- Department of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
Gender-fair language (GFL) aims at reducing gender stereotyping and discrimination. Two principle strategies have been employed to make languages gender-fair and to treat women and men symmetrically: neutralization and feminization. Neutralization is achieved, for example, by replacing male-masculine forms (policeman) with gender-unmarked forms (police officer), whereas feminization relies on the use of feminine forms to make female referents visible (i.e., the applicant… he or she instead of the applicant… he). By integrating research on (1) language structures, (2) language policies, and (3) individual language behavior, we provide a critical review of how GFL contributes to the reduction of gender stereotyping and discrimination. Our review provides a basis for future research and for scientifically based policy-making.
Linguistic gender asymmetries are ubiquitous, as documented in the contributions in Hellinger and Bußmann (2001, 2002, 2003), which analyze 30 languages (e.g., Arabic, Chinese, English, Finnish, Hindi, Turkish, Swahili) from various language families. An almost universal and fundamental asymmetry lies in the use of masculine generics. In English, for example, generic he can be used when gender is irrelevant (e.g., the user… he) and in German, masculine role nouns serve as labels for mixed gender groups (e.g., einige Lehrer, masc.pl ‘several teachers’ for a group of male and female teachers). Thus, masculine forms not only designate men but also mixed-gender groups or referents whose gender is unknown or unspecified (see Stahlberg et al., 2007). Feminine forms, on the other hand, do not function generically but refer to women only (Hellinger and Bußmann, 2001).
That masculine forms are used to represent all human beings is in accord with the traditional gender hierarchy, which grants men more power and higher social status than women (Ridgeway and Correll, 2004). A large-scale content analysis of 800,000 Reuters news messages (published in English between 1996 and 1997) found that the pronoun he was more frequent than she in the news and also appeared in more positive contexts (Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2014). The interrelation of language and the gender hierarchy has also been documented in a study which analyzed the ratio of male to female pronouns (e.g., he/she, his/hers) in written texts (full texts of about 1.2 million U.S. books, years 1900–2008; from the Google Books database; Twenge et al., 2012). This ratio was found to reflect the status of women in the United States during the 20th century. When women’s status was high (as indicated by educational attainment, labor force participation, etc.), the proportion of female pronouns was higher; when women’s status was low, female pronouns were less frequent.
Gender-fair language (GFL)1 was introduced as a response to this structural asymmetry and as part of a broader attempt to reduce stereotyping and discrimination in language (see Fairclough, 2003; Maass et al., 2013, for the political correctness debate). GFL aims to abolish asymmetries in referring to and addressing women and men, for example, by replacing masculine forms (policeman) with gender-unmarked forms (police officer), or by using both masculine and feminine forms (i.e., the applicant… he or she instead of the applicant… he).
In this paper, we review theoretical and empirical work on the role of GFL in sustaining or reducing gender stereotyping and social discrimination, as a follow-up on a comprehensive research program (the Marie Curie Initial Training Network - Language, Cognition, and Gender, ITN LCG, http://www.itn-lcg.psy.unibe.ch/content/index_eng.html). In this framework, we survey research on (1) language structures, (2) language policies, and (3) individual language behavior in order to draw conclusions on the effectiveness of GFL and to identify boundary conditions and obstacles for its implementation. Our aim is to critically discuss and integrate research findings to answer the question of whether and under what circumstances GFL contributes to the reduction of gender stereotyping and discrimination. Hopefully, this review will provide a useful basis for future research and for scientifically based policy-making.
Language Structures
Although gender asymmetries exist in most, if not all, languages, they may be more or less conspicuous, depending on the structure of the language. Three types of languages can be distinguished: grammatical gender languages, natural gender languages, and genderless languages (see Stahlberg et al., 2007). Table 1 gives an overview of this typology, describing the main characteristics of the different types with regard to gender and gender asymmetries as well as preferred strategies of linguistic gender-fairness. German, French, and Czech, for example, are grammatical gender languages. In these languages, every noun has a grammatical gender and the gender of personal nouns tends to express the gender of the referent. In natural gender languages (English or Swedish)2 personal nouns tend to be gender-neutral (e.g., neighbor) and referential gender is expressed pronominally (e.g., he/she). In genderless languages such as Finnish or Turkish neither personal nouns nor pronouns signal gender. Here, gender is only expressed through attributes such as ‘male/female [teacher]’ or in lexical gender words such as ‘woman’ or ‘father.’ Consequently, gender and linguistic gender asymmetries are much more visible in grammatical gender languages than in natural gender languages or genderless languages (Hellinger and Bußmann, 2001).
The way gender is encoded in a language may be associated with societal gender equality (Stahlberg et al., 2007). This assumption was tested empirically for 111 countries with different language systems, controlling for geographic, religious, political, and developmental differences (Prewitt-Freilino et al., 2012). In this research, the Global Gender Gap Index of the World Economic Forum was used to determine gender equality (GGI; Hausmann et al., 2009). Countries with grammatical gender languages were found to reach lower levels of social gender equality than countries with natural gender languages or genderless languages. This suggests that a higher visibility of gender asymmetries is accompanied by societal gender inequalities. A survey on sexist attitudes yielded additional evidence for this relationship (Wasserman and Weseley, 2009): respondents (native speakers of English as well as bilinguals) exhibited more sexist attitudes when the survey was conducted in a grammatical gender language (Spanish or French) than in a natural gender language (English). These findings document that, from the perspective of gender-fairness or gender equality, grammatical gender languages present a particularly complex and difficult case.
Research has consistently revealed that masculine generics evoke a male bias in mental representations and make readers or listeners think more of male than female exemplars of a person category (Stahlberg et al., 2007). Effects of linguistic forms on mental representations were measured with the help of various experimental methodologies, for instance, (1) completing sentences with different pronouns and nouns (e.g., he, she, he/she, the lawyer, the client; Jacobson and Insko, 1985), (2) writing stories about fictitious people following an introductory sentence in the masculine or in gender-fair wording (Heise, 2000), (3) naming female or male representatives (e.g., favorite musician) in response to either masculine nouns or combinations of feminine and masculine forms (Stahlberg et al., 2001), (4) estimating the proportion of women and men in certain roles (e.g., participants at a congress of nutritionists versus geophysicists; Braun et al., 1998), (5) measuring reading time as an indicator of fit between sentences about social groups denoted by nouns with different grammatical gender and sentences that contained a reference to the social group that qualified the group members as female, male, or neither one (Irmen and Roßberg, 2004), or (6) measuring reaction times when classifying gender-related (e.g., she, he) or neutral pronouns (e.g., it, me) as female or male after perceiving gender-related (e.g., mother, father, nurse, doctor) or gender-neutral primes (e.g., parent, student; Banaji and Hardin, 1996). The masculine bias in language has been observed in English (e.g., Crawford and English, 1984; Hamilton, 1988; Gastil, 1990; Ng, 1990), French (e.g., Chatard et al., 2005; Gabriel et al., 2008), German (e.g., Heise, 2000; Stahlberg et al., 2001; Braun et al., 2005; Irmen, 2007), Italian (e.g., Cacciari and Padovani, 2007), Polish (e.g., Bojarska, 2011), and Spanish (Carreiras et al., 1996). In a study with German and Belgian school children, the grammatical form of job titles was found to influence the children’s perceptions of typically male jobs: when occupations were presented in the masculine (e.g., German Ingenieure, masc.pl ‘engineers’) the mental accessibility of female jobholders was lower than with feminine-masculine word pairs (e.g., Ingenieurinnen und Ingenieure, fem.pl and masc.pl ‘[female and male] engineers’; Vervecken et al., 2013). In another study, adult speakers as well envisaged more men in an occupation when job advertisements included more masculine than feminine forms (Gaucher et al., 2011). In all, both the range of methods as well as the number of languages for which the male bias of masculine generics has been documented attests to the validity of the finding.
