Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 07 September 2015
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Do both psychopathology and creativity result from a labile wake-sleep-dream cycle? View all 11 articles

Dissociative states in dreams and brain chaos: implications for creative awareness

  • 1Center for Neuropsychiatric Research of Traumatic Stress, Department of Psychiatry and UHSL, First Faculty of Medicine, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
  • 2Central European Institute of Technology, Faculty of Medicine, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
  • 3Department of Public Health Sciences, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA
  • 4Sofia University, Palo Alto, CA, USA

This article reviews recent findings indicating some common brain processes during dissociative states and dreaming with the aim to outline a perspective that neural chaotic states during dreaming can be closely related to dissociative states that may manifest in dreams scenery. These data are in agreement with various clinical findings that dissociated states can be projected into the “dream scenery” in REM sleep periods and dreams may represent their specific interactions that may uncover unusual psychological potential of creativity in psychotherapy, art, and scientific discoveries.

Introduction

A particular unexplained question is whether it may be possible for an individual to have a “transcendental experience” in which consciousness transcends its own symbolic self-understanding and abstract thinking and how these experiences may be related to particular aspects of consciousness and their complex organization in the brain. Recent findings indicate that conscious states can be described as representations of brain states and related to dynamics of large-scale neuronal networks (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Singer, 2001; Varela et al., 2001; Rees et al., 2002; Fingelkurts et al., 2010, 2013; Bob, 2011; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2012).

According to some recent theoretical viewpoints subjective experience can be represented in the brain by a “cerebral mental field” which refers to the mind as a system property related to the synchronized activities of large numbers of neurons (Libet, 2006). These specific complex processes linking the brain and its cognitive “mental field” may help to describe relationships between subjective experience and brain activation patterns on various functional levels of organization (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Melancon and Joanette, 2000; Varela et al., 2001; Korn and Faure, 2003). In addition, these findings show that not only brain generates consciousness, but also consciousness affects the brain. This complex organization of subjective mental experiences plays a role in spatio-temporal activities of the brain, which via various feedback loops are associated with the spatial-temporal organization of the mind processes (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Fingelkurts et al., 2010, 2013; Bob, 2011; Fingelkurts and Fingelkurts, 2012; Bass, 2014; Tang et al., 2015).

In this context, recent findings indicate that distributed patterns of neuronal activities are specifically linked within neural assemblies that tend to create coherent systems, which enable mental representations related to perceptual and cognitive functions, memory processes, and conscious awareness (Freeman, 1991; Singer, 2001; Rees et al., 2002). In addition, according to the recent evidence these neural mechanisms that enable conscious integration are significantly affected by various stressful and traumatic experiences that may determine dissociation of conscious awareness and memory (Li and Spiegel, 1992; Putnam, 1997; Bob, 2003; Spiegel, 2012).

Dissociative States in Dreams

Dissociative states usually emerge as a disconnection and switch between different mental states due to a disconnection between memories related to traumatic or stressful experiences that disturb conscious awareness and experience of the self (Li and Spiegel, 1992; Putnam, 1997; Bob, 2003; Spiegel, 2012). In this context, dissociation can be defined as a partial or total disconnection between memories of the past, awareness of identity and of immediate sensations, and control of bodily movements often resulting from traumatic experiences, intolerable problems, or disturbed relationships (Li and Spiegel, 1992; Bob, 2003; Colman, 2003). Due to this disconnection of memory and identity dissociation is related to disability to integrate some mental contents into conscious awareness (Bernstein and Putnam, 1986). In agreement with new DSM-V criteria, dissociation may be defined: “as a disruption of and/or discontinuity in the normal, subjective integration of one or more aspects of psychological functioning, including—but not limited to—memory, identity, consciousness, perception, and motor control” (Spiegel, 2012; American Psychiatric Association, 2014).

