a positive influence on parenting, it is also important to note that it can exert an adverse influence, in terms of stressful or non-supportive relationships that lead to decreased wellbeing (Ingersoll-Dayton et al., 1997).

Interestingly, Herwig et al. (2004) have simultaneously considered maternal parenting and depression, couple satisfaction and social support in their associations with child outcomes, reporting the predictive role of maternal parenting and couple satisfaction and the indirect influence of depression and social support on child development.

Aims and Hypothesis

To our knowledge, to date no studies have examined the quality of mother–infant interactions in the context of infant massage through the application of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS; Biringen, 2008), nor have adopted a longitudinal approach. The present preliminary research aimed to investigate mother–child EA during infant massage classes. EA refers to the capacity of a dyad to share an emotionally healthy relationship and has been widely used in research to assess the quality of parenting and of adult–child relationships. Moreover, we adopted a longitudinal perspective, in order to verify whether EA changed or remained stable during the unfolding of infant massage lessons. Finally, according to the extant literature that highlights the role of multiple factors in shaping the quality of early parenting practices, we investigated whether EA was associated to mothers’ perception of couple adjustment, social support and psychological wellbeing. An increase in dyadic EA was expected and we hypothesized that more optimal adult–child interactions would be associated with a lower degree of maternal psychological distress, and with a higher level of couple satisfaction and perceived social support.

Materials and Methods

The present study adopted a descriptive and correlational design for providing preliminary data on the longitudinal investigation of mother–infant EA during massage classes.

Participants1

The sample of the study was composed of 20 mother–child pairs selected from a larger group of participants enrolled in an infant massage course. The children’s (10 boys and 10 girls) age ranged from 2 to 7 months (M = 2.6, SD = 1.392). Sixty-five percent of the subjects could be assessed longitudinally. The remaining subjects (35%) were not tested longitudinally due to obstacles preventing mothers from attending the scheduled lessons. The mothers were all Caucasian; their age ranged from 27 to 38 years old (M = 31.75, SD = 2.751). Inclusion criteria for the study included: being primiparous, being subject to the first experience of infant massage, having attended all massage classes with the same conductor, absence of infant pathology.

Socio-demographic information regarding different domains such as level of education and occupation was collected through a self-report questionnaire. Twenty percent of the mothers reported to be an only child, while the others reported one (40%) or more (40%) siblings. Concerning education, the 10% of the group declared to have a middle school certificate, while the others reported to have an high school diploma (35%) or an academic degree (50%). As far as it regards work, 15% of the mothers reported to be unemployed, while the remaining declared to have a job in the working class (10%), as employees (45%) or in other forms (30%).

Procedure and Instruments

Massage courses were presented during childbirth classes in different Venetian sanitary districts. Once enrolled the women were contacted after delivery and invited to participate to the course. Participation was free. The dyads were divided randomly in groups of 5, each of these represented a massage class. At the beginning of the course each mother was given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home, aimed at investigating socio-demographic information, maternal psychological well being, marital relationships and perceived social support (see the section Quality of Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons). The participants were told that they would have been videotaped three times during the cycle of massage classes (during T1, T3, and T4). Informed consent was asked to both parents before videotaping the baby. Figure 1 resumes the research design, and the variables assessed during the different times of measurement.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Times of measurement, intervention, and variables assessed.

The Infant Massage Program

The dyads were offered a cycle of four weekly lessons on infant massage. Each encounter lasted about an hour and a half. Massage courses were provided by a trained psychotherapist. During the course each room was warm and quiet in its atmosphere. Cameras were located in different angles of the room at a distance that allowed to capture the most salient aspects of each dyad’s interaction without interfering with the appropriate atmosphere.

Every mother was told to bring a cushion big enough to contain the baby and some natural oil. Activities took place on the floor, sitting in circle on wickers, in front of their babies, holding cushions firm with their legs. This condition allowed the mothers to hold their babies and to enhance visual contact with them. The conductor sit in the center of the circle in order to be visible for all the participants, and reproduced on a doll the various techniques taught during each encounter. In this way, the conductor did not touch the babies but only showed the participants how to massage.

The first lesson (T1) concerned an initial introduction to the massage course and to the research project. After a brief presentation of the equipe, the mothers were given notions in order to experience in the best way possible the encounters: they were invited to pay attention and to follow their children’s needs, to wait for optimal moments before beginning massage sessions, to feed their babies or to let them sleep whether necessary, to calm them down as they were used to. After a brief moment of relaxation, the mothers were invited to undress their infants from waist down, in order to be able to massage their legs and their abdomen. Instructions focused on how to handle the baby and how to touch, on how to pay attention and to become aware of the baby’s signals. The mothers were lead through a group discussion on massage benefits and they were invited to repeat the procedure at home. The first encounter was video-recorded.

During the second lesson (T2) the conductor introduced another sequence of massage procedures, focusing on face, superior arts, and chest. Alternative positions to use massage were presented and parents were positively reinforced during their efforts. Moreover, the mothers were given the possibility to share their feelings about maternity with the group. The conductor stressed the fact that the massage represents a technique with a particular focus on mother–child relationship, a relationship that can be improved dedicating more time to practice at home and, in turn, to the relationship.

The third lesson (T3) concerned the repetition of the entire sequence applied to the frontal body, also adding some suggestions on how to approach the back during massage. Again, some space during this encounter was left to allow the mothers to talk about their experience of the postpartum period. A bibliography was suggested to acquire more knowledge regarding the themes emerged during the cycle of lessons. This encounter was video-recorded.

After a month the group met again for a so-called moment of reinforce and of review of the techniques previously learned. This session was videotaped (T4) and considered as a follow-up.

Quality of Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons

The first (T1), the third (T3), and the fourth (T4) lessons were videotaped. For every dyad 20 min of mother–child interaction were recorded during each episode. The interactions were coded using the fourth version of the EAS (Biringen, 2008). The coding system is composed of six scales/dimensions, four for the adult (sensitivity, structuring, non-intrusiveness, and non-hostility) and two aimed at evaluating the child’s contribution (responsiveness, involvement of the adult).

Adult sensitivity refers to quality of adult affects, clarity of perceptions and appropriate responsiveness, awareness of timing, flexibility, variety and creativity during play, acceptance of the child, amount of interactions, and adequate resolution of conflicts.

Adult structuring concerns the use of proactive guidance, the success of attempts, the amount of guidance, the ability to set limits and to remain firm in the face of pressure, the use of both verbal and non-verbal suggestions and the ability to assume an adult role rather than a peer one.

Adult non-intrusiveness refers to the ability to follow the child’s lead, to the use of non-interruptive ports of entry into interaction, to the modest use of commands and directives, to the appropriateness of teaching and adult talking, to the absence of interferences and of child’s signals that indicate that the adult is perceived as intrusive.