In general, different strategies can be used to make language gender-fair and avoid detrimental effects of masculine generics: neutralization, feminization and a combination of the two. Which strategy is the appropriate one depends on the type of language concerned (grammatical gender language, natural gender language, or genderless language, Bußmann and Hellinger, 2003).
In the framework of neutralization gender-marked terms are replaced by gender-indefinite nouns (English policeman by police officer). In grammatical gender languages, gender-differentiated forms are replaced, for instance, by epicenes (i.e., forms with invariant grammatical gender which refer to female as well as male persons; e.g., German Staatsoberhaupt, neut. ‘head of state’ or Fachkraft, fem. ‘expert’ in German). Neutralization has been recommended especially for natural gender languages (e.g., Hellinger and Bußmann, 2003; for English; Norwegian; Danish) and genderless languages (e.g., Engelberg, 2002, for Finnish), as it is fairly easy to avoid gender markings in these languages. Thus, neither generic he nor the combination he/she, but “singular they is the dominant epicene pronoun in modern written British English. However, despite its use, singular they has never been endorsed by institutions of the English language, such as major dictionaries and style guides (although many style guides now reject generic he…)” (Paterson, 2014, p. 2). Recently, a gender-neutral third person pronoun was invented in Swedish: hen. This neologism first appeared in 2012 in a children’s book where it served as an alternative to the gender-marked pronouns ‘she’ (hon) and ‘he’ (han; Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015).
In contrast, feminization is based on the explicit inclusion of women. Thus, masculine generics are replaced by feminine-masculine word pairs (e.g., German Elektrikerinnen und Elektriker ‘[female and male] electricians’; Polish nauczycielki i nauczyciele ‘[female and male] teachers’) or abbreviated forms with slashes (e.g., German Elektriker/in; Polish nauczyciel/ka) or brackets (e.g., Elektriker[in]; nauczyciel[ka]). Feminization has been recommended for grammatical gender languages such as German, Spanish, Czech, and Italian (Hellinger and Bußmann, 2003; Moser et al., 2011), usually in combination with neutralizing in order to avoid overly complex sentence structures.
However, feminization is not always advantageous for women. The Italian feminine suffix -essa, for example, has a slightly derogatory connotation (e.g., Marcato and Thüne, 2002). Accordingly, a woman introduced as professoressa ‘female professor’ was perceived as less persuasive than a man or than a woman referred to with the masculine form professore (Mucchi-Faina, 2005). Masculine terms used in reference to a female jobholder were associated with higher status than feminine job titles with -essa (Merkel et al., 2012). Another example is the German (originally French) suffix-euse or -öse. Feminine terms such as Masseuse ‘(female) masseur’ and Frisöse ‘(female) hair dresser’ evoke sexual or frivolous associations, so that the neutral suffix -in is usually preferred, as in Ingenieur-in ‘female engineer,’ or Spediteur-in ‘female forwarding agent.’ Especially in Slavic languages feminine job titles tend to be associated with lesser status, with rural speech, or with the meaning ‘wife of…’ rather than ‘female job holder’ (for Russian: Doleschal and Schmid, 2001; for Serbian: Hentschel, 2003; for Polish: Koniuszaniec and Blaszkowa, 2003). There are also asymmetries in meaning between feminine and masculine forms, as with Polish sekretarka ‘female secretary,’ which designates a personal assistant, whereas the masculine sekretarz refers also to a high governmental function. In Polish, the feminine suffix -ka not only derives feminine occupational terms (such as nauczyciel-ka ‘female teacher’ from masculine nauczyciel ‘teacher’) but also words for inanimate objects such as marynar-ka ‘jacket’ from masculine marynarz ‘sailor.’ Problems of this kind can limit the possibilities of feminization in some languages. Where feminization faces such structural problems, its use is less widespread and may have negative effects (Italian: Mucchi-Faina, 2005; Polish: Formanowicz et al., 2013, 2015). But where feminine suffixes are productive feminization can became a linguistic norm and can be evaluated positively (German: Vervecken and Hannover, 2012).
The focus of early research on GFL was mostly on the masculine bias associated with masculine generics. But although these findings suggest that linguistic asymmetries may have farther-reaching consequences, this line of research has made no further progress until recently. The latest findings are more comprehensive and indicate how linguistic asymmetries may facilitate (unintended) forms of social discrimination (Mucchi-Faina, 2005; Stahlberg et al., 2007). For example, adult women were reluctant to apply to gender-biased job advertisements (e.g., English job titles ending in -man) and were more interested in the same job when the advertisement had an unbiased form (Bem and Bem, 1973). Also, the likelihood of naming women as possible candidates for the office of chancellor in Germany was found to depend on the grammatical gender of the word ‘chancellor’ in the question (Stahlberg and Sczesny, 2001). When the masculine generic (Kanzler) was used, fewer respondents suggested female politicians compared to a combination of masculine and feminine form (Kanzler oder Kanzlerin ‘[male or female] chancellor’). Moreover, self-evaluation and evaluations by others were found to be influenced by linguistic forms. Thus, girls assumed women to be less successful in typically male occupations when the jobs were described with masculine rather than gender-fair forms, and they were also less interested in these occupations (see also Chatard et al., 2005; Vervecken et al., 2013). Using feminine-masculine word pairs rather than masculine forms for traditionally male occupations boosted children’s self-efficacy (Vervecken and Hannover, 2015). Furthermore, occupations described in pair forms mitigated the difference between ascribed success to female and male jobholders in gendered occupations (Vervecken et al., 2015). Also, women’s perceptions of belonging were found to mediate the effect that women found jobs advertised in the masculine less appealing (Gaucher et al., 2011). Accordingly, women experienced the use of gender-exclusive language during a mock job interview as ostracism (Stout and Dasgupta, 2011). They reported a lower sense of belonging when gender-exclusive language (he) was used compared to gender-inclusive (he or she) or gender-neutral (one) forms. In a study on Austrian German, the wording of job advertisements influenced the evaluation of candidates for leadership positions (Horvath and Sczesny, 2015): men were perceived as fitting a high-status leadership position better than women when a masculine job title was used (Geschäftsführer, masc. ‘chief executive officer, CEO’). But when the job ad was gender-fair (Geschäftsführerin/Geschäftsführer, fem./masc. ‘[female/male] CEO’), women and men were judged as equally suited. In the context of a lower-status position (project leader) no differences of this kind occurred.
Language Policies
Many countries have pledged themselves to an equal treatment of women and men (e.g., the member states of the European Union and associated states in the Treaty of Lisbon- European Commission, 2007), and the use of GFL is widely recommended (Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei, 1996, revised in 2009; UNESCO, 1999; National Council of Teachers of English, 2002; European Commission, 2008; American Psychological Association, 2009). But the implementation of GFL has reached different stages in different countries and speech communities.