Dissociative mental states also manifest during dreams and may be typically related to discontinuities and shifts in dream scenes, which according to some psychological studies of dreams may specifically manifest in pathological processes related to nightmares and recurrent dreams linked to traumatic experiences (Ferenczi, 1934; Levitan, 1980; Hartmann, 1998; Bob, 2004; Schonhammer, 2005). Intriguing relationships between dreams and the dissociative states were also reported in cases of the so-called “alter personalities” in multiple personality disorder (MPD), which may manifest on parallel levels in dreams and during personality alterations in hypnosis (Jeans, 1976; Marmer, 1980a,b; Salley, 1988; Putnam, 1989; Brenner, 1996, 1999, 2001; Bob, 2004). For example, Barrett (1994, 1995, 1996) reported some cases of MPD in which alter personalities manifested in dream scenes. These case studies suggest that dream “personalities” may manifest as hallucinated projections of various aspects of the fragmented self that may be understood as prototypes of these alter personalities (Bowers and Brecher, 1955; Watkins and Watkins, 1979–1980;; Merskey, 1992; Rickeport, 1992; Watkins, 1993; Lynn et al., 1994; Barrett, 1995; Bob, 2004). In this context, Bowers and Brecher (1955) reported that fragmented aspects of the self, very similar to altered personalities in MPD, may manifest during hypnosis also in individuals without multiple personality structures and also they may be observed in dreams and associations using Rorschach test. Taken together these reported studies suggest that dissociative mental states may be projected as specific types of hallucinatory experiences during dreams and may be identified as parts of dream scenes (Gabel, 1989; Hartmann, 1998; Bob, 2004). An important implication of these data is that dreaming processes may represent a conscious reflection of dissociative states represented during memory processing in REM sleep (Gabel, 1989; Rotenberg, 1992; Bob, 2004; Eisser, 2005; Stickgold and Walker, 2005).

Dreams and Brain Chaos

Recent findings indicate that some modalities of awareness and attention used for self-monitoring and for the internal explorations of one’s consciousness such as dreams, meditation and various forms of creative awareness may be at certain levels of brain functions understood using theories of non-linear dynamics, chaos and self-organization (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Elbert et al., 1994; Melancon and Joanette, 2000; Bob, 2003, 2011; Korn and Faure, 2003; Fingelkurts et al., 2013).

In the scientific history, the chaotic processes were for the first time documented in the last years of 19th century in the work by Poincaré (1908/1998). In his “Science and method” Poincaré (1908/1998, p. 68) wrote: “A very small, unnoticeable cause can determine a visible very large effect; in this case we claim that this effect is a product of random …”.

In the dynamic state of chaos very small changes in a system can result to very large differences in the system’s behavior (the so-called butterfly effect, which metaphorically means that the flapping of a butterfly’s wings in one part of the world later may cause a tornado in a distant part of the world). Chaos represents a dynamic pattern of activity that occurs when a system involves a large number of interlinked and simultaneously active states, which can lead to self-organization (Freeman, 1991, 2000; Elbert et al., 1994; Korn and Faure, 2003; Bob, 2015).

Seminal contributions to this field of research reported Freeman (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Elbert et al., 1994; Bob, 2011), who was particularly interested to explore how brain generates cognitive processing, intentionality, and meaning. In his research Freeman found that brain activities may manifest chaotic behavior. Freeman also proposed that chaos could underlie basic forms of collective neural activity in perceptual processing including ability to access memorized sensory patterns and learning of novel sensory information. In cognitive processes chaos may explain brain ability to respond flexibly to the outside world and to generate novel activity patterns that are subjectively experienced as “novel” ideas that enable complex dynamic behavior of the brain and intentional behavior (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000).

