Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Psychol., 30 March 2016
Sec. Consciousness Research

The Prefrontal Cortex and Suggestion: Hypnosis vs. Placebo Effects

  • Department of Psychology, Faculty of Science and Technology, Bournemouth University, Poole, UK

Suggestion has been defined as a form of communicable ideation or belief, that once accepted has the capacity to exert profound changes on a person's mood, thoughts, perceptions and behaviors (Halligan and Oakley, 2014). The prefrontal region (the region of the frontal cortex anterior to the motor areas) of the human cerebral cortex appears to play an important role in suggestion (Asp et al., 2012). Children, with still-developing prefrontal cortices, are more susceptible to suggestion (Ceci et al., 1987; Bruck and Ceci, 1995). Older adults, who experience atrophy of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) as a result of the normal processes of aging, are also more open to suggestion (Cohen and Faulkner, 1989). Damasio (1994) described how patients with damage to the PFC are more vulnerable to “snake-oil” salesmen and disreputable characters. Asp et al. (2012) showed that patients with ventromedial PFC damage were more likely to believe in misleading advertisements. The role of the frontal cortices in suggestion fits with the putative role of the prefrontal cortex in the control of thought and behavior (Miller and Cohen, 2001).

Halligan and Oakley (2014) recently called for increased study of phenomena associated with suggestion, noting the importance of suggestion to many forms of human behavior. They compared various forms of suggestion in an attempt to elucidate shared underlying psychological properties. In separate work, they have noted the growing interest in hypnosis and hypnotic suggestion following the application of methods of cognitive neuroscience to the study of these and related phenomena (Oakley and Halligan, 2009). Here the contributions of the prefrontal cortex to hypnosis and placebo effects will be described in an attempt to highlight how methods of cognitive neuroscience might complement other approaches when investigating different forms of suggestion.

Hypnosis involves suggestion. Cardeña and Spiegel (1991) described hypnotic phenomenon as comprising three integrative dimensions, one of which is suggestibility (with the remaining two being absorption and dissociation). Commonly used scales that measure “hypnotisability” are actually measuring suggestibility under hypnosis (Kirsch et al., 2011). Thus, hypnosis is intrinsically linked with suggestion and suggestion is in fact a conditio sine qua non to create and measure hypnotic phenomena.

Hypnosis usually takes the form of an interaction in which one person, the hypnotist, relaxes another individual by talking him/her through a series of visualizations involving muscle relaxation and then counting him/her up into a “state” of hypnosis (known as the induction). The hypnotized individual will be concentrating on the hypnotist's voice relaying a series and variety of suggestions to which the individual may or may not respond. Under the influence of hypnotic suggestion (or post-hypnotic suggestion which is a suggestion given under hypnosis but activated post-hypnosis) the experience of pain (Derbyshire et al., 2004), color perception (Kosslyn et al., 2000), cognitive conflict (Raz et al., 2002) and delusions (Rahmanovic et al., 2011) can be produced or extinguished (see Oakley and Halligan, 2009, for a recent review).

Placebo effects also involve suggestion and are commonly employed in medicine and clinical practice. They are also used as a control condition in randomized controlled trials assessing the clinical significance of new treatments and drugs (Finniss et al., 2010). Comparisons have been drawn between hypnosis and the placebo effect with hypnosis being described as placebo without deception (Wickless and Kirsch, 1989; Kirsch, 1999; Raz, 2007; Kirsch et al., 2008). Raz (2007) made the case that by studying hypnosis and using it as a correlate of placebo one can avoid the ethical considerations associated with deception in placebo, and still understand the mechanisms that underpin it. More specifically, the case was made that the recent finding that a specific COMT polymorphism correlates with hypnotisability indicates that similar associations may tap at least some form of Good Placebo Responder (GPR), and that regardless of the formal relationship between hypnosis and placebo it may be helpful to apply hypnosis for its placebo value in generating positive expectations. A further argument for the association between hypnosis and placebo effects is that they are both influenced by expectancy (Kirsch, 1999), although the case for expectancy as an important determinant appears more convincing in the case of placebos (Benham et al., 1998). Hypnotic suggestibility and response to placebo also both appear to increase following inhalation of oxytocin (Bryant et al., 2013; Kessner et al., 2013; although see Parris et al., 2014 for an example of how oxytocin impedes the effect of a post-hypnotic suggestion), and have similar analgesic effects (Kupers et al., 2005).