Adult non-hostility refers to the lack of negativity in face or voice and to the lack of ridiculing or other disrespectful behaviors toward the child. A non-hostile adult does not threat to separate, is not frightening, maintains cool during challenging situations and does not use threats of hostile play themes during interactions.

Child responsiveness takes into account quality of child’s affects and organization of behaviors, the ability and the willingness to respond to the adult’s bids without anxiety or role reversal. It also considers positive physical positioning, concentration on task and the presence of avoidance or of over responsiveness and role reversal.

Child involvement of the adult concerns the use of simple and elaborative initiative to involve the adult, the affective use of the adult (rather than instrumental), the lack of negative/over involving behaviors, and the use of verbal and non-verbal channels.

Each EA dimension is given a global score on a 7-point scale, with higher ratings referring to more optimal features. Scores between 5 and 7 are considered adequate and index of a healthy relationship. Scores around 4 indicate inconsistency, (i.e., behaviors that are appropriate in some way but that are not fully healthy). Scores of 3 or below indicate less optimal interactions were problematic behaviors might arise (scores of 1 or 2). The coding refers to the global quality of the interaction observed rather than on specific behaviors. To get a more specific profile of the adult–child relationship, the EA assessment system provides the coders also a Clinical-Screener that allows to attribute each member of the dyad to one of four “zones” (according to the scores given to maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness), which represent four possible categories of EA: the Emotionally Available zone, the Complicated zone, the Detached zone and the Problematic zone. Mother–child interactions were coded by two independent raters previously trained on the EA coding system in order to reach satisfactory reliability with the Biringen’s lab. Inter-rater reliability was calculated on the 20% of the videos using ICCs which ranged from 0.80 to 0.95.

Self-report Measures

At the beginning of the massage course the mothers were given a battery of self-report questionnaires to fill in at home before the second lesson. The instruments aimed to assess socio-demographic information, maternal psychological wellbeing, marital relationships and perceived social support.

The Symptom Checklist-90 Revised (SCL-90-R; Derogatis, 1977; Italian version by Sarno et al., 2011) is a brief questionnaire designed to evaluate the presence of psychological distress and a range of psychopathological symptoms. It consists of 90 items and yields nine scores along primary symptom dimensions and three scores that refer to global distress indexes. The primary assessed symptom dimensions are somatization, obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxiety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. The three global indexes refer to global psychological distress status (Global Severity Index – GSI), to the total number of symptoms reported (Positive Symptom Total – PST) and to the intensity of reported distress (Positive Symptom Distress Index – PSDI). According to T-scores, each item can be interpreted as below average scores, within the median range, above average scores and definitely above the average scores of the normative sample (thus indicating severe symptomatology).

The Dyadic Adjustment Scale (DAS; Spanier, 1976; Italian version by Gentili et al., 2002) is a multidimensional tool that allows to assess conjugal adjustment. The sum of the 32 items lead to four dimension of conjugal adjustment (Dyadic Consensus, Dyadic Satisfaction, Dyadic Cohesion, and Affective Expression) and to a global score (Total Adjustment Score) that represents the degree of conjugal adjustment perceived by the partners.

The Multidimensional Scale on Perceived Social Support (MSPSS; Zimet et al., 1988; Italian version by Prezza and Principato, 2002) is a self-report questionnaire about the perceived adequacy of support given by the partner, family and friends. The 12 items are given a score on a 7-point Likert Scale and their sum leads to three dimensions concerning support received by the family, by friends and by a significant other.

Results

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS statistics vers. 23. Firstly, descriptive statistics (frequencies, mean scores, and percentages) were examined. Secondly, non-parametric tests were applied; more specifically, we adopted the Friedman and Wilcoxon signed-rank and the Spearman’s Rho to test for any associations between the different instruments adopted in the research design.

Preliminary Analysis

During preliminary analysis, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient was used to assess the reliability of the instruments. Descriptive statistics (average scores, frequencies, and percentages) were examined.

Mother–Child Interactions during Massage Lessons

The application of Cronbach’s alpha coefficient, to all the three periods considered, reported good reliability for EA maternal scales (0.85 ≤ α ≤ 0.89), for the EA child’s scales (0.73 ≤ α ≤ 0.80) and for all the six scales considered globally (0.87 ≤ α ≤ 0.90). Tables 1 and 2 report average scores, standard deviations and the distribution of the dyads assessed through the EAS and the EA clinical screener. As it is possible to observe from Table 1, regarding the EA Scales, during T1 the dyads reported on average score 4 (indicating inconsistency) in two maternal dimensions and in both child dimensions, while scores on the other dimensions resulted adequate (≥5). During T3 and T4 average scores resulted adequate (≥5) for all of the six dimensions. As far as it concerns the distribution of the dyads in the zones of the EA clinical screener, as it is possible to see in Table 2 during the periods considered a progressively larger amount of dyads fell in the Emotionally Available (EA) zone, while the “lower” zones were progressively less represented.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Average scores and standard deviations of the Emotional Availability Scales (EAS) applied during T1, T3, and T4.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Distribution of the dyads on the zones of the emotional availability (EA) clinical screener during T1, T3, and T4.

Psychological Distress

As far as it concerns psychological distress, Cronbach’s alpha was applied to the SCL-90-R symptom dimensions and to the global distress indexes. Good internal consistency was reported for obsessive-compulsive (0.82), interpersonal sensitivity (0.72), social phobia (0.72), paranoid ideation (0.78), psychoticism (0.80), and for the total of the items (0.90). Reliability was acceptable for somatization (0.67) and depression (0.70), poor for anxiety (0.56) and unacceptable for hostility (≤0.50), which was excluded from subsequent analysis. Table 3 reports average scores, standard deviations and the distribution among normative cut-offs for the SCL-90-R scores. As it is possible to see, the majority of the subjects fell within the SCL-90-R normative cut-off values while a smaller percentage of mothers reported values above the norm, suggesting the presence of significant psychological distress.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the SCL-90-R.

Couple Adjustment

Regarding couple adjustment, Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported good internal consistency for the DAS total score (0.77) and for the subscales concerning Dyadic Consensus (0.69) and Dyadic Satisfaction (0.68). Reliability was unacceptable as far as it concerns Dyadic Cohesion and Affective Expression (α ≤ 0.50). Thus, these subscales were excluded from subsequent analysis. Normative cut-offs were computed from average scores and standard deviations reported in the article of the Italian validation of the DAS (Gentili et al., 2002). All the mothers in the present study reported scores that fell in the normative range (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers’ scores in the DAS.

Perceived Social Support

The Cronbach’s alpha coefficient reported very good reliability for all the scales of the MSPSS, considering both the different subscales and the whole scale (0.76 ≤ α ≤ 0.97). Table 5 reports average scores and standard deviations for the MSPSS. Normative cut-offs were extracted from the validation study by Prezza and Principato (2002). As it is possible to see, the majority of the subjects reported the perception of satisfactory support provided by family, friends and a significant other.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Average scores, standard deviations, and distribution of the mothers in the MSPSS.