Recent findings indicate that deterministic mechanisms of brain activity as well as chaotic neural patterns are important for brain functioning (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Elbert et al., 1994; Birbaumer et al., 1995; Kantz and Schreiber, 1997; Bob, 2011). In this context, mental states and brain activities related to intentional goals are predictable and deterministic. On the other hand brain processes related to great sensitivity to very small changes, mainly in initial stages of a neural processes, seem to be characterized by chaotic neural activities and mental states, for example during free floating divergent thinking or search related mental events (Freeman, 1991, 1999, 2000; Elbert et al., 1994; Globus and Arpaia, 1994; Birbaumer et al., 1995; Faure and Korn, 2001; Meyer-Lindenberg et al., 2002; Bob, 2011; Yoruk and Runco, 2014). These chaotic brain activities with increased sensitivity to “initial conditions” underlying novel trends in brain processes are related to a very large number of interacting and interlinked neural states that are sensitive and unstable because of a competition of many neural patterns (Freeman, 2000; Korn and Faure, 2003). According to these findings chaos may enable flexible brain responses to some external stimuli associated with novel neural activities, behavior and cognitive processing, for example experiences of original ideas and creativity (Skarda and Freeman, 1987; Freeman, 1991, 2000, 2001; Elbert et al., 1994; Melancon and Joanette, 2000; Korn and Faure, 2003; Bob, 2011). Chaotic states in the brain may manifest in various cognitive processes as for example in dreams (Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Kahn et al., 2000, 2002; Bob, 2011; Kahn, 2013), dissociative states (Pediaditakis, 1992; Putnam, 1997; Sel, 1997; Bob, 2003) and may characterize specific processes in development of mental disorders, as for example in depression or schizophrenia (Gottschalk et al., 1995; Huber et al., 1999; Paulus and Braff, 2003; Bob, 2011).

Chaotic brain states are usually related to activities in various independent regions that process information in parallel distributed mode (Tirsch et al., 2004). This parallel distributed processing may manifest in transient states and fluctuations of increased or decreased complexity that in cases of low associated strength among the parallel distributed information subsystems may lead to dissociated mental states (Mc Clelland et al., 1986; Li and Spiegel, 1992; Butler et al., 1996; Bob, 2003). Due to these decreased levels of association between some information processes, dissociation manifests as disturbed continuity between interacting mental states (Li and Spiegel, 1992; Bob, 2003).

Dream scenes mainly occur in the REM sleep and include mental images, thoughts, sounds, and other sensory experiences in various sequences that may be episodically disturbed by discontinuous shifts in dream narratives (Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Faw, 1997; Kahn et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Stickgold et al., 2001; Hobson and Pace-Schott, 2002; Kahn, 2013). According to some findings neural correlates of these discontinuous shifts in a dream scenery are rapid shifts in neural patterns related to self-organized neural activities, mainly associated with activities of cholinergic pontogeniculoocipital (PGO) systems (Hobson and McCarley, 1977; Quattrochi et al., 1989; Hobson, 1990; Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Stickgold et al., 1994, 2001; Kahn et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Kahn, 2013). In addition, there is also evidence that the PGO activity is correlated with increased firings in the visual cortex and lateral geniculate bodies participating in formation of images (Callaway et al., 1987; Singer, 1989; Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Porte and Hobson, 1996; Stickgold et al., 2001; Kahn, 2013). During these self-organizing processes brain responses may become very sensitive with respect to very small stimuli which may lead to chaotic bifurcations that cause rapid shifts in patterns of neural activities related to increased cholinergic and decreased aminergic input onto forebrain structures (Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Stickgold et al., 1994; Kahn et al., 2000, 2002; Kahn, 2013). According to recent findings these chaotic processes may be characterized by significant transitions of dream objects and sceneries due to a competition and interference of various dream images, which lead to dream discontinuities and multiple transitions of neural firing patterns (Tender and Kramer, 1971; Kaczmarek and Babloyantz, 1977; Elazar and Hobson, 1985; Rotenberg, 1992; Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Kahn et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Kahn, 2013).