Research using Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) suggests that placebo effects require the involvement of the prefrontal cortex whereas hypnotic suggestibility is increased when the prefrontal cortex is hypoactive. Applying TMS to the left dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) to impair function in this region results in increased hypnotic suggestibility (Dienes and Hutton, 2013). In their study, medium suggestible participants received 5 min of 1 Hz repetitive TMS to either the left DLPFC or the vertex (control location) in four separate sessions. In the 5 min of residual cortical disruption that followed stimulation participants were given a brief hypnotic induction and two hypnotic suggestions (magnetic hands and arm levitation). In a second session involving stimulation of the same cortical site, a further two hypnotic suggestions were delivered (rigid arm and taste hallucination). Relative to stimulation of the vertex, low frequency rTMS to the left DLPFC resulted in an increase in hypnotic susceptibility (a 6% increase) to all suggestions as established using both subjective and objective reports of performance. Importantly, their result was not related to expectancy since expectancies were identical between the two conditions. In contrast, Krummenacher et al. (2010) have shown that rTMS to the right and left DLPFC completely blocked expectation-related placebo analgesia. In this study participants received 1 Hz rTMS for 15 min after which heat pain was delivered to their forearms. Compared to a sham condition, those receiving rTMS to the DLPFC did not show placebo analgesic effects (the TMS equipment was used as the placebo) and this was irrespective of the laterality of stimulation. Indeed, the placebo response was completely blocked by inhibition of the DLPFC. The authors hypothesized that the results were due to a disruption of the cognitive representation of pain analgesia and the resulting suppression of an expectation-related placebo effect. In sum, these two studies on two forms of suggestion illustrate a differential and opposing effect of stimulation of the left DLPFC.

There are important differences between these studies that may have a bearing on their outcomes. In Dienes and Hutton's study brain stimulation occurred before the hypnotic induction and suggestions were given, whereas in Krummenacher et al. stimulation occurred after the suggestion of placebo analgesia was given. It is possible that TMS of DLPFC after hypnotic induction and suggestion would also inhibit the effects of suggestion. The effects of TMS might also be dosage dependent: Dienes and Hutton delivered 5 min of stimulation, whereas Krummenacher et al. delivered 15 min. It is possible that had stimulation been delivered for 15 min in the hypnosis study, the effect size would have been larger or the observed effect would have been in the opposite direction. It is also not clear in either case that the result was a consequence of stimulation of the DLPFC since TMS can have indirect effects through synaptic connections. Stimulation of the DLPFC results in activation of the VLPFC (Eisenegger et al., 2008) which has been shown to be activated by surprising and rewarding stimuli (Parris et al., 2007, 2009; Rolls et al., 2008). It has also been shown that rTMS of the DLPFC results in blood flow changes in the ACC and midbrain neurons (Speer et al., 2003).

Despite these issues, the findings from these two studies seem to mirror findings in both literatures. Benedetti (2009) has noted that without PFC there is no placebo effect. Benedetti et al. (2006) studied Alzheimer's patients at the initial stage of the disease and 1-year later to investigate whether Alzheimer's Disease altered susceptibility to placebo effects. They found that the reduced frontal connectivity in patients predicted a smaller placebo response. Stein et al. (2012) have also shown that greater white matter integrity in the DLPFC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex, and the periacaqueductal gray was associated with greater placebo analgesia.