As it is possible to see from the data above, from a descriptive point of view at the beginning of the massage course all the mothers seemed to be able to rely on satisfactory couple adjustment, showing scores within the normative range in all the DAS scales. A similar consideration could be made for social support; although few subjects reported MSPSS scores below average, the majority of the participants seemed to experience a sufficient amount of support provided by family, friends and a significant other. As far as it concerns psychological wellbeing, although the majority of the subjects reported scores that fell into the SCL-90-R normative range, some mothers reported the presence of significant psychological distress as well. It is noteworthy, however, that rarely this perception reached the clinical cut-off. Finally, considering mother–child interactions, it is possible to see how, during T1, most of the mother–child dyads fell in the complicated zone of EA, indicating the presence of an emotional connection but the existence of difficulties as well. The complicated zone, and the zones below were progressively less represented during the ongoing of massage classes. During the follow up (T4), all the dyads that completed the program fell in the emotional available zone.

Change vs. Stability in Quality of Mother–Child Interactions

To assess change vs. stability of EA during the infant-massage course the Friedman test was applied. This non-parametric statistical test can be considered a valid alternative of the parametric repeated measures ANOVA. The results reported a statistically significant increase in maternal sensitivity (X2 = 18.650, p = 0.001), structuring (X2= 17.190, p = 0.001), non-intrusiveness (X2 = 15.864, p = 0.001), and non-hostility (X2 = 7.400, p = 0.025), just as in child responsiveness (X2 = 15.650, p = 0.001) and involvement (X2 = 15.476, p = 0.001), indicating thus an improvement in all the EA dimensions during the massage course. To investigate the specific patterns of change in the scales, the Wilcoxon signed-rank test was applied to compare the different periods of infant massage course. This non-parametric statistical hypothesis test can be considered as an alternative to the paired Student’s t-test and can be used to compare repeated measures on a single sample to assess differences in the population mean ranks. The results reported a statistically significant increase in maternal sensitivity (Z = -2.553, p = 0.011), structuring (Z = -2.335, p = 0.020), non-intrusiveness (Z = -3.357, p = 0.001), just as in child responsiveness (Z = -2.626, p = 0.009), and involvement (Z = -2.120, p = 0.034) from T1 to T3. Regarding the transition from T3 to T4, statistically significant improvements were highlighted for maternal sensitivity (Z = -2.070), structuring (Z = -2.384, p = 0.017), non-intrusiveness (Z = -2.059, p = 0.040) and in child responsiveness (Z = -2.121, p = 0.034).

Associations between Emotional Availability and Psychological Distress

To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and maternal psychological distress, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the SCL-90-R reliable scores (obsessive-compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, social phobia, paranoid ideation, psychoticism, somatization, depression, anxiety, GSI, PST, and PSDI) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. Table 6 reports associations between EA and psychological distress. As it is possible to observe, regarding T1, correlations were found between maternal non-intrusiveness and psychoticicsm. During T3, negative correlations were found between psychoticism, maternal structuring, non-intrusiveness, non-hostility, and for child responsiveness. Moreover, anxiety resulted negatively associated with maternal non-intrusiveness and child responsiveness. As far it concerns T4, statistically significant inverse associations were found between maternal sensitivity and obsessive-compulsive, the GSI, and the PSDI.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Associations between EA and psychological distress.

Associations between Emotional Availability and Couple Adjustment

To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and couple adjustment, the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the DAS reliable scores (Dyadic Consensus – DC, Dyadic Satisfaction – DS, and DAS total scores) and to the scores obtained through the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. Table 7 reports associations between EA, couple adjustment and perceived social support. As it is possible to see, regarding T1, statistically significant inverse correlations were reported between maternal sensitivity, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score, and between maternal structuring and Dyadic Satisfaction. During T3, significant negative correlations were reported between maternal structuring, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score, and between maternal non-intrusiveness, Dyadic Consensus and the DAS total score. No statistically significant associations between Couple adjustment and EAS scores during T4 were found.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 7. Associations between EA, couple adjustment, and perceived social support.

Associations between Emotional Availability and Perceived Social Support

To test for associations between quality of mother–child interactions and the dimensions of perceived social support the Spearman’s Rho coefficient was applied to the scores of the MSPSS and to the EAS during T1, T3, and T4. As it is possible to observe in Table 7, regarding T1, negative associations were found between maternal non-hostility, the social support received by a significant other, and the total score of the MSPSS. No associations were found between EAS during T3 and T4 and the perceived social support.

Associations between Psychological Distress, Couple Adjustment, and Perceived Social Support

Spearman’s Rho was applied to test for associations between perceived social support, couple adjustment, and psychological distress. No statistically significant associations were found between couple adjustment and perceived social support, neither between couple adjustment and psychological distress. With respect to psychological distress and perceived social support, statistically significant correlations were found between paranoid ideation and the support received by a significant other (r = -0.492, p = 0.027), the total score of the MSPSS (r = -0.512, p = 0.021).

Associations between Improvements in Emotional Availability, Psychological Distress, Couple Adjustment, and Perceived Social Support

In order to assess whether there were associations between EA improvements among the three different timepoints and the other measures, the variance score of EA between timepoints (i.e., from T1 to T3, from T3 to T4, and from T1 to T4) was calculated subtracting the scores at later stages with scores at previous stages. Subsequently, Spearman’s Rho correlations were run between the variance scores of EA and the other measures (i.e., psychological distress, perceived dyadic satisfaction, and perceived social support). Some associations were detected for SCL-90-R, namely a negative relationship between higher psychological distress at three scales of the SCL-90-R (assessed at time 1) and lower variance scores between T1 and T4. More specifically, a negative relationship was detected between Interpersonal Sensitivity and variance score of Child Responsiveness (r = -537, p < 0.05), Social Phobia and variance score of Child Involvement (r = -519, p < 0.05), Psychoticism and variance score of Child Responsiveness (r = -531, p < 0.05). Taken together, these results indicate that the higher psychological distress mothers experience at T1, the less their interaction quality improved during massage lessons.