Dreams, Conscious Awareness, and Novel Ideas

Reported findings suggest that dreams may reflect dissociative mental states that especially manifest as discontinuous jumps in the dream scenery (Salley, 1988; Gabel, 1989; Bob, 2004), which in a similar way as dissociative states during waking may be related to chaos and self-organization in the brain (Pediaditakis, 1992; Putnam, 1997; Sel, 1997; Bob, 2003, 2011). These findings are in accordance with experiences and case studies in psychotherapy, which indicate that dreams are not random processes and may be meaningful for self-discovery and personal growth (Jeans, 1976; Marmer, 1980a,b; Salley, 1988; Barrett, 1994, 1995, 1996; Brenner, 1996, 1999, 2001; Bob, 2004).

A specific feature of chaotic neural states is that they can generate novel patterns of neural activities and novel synaptic connections, which may link less associated or dissociative mental states into a coherent whole (Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Kahn et al., 2000). For example, several findings suggest that high complexity and self-organization manifest also during creative divergent thinking (Combs, 1996; Mölle et al., 1996). Specifically in the REM sleep these integrative processes support “binding” functions of dreams, which connect dream images coherently together that enables novel creative unity of conscious experience (Revonsuo and Tarkko, 2002). These findings suggest that dreams may reflect new integrations of dissociated mental states related to traumatic and stressful experiences and may also increase creative potentialities in various cases of artistic inventions, scientific discoveries and deep insights related to meta-cognitive, transcendental or religious experiences reflecting certain forms of “preconscious” or “unconscious” intelligence (Haule, 1984; Barrett, 1993; Strunz, 1993; Baylor, 2001; Schaverien, 2005; Louchakova, 2006; Edwards et al., 2013; Dresler et al., 2015).

Conclusion

Chaos and self-organization manifest in brain and cognitive functions and may be specifically related to dissociative states characterized by rapid shifts between disconnected mental states that in the case of dreams are likely specifically related to PGO bursts of neural activity (Kahn and Hobson, 1993; Putnam, 1997; Bob, 2003, 2011; Korn and Faure, 2003; Kahn, 2013).

Although the current literature and evidence about these links between dreams and dissociated states are limited, they provide useful explanatory scheme for future research that could explain more detailed connections of self-organizing theory of dreams and dissociative states. For example, Tart (2009) suggests that hypnotic techniques provide a useful framework for experimental studies of dreams and other clinical data show that also dreams in patients who experienced serious traumatic events could open new perspectives in this research (Bob, 2004; Rotenberg, 2014). These studies may also provide new theoretical framework for understanding of connections between dreams and brain functions related to unusual creative potential of unconscious mental processing as well as important findings for psychotherapy that using “dream analysis” may open novel insights and creativity in individual life and help to resolve traumatic experiences linked to various mental disorders.

Taken together these data suggest a perspective for further research that dissociated mental states may be projected into a dream scenery during REM sleep and dreams may represent specific interactions of dissociated contents, which may create the content of the dream (Salley, 1988; Gabel, 1989; Bob, 2004). These data also suggest that neural chaotic states during dreaming may represent underlying neural processes that enable new integration of dissociated contents of memory which may generate novel ideas, insights and other creative conscious states.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The study was supported by projects PRVOUK provided by Charles University, project GACR P407/12/1957 and the project “CEITEC – Central European Institute of Technology” (CZ.1.05/1.1.00/02.0068) from European Regional Development Fund.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2014). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (1993). The “Committee of sleep”: a study of dream incubation of problem solving. Dreaming 3, 115–122. doi: 10.1037/h0094375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (1994). Dreams in dissociative disorders. Dreaming 4, 165–175. doi: 10.1037/h0094410

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (1995). The dream character as prototype for the multiple personality alter. Dissociation 8, 66–68.

Google Scholar

Barrett, D. (1996). “Dreams in multiple personality disorder,” in Trauma and Dreams, ed. D. Barrett (Cambridge: Harvard University Press), 68–81.