In contrast to Stein et al. a recent study revealed no relationship between white (or gray) matter microstructure and hypnotic suggestibility (Hoeft et al., 2012; although see Huber et al., 2014). However, a relatively well-established finding in the hypnosis literature is that hypnosis itself leads to reduced frontal lobe functioning (Sheehan et al., 1988; Farvolden and Woody, 2004; Jamieson and Sheehan, 2004; Wagstaff et al., 2007), which supports the notion that disrupting frontal activity using TMS would facilitate being hypnotized. One can also consider hypnotic suggestibility itself and whether there is evidence that frontal lobe function is initially relatively impaired in highly suggestible individuals, again facilitating the induction of hypnosis. Consistent with this, studies employing the Stroop task, a task known to involve the DLPFC, report poorer performance by highs at baseline (Dixon et al., 1990; Dixon and Laurence, 1992; Farvolden and Woody, 2004) indicating poorer DLPFC function independent of hypnosis. However, studies with large sample sizes have reported no baseline performance differences between suggestible and less suggestible people on tasks thought to index frontal lobe function (Dienes et al., 2009; Varga et al., 2011), although these latter findings could be due to a failure to titrate task difficulty and involve tasks that do not so clearly rely on DLPFC.

The PFC is a large region of the brain, and one might consider the role of different regions of the PFC in the varieties of suggestion. The involvement of the DLPFC could differentiate hypnosis and placebo effects. The role of more ventromedial regions in other forms of suggestion (Asp et al., 2012) indicates a further potential dissociation between neural substrates underpinning the varieties of suggestion.

Lifshitz et al. (2012) draw a distinction between two types of suggestibility: (1) suggestibility that is largely determined by underlying cognitive aptitude, and (2) suggestibility that is best considered as a flexible skill amenable to attitudinal factors including beliefs and expectations. The differential involvement of the PFC in hypnosis and placebo, and the identification of individual differences in the former and not the latter, appears to indicate that aptitude is most important in hypnosis, whilst attitudinal factors might be more important in the placebo response. Expectancy appears to be a more important determinant in the case of placebos (Benham et al., 1998; Benedetti, 2009; Lifshitz et al., 2012). Indeed, in Dienes and Hutton's TMS study, their effect of TMS on hypnotic suggestibility was independent of expectancy, but Krummenacher et al.'s TMS study reports disruption to an expectancy-based (as opposed to a conditioning-based) placebo.

Notably, research suggests that measures of placebo suggestibility do not correlate with hypnotic/imaginative suggestibility measures (Kihlstrom, 2008) indicating another reason to consider them different entities, at least in some regards. Evidencing a complex relationship between different forms of suggestion, a recent study reported similar levels of placebo analgesia in high and low suggestible individuals despite differential neural activity underpinning it (Huber et al., 2013).

In conclusion, whilst there are potentially informative similarities between hypnotic suggestion and placebos, their differences, particularly with regard to the differential contributions of regions of the prefrontal cortex, are also potentially informative as to the nature of suggestion more generally.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This research was supported by Bournemouth University.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Asp, E., Manzel, K., Koestner, B., Cole, C. A., Denburg, N. L., and Tranel, D. (2012). A neuropsychological test of belief and doubt: damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex increases credulity for misleading advertising. Front. Neurosci. 6:100. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2012.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F. (2009). Placebo Effects. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Benedetti, F., Arduino, C., Costa, S., Vighetti, S., Tarenzi, L., Rainero, I., et al. (2006). Loss of expectation-related mechanisms in Alzheimer's disease makes analgesic therapies less effective. Pain 121, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benham, G., Bowers, S., Nash, M., and Muenchen, R. (1998). Self-fulfilling prophecy and hypnotic response are not the same thing. J. Person. Soc. Psychol. 75, 1604–1613. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.75.6.1604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruck, M., and Ceci, S. J. (1995). Amicus brief for the case of State of New Jersey v. Michaels presented by Commitee of Concerned Social Scientists. Psychol. Public Policy Law 1, 272–322. doi: 10.1037/1076-8971.1.2.272

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., Hung, L., Dobson-Stone, C., and Schofield, P. R. (2013). The association between the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) and hypnotizability. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 1979–1984. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardeña, E., and Spiegel, D. (1991). “Suggestibility, absorption, and dissociation: an integrative model of hypnosis,” in Human Suggestibility: Advances in Theory, Research, and Application ed J. F. Schumaker (Florence, KY: Taylor & Frances/Routledge), 93–107.