Discussion

The first aim of the present study was to investigate EA during infant massage classes and to observe if an improvement in mother–child interactions occurred. Up to date, only few studies applied the EAS in the context of infant massage (Hays, 2014) and, at least to our knowledge, none to unselected populations. Secondly, according to the literature that highlights the intervention of multiple factors in determining the quality of parenting practices (Belsky, 1984; Feiring et al., 1987; Jennings et al., 1991; Melson et al., 1993; Bender and Losel, 1998; Singer et al., 2003; Favez et al., 2006), we aimed to test whether aspects such as the maternal perception of couple adjustment, social support and psychological wellbeing were associated to mother–child EA. At the beginning of the courses, the dyads showed scores that ranged from adequate to complicated EA. However, the results reported an increase in all the six dimensions concurrently with the ongoing of the infant massage course. Mothers enrolled in the courses seemed to gradually become more sensitive and responsive toward their children’s bids, as well as more able to provide adequate scaffolding during the lessons. At the same time, they seemed also to become less intrusive, providing more space to the interactions and being less interfering, both psychologically and physically, although the specific setting of infant massage explicitly requires tactile stimulation and the physical manipulation of the infant’s body. Simultaneously, a significant increase in child responsiveness was recorded, suggesting an improvement in the ability to find an adequate balance between self- and interactive-regulation and in the possibility to organize affects and behaviors in a coordinated way in order to respond to the caregiver’s bids. This parallel increase of maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness, the major dimensions indicative of the adult’s and the child’s EA, seems to support the hypothesis of bi-directionality and reciprocity within adult–child relationships (Sander, 1977; Tronick, 1989; Beebe and Lachmann, 2002). Although these improvements are not directly attributable to the course of infant massage, also due to the absence of a control group, this seems a plausible hypothesis, especially considering the literature concerning short-term stability of EA (Robinson et al., 1993; Biringen et al., 1995; Bornstein et al., 2006). Moreover, several studies report a positive influence on adult–child relationships exerted by infant massage (Onozawa et al., 2001; Lee, 2006). More research is needed in future in order to discern more clearly how these improvements in adult–child interactions might be affected by the massage course itself, by developmental processes or by the progressive mutual adjustment that the dyad reaches after delivery.

As far as it concerns maternal psychological wellbeing, as expected, a higher degree of adult psychopathology resulted associated with less optimal mother–child interactions, supporting the hypothesis that experiencing some kind of psychological distress might affect different domains of life, including the one of everyday interactions with one’s own child (Rogosch et al., 1992; Tronick and Weinberg, 1997; Anke, 2012). In particular, in our study, the major symptomatic scales negatively associated with the quality of mother–child exchanges were anxiety and psychoticism, considered as a graduated continuum from mild interpersonal alienation to first-rank symptoms of psychosis (Derogatis, 1977). Given the preliminary nature of this study, self-report measures were administered only during the first period considered; it would be interesting, in the future, to administer them also at the end of the massage course, in order to see whether an improvement in maternal psychological wellbeing occurs parallel to the improvement of mother–child interactions.

Our hypotheses were not confirmed as far as it concerns the associations between mother–child interactions, couple adjustment and social support. The lack of associations seems to reflect in part the non-univocal results reported by the literature. In fact, although several studies reported the presence of associations between marital quality and parenting (Erel and Burman, 1995) the nature of these associations was not always clear (Grych, 2002). Sometimes they appeared positive (Carneiro et al., 2006), sometimes they were negative (Favez et al., 2006), in other cases they were absent (Barnett et al., 2008; Favez et al., 2013). This incongruence could be due to many methodological reasons, such as different samples, the different periods when the measures were administered as well to the use of different kinds of measures (observational vs. self-report). Moreover it is possible that the “spill over” effect (Engfer, 1988; Katz and Gottman, 1996) expected from marital quality and perceived social support toward adult–child interactions might differ according to the family system investigated (i.e., parental-dyadic vs. co-parental-dyadic) (McHale et al., 2000; Johnson, 2001; Lindsey and Caldera, 2006; Stroud et al., 2015). It is important to note that, differently from what it is often reported in the literature, all our mothers held satisfactory couple relationships. Maybe they were still experiencing the so-called “baby honeymoon” (Hobbs, 1965; Wallace and Gotlib, 1990) or maybe they were part of that portion of couples that do not face a decline in marital satisfaction after delivery (Holmes et al., 2013). Anyway, further analysis will be required in future, also expanding and improving research designs, in order to confirm or disconfirm these associations and to examine in depth their nature.

The present study shows a series of limitations that might offer useful suggestions for future research. The first limit regards the sample; the small amount of the participants and the absence of a non-treated control group, in fact, prevent us to generalize the obtained results. A larger sample would allow to adopt more sound statistical analysis, while the presence of a control group would allow to compare the development of mother–child interactions between dyads that undergo infant massage courses and dyads without intervention, thus leading to a better explanation of the effective influence of infant massage upon the establishment of early adult–child interactions.

Another limit regards the absence of a baseline assessment of mother–child interactions. Videotaping the dyads during massage lessons might have influenced the nature of mother–child interactions both in positive or in negative. On one side, in fact, the massage context might have acted as a buffering factor, preventing the mothers from enacting dysfunctional behaviors that otherwise could have been adopted; on the other hand, instead, finding themselves in a new situation and being asked to do something new (massaging their babies while being videotaped) might have made interactions more challenging for these women. In this sense, including a baseline assessment in future would favor a better control of the different intervening variables. Moreover, it should be taken into account that our study only included mothers and did not involve fathers. Expanding the research design in this direction in future would lead to two major consequences: first of all, the possibility to support and sustain also paternal functioning during the postpartum period; secondly the opportunity to increase and to deepen the comprehension of family processes.

Finally, some considerations should be dedicated to the clinical implications of our study. These preliminary findings, in fact, seem to suggest the usefulness of infant massage for the strengthening and the enhancement of early healthy adult–child interactions. This cost-saving technique could provide a simple but effective way to favor the construction of early affective bonds; in this way, it could accompany a process existing per se and sustain the dyad during expected developmental challenges, whether necessary. Especially in a delicate interval such as the post-partum period this practice could become extremely important, since it could help the dyad to face the need of mutual adjustment, facilitating regulatory processes and the establishment of sleep-wake cycles. Moreover, a “guided” emotionally intense approach toward the infant could reassure the mothers, who often perceive the newborns as fragile and are afraid to touch them, making them more confident when handling their babies. From this perspective, infant massage constitutes a precious resource in terms of primary prevention, i.e., in terms of those interventions aimed at sustaining and enhancing the existing resources within the family system, since it can be offered as an enriching support also in the absence of adult psychopathology. A replication of these results in larger samples would thus encourage the diffusion of this non-invasive technique in terms both of relational support and enhancement of parenting abilities.

Ethics Statement

The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015. Written consent was obtained from the participants.

Author Contributions

AS prepared the study design and supervised the research team; GB carried out the massage courses and recruited the sample. SF collected data and prepared data set. AP and MP wrote the introduction section of the manuscript, performed statistical analyses, and prepared tables and figures. AS, MP, and AP wrote the discussions section of the manuscript. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Ethics approved by the General Assembly of the Italian Association of Psychology held on March 27, 2015 with written informed consent from all subjects.