In the 1970s, guidelines for GFL were introduced in particular professional domains across national and linguistic boundaries, for example, by the American Psychological Association (1975), by the McGraw-Hill Book Company (1974; see also Britton and Lumpkin, 1977; Sunderland, 2011) and the Macmillan Publishing Company (1975). These guidelines demand that authors of (psychological) articles, books, teaching materials, or fiction treat women and men equally, including the language they use (see also Sadker et al., 1991). Publication guidelines of this kind have been effective, because authors need to follow the rules if they want to see their manuscripts published. In texts written by Australian academics (Pauwels, 2003), for example, masculine generic pronouns were infrequent. Similarly, an analysis of American Psychological Association journal articles from the years 1965–2004 revealed a complete absence of generic he from 1985 onward, even if the articles still contained other, more subtle gender biases such as androcentric reporting in tables and graphs (Hegarty and Buechel, 2006).
In 1987 representatives of Canada and the Nordic countries argued for an adoption of GFL by the United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization. This resulted in the creation of guidelines in UNESCO (1999). UNESCO’s position in favor of GFL is described in their gender equality guidelines: “This development indicated a growing awareness that language does not merely reflect the way we think: it also shapes our thinking. If words and expressions that imply that women are inferior to men are constantly used, that assumption of inferiority tends to become part of our mindset; hence the need to adjust our language when our ideas evolve” (UNESCO, 2011, p. 4). The document not only became the most widely recognized international standard for GFL, it also regulates language use in internal documents and publications of UNESCO. Similar guidelines for publications were issued by the European Commission (2008), referring to all working languages of the European Union (EU). Yet, the standards promoted by UNESCO and the EU do not regulate language use in the different countries and are not considered mandatory within their member states.
The availability of GFL policies and the extent of their implementation, that is, their dissemination and execution, also vary considerably between countries (Moser et al., 2011). In Italy, for instance, guidelines for GFL were issued in Sabatini (1987), in the German-speaking area most guidelines appeared in the 1990s (e.g., Hellinger and Bierbach, 1993; Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei, 1996; revised in 2009), and in the Czech Republic guidelines were published only in Valdrová et al. (2010). In other countries such as Poland there are as yet no official guidelines at all. While in some states GFL policies are mentioned only on the website of a ministry (e.g., Czech Republic; Valdrová et al., 2010), use of GFL is mandatory in job ads and public administration in Austria. Since the 1990s the German Duden dictionaries, for example, have included not only the masculine form of personal nouns and job titles but routinely cite the corresponding feminine forms (Kunkel-Razum, 2004). The dictionary lists even feminine forms that are infrequent in texts. An example is the word Päpstin ‘female pope,’ which has been listed in the Grosses Wörterbuch der deutschen Sprache (Large dictionary of the German language) from the year 1999 onward, even though obviously there never was a female pope in the history of the Catholic Church (Kunkel-Razum, 2004). Moreover, the Duden editors decided to include a chapter on the “equal treatment of women and men in language” in the ninth volume of the series Richtiges und gutes Deutsch (Correct and good German). The chapter describes the linguistic potential which the German language offers for speaking or writing in a gender-fair way.
In the German-speaking countries, language policies have become part of the organizational culture of various institutions such as universities and administrations (e.g., Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei, 1996, revised in 2009; Merkel, 2011; Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich, 2011; Gendup – Zentrum für Gender Studies und Frauenförderung, 2012). Even so, Austria is the only country where the use of GFL in job advertisements is strictly prescribed and companies are fined for failing to address both genders in their job ads (Bundesministerium für Frauen und Öffentlichen Dienst, 2009). This may be the reason why the proportion of job ads worded in GFL differs between Austria and German-speaking Switzerland: only 9% of Austrian job advertisements contain masculine generics, whereas it is 27% in Switzerland (Hodel et al., 2013).
School and education are of particular importance for the implementation of GFL. In most countries there are few official GFL guidelines for authors of educational materials (Eurydice, 2009) and regulations concerning schoolbooks exist only in certain countries (e.g., Germany, Ireland, or Iceland). Similarly, only a few countries require schoolbooks to be officially evaluated or approved. In the UK, for example, educational authorities do not monitor teaching materials and schools choose them autonomously. Today German schoolbooks for mathematics and German mostly use gender-neutral forms, followed by masculine generics and feminine-masculine word pairs, (Moser and Hannover, 2014). The two gender-fair options together (word pairs and neutralizing) outweighed the masculine in the schoolbook sample that was analyzed. Since earlier studies on German schoolbooks (e.g., Lindner and Lukesch, 1994; Preinsberger and Weisskircher, 1997) reported a predominance of masculine generics, this finding indicates an increase of GFL in schoolbooks. In some of the texts, however, feminine-masculine word pairs were mixed with masculine generics (see also Markom and Weinhäupl, 2007). This inconsistency is problematic because in the presence of word pairs masculine forms may be understood as referring to male persons only (e.g., Gabriel et al., 2008).
Individual Language Behavior
Apart from language structures and country-specific aspects, there are a number of factors that make individuals use or reject GFL. One major factor is the novelty of gender-fair forms, which conflicts with speakers’ linguistic habits (Blaubergs, 1980). As long as this is the case, people may experience GFL as irritating, and consequentially may refrain from using it. This could explain why negative effects of GFL have been found especially in the initial phases of language reform such as, for instance, in English in the 1990s (McConnell and Fazio, 1996), and in Italian and Polish in the beginning of the 21st century (Mucchi-Faina, 2005; Merkel et al., 2012; Formanowicz et al., 2013).
Moreover, initiatives for GFL were first instigated by activist movements (e.g., Silveira, 1980; Pusch, 1984) and for that reason often met with negative reactions (Blaubergs, 1980; Parks and Roberton, 1998; Formanowicz et al., 2013). It is conceivable that individual reactions toward GFL are not only caused by its novelty, but also depend on attitudes toward gender arrangements (Jost and Kay, 2005; Carney et al., 2008), for conservative political attitudes are associated both with lesser openness for novelty (Carney et al., 2008) and with stronger support for traditional gender arrangements (Jost et al., 2003, 2008; Hoyt, 2012). Thus, speakers of Polish with more conservative attitudes devaluated female job applicants referring to themselves with a feminine job title compared to female and male applicants using a masculine job title (Formanowicz et al., 2013).
Another factor for individual speakers’ use of GFL might be speakers’ gender: women could be expected to hold more favorable attitudes toward GFL than men and they might be more inclined to use it in their own speech. However, research findings on this point are mixed. While in some studies men rejected GFL more than women did (e.g., Parks and Roberton, 2004; Douglas and Sutton, 2014), other studies found no gender difference in attitudes toward GFL (e.g., Sczesny et al., 2015). Gender differences were mediated by participants’ attitudes toward women, which were, in turn, driven by more comprehensive ideologies that justified the social gender hierarchy (i.e., gender-specific system justification and social dominance orientation; Douglas and Sutton, 2014).