Ceci, S. J., Ross, D. F., and Toglia, M. P. (1987). Suggestibility of children's memory: Psycholegal implications. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 116, 38–49. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.116.1.38

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, G., and Faulkner, D. (1989). Age differences in source forgetting: effects on reality monitoring and on eyewitness testimony. Psychol. Aging 4, 10–17.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes' Error: Emotion, Rationality and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Putnam.

Derbyshire, S. W. G., Whalley, M. G., Stenger, V. A., and Oakley, D. A. (2004). Cerebral activation during hypnotically induced and imagined pain. Neuroimage 23, 392–401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dienes, Z., Brown, E., Hutton, S., Kirsch, I., Mazzoni, G., and Wright, D. B. (2009). Hypnotic suggestibility, cognitive inhibition and dissociation. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 837–847. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dienes, Z., and Hutton, S. (2013). Understanding hypnosis metacognitively: rTMS applied to left DLPFC increases hypnotic suggestibility. Cortex 49, 386–392. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dixon, M., Brunet, A., and Laurence, J. (1990). Hypnotizability and automaticity: toward a parallel distributed processing model of hypnotic responding. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 99, 336–343.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Dixon, M., and Laurence, J.-R. (1992). Hypnotic susceptibility and verbal automaticity: automatic and strategic processing difference in the Stroop colour-naming task. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 101, 344–347.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Eisenegger, C., Treyer, V., Fehr, E., and Knoch, D. (2008). Time-course of “off-line” prefrontal rTMS effects—a PET study. Neuroimage 42, 379–384. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farvolden, P., and Woody, E. Z. (2004). Hypnosis, memory, and frontal executive functioning. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hyp. 52, 3–26. doi: 10.1076/iceh.52.1.3.23926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finniss, D. G., Kaptchuk, T. J., Miller, F., and Benedetti, F. (2010). Biological, clinical, and ethical advances of placebo effects. Lancet 375, 686–695. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61706-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halligan, P. W., and Oakley, D. A. (2014). Hypnosis and beyond: exploring the broader domain of suggestion. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 1, 105–122. doi: 10.1037/cns0000019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoeft, F., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Haas, B. W., Bammer, R., Menon, V., et al. (2012). Functional brain basis of hypnotizability. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 69, 1064–1072. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, A., Lui, F., Duzzi, D., Pagnoni, G., and Porro, C. A. (2014). Structural and functional cerebral correlates of hypnotic suggestibility. PLoS ONE 9:e93187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Huber, A., Lui, F., and Porro, C. A. (2013). Hypnotic susceptibility modulates brain activity related to experimental placebo analgesia. Pain 154, 1509–1518. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamieson, G. A., and Sheehan, P. W. (2004). An empirical test of Woody and Bowers's dissociated-control theory of hypnosis. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 52, 232–249. doi: 10.1080/0020714049052349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessner, S., Sprenger, C., Wrobel, N., Wiech, K., and Bingel, U. (2013). Effect of oxytocin on placebo analgesia: a randomized study. JAMA 310, 1733–1735. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.277446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kihlstrom, J. F. (2008). “The domain of hypnosis, revisited,” in The Oxford Handbook of Hypnosis: Theory, Research and Practice, eds M. R. Nash and A. J. Barnier (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 19–52.

Kirsch, I. (1999). Hypnosis and placebos: response expectancy as a mediator of suggestion effects. Ana. Psicol. 15, 99–110.

Google Scholar

Kirsch, I., Cardeña, E., Derbyshire, S., Dienes, Z., Heap, M., Kallio, S., et al. (2011). Definitions of hypnosis and hypnotizability and their relation to suggestion and suggestibility: a consensus statement. Contemp. Hypn. Integr. Ther. 28, 107–115.

Google Scholar

Kirsch, I., Wickless, C., and Moffitt, K. H. (2008). Expectancy and suggestibility: are the effects of environmental enhancement due to detection? Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 47, 40–45. doi: 10.1080/00207149908410021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Costantini-Ferrando, M. F., Alpert, N. M., and Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters color processing in the brain. Am. J. Psychiat. 157, 1279–1284. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.8.1279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krummenacher, P., Candia, V., Folkers, G., Schedlowski, M., and Schönbächler, G. (2010). Prefrontal cortex modulates placebo analgesia. Pain 148, 368–374. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kupers, R., Faymonville, M.-E., and Laureys, S. (2005). “The cognitive modulation of pain: hypnosis- and placebo-induced analgesia,” in The Boundaries of Consciousness: Neurobiology and Neuropathology, ed S. Laureys, (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 251–600.