References

Acolet, D., Modei, N., Ciannakoilopoulos, X., Bond, C., Clow, A., and Glover, V. (1993). Changes in plasma cortisol and catecholamine concentrations in response to massage in preterm infants. Arch. Dis. Child 68, 29–31. doi: 10.1136/adc.68.1_Spec_No.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adamson, S. (1996). Teaching baby massage to new parents. Complement. Ther. Nurs. Midwifery 2, 151–159. doi: 10.1016/S1353-6117(96)80049-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anke, T. (2012). Mothers with severe mental illnes and infants-interactions and treatment. Tidsskr. Nor. Psykol. 49, 1050–1060.

Balsamo, E. (2002). “Bambini immigrati e bisogni insoddisfatti: la via dell’etnopediatria,” in Mille Modi di Crescere. Bambini Immigrati e Modi di Cura, ed. La Casa di Tutti i Colori (Milano: Franco Angeli).

Google Scholar

Balsamo, E. (2007). Sono Qui Con te. L’arte del Maternage. Torino: Il Leone Verde.

Google Scholar

Barnett, M. A., Deng, M., Mills-Koonce, R., Willoughby, M., and Cox, M. (2008). Interdependence of parenting of mothers and fathers of infants. J. Fam. Psychol. 22, 561–573. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beebe, B., and Lachmann, F. (2002). Infant Research e Trattamento Degli Adulti: un Modello Sistemico-Diadico Delle Interazioni. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore, 2003.

Google Scholar

Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K. A., Chen, H., et al. (2012). Maternal postpartum depressive symptoms and 4-month mother–infant interaction. Psychoanal. Psychol. 29, 383. doi: 10.1037/a0029387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1984). The determinants of parenting: a process model. Child Dev 55, 83–96. doi: 10.2307/1129836

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1986). Infant day care: a cause for concern? Zero Three 7, 1–7.

Google Scholar

Belsky, J., and Rovine, M. (1990). Patterns of marital change across the transition to parenthood: pregnancy to three years postpartum. J. Marriage Fam. 52, 5–19. doi: 10.2307/352833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., Spanier, G. B., and Rovine, M. (1983). Stability and change in marriage across the transition to parenthood. J. Marriage Fam. 52, 567–577. doi: 10.2307/351661

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bender, D., and Losel, F. (1998). “Protektive Faktoren der psyschisch gesunden Entwicklung junger menschen: ein beitrag zur kontroverse um saluto- versus pathogenetische Ansätze,” in Gesundheits- oder Krankheitstheorie? Saluto- versus Pathogenetisceh Ansatze im Gesundheitswesen, eds J. Margraf, J. Siegrist, and S. Neumer (Berlin: Springer), 117–145.

Biringen, Z. (2008). Emotional Availability (EA) Scales Manual, 4th Edn. Boulder: Colorado State University.

Google Scholar

Biringen, Z., Emde, R. N., Campos, J. J., and Appelbaum, M. I. (1995). Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its timing. Child Dev. 66, 499–514. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1995.tb00886.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biringen, Z., and Easterbrooks, M. A. (2012). Emotional availability: concept, research, and window on developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 24, 1–8. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blackwell, P. (2000). The influence of touch on child development: implications for intervention. Infants Young Child. 13, 25–39. doi: 10.1097/00001163-200013010-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. H. (2002). “Parenting infants,” in Handbook of Parentin: Children and Parenting, Vol. 1, ed. H. M. Bornestein (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 3–43.

Google Scholar

Bornstein, M. H., Gini, M., Suwalsky, J. T. D., Putnick, D. L., and Haynes, O. M. (2006). Emotional availability in mother-child dyads: short-term stability and continuity from variable-centered and person-centered perspectives. Merrill Palmer Q. 52, 547–571. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bower, D., Jia, R., Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J., Mangelsdorf, S. C., and Brown, G. L. (2013). Trajectories of couple relationship satisfaction in families with infants the roles of parent gender, personality, and depression in first-time and experienced parents. J. Soc. Pers. Relatsh. 30, 389–409. doi: 10.1177/0265407512457656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brazelton, T. B. (1990). The Earliest Relationship. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Google Scholar

Carneiro, C., Corboz-Warnery, A., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (2006). The prenatal lausanne trilogue play: a new observationsl assessment tool of the prenatal co-parenting alliance. Infant Ment. Health J. 27, 207–228. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chong, A., and Mickelson, K. D. (2013). Perceived fairness and relationship satisfaction during the transition to parenthood the mediating role of spousal support. J. Fam. Issues 18, 1–26. doi: 10.1177/0192513X13516764

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, C. P., and Cowan, P. A. (2000). When Partners Become Parents: The Big Life Change for Couples. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Google Scholar

Cowan, P., and Hetherington, M. (2001). Family Transitions. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Cutrona, C. E. (1996). Social Support in Couples: Marriage as a Resource in Times of Stress, Vol. 13. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Czarnocka, J., and Slade, P. (2000). Prevalence and predictors of post-traumatic stress symptoms following childbirth. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 39, 35–51. doi: 10.1348/014466500163095

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Chateau, P. (1976). The influence of early contact on maternal and infant behavior on primaparae. Birth Fam. J. 3, 149–155. doi: 10.1111/j.1523-536X.1976.tb01186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derogatis, L. R. (1977). SCL-90-R, Administration, Scoring, and Procedures Manual for the R(evised) Version. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University, School of Medicine.

Google Scholar

Dew, J., and Wilcox, W. B. (2011). If Momma ain’t happy: explaining declines in marital satisfaction among new mothers. J. Marriage Fam. 73, 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00782.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diego, M. A., Field, T., and Hernandez-Reif, M. (2008). Temperature increases in preterm infants during massage therapy. Infant Behav. Dev. 31, 149–152. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dieter, J., Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Emory, E. K., and Redzepi, M. (2003). Stable preterm infants gain more weight and sleep less after five days of massage therapy. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 28, 403–411. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsg030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dieter, J. N. I., Field, T., Hernandez Reif, M., Emory, E. K., and Redzepi, M. (2001). Massage therapy promotes weight gain in preterm infants after only five days. Acta Pediatrica

Doss, B. D., Rhoades, G. K., Stanley, S. M., and Markman, H. J. (2009). The effect of the transition to parenthood on relationship quality: an 8-year prospective study. J. pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 601–619. doi: 10.1037/a0013969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, L., Li, W., and Shi, F. (2002). Studying impact of touching on growth and development of infants. Chin. Nurs. Res. 16:5.

Google Scholar

Engfer, A. (1988). “The interrelatedness of marriage and the mother-child relationship,” in Relationships Within Families: Mutual Influences, ed. R. H. J. Stevenson-Hinde (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 83–103.