Language use has been viewed as associated with speakers’ sexist attitudes, so much so that the use of sexist language has been regarded as an example of subtle sexism (Swim et al., 2004). Modern sexism, for instance, is a view that denies that women are still discriminated against and disapproves of policies promoting gender equality (Swim et al., 1995). In fact, participants with modern sexist beliefs were found to use more traditional, gender-unfair language (Swim et al., 2004). Correspondingly, speakers with stronger sexist attitudes toward women used gender-fair pronouns less frequently than speakers with less sexist attitudes (Jacobson and Insko, 1985). Speakers with progressive gender role perceptions, on the other hand, exhibited a tendency to avoid sexist language when writing an essay (McMinn et al., 1991).
This raises the question how sexist or non-sexist ideologies translate into actual language behavior. Spontaneous use of GFL was found to be guided by explicit intentions to use GFL as well as more implicit processes involving use of GFL in the past (Sczesny et al., 2015). GFL use was not predicted directly by sexist beliefs but by intentions and habits. In other words, sexist speakers do not avoid GFL just because they are reluctant to change their linguistic habits, they deliberately employ a form of language that treats males as the norm and makes women less visible. Habits guide speakers’ linguistic behavior without their being aware of it (Sczesny et al., 2015), and learning processes play a role for GFL to become a habit. Speakers who grew up with schoolbooks using predominantly masculine generics (e.g., English: Hellinger, 1980; Campbell and Schram, 1995; Lee and Collins, 2008; German: Lindner and Lukesch, 1994; Preinsberger and Weisskircher, 1997) tend not to question this usage. But once speakers have acquired the habit of using GFL they will rely on this language form. Establishing GFL habits via teaching and practicing current linguistic standards (e.g., Duden; Kunkel-Razum, 2004) is a promising approach which should follow the initial phase of GFL implementation and may reduce political controversies. In this sense, a prevalence of GFL in the media could also promote the use of GFL by individual speakers.
So far, few studies have investigated how speakers can be made to use and approve of GFL. After training interventions, speakers of English used slightly more gender-fair pronouns in completing sentences than non-attendants (McMinn and Foster, 1991; McMinn et al., 1991; Prentice, 1994). Their attitudes, however, did not change (Prentice, 1994). German speakers as well used more GFL after being exposed to arguments for GFL than in a control condition (Koeser and Sczesny, 2014), but this did not affect their attitudes toward GFL. Interestingly, merely reading texts in gender-fair wording can also increase speakers’ own use of GFL: female speakers of German employed more gender-fair forms after reading a gender-fair text than after other texts, but there was no such effect for men (Koeser et al., 2015). Male speakers increased their use of gender-fair forms only when their attention was drawn to GFL forms. These findings indicate that it is more difficult to change attitudes than to promote speakers’ actual use of GFL.
Overcoming Gender Stereotyping and Discrimination with Gender-Fair Language?
Over the past decades, a large body of research—based on various experimental methodologies, from storytelling to measuring reaction times—has confirmed the influence of linguistic forms on the accessibility of mental representations of women and men (see Stahlberg et al., 2007). Regardless of language structure and of the ease of implementing GFL (Bußmann and Hellinger, 2003), a consistent finding is that speakers do not understand masculine forms as referring to both genders equally but that they interpret them in a male-biased way. This underscores the importance of implementing GFL in everyday language and of using it consistently, so that speakers take up this usage in their own texts and utterances.
How successful have the respective language policies been so far? In natural gender languages, neutralization has been fairly easy to adopt and implement (e.g., English, Danish). But even in these language communities people are guided by their knowledge about typical gender distributions in social roles. Thus, English readers tend to associate different occupations or role nouns with men or women, since gender stereotypes are incorporated in their mental representations (Oakhill et al., 2005); and even though there are fewer gender-marked forms in natural gender languages, masculine generics exist and their use can result in social discrimination (Stout and Dasgupta, 2011). In grammatical gender languages, feminization as the main strategy of GFL still poses challenges. This is especially true for some languages, e.g., Italian (Merkel et al., 2012) and Slavic languages (Koniuszaniec and Blaszkowa, 2003), where the creation of feminine forms can be problematic, as outlined above. Refusal of GFL can still be observed (Formanowicz and Sczesny, 2014). Such disadvantages are likely to occur while the change is in progress (Formanowicz et al., 2015).
Moreover, our review suggests that—independent of language structure—GFL is more frequent and more accepted when it is backed by official regulations and when the use of biased language is sanctioned in some way (e.g., in official publications or texts; American Psychological Association, 1975, 2009; Bundesministerium für Frauen und Öffentlichen Dienst, 2009; see Hodel et al., 2013). The relationship between policy-making and social change is surely bidirectional. On the one hand, gender equality movements and their demands find their way into legislation. On the other hand, official regulations may stipulate social change by facilitating the internalization of new norms and enforcing their execution. Public discussions over policies also enhance public awareness for GFL (see above the singular pronouns they in English and hen in Swedish). The contribution of language reforms to gender equality in a society/speech community can best be assessed with investigations that compare countries sharing the same language (e.g., French in Canada and in France) as well as countries with different languages (e.g., Polish and German, two grammatical languages at different stages of implementing GFL). Although there have been some attempts at this type of research (Formanowicz et al., 2015; Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015) more research is needed to evaluate the effectiveness of language-related policies and provide an evidence-based rationale for policy-making.
As mentioned above, speakers’ use of GFL results from deliberate processes, involving attitudes and intentions, and habitual processes, involving repetition of past behavior (Sczesny et al., 2015). Both types of processes are relevant for the successful implementation of GFL. Despite the various guidelines and legal regulations for GFL that exist on global and national levels, spontaneous use of GFL by individual speakers still seems to be infrequent. For instance, use of GFL in a gap-filling task was quite low among speakers of German from Germany and Switzerland, although GFL policies are fairly advanced in both countries. Most of the participants used more masculine generics than gender-fair forms. As language use is an action performed in a wide range of circumstances, future research should also assess the contiguity between behavior and context. Speakers may employ GFL when writing official texts, for instance, but not when talking or writing to friends. Moreover, attitudes, norms, and intentions concerning GFL in general seem to be only moderately favorable. Even though positive arguments for GFL can help to promote a change in language behavior (Koeser and Sczesny, 2014), future research should attempt to identify factors that are crucial for a deliberate use of GFL. For instance, it might be worthwhile to determine the content and strength of attitudes in different groups of speakers, namely speakers who use GFL regularly compared to speakers who use GFL only occasionally and others who do not use it at all. To gain a more comprehensive understanding of the processes underlying a rejection of GFL, future research could also take a closer look at people’s political attitudes (Formanowicz et al., 2013), their preference for status quo, and their acceptance of traditional gender arrangements (Jost et al., 2008).
In any case, attitudes toward GFL may become more favorable the more frequently and longer GFL has been used (in addition to a mere exposure effect, Zajonc, 1968, see also the existence bias: people treat the existence of something as evidence of its goodness; Eidelman et al., 2009). The role of familiarity for an active use of GFL can best addressed with longitudinal studies. In Sweden, for example, speakers’ attitudes toward the gender-neutral pronoun hen have become more positive over time (Gustafsson Sendén et al., 2015). A meta-analytical approach would constitute another way of capturing the dynamics of GFL implementation, taking into account the time when the studies were conducted but also the availability of policies and the structure of the languages concerned. This approach might help to determine whether a language has left the phase where GFL evokes negative associations as well as the role of other factors (such as language policies).