Google Scholar

Bass, L. (2014). Quantum theory of iconic memory. Act. Nerv. Super. 56, 57–61.

Google Scholar

Baylor, G. W. (2001). What do we really know about Mendeleev’s dream of the periodic table? A note on dreams of scientific problem solving. Dreaming 11, 89–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1009484504919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernstein, E. M., and Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a dissociation scale. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 174, 727–735. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198612000-00004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Birbaumer, N., Flor, H., Lutzenberger, W., and Elbert, T. (1995). Chaos and order in the human brain. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl. 44, 450–459.

Google Scholar

Bob, P. (2003). Dissociation and neuroscience: history and new perspectives. Int. J. Neurosci. 113, 903–914. doi: 10.1080/00207450390220376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bob, P. (2004). Dissociative processes, multiple personality and dream functions. Am. J. Psychother. 58, 139–149.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bob, P. (2011). Brain, Mind and Consciousness: Advances in Neuroscience Research. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Bob, P. (2015). The Brain and Conscious Unity: Freud’s Omega. New York, NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Bowers, M. K., and Brecher, S. (1955). The emergence of multiple personalities in the course of hypnotic investigation. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Hypn. 3, 188–199. doi: 10.1080/00207145508410151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brenner, I. (1996). The characterological basis of multiple personality. Am. J. Psychother. 52, 154–166.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brenner, I. (1999). Deconstructing DID. Am. J. Psychother. 53, 344–360.

Google Scholar

Brenner, I. (2001). Dissociation of Trauma: Theory, Phenomenology and Technique. Boston: International Universities Press.

Google Scholar

Butler, L. D., Duran, R. E. F., Jasiukaitis, P., Koopman, C. H., and Spiegel, D. (1996). Hypnotizability and traumatic experience: a diathesis-stress model of dissociative symptomatology. Am. J. Psychiatry (Festschrift Suppl.t) 153, 42–62.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Callaway, C. W., Lydic, R., Baghdoyan, H. A., and Hobson, J. A. (1987). Pontogeniculooccipital waves: spontaneous visual system activity during rapid eye movement sleep. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 7, 105–109. doi: 10.1007/BF00711551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colman, A. M. (2003). A Dictionary of Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Combs, A. (1996). The Radiance of Being: Complexity, Chaos, and the Evolution of Consciousness. Saint Paul, MN: Paragon House.

Google Scholar

Dresler, M., Wehrle, R., Spoormaker, V. I., Steiger, A., Holsboer, F., Czisch, M., et al. (2015). Neural correlates of insight in dreaming and psychosis. Sleep Med. Rev. 20, 92–99. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, C. L., Ruby, P. M., Malinowski, J. E., Bennett, P. D., and Blagrove, M. T. (2013). Dreaming and insight. Front. Psychol. 4:979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisser, A. S. (2005). Physiology and psychology of dreams. Semin. Neurol. 25, 97–105. doi: 10.1055/s-2005-867078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elazar, Z., and Hobson, J. A. (1985). Neuronal excitability control in health and disease: a neurophysiological comparison of REM sleep and epilepsy. Prog. Neurobiol. 25, 141–188. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(85)90002-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elbert, T., Ray, W. J., Kowalik, Z. J., Skinner, J. E., Graf, K. E., and Birbaumer, N. (1994). Chaos and physiology: deterministic chaos in excitable cell assemblies. Physiol. Rev. 74, 1–47.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Faure, P., and Korn, H. (2001). Is there chaos in the brain? I. Concepts of nonlinear dynamics and methods of investigation. C. R. Acad. Sci. III 324, 773–793. doi: 10.1016/S0764-4469(01)01377-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faw, B. (1997). Outlining a brain model of mental imaging abilities. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 283–288. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00026-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferenczi, S. (1934). Gedanken über das trauma. Int. Z. Psychoanal. 20, 5–12.