PubMed Abstract

Lifshitz, M., Howells, C., and Raz, A. (2012). Can expectation enhance response to suggestion? De-automatization illuminates a conundrum. Conscious. Cogn. 21, 1001–1008. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oakley, D. A., and Halligan, P. W. (2009). Hypnotic suggestion and cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 264–270. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B. A., Dienes, Z., Bate, S., and Gothard, S. (2014). Oxytocin impedes the effect of the word blindness post-hypnotic suggestion on Stroop task performance. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 895–899. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B. A., Kuhn, G., Mizon, G. A., Benattayallah, A., and Hodgson, T. L. (2009). Imaging the impossible: an fMRI study of impossible causal relationships in magic tricks. Neuroimage 45, 1033–1039. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parris, B. A., Thai, N. J., Benattayallah, A., Summers, I. R., and Hodgson, T. L. (2007). The role of the lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate in stimulus-response association reversals. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 13–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rahmanovic, A., Barnier, A. J., Cox, R. E., Langdon, R. A., and Coltheart, M. (2011). “That's not my arm”: a hypnotic analogue of somatoparaphrenia. Cogn. Neuropsychiat. 17, 36–63. doi: 10.1080/13546805.2011.564925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, A. (2007). Hypnobo: Perspectives on Hypnosis and Placebo. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 50, 29–36. doi: 10.1080/00029157.2007.10401595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, A., Shapiro, T., Fan, J., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Hypnotic suggestion and the modulation of Stroop interference. Arch. Gen. Psychiat. 59, 1155–1161. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.12.1155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolls, E. T., Grabenhorst, F., and Parris, B. A. (2008). Warm pleasant feelings in the brain. Neuroimage 41, 1504–1513. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, P. W., Donovan, P., and Macleod, C. M. (1988). Strategy manipulation and the Stroop effect in hypnosis. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 455–460. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.4.455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Speer, A. M., Willis, M. W., Herscovitch, P., Daube-Witherspoon, M., Repella Shelton, J., Benson, B. E., et al. (2003). Intensity-dependent regional cerebral blood flow during 1-Hz repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in healthy volunteers studied with H215O positron emission tomography: i. effects of primary motor cortex rTMS. Biol. Psychiat. 54, 818–825. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00002-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Stein, N., Sprenger, C., Scholz, J., Wiech, K., and Bingel, U. (2012). White matter integrity of the descending pain modulatory system is associated with interindividual differences in placebo analgesia. Pain 153, 2210–2217. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varga, K., Németh, Z., and Szekely, A. (2011). Lack of correlation between hypnotic susceptibility and various components of attention. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1872–1881. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagstaff, G. F., Cole, J. C., and Brunas-Wagstaff, J. (2007). Effects of hypnotic induction and hypnotic depth on phonemic fluency: a test of the frontal inhibition account of hypnosis. Int. J. Psychol. Psychol. Ther. 7, 27–40.

Google Scholar

Wickless, C., and Kirsch, I. (1989). Effects of verbal and experiential expectancy manipulations on hypnotic susceptibility. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 57, 762–768. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.762

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: suggestion, hypnosis, placebo, prefrontal cortex (PFC), executive function

Citation: Parris BA (2016) The Prefrontal Cortex and Suggestion: Hypnosis vs. Placebo Effects. Front. Psychol. 7:415. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00415

Received: 21 December 2015; Accepted: 09 March 2016;
Published: 30 March 2016.

Edited by:

Adam B. Barrett, University of Sussex, UK

Reviewed by:

Arianna Palmieri, University of Padova, Italy

Copyright © 2016 Parris. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Benjamin A. Parris, YnBhcnJpc0Bib3VybmVtb3V0aC5hYy51aw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.