Interventions aiming to increase the use of GFL could focus on a simple repetition of non-sexist expressions, so that these become established habits (Koeser et al., 2015; Wood and Rünger, 2016). This would be a very subtle and implicit way of promoting use of GFL. The development and evaluation of GFL interventions/trainings has not yet been investigated systematically. Future research should take both deliberate and habitual processes of GFL use into consideration, for instance, by analyzing whether children—exposed to and trained in GFL at school (with the help of current schoolbooks)—will later use GFL habitually and consequently hold less gender-stereotypic beliefs.
Finally, there are still obstacles that prevent GFL from becoming a linguistic norm/standard and prevent the change toward an equal treatment of women and men. First, the male bias of linguistic asymmetries in mental representations is backed by a higher prevalence of men in certain social roles (e.g., heroes, politicians), which facilitates their cognitive accessibility (Stahlberg and Sczesny, 2001). Once women and men occupy all social roles to a similar extent (see social role theory, which poses that gender stereotype content results from observing women and men in certain societal roles; Eagly, 1987; Bosak et al., 2012), this difference in accessibility should decrease and more gender-balanced mental representations should emerge. Ironically, recent research has documented that linguistic asymmetries prevent girls and women from aspiring to male-dominated roles (see Chatard et al., 2005; Gaucher et al., 2011; Stout and Dasgupta, 2011; Vervecken et al., 2013; Vervecken and Hannover, 2015) and thereby perpetuate the higher accessibility of men in these roles.
Second, the use of gender-unfair language, especially of masculine generics, restricts the visibility of women and the cognitive availability of female exemplars (Stahlberg et al., 2007), which may be disadvantageous for women (e.g., in personnel selection; Stout and Dasgupta, 2011; Horvath and Sczesny, 2015). However, increasing the visibility of women with the help of novel feminine forms may also have negative consequences and may therefore be avoided, for instance, in women’s professional self-reference (Merkel et al., 2012; Formanowicz et al., 2013). Thus, the avoidance of GFL by women (e.g., avoidance of feminine job titles in grammatical gender languages), in order to protect themselves from ascriptions of incompetence or lower status, also perpetuates the reduction of gender stereotyping and social discrimination.
Third, arguments against GFL have routinely included the presumed difficulty of understanding GFL texts (Parks and Roberton, 1998). Empirical investigations have refuted this argument and have shown that text quality (Rothmund and Christmann, 2002) and cognitive processing were not damaged (Braun et al., 2007). When GFL texts were compared to (generic) masculine texts, there were no differences in readability and esthetic appeal (Blake and Klimmt, 2010). In all, the empirical evidence does not confirm the alleged disadvantage of GFL. Yet, these findings and the scientific evidence for serious disadvantages of masculine generics (see above) have largely been ignored in political controversies and public discussions about GFL. In all, there is a lack of transfer of scientific knowledge which prevents the understanding of linguistic asymmetries as part of a broader gender imbalance and hinders social change. Education and policy-making therefore need to increase the efforts of circulating new scientific insights about GFL to break the vicious circle of ill-informed controversies and discussions about GFL.
At first glance linguistic gender asymmetries seem to affect mostly women. When masculine forms are used it is women who are seen as less prototypical category exemplars, it is women who feel less adequate or are less preferred as job candidates, and it is women who profit from GFL. Therefore, the question arises whether GFL benefits men as well. First, the introduction of GFL might represent a particular challenge for men. In a study by Crawford and English (1984) both male and female participants read a text whose title contained either masculine generics (Psychologist and his work?) or GFL (Psychologist and their work?) and were to recall the text after 2 days. As the results showed, men’s recall was better in the masculine and women’s recall in the GFL condition. This finding indicates that learning to use GFL involves more than overcoming linguistic novelty. For men, GFL means an unwelcome loss of their privileged position in language. Only in few situations have they something to gain through GFL. If all job advertisements would contain GFL, for instance, men might be more included in traditionally female jobs which used to be referred to in the feminine. Future research should also consider the perspective of men and examine how GFL can turn into a win–win situation for women and men in modern societies.
To conclude, past research has revealed that GFL has the potential to make significant contributions to the reduction of gender stereotyping and discrimination. But as the body of existing evidence is based mainly on experimental paradigms with different kinds of measures, future research should take a closer look on people’s actual language use in everyday life (e.g., in conversations, in the classroom, in social media or organizational communication). Moreover, it will be fruitful to further investigate the dynamics of GFL usage and its effects from cross-linguistic and cross-cultural perspectives (see above the Marie Curie Initial Training Network - Language, Cognition, and Gender, ITN LCG, which can be regarded as a first step in this direction). Speakers’ willingness to use GFL in everyday life is crucial in order to profit from the impact of GFL on the (linguistic and social) treatment of women and men in society. But a deliberate effort is required before the use of GFL can become habitual. Education and policy-making can facilitate these processes. When employed consistently over a longer period, and especially when supported by well-informed controversies and discussions, GFL will contribute even more to the reduction of gender stereotyping and discrimination and may thus function as another barometer for change (like the decrease in gender-stereotypical social perception of leadership, Schein, 2001).
Funding
This research was conducted within the Marie Curie Initial Training Network: Language, Cognition, and Gender, ITN LCG, funded by the European Commission’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013) under grant agreement n°237907 (www.itn-lcg.eu). We thank Friederike Braun for her valuable comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer Simona Mancini and handling Editor Manuel Carreiras declared their shared affiliation, and the handling Editor states that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.
Footnotes
- ^ In the literature, GFL is also referred to with other terms, e.g., gender-neutral language: Sarrasin et al. (2012); gender-inclusive language: Stout and Dasgupta (2011); non-sexist language: Douglas and Sutton (2014).
- ^ According to McConnell-Ginet (2013), however, the concept of natural gender language is a myth, and she suggests calling the respective languages “notional” gender languages, since, for example in English, “concepts and ideas about biological sex matter at least as much as sex itself to the choice of English third-person pronouns.” (p. 3).
References
American Psychological Association (1975). Guidelines for nonsexist use of language. Am. Psychol. 30, 682–684. doi: 10.1037/h0076869
American Psychological Association (2009). Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association: Supplemental Material, 6th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Banaji, M. R., and Hardin, C. D. (1996). Automatic stereotyping. Psychol. Sci. 7, 136–141. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x
Bem, S. L., and Bem, D. J. (1973). Does sex-biased job advertising “aid and abet” sex discrimination? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 3, 6–18. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1973.tb01290.x
Blake, C., and Klimmt, C. (2010). Geschlechtergerechte Formulierungen in Nachrichtentexten [Gender-fair formulations in news texts]. Publizistik 55, 289–304. doi: 10.1007/s11616-010-0093-2
Blaubergs, M. S. (1980). An analysis of classic arguments against changing sexist language. Womens Stud. Int. Q. 3, 135–147. doi: 10.1016/S0148-0685(80)92071-0
Bojarska, K. (2011). Wpływ androcentrycznych i inkluzywnych płciowo konstrukcji językowych na skojarzenia z płcią [The impact of the andocentric and gender-inclusive language constructions on the gendered asssociations]. Stud. Psychol. 49, 53–68. doi: 10.2478/v10167-011-0010-y
Bosak, J., Sczesny, S., and Eagly, A. H. (2012). The impact of social roles on trait judgments – a critical re-examination. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 38, 429–440. doi: 10.1177/0146167211427308
Braun, F., Gottburgsen, A., Sczesny, S., and Stahlberg, D. (1998). Können Geophysiker Frauen sein? Generische Personenbezeichnungen im Deutschen [Can geophysicists be women? Generic terms for describing persons in German]. Z. Ger. Linguist. 26, 177–195. doi: 10.1515/zfgl.1998.26.3.265
Braun, F., Oelkers, S., Rogalski, K., Bosak, J., and Sczesny, S. (2007). “For reasons of intelligibility.” How masculine generics and alternative forms affect the cognitive processing of a text. Psychol. Rundsch. 58, 183–189. doi: 10.1026/0033-3042.58.3.183
Braun, F., Sczesny, S., and Stahlberg, D. (2005). Cognitive effects of masculine generics in German: an overview of empirical findings. Communications 30, 1–21. doi: 10.1515/comm.2005.30.1.1
Britton, G. E., and Lumpkin, M. C. (1977). For sale: subliminal bias in textbooks. Read. Teach. 31, 40–45.