Google Scholar

Fingelkurts, A. A., and Fingelkurts, A. A. (2012). Mind as a nested operational architectonics of the brain: comment on “Neuroontology, neurobiological naturalism, and consciousness: a challenge to scientific reduction and a solution” by Todd E. Feinberg. Phys. Life Rev. 9, 49–50. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2011.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., and Neves, C. F. (2010). Natural world physical, brain operational, and mind phenomenal space-time. Phys. Life Rev. 7, 195–249. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fingelkurts, A. A., Fingelkurts, A. A., and Neves, C. F. (2013). Consciousness as a phenomenon in the operational architectonics of brain organization: criticality and self-organization considerations. Chaos Solitons Fractals 55, 13–31. doi: 10.1016/j.chaos.2013.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (1991). The physiology of perception. Sci. Am. 264, 34–41. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0291-78

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (1999). Consciousness, intentionality, and causality. J. Conscious. Stud. 6, 143–172.

Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (2000). Mesoscopics neurodynamics: from neuron to brain. J. Physiol. Paris 94, 303–322. doi: 10.1016/S0928-4257(00)01090-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeman, W. J. (2001). Biocomplexity: adaptive behavior in complex stochastic dynamical systems. BioSystems 59, 109–123. doi: 10.1016/S0303-2647(00)00146-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gabel, S. (1989). Dreams as a possible reflection of dissociated self-monitoring system. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 177, 560–568. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198909000-00008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Globus, G. C., and Arpaia, J. P. (1994). Psychiatry and the new dynamics. Biol. Psychiatry 32, 352–364. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)90039-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gottschalk, A. M., Bauer, M. S., and Whybrow, P. C. (1995). Evidence of chaotic mood variation in bipolar disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 52, 947–959. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950230061009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartmann, E. (1998). Nightmare after trauma as paradigm for all dreams: a new approach to the nature and functions of dreaming. Psychiatry 61, 223–238.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Haule, J. R. (1984). From somnambulism to the archetypes: the French Roots of Jung’s Split with Freud. Psychoanal. Rev. 71, 636–689.

Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A. (1990). Sleep and dreaming. Neuroscience 10, 371–382.

Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A., and McCarley, R. W. (1977). The brain as a dream-state generator: an activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process. Am. J. Psychol. 134, 1335–1368. doi: 10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobson, J. A., and Pace-Schott, E. F. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of sleep: neural systems, consciousness and learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 679–693. doi: 10.1038/nrn915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huber, M. T., Braun, H. A., and Krieg, J. C. (1999). Consequences of deterministic and random dynamics for the course of affective disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 256–262. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00311-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeans, R. F. (1976). The three faces of evelyn: a case report. I. An independently validated case of multiple personalities. J. Abnorm Psychol. 85, 249–255. doi: 10.1037/h0078100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaczmarek, L. K., and Babloyantz, A. (1977). Spatiotemporal patterns in epileptic seizures. Biol. Cybern. 26, 199–208. doi: 10.1007/BF00366591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, D. (2013). Brain basis of self: self-organization and lessons from dreaming. Front. Psychol. 4:408. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, D., Combs, A., and Krippner, S. (2002). Dreaming as a function of chaos-like stochastic processes in the self-organizing brain. Nonlinear Dyn. Psychol. Life Sci. 6, 311–322. doi: 10.1023/A:1019758527338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, D., and Hobson, J. A. (1993). Self-organization theory of dreaming. Dreaming 3, 151–178. doi: 10.1037/h0094378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, D., Krippner, S., and Combs, A. (2000). Dreaming and the self-organizing brain. J. Conscious. Stud. 7, 4–11.

Google Scholar

Kahn, D., Pace-Schott, E. F., and Hobson, J. A. (1997). Consciousness in waking and dreaming: the roles of neuronal oscillation and neuromodulation in determining similarities and differences. Neuroscience 78, 13–38.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kantz, H., and Schreiber, T. (1997). Nonlinear Time Series Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Korn, H., and Faure, P. (2003). Is there chaos in the brain? II. Experimental evidence and related models. C. R. Biol. 326, 787–840. doi: 10.1016/j.crvi.2003.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levitan, H. (1980). “The dream in traumatic states,” in The Dream in Clinical Practice, ed. J. M. Natterson (New York, NY: Jason Aronson).