Google Scholar

Erel, O., and Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and parent-child relations: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 118, 108–132. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.118.1.108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favez, N., Frascarolo, F., and Fivaz-Depeursinge, E. (2006). Family alliance stability and change from pregnancy to toddlerhood and marital correlates. Swiss J. Psychol. 65, 213–220. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185.65.4.213

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favez, N., Frascarolo, F., Lavancy Scaiola, C., and Corboz-warnery, A. (2013). Prenatal representations of family in parents and coparental interactions as predictors of triadic interactions during infancy. Infant Ment. Health J. 34, 25–36. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Favez, N., Lopes, F., Bernard, M., Frascarolo, F., Lavanchy Scaiola, C., Corboz-Warnery, A., et al. (2012). The development of family alliance from pregnancy to toddlerhood and child outcomes at 5 years. Fam. Process 51, 542–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01419.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feijò, L., Hernandez-Reif, M., Field, T., Burns, W., Valley-Gray, S., and Simco, E. (2006). Mothers’ depressed mood and anxiety levels are reduced after massaging their preterm infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 476–480. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feiring, C., Fox, N. A., Jaskir, J., and Lewis, M. (1987). The relation between social support, infant risk status and mother–infant interaction. Dev. Psychol. 23, 400. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.3.400

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferber, S. G., Feldman, R., Kohelet, D., Kuint, J., Dollberg, S., Arbel, E., et al. (2005). Massage therapy facilitates mother–infant interaction in premature infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 28, 74–81. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.07.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferber, S. G., Kuint, J., Weller, A., Feldman, R., Dollberg, S., Arbel, E., et al. (2002). Massage therapy by mothers and trained professional enhances weight gain in preterm infants. Early. Hum. Dev 67, 37–45. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(01)00249-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (2002). “Preterm infant massage therapy studies: an American approach,” in Seminars in Neonatology, Vol. 7, (Philadelphia, PA: WB Saunders), 487–494. doi: 10.1053/siny.2002.0153

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Grizzle, N., Scafidi, F., Abrams, S., and Richardson, S. (1996). Massage therapy for infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 19, 107–112. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(96)90048-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Hernandez-Reif, M., Diego, M., Feijo, L., Vera, Y., and Gil, K. (2004). Massage therapy by parents improves early growth and development. Infant Behav. Dev. 27, 435–442. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Scafidi, F., and Schanberg, S. M. (1987). Massage of preterm newborns to improve growth and development. Pediatr. Nurs. 13, 385–387.

Google Scholar

Findler, L., Taubman-Ben-Ari, O., and Jacob, K. (2008). Internal and external contributors to maternal mental health and marital adaptation one year after birth: comparisons of mothers of pre-term and full-term twins. Women and health 46, 39–60. doi: 10.1300/J013v46n04_03

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forman, D. R., O’hara, M. W., Stuart, S., Gorman, L. L., Larsen, K. E., and Coy, K. C. (2007). Effective treatment for postpartum depression is not sufficient to improve the developing mother–child relationship. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 585–602. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gavin, A. R., Hill, K. G., Hawkins, J. D., and Maas, C. (2011). The role of maternal early-life and later-life risk factors on offspring low birth weight: findings from a three-generational study. J. Adolesc. Health 49, 166–171. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.11.246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gentili, P., Contreras, L., Cassaniti, M., and D’Arista, F. (2002). La dyadic adjustment scale. Una misura dell’adattamento di coppia. Minerva Psichiatr. 43, 107–116.

Google Scholar

Gjerdingen, D. K., and Center, B. A. (2005). First-time parents’ postpartum changes in employment, childcare, and housework responsibilities. Soc. Sci. Res. 34, 103–116. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glasheen, C., Colpe, L., Hoffman, V., and Warren, L. K. (2015). Prevalence of serious psychological distress and mental health treatment in a national sample of pregnant and postpartum women. Matern. Child Health J. 19, 204–216. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1511-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glazier, R. H., Elgar, F. J., Goel, V., and Holzapfel, S. (2004). Stress, social support, and emotional distress in a community sample of pregnant women. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 25, 247–255. doi: 10.1080/01674820400024406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, S. H. (2007). Depression in mothers. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 3, 107–135. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, S. H., Rouse, M. H., Connell, A. M., Broth, M. R., Hall, C. M., and Heyward, D. (2011). Maternal depression and child psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 14, 1–27. doi: 10.1007/s10567-010-0080-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grych, J. H. (2002). “Marital relationships and parenting,” in Handbook of Parenting, Vol. 4, ed. M. H. Bornstein (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 231–251.

Google Scholar

Harlow, H. F., and Zimmermann, R. R. (1958). The development of affectional responses in infant monkeys. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 102, 501–509.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hay, D. F., and Kumar, R. (1995). Interpreting the effects of mothers’ postnatal depression on children’s intelligence: a critique and re-analysis. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 25, 165–181. doi: 10.1007/BF02251301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hays, L. J. (2014). Impact Upon Emotional Availability: Infant GERD and Infant Massage Therapy. Ph.D. thesis, University of Colorado, Denver, CO.

Google Scholar

Hernandez-Reif, M., Diego, M., and Field, T. (2007). Preterm infants show reduced stress behaviors and activity after 5 days of massage therapy. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 557–561. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herwig, J. E., Wirtz, M., and Bengel, J. (2004). Depression, partnership, social support, and parenting: interaction of maternal factors with behavioral problems of the child. J. Affect. Disord. 80, 199–208. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(03)00112-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobbs, D. (1965). Parenthood as crisis: a third study. J. Marriage Fam. 27, 367–372. doi: 10.2307/350281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmes, E. K., Sasaki, T., and Hazen, N. L. (2013). Smooth versus rocky transitions to parenthood: family systems in developmental context. Fam. Relat. 62, 824–837. doi: 10.1111/fare.12041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horwitz, S. M., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Storfer-Isser, A., and Carter, A. S. (2007). Prevalence, correlates, and persistence of maternal depression. J. Women’s Health 16, 678–691. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2006.0185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Morgan, D., and Antonucci, T. (1997). The effects of positive and negative social exchanges on aging adults. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 52, S190–S199. doi: 10.1093/geronb/52B.4.S190

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, J. B., Miller, R. B., Oka, M., and Henry, R. G. (2014). Gender differences in marital satisfaction: a meta-analysis. J. Marriage Fam. 76, 105–129. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jean, A. D. L., and Stack, D. M. (2009). Functions of maternal touch and infants’ affect during face-to-face interactions: new directions for the still-face. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 123–128. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jean, A. D. L., Stack, D. M., Girouard, N., and Fogel, A. (2004). “Maternal touching during interactions: the influence of infant age and social context,” in Proceedings of the International Conference of Infant Studies, Chicago, IL.

Google Scholar

Jennings, K. D., Stagg, V., and Connors, R. E. (1991). Social networks and mothers’ interactions with their preschool children. Child Dev 62, 966–978. doi: 10.2307/1131146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jinon, S. (1996). “The effect of infant massage on growth of the preterm infant,” in Increasing Safe and Successful Pregnancy, eds C. Yarbes Almirante and M. De Luma (Amsterdam: Elsevier Science), 265–269.