Bundesministerium für Frauen und Öffentlichen Dienst (2009). Geschlechtergerechte Stellenausschreibungen [Gender-Fair Job Advertisements]. Unabhängiger Bericht der Gleichbehandlungsanwaltschaft iS §3 Abs 5 GBK/GAW-Gesetz, Wien.
Bußmann, H., and Hellinger, M. (2003). “Engendering female visibility in German,” in Gender Across Languages. The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 3, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 141–173.
Cacciari, C., and Padovani, R. (2007). Further evidence on gender stereotype priming in language: semantic facilitation and inhibition on Italian role nouns. Appl. Psycholinguist. 28, 277–293. doi: 10.1017/S0142716407070142
Campbell, R., and Schram, P. J. (1995). Feminist research methods. A content analysis of psychology and social science textbooks. Psychol. Women Q. 19, 85–106. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1995.tb00280.x
Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Samuel, D., Gosling, S. D., and Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Polit. Psychol. 29, 807–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9221.2008.00668.x
Carreiras, M., Garnham, A., Oakhill, J. V., and Cain, K. (1996). The use of stereotypical gender information in constructing a mental model: evidence from English and Spanish. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 49, 639–663. doi: 10.1080/027249896392531
Chatard, A., Guimond, S., and Martinot, D. (2005). Impact de la féminisation lexicale des professions sur l’auto-efficacité des élèves: une remise en cause de l’universalisme masculin? Année Psychol. 105, 249–272. doi: 10.3406/psy.2005.29694
Crawford, M., and English, L. (1984). Generic versus specific inclusion of women in language: effects on recall. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 13, 373–381. doi: 10.1007/BF01068152
Doleschal, U., and Schmid, S. (2001). “Doing gender in Russian,” in Gender Across Languages. The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 1, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 253–282.
Douglas, K. M., and Sutton, R. M. (2014). “A giant leap for mankind” but what about women? The role of system-justifying ideologies in predicting attitudes toward sexist language. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 33, 667–680. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14538638
Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex Differences in Social Behavior. A Social-Role Interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Eidelman, S., Crandall, C. S., and Pattershall, J. (2009). The existence bias. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 97, 765–775. doi: 10.1037/a0017058
Engelberg, M. (2002). “The communication of gender in Finnish,” in Gender Across Languages. The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 2, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 109–132.
European Commission (2007). Treaty of Lisbon. Available at: http://ec.europa.eu/archives/lisbon_treaty/full_text/index_en.htm
European Commission (2008). Gender-Neutral Language in the European Parliament. Available at: http://www.europarl.europa.eu/RegData/publications/2009/0001/P6_PUB(2009)0001_EN.pdf
Eurydice (2009). Gender Differences in Educational Outcomes: Study on the Measures Taken and the Current Situation in Europe. Available at: http://eacea.ec.europa.eu/education/eurydice/thematic_reports_en.php
Fairclough, N. (2003). Political correctness: the politics of culture and language. Discourse Soc. 14, 17–28. doi: 10.1177/0957926503014001927
Formanowicz, M., Bedynska, S., Cisłak, A., Braun, F., and Sczesny, S. (2013). Side effects of gender-fair language: how feminine job titles influence the evaluation of female applicants. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 62–71. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1924
Formanowicz, M., and Sczesny, S. (2014). Gender-Fair language and professional self-reference: the case of female psychologists in Polish. J. Mix. Methods Res. 10, 64–81. doi: 10.1177/1558689814550877
Formanowicz, M. M., Cisłak, A., Horvath, L. K., and Sczesny, S. (2015). Capturing socially motivated linguistic change: how the use of gender-fair language affects support for social initiatives in Austria and Poland. Front. Psychol. 6:1617. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01617
Gabriel, U., Gygax, P., Sarrasin, O., Garnham, A., and Oakhill, J. (2008). Au-pairs are rarely male: role names’ gender stereotype information across three languages. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 206–212. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.1.206
Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: the oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles 23, 629–643. doi: 10.1007/BF0028925
Gaucher, D., Friesen, J., and Kay, A. C. (2011). Evidence that gendered wording in job advertisement exists and sustains gender inequality. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 101, 109–128. doi: 10.1037/a0022530
Gendup – Zentrum für Gender Studies und Frauenförderung (2012). Leitfaden für einen gerechten Sprachgebrauch [Guideline for Gender-Fair Language]. Available at: http://www.uni-salzburg.at/fileadmin/oracle_file_imports/2103374.PDF
Gustafsson Sendén, M., Bäck, E. A., and Lindqvist, A. (2015). Introducing a gender-neutral pronoun in a natural gender language: the influence of time on attitudes and behavior. Front. Psychol. 6:893. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00893
Gustafsson Sendén, M., Lindholm, T., and Sikström, S. (2014). Biases in news media as reflected by personal pronouns in evaluative contexts. Soc. Psychol. 45, 103–111. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000165
Hamilton, M. C. (1988). Using masculine generics: does generic he increase male bias in the user’s imagery? Sex Roles 19, 785–799. doi: 10.1007/bf00288993
Hausmann, R., Tyson, L. D., and Zahidi, S. (2009). The Global Gender Gap Report 2009. Geneva: World Economic Forum.
Hegarty, P., and Buechel, C. (2006). Androcentric reporting of gender differences in APA journals: 1965-2004. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 10, 377–389. doi: 10.1037/1089-2680.10.4.377
Heise, E. (2000). Sind Frauen mitgemeint? Eine empirische Untersuchung zum Verständnis des generischen Maskulinums und seiner Alternativen [Are women included? An empirical investigation of interpretations of masculine generics and their alternatives]. Sprache Kogn. 19, 3–13. doi: 10.1024//0253-4533.19.12.3
Hellinger, M. (1980). For men must work and women must weep: sexism in English language textbooks used in German schools. Womens Stud. Int. Q. 3, 267–275. doi: 10.1016/S0148-0685(80)92323-4
Hellinger, M., and Bierbach, C. (1993). Eine Sprache für beide Geschlechter. Richtlinien für einen nicht-sexistischen Sprachgebrauch. [A Language for Both Genders. Guidelines for a Non-Sexist Language Use]. Available at: http://www.unesco.de/fileadmin/medien/Dokumente/Bibliothek/eine_sprache.pdf
Hellinger, M., and Bußmann, H. (2001, 2002, 2003). Gender Across Languages: The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 1, 2, 3. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Hentschel, E. (2003). “Serbian: the expression of gender in Serbian,” in Gender Across Languages: The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 3, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 287–309.