Google Scholar

Li, D., and Spiegel, D. (1992). A neural network model of dissociative disorders. Psychiatr. Ann. 22, 144–147. doi: 10.3928/0048-5713-19920301-11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libet, B. (2006). Reflections on the interaction of the mind and brain. Prog. Neurobiol. 78, 322–326. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2006.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Louchakova, O. (2006). “Ontopoiesis and union in the prayer of the heart: contributions to psychotherapy and learning,” in Logos of Phenomenology and Phenomenology of the Logos. Book four. The Logos of Scientific Interrogation. Participating in Nature-Life-Sharing in Life. Analecta Husserliana, ed. A. T. Tymieniencka (New York, NY: Springer), 289–311.

Google Scholar

Lynn, S. J., Maré, C., Kvaal, S., Segal, D., and Sivec, H. (1994). The hidden observer, hypnotic dreams, and age regression: clinical implications. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 37, 130–142. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1994.10403125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marmer, S. S. (1980a). “The dream in dissociative states,” in The Dream in Clinical Practic, ed. J. M. Natterson (New York, NY: Jason Aronson).

Google Scholar

Marmer, S. S. (1980b). Psychoanalysis of multiple personality. Int. J. Psycho-Anal. 61, 439–459.

Google Scholar

Mc Clelland, J. L., Rumelhart, D. E., and PDP research group. (1986). Parallel Distributed Processing I, II. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Melancon, G., and Joanette, Y. (2000). Chaos, brain and cognition: toward a nonlinear order? Brain Cogn. 42, 33–36. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1999.1154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merskey, H. (1992). The manufacture of personalities. The production of multiple personality disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry 160, 327–340.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Zeman, U., Hajak, G., Cohen, L., and Berman, K. F. (2002). Transitions between dynamical states of differing stability in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 10948–10953. doi: 10.1073/pnas.162114799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mölle, M., Marshall, L., Lutzenberger, W., Pietrowsky, R., Fehm, H. L., and Born, J. (1996). Enhanced dynamic complexity in the human EEG during creative thinking. Neurosci. Lett. 208, 61–64. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(96)12539-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paulus, M. P., and Braff, D. L. (2003). Chaos and schizophrenia: does the method fit the madness? Biol. Psychiatry 53, 3–11. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01701-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pediaditakis, N. (1992). Deterministic non-linear chaos in brain function and borderline psychopathological phenomena. Med. Hypotheses 39, 67–72. doi: 10.1016/0306-9877(92)90143-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poincaré, H. (1908/1998). Science and Method. London: Thomas Nelson and Sons.

Google Scholar

Porte, H., and Hobson, J. A. (1996). Physical motion in dreams: one measure of three theories. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 105, 329–335. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.3.329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putnam, F. (1997). Dissociation in Children and Adolescents. A Developmental Perspective. London: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Putnam, F. W. (1989). Diagnosis and Treatment of Multiple Personality Disorder. London: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Quattrochi, J. J., Mamelak, A. N., Madison, R. D., Macklis, J. D., and Hobson, J. A. (1989). Mapping neuronal inputs to REM sleep induction sites with carbachol-fluorescent microspheres. Science 245, 984–986. doi: 10.1126/science.2475910

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rees, G., Kreiman, G., and Koch, C. (2002). Neural correlates of consciousness in humans. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 261–270. doi: 10.1038/nrn783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Revonsuo, A., and Tarkko, K. (2002). Binding in dreams - the bizarreness of dream images and the unity of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 9, 3–24.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rickeport, M. M. (1992). The interface between multiple personality, spirit mediumship and hypnosis. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 34, 168–177. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1992.10402841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotenberg, V. S. (1992). Sleep and memory II: investigations on humans. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 16, 503–505. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80191-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotenberg, V. S. (2014). Probability forecast in different types of behavior and in REM sleep in frame of search activity concept. Act. Nerv. Super. 56, 95–104.