Google Scholar

Johnson, V. K. (2001). Marital interaction, family organization, and differences in parenting behavior: explaining variations across family interaction contexts. Fam. Process 40, 333–342. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2001.4030100333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, B. H., Cassel, J. C., and Gore, S. (1977). Social support and health. Med. Care 15, 47–58. doi: 10.1097/00005650-197705001-00006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katz, L., and Gottman, J. M. (1996). “Spillover effects of marital conflict: in search of parenting and coparenting mechanisms,” in Understanding how Family-Leve Dynamics Affect Children’s Development: Studies of Two-Parents Families, eds J. McHale and P. A. Cowan (San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass), 57–76.

Google Scholar

Ke, G., Ling, X., and Li, L. (2001). Clinical survey of improved massage method developing infants’ growth and development. China Nurse 36, 278–280.

Google Scholar

Kim, T. I., Shin, Y. H., and White-Traut, A. C. (2003). Multisensory intervention improves physical growth and illness rates in Korean orphaned newborn infants. Res. Nurs. Health 26, 424–433. doi: 10.1002/nur.10105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kohn, J. L., Rholes, W., Simpson, J. A., Martin, A., Tran, S., and Wilson, C. L. (2012). Changes in marital satisfaction across the transition to parenthood: the role of adult attachment orientations. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 1506–1522. doi: 10.1177/0146167212454548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koniak-Griffin, D., and Ludington-Hoe, S. M. (1988). Developmental and temperament outcomes of sensory stimulation in healthy infants. Nurs. Res. 37, 70–76. doi: 10.1097/00006199-198803000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, C., Schandberg, S., Field, T., Symansky, R., Zimmerman, E., Scafidi, F., et al. (1991). Tactile kinesthetiv stimulation effects on sympathetic and adrenocortical function in preterm infants. J. Pediatr. 119, 434–440. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)82059-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, E., Rothman, A. D., Cobb, R. J., Rothman, M. T., and Bradbury, T. N. (2008). Marital satisfaction across the transition to parenthood. J. Fam. Psychol. 22, 41–50. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.22.1.41

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H. (2006). The effects of infant massage on weight, height, and mother-infant interaction. J. Korean Acad. Nurs. 36, 1331–1339.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lindsey, E. W., and Caldera, Y. M. (2006). Mother-father-child triadic interaction and mother-child dyadic interaction: gender differences within and between contexts. Sex Roles 55, 511–521. doi: 10.1007/s11199-006-9106-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C. (2005). The effect of touching on healthy infants’weight. J. Huaihai Med. 23, 137.

Lu, J., Li, J., and Wu, L. (2005). A study of the effect of touch on health in infants. Pract. Clin. Med. 6, 119–121.

McHale, J. P., Kuersten-Hogan, R., Lauretti, A., and Rasmussen, J. L. (2000). Parental reports of coparenting and observed coparenting behavior during the toddler period. J. Fam. Psychol. 14, 220–234. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.2.220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melson, G. F., Ladd, G. W., and Hsu, H. C. (1993). Maternal support networks, maternal cognitions, and young children’s social, and cognitive development. Child Dev. 64, 1401–1417. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02960.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R. E., and Trickett, E. J. (1980). Task force report: social networks as mediators of social support. Community Ment. Health J. 16, 27–44. doi: 10.1007/BF00780665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montagu, A. (1971). Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. Oxford: Columbia Univ Press.

Google Scholar

Moszkowski, R. J., and Stack, D. M. (2007). Infant touching behaviour during mother-infant face-to-face interactions. Infant Child Dev. 16, 307–319. doi: 10.1002/icd.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moyer Mileur, L., Luetkemeier, M., Boomer, L., and Chan, G. M. (1995). Effect of physical activity on bone mineralization in premature infants. J. Pediatr. 127, 620–625. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(95)70127-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Fearon, P., and Cooper, P. (2015). “Postnatal depression, mother–infant interactions, and child development. prospects for screening and treatment,” in Identifying Perinatal Depression and Anxiety: Evidence-based Practice in Screening, Psychosocial Assessment and Management, eds J. Milgrom and A. W. Gemmill (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons), 139–164.

Na, Z., Xie, H. Y., and Huang, J. H. (2005). The effect of infant massage on growth. New J. Tradit. Chin. Med. 37, 69–70.

Google Scholar

Nelson, C., and Bosquet, M. (2000). “Neurobiology of fetal and infant development: implications for infant mental health,” in Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 2nd Edn, ed. C. H. Zeanah Jr. (New York, NY: Guilford Press).

Google Scholar

O’Higgins, M., Roberts, I., and Glover, V. (2008). Postnatal depression and mother and infant outcomes after infant massage. J. Affect. Disord. 109, 189–192. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2007.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onozawa, K., Glover, V., Adams, D., Modi, N., and Kumar, R. C. (2001). Infant massage improves mother–infant interaction for mothers with postnatal depression. J. Affect. Disord. 63, 201–207. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00198-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oswalt, K. L., Biasini, F. J., Wilson, L. L., and Mrug, S. (2009). Outcomes of a massage intervention on teen mothers: s pilot study. Pediatr. Nurs. 35, 284–317.

Google Scholar

Pinelli, J. (2000). Effects of family coping and resources on family adjustment and parental stress in the acute phase of the NICU experience. Neonatal Netw. 19, 27–37. doi: 10.1891/0730-0832.19.6.27

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prezza, M., and Principato, M. C. (2002). “La rete sociale e il sostegno sociale,” in Conoscere la Comunità, eds M. Prezza and M. Santinello (Bologna: Il Mulino), 193–234.

Google Scholar

Relier, J. P. (1994). Amarlo Prima che Nasca. Il Legame Madre-Figlio Prima Della Nascita. Firenze: Le Lettere.

Google Scholar

Robertson, J., and Bowlby, J. (1952). Responses of young children to separation from their mothers. Courrier Centre Int. Enfance 2, 131–142.

Robinson, J., Little, C., and Biringen, Z. (1993). Emotional communication in mother-toddler relationships: evidence for early gender differentiation. Merrill Palmer Q. 39, 496–517.

Google Scholar

Rogosch, F. A., Mowbray, C. T., and Bogat, G. A. (1992). Determinants of parenting attitudes in mothers with severe psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 4, 469–487. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400000900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, J., and Jones, L. (2010). Discharge and beyond. A longitudinal study comparing stress and coping in parents of preterm infants. J. Neonatal Nurs. 16, 258–266. doi: 10.1016/j.jnn.2010.07.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sander, L. (1977). “The regulation of exchange in the infant-caretaker system and some aspect of the context-content realationship,” in Interaction, Conversation, and the Development of Language, eds M. Lewis and L. Rosemblum (New York, NY: Wiley).