Hodel, L., Formanowicz, M., Sczesny, S., Valdrova, J., and von Stockhausen, L. (2013). “Gender fair language use in online job Avertisements,” in Proceedings of the 16th Conference of European Association of Work & Organisational Psychology (EAWOP), Münster.
Horvath, L. K., and Sczesny, S. (2015). Reducing women’s lack of fit with leadership? Effects of the wording of job advertisements. Eur. J. Work Organ. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/1359432X.2015.1067611
Hoyt, C. L. (2012). Gender bias in employment contexts: a closer examination of the role incongruity principle. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 48, 86–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.08.004
Irmen, L. (2007). What’s in a (role) name? Formal and conceptual aspects of comprehending personal nouns. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 36, 431–456. doi: 10.1007/s10936-007-9053-z
Irmen, L., and Roßberg, N. (2004). Gender markedness of language. The impact of grammatical and nonlinguistic information on the mental representation of person information. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 23, 272–307. doi: 10.1177/0261927X04266810
Jacobson, M. B., and Insko, W. R. Jr. (1985). Use of non-sexist pronouns as a function of one’s feminist orientation. Sex Roles 13, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/BF00287456
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., and Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychol. Bull. 129, 339–375. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.33
Jost, J. T., and Kay, A. C. (2005). Exposure to benevolent sexism and complementary gender stereotypes: consequences for specific and diffuse forms of system justification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 498–509. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.3.498
Jost, J. T., Nosek, B. A., and Gosling, S. D. (2008). Ideology: its resurgence in social, personality, and political psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 3, 126–136. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00070.x
Koeser, S., Kuhn, E. A., and Sczesny, S. (2015). Just reading? How gender-fair language triggers readers‘ use of gender-fair forms. Advance online publication. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 34, 343–357. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14561119
Koeser, S., and Sczesny, S. (2014). Promoting gender-fair language: the impact of arguments on language use, attitudes, and cognitions. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 33, 548–560. doi: 10.1177/0261927X14541280
Koniuszaniec, G., and Blaszkowa, H. (2003). “Language and gender in Polish,” in Gender Across Languages. The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 3, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 259–285.
Kunkel-Razum, K. (2004). “Die Frauen und der Duden – der Duden und die Frauen [The women and the Duden – the Duden and the women],” in Adam, Eva und Die Sprache, Beiträge zur Geschlechterforschung [Adam, Eve and the Language], ed. K. M. Eichhoff-Cyrus (Mannheim: Dudenverlag), 308–315.
Lee, J. F. K., and Collins, P. (2008). Gender voices in Hong Kong English textbooks – some past and current practices. Sex Roles 59, 127–137. doi: 10.1007/s11199-008-9414-6
Lindner, V., and Lukesch, H. (1994). Geschlechtsrollenstereotype im Deutschen Schulbuch. [Gender Stereotypes in German Schoolbooks]. Regensburg: S. Roderer Verlag.
Maass, A., Suitner, C., and Merkel, E. (2013). “Does political correctness make (social) sense?,” in Social Cognition and Communication, eds J. P. Forgas, O. Vincze, and J. Laszlo (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 331–346.
Macmillan Publishing Company (1975). Guidelines for Creating Positive Sexual and Racial Images in Educational Materials. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Marcato, G., and Thüne, E. M. (2002). “Gender and female visibility in Italian,” in Gender Across Languages: The Linguistic Representation of Women and Men, Vol. 2, eds M. Hellinger and H. Bußmann (Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company), 187–217.
Markom, C., and Weinhäupl, H. (2007). Die Anderen im Schulbuch. Rassismen, Exotismen, Sexismen und Antisemitismus in österreichischen Schulbüchern. [The Others in Schoolbooks. Racism, Exocitism, Sexism, and Anti-Semitism in Austrian Schoolbooks]. Wien: Braumüller.
McConnell, A. R., and Fazio, R. H. (1996). Women as men and people: effects of gender-marked language. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 22, 1004–1013. doi: 10.1177/01461672962210003
McConnell-Ginet, S. (2013). “Gender and its relation to sex: the myth of ‘natural’ gender,” in The Expression of Gender, ed. G. G. Corbett (Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton), 3–38.
McGraw-Hill Book Company (1974). Guidelines for Equal Treatment of the Sexes. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company.
McMinn, M. R., and Foster, J. D. (1991). A computer program to teach nonsexist language. Teach. Psychol. 18, 115–117. doi: 10.1207/s15328023top1802_16
McMinn, M. R., Troyer, P. K., Hannum, L. E., and Foster, J. D. (1991). Teaching nonsexist language to college students. J. Exp. Educ. 59, 153–161. doi: 10.1080/00220973.1991.10806558
Merkel, E. (2011). Geschlechtergerechte Sprache in den universitären Gleichstellungskonzepten: Eine vergleichende Analyse [Gender-Fair Language in University Concepts of Gender-Equality: A Comparative Analysis]. Essen: Netzwerk Frauen– und Geschlechterforschung NRW.
Merkel, E., Maass, A., and Frommelt, L. (2012). Shielding women against status loss. The masculine form and its alternatives in the Italian language. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 31, 311–320. doi: 10.1177/0261927X12446599
Moser, F., and Hannover, B. (2014). How gender fair are German schoolbooks in the twenty-first century? An analysis of language and illustrations in schoolbooks for mathematics and German. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 29, 387–407. doi: 10.1007/s10212-013-0204-3
Moser, F., Sato, S., Chiarini, T., Dmitrov-Devold, K., and Kuhn, E. (2011). Comparative Analysis of Existing Guidelines for Gender-Fair Language within the ITN LCG Network (Work Package B ITN LCG). Available at: http://www.unifr.ch/psycho/itn-lcg/assets/files/ITN_publications/guidelines_final_may2011.pdf
Mucchi-Faina, A. (2005). Visible or influential? Language reforms and gender (in)equality. Soc. Sci. Inform. 44, 189–215. doi: 10.1177/0539018405050466
National Council of Teachers of English (2002). Guidelines for Gender-Fair Use of Language. Available at: www.ncte.org/positions/statements/genderfairuseoflang
Ng, S. H. (1990). Androcentric coding of man and his in memory by language users. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 26, 455–464. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(90)90069-x
Oakhill, J., Garnham, A., and Reynolds, D. (2005). Immediate activation of stereotypical gender information. Mem. Cognit. 33, 972–983. doi: 10.3758/bf03193206
Parks, J. B., and Roberton, M. A. (1998). Contemporary arguments against nonsexist language: Blaubergs (1980) revisited. Sex Roles 39, 445–461. doi: 10.1023/A:1018827227128
Parks, J. B., and Roberton, M. A. (2004). Attitudes toward women mediate the gender effect on attitudes toward sexist language. Psychol. Women Q. 28, 233–239. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.2004.00140.x
Paterson, L. L. (2014). British Pronoun Use, Prescription, and Processing: Linguistic and Social Influences Affecting ‘They’ and ‘He’. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.
Pauwels, A. (2003). “Linguistic sexism and feminist linguistic activism,” in The Handbook of Language and Gender, eds M. Meyerhoff and J. Holmes (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing), 550–570.
Preinsberger, A., and Weisskircher, E. (1997). “Mathematikschulbücher – eine aktuelle Untersuchung [Mathematics books – a recent examination],” in Schule Weiblich – Schule Männlich. Zum Geschlechterverhältnis im Bildungswesen [Female School – Male School. On Gender Relations in the Educational System], eds L. Lassnigg and A. Paseka (Innsbruck: Studien Verlag), 132–143.
Prentice, D. A. (1994). Do language reforms change our way of thinking? J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 13, 3–19. doi: 10.1177/0261927X94131001
Prewitt-Freilino, J. L., Caswell, T. A., and Laakso, E. K. (2012). The gendering of language: a comparison of gender equality in countries with gendered, natural gender, and genderless languages. Sex Roles 66, 268–281. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-0083-5
Pusch, L. F. (1984). Das Deutsche als Männersprache. Aufsätze und Glossen zur Feministischen Linguistik [German as a Male language]. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp.
Ridgeway, C. L., and Correll, S. J. (2004). Unpacking the gender system: a theoretical perspective on gender beliefs and social relations. Gend. Soc. 18, 510–531. doi: 10.1177/0891243204265269
Rothmund, J., and Christmann, U. (2002). Auf der Suche nach einem geschlechtergerechten Sprachgebrauch. Führt die Ersetzung des generischen Maskulinums zu einer Beeinträchtigung von Textqualitäten? [In search of gender-fair language. Does replacement of masculine generics impair text quality?]. Muttersprache 112, 115–135.
Sabatini, A. (1987). Il Sessismo Nella Lingua Italiana, Commissione Nazionale per la Realizzazione Della Parità tra Uomo e Donna, Roma. Available at: http://www.funzionepubblica.gov.it/media/962032/il%20sessismo%20nella%20lingua%20italiana.pdf
Sadker, M., Sadker, D., and Klein, S. (1991). The issue of gender in elementary and secondary eduation. Rev. Res. Educ. 17, 269–334. doi: 10.2307/1167334
Sarrasin, O., Gabriel, U., and Gygax, P. (2012). Sexism and attitudes toward gender-neutral language. The case of English, French, and German. Swiss J. Psychol. 71, 113–124. doi: 10.1024/1421-0185/a000078
Schein, V. E. (2001). A global look at psychological barriers to women’s progress in management. J. Soc. Issues 57, 675–688. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00235
Schweizerische Bundeskanzlei (1996). Geschlechtergerechte Sprache. Leitfaden zum Geschlechtergerechten Formulieren im Deutschen. [Gender-Fair Language. Guideline for Gender-Fair Formulation in German]. Available at: http://www.bk.admin.ch/dokumentation/sprachen/04915/05313/index.html (revised in 2009).
Sczesny, S., Moser, F., and Wood, W. (2015). Beyond sexist beliefs: how do people decide to use gender-inclusive language? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 41, 943–954. doi: 10.1177/0146167215585727
Silveira, J. (1980). Generic masculine words and thinking. Womens Stud. Int. Q. 3, 165–178. doi: 10.1016/s0148-0685(80)92113-2
Stahlberg, D., Braun, F., Irmen, L., and Sczesny, S. (2007). “Representation of the sexes in language,” in Social Communication. A Volume in the Series Frontiers of Social Psychology, ed. K. Fiedler (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 163–187.
Stahlberg, D., and Sczesny, S. (2001). Effekte des generischen Maskulinums und alternativer Sprachformen auf den gedanklichen Einbezug von Frauen [The impact of masculine generics on the cognitive inclusion of women]. Psychol. Rundsch. 52, 131–140. doi: 10.1026//0033-3042.52.3.131
Stahlberg, D., Sczesny, S., and Braun, F. (2001). Name your favorite musician: effects of masculine generics and of their alternatives in German. J. Lang. Soc. Psychol. 20, 464–469. doi: 10.1177/0261927x01020004004
Stout, J. G., and Dasgupta, N. (2011). When he doesn’t mean you: gender-exclusive language as ostracism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 3, 757–769. doi: 10.1177/0146167211406434
Swim, J. K., Aikin, K. J., Hall, W. S., and Hunter, B. A. (1995). Sexism and racism: old-fashioned and modern prejudices. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 68, 199–214. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.2.199
Swim, J. K., Mallet, R., and Stangor, C. (2004). Understanding subtle sexism: detection and use of sexist language. Sex Roles 51, 117–128. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000037757.731.06
Swiss Federal Institute of Technology Zurich (2011). Die Zwölf Sprachregeln [Twelve Language Rules]. Available at: http://www.equal.ethz.ch/rules
Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., and Gentile, B. (2012). Male and female pronoun use in US books reflects women’s status, 1900–2008. Sex Roles 67, 488–493. doi: 10.1007/s11199-012-0194-7
UNESCO (1999). Guidelines for Gender-Neutral Language. Available at: http://unesdoc.unesco.org/images/0011/001149/114950mo.pdf
UNESCO (2011). Priority Gender Equality Guidelines. Available at: http://www.unesco.org/new/fileadmin/MULTIMEDIA/HQ/BSP/GENDER/GE%20Guidelines%20December%202_FINAL.pdf
Valdrová, J., Knotková-Capková, B., and Pacliková, P. (2010). Kultura Genderove Vyvazeneho Vyjadrovani. Available at: http://data.idnes.cz/soubory/studium/A100125_BAR_GENDER_PRIRUCKA.PDF
Vervecken, D., Gygax, P. M., Gabriel, U., Guillod, M., and Hannover, B. (2015). Warm-hearted businessmen, competitive housewives? Effects of gender-fair language on adolescents’ perceptions of occupations. Front. Psychol. 6:1437. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01437
Vervecken, D., and Hannover, B. (2012). Ambassadors of gender equality? How use of pair forms versus masculines as generics impacts perception of the speaker. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 42, 754–762. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1893
Vervecken, D., and Hannover, B. (2015). Yes I can! Effects of gender fair job descriptions on children’s perceptions of job status, job difficulty, and vocational self-efficacy. Soc. Psychol. 46, 76–92. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000229
Vervecken, D., Hannover, B., and Wolter, I. (2013). Changing (s)expectations: how gender-fair job descriptions impact children’s perceptions and interest regarding traditionally male occupations. J. Vocat. Behav. 82, 208–220. doi: 10.1016/j.jvb.2013.01.008
Wasserman, B. D., and Weseley, A. J. (2009). Qué? Quoi? Do languages with grammatical gender promote sexist attitudes? Sex Roles 61, 634–643. doi: 10.1007/s11199-009-9696-3
Wood, W., and Rünger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 11.1–11.26. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033417
Keywords: gender stereotypes, gender-fair language, social discrimination, gender equality, social change
Citation: Sczesny S, Formanowicz M and Moser F (2016) Can Gender-Fair Language Reduce Gender Stereotyping and Discrimination? Front. Psychol. 7:25. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00025
Received: 31 May 2015; Accepted: 07 January 2016;
Published: 02 February 2016.
Edited by:
Manuel Carreiras, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, SpainReviewed by:
Simona Mancini, Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language, SpainJuan I. Aragones, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Spain
Copyright © 2016 Sczesny, Formanowicz and Moser. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sabine Sczesny, c2FiaW5lLnNjemVzbnlAcHN5LnVuaWJlLmNo