Google Scholar

Sarno, I., Preti, E., Prunas, A., and Madeddu, F. (2011). SCL-90-R. Symptom Checklist-90-R. Adattamento italiano. Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.

Google Scholar

Saurel-Cubizolles, M. J., Romito, P., and Lelong, N. (2007). Women’s psychological health according to their maternal status: a study in France. J. Psychosom. Obstet. Gynecol. 28, 243–249. doi: 10.1080/01674820701350351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scafidi, F., and Field, T. (1996). Massage therapy improves behaviour in neonates born to HIV positive mothers. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 21, 889–898. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/21.6.889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scafidi, F., Field, T., Schanberg, S., Bauer, C., Tucci, K., Roberts, J., et al. (1990). Massage stimulates growth in preterm infants: a replication. Infant Behav. Dev. 13, 167–188. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(90)90029-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scafidi, F., Field, T. M., Schanberg, S. M., Bauer, C. R., Vega-Lahr, N., Garcia, R., et al. (1986). Effects of tactile/kinesthetic stimulation on the clinical course and sleep/wake behavior of preterm neonates. Infant Behav. Dev. 9, 91–105. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90041-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shapiro, A. F., Gottman, J. M., and Carrere, S. (2000). The baby and the marriage: identifying factors that buffer against decline in marital satisfaction after the first baby arrives. J. Fam. Psychol. 14, 59. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.14.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, L. T., Fulton, S., Davillier, M., Koshy, D., Salvator, A., and Baley, J. E. (2003). Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 24, 233–241. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200308000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: new scales for assessing the quality of marriage and similar dyads. J. Marriage Fam. 38, 15–28. doi: 10.2307/350547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stack, D. M. (2004). “Touching during mother-infant interactions,” Touch and Massage in Early Child Development, ed. T. M. Field (New York: Johnson and Johnson Pediatric Institute), 49-81.

Google Scholar

Stack, D. M., and Muir, D. W. (1990). Tactile stimulation as a component of social interchange: new interpretations for the still-face effect. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 8, 131–145. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1990.tb00828.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stack, D. M., and Muir, D. W. (1992). Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates five-month-olds’ affect and attention. Child Dev. 63, 1509–1525. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01711.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stemp, P. S., Turner, R. J., and Noh, S. (1986). Psychological distress in the postpartum period: the significance of social support. J. Marriage Fam. 48, 271–277. doi: 10.2307/352394

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stork, H. (1986). Enfances Indiennes. Étude de Psychologie Transculturelle et Comparée du Jeun Enfant. Paris: Paidos-Le Centurion.

Stroud, C. B., Meyers, K. M., Wilson, S., and Durbin, E. (2015). Marital quality spillover and young children’ìs adjustment: evidence for dyadic and triadic parenting as mechanisms. J. Clin. Chind Adolesc. Psychol. 44, 800–813. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2014.900720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutter-Dallay, A. L. (2006). Impact des Symptômes Dépressifs Maternels Postnataux Précoces sur le Développement Cognitif et Moteur du Nourrisson. Ph.D. thesis, University of Bordeaux, Bordeaux, 2.

Google Scholar

Taylor, S. E. (2011). “Social support: a review,” in The Handbook of Health Psychology, ed. M. S. Friedman (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 189–214.

Google Scholar

Terry, D. J., McHugh, T. A., and Noller, P. (1991). Role dissatisfaction and the decline in marital quality across the transition to parenthood. Aust. J. Psychol. 43, 129–132. doi: 10.1080/00049539108260136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E. Z. (1995). “Touch in mother-infant interactions,” in Touch Early Development, ed. T. M. Field (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.) 53–65.

Google Scholar

Trillingsgaard, T., Baucom, K. J., and Heyman, R. E. (2014). Predictors of change in relationship satisfaction during the transition to parenthood. Fam. Relat. 63, 667–679. doi: 10.1111/fare.12089

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. Am. Psychol. 44, 112–119. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E. Z., and Weinberg, M. K. (1997). “Le madri depresse e i loro bambini: l’insuccesso nella formazione di stati di coscienza diadici,” in Depressione del Post-Partum e Sviluppo del Bambino, eds L. Murray and P. J. Cooper (Roma: Cic Edizioni Internazionali), 47–72.

Google Scholar

Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., and Foster, C. A. (2003). Parenthood and marital satisfaction: a meta-analytic review. J. Marriage Fam. 65, 574–583. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00574.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Underdown, A. (2009). “The power of touch–exploring infant massage,” in Keeping the Baby in Mind. Infant Mental Health Practice, eds J. Barlow and P. O. Svanberg (London: Routledge), 17–28.

Google Scholar

Wallace, P. M., and Gotlib, I. H. (1990). Marital adjustment during the transition to parenthood: stability and predictors of change. J. Marriage Fam. 52, 21–29. doi: 10.2307/352834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinberg, M. K., and Tronick, E. Z. (1997). Maternal depression and infant maladjustment: a failure of mutual regulation. handb. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1, 177–191.

Google Scholar

Weiss, S. J., Wilson, P., Hertenstein, M. J., and Campos, R. (2000). The tactile context of a mother’s caregiving: implications for attachment of low birth weight infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 23, 91–111. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(00)00030-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeden, A., Scafidi, F., Field, T., Ironson, G., Valdeon, C., and Bandstra, E. (1993). Massage effects on cocaine exposed preterm neonates. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 14, 318–322. doi: 10.1097/00004703-199310000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whisman, M. A. (2001). “The association between depression and marital dissatisfaction,” in Marital and Family Processes in Depression: A Scientific Foundation for Clinical Practice, ed. S. R. H. Beach (Washington, D.C: American Psychological Association), 3–24.

Google Scholar

Zimet, G. D., Powell, S. S., Farley, G. K., Werkman, S., and Berkoff, K. A. (1988). Psychometric characteristics of the multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J. Pers. Assess. 55, 610–617. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: infant massage, mother–child interactions, child development, parenting, early relationships

Citation: Porreca A, Parolin M, Bozza G, Freato S and Simonelli A (2017) Infant Massage and Quality of Early Mother–Infant Interactions: Are There Associations with Maternal Psychological Wellbeing, Marital Quality, and Social Support?. Front. Psychol. 7:2049. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.02049

Received: 26 September 2015; Accepted: 19 December 2016;
Published: 17 January 2017.

Edited by:

Francesco Pagnini, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy

Reviewed by:

Colin M. Bosma, Harvard University, USA
Gabriela Markova, University of Vienna, Austria
Eleonora Volpato, Fondazione Don Gnocchi, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Porreca, Parolin, Bozza, Freato and Simonelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alessio Porreca, YWxlc3Npby5wb3JyZWNhQGxpYmVyby5pdA==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.