Google Scholar

Salley, R. D. (1988). Subpersonalities with dreaming functions in a patient with multiple personalities. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 176, 112–115. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198802000-00007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaverien, J. (2005). Art, dreams and active imagination: a post-Jungian approach to transference and the image. J. Anal. Psychol. 50, 127–153. doi: 10.1111/j.0021-8774.2005.00519.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schonhammer, R. (2005). ‘Typical dreams’- reflections of arousal. J. Conscious. Stud. 12, 18–37.

Google Scholar

Sel, R. (1997). Dissociation as complex adaptation. Med. Hypotheses 48, 205–208. doi: 10.1016/S0306-9877(97)90307-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, W. (1989). “The role of acetylcholine in use-dependent plasticity of the visual cortex,” in Brain Cholinergic Systems, eds M. Steriade and D. Biesold (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Singer, W. (2001). Consciousness and the binding problem. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 929, 123–146. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05712.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skarda, C. H. A., and Freeman, W. J. (1987). How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 161–195. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00047336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiegel, D. (2012). Divided consciousness: dissociation in DSM-5. Depress. Anxiety 29, 667–670. doi: 10.1002/da.21984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R., Hobson, J. A., Fosse, R., and Fosse, M. (2001). Sleep, learning, and dreams: off-line memory reprocessing. Science 294, 1052–1057. doi: 10.1126/science.1063530

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R., Rittenhouse, C., and Hobson, J. A. (1994). Dream splicing: a new technique for assessing thematic coherence in subjective reports of mental activity. Conscious. Cogn. 3, 114–128. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1994.1008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stickgold, R., and Walker, M. P. (2005). Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: what is the role of sleep. Trends Neurosci. 28, 408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strunz, F. (1993). Preconscious mental activity and scientific problem-solving: a critique of the Kekulé dream controversy. Dreaming 3, 281–294. doi: 10.1037/h0094386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Y. Y., Hölzel, B. K., and Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of mindfulness meditation. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 213–225. doi: 10.1038/nrn3916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tart, C. (2009). Hypnotic suggestion as a technique for the control of dreaming. Act. Nerv. Super. 51, 77–82.

Google Scholar

Tender, J., and Kramer, M. (1971). Dream recall. Am. J. Psychiatry 128, 3–10.

Tirsch, W. S., Stude, P., Scherb, H., and Keidel, M. (2004). Temporal order of nonlinear dynamics in human brain. Brain Res. Rev. 45, 79–95. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2004.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., and Martinerie, J. (2001). The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. doi: 10.1038/35067550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, H. H. (1993). Ego-state therapy: an overview. Am. J. Clin. Hypn. 35, 232–240. doi: 10.1080/00029157.1993.10403014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watkins, J. G., and Watkins, H. H. (1979–1980). Ego states and hidden observers. J. Altered States Conscious. 5, 3–18.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yoruk, S., and Runco, M. A. (2014). The neuroscience of divergent thinking. Act. Nerv. Super. 56, 1–16.

Google Scholar

Keywords: dreams, dissociation, self-organization, chaos, creativity

Citation: Bob P and Louchakova O (2015) Dissociative states in dreams and brain chaos: implications for creative awareness. Front. Psychol. 6:1353. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01353

Received: 01 May 2015; Accepted: 24 August 2015;
Published: 07 September 2015.

Edited by:

Sue Llewellyn, University of Manchester, UK

Reviewed by:

Andrew A. Fingelkurts, BM-Science – Brain and Mind Technologies Research Centre, Finland
Richard James Brown, University of Manchester, UK

Copyright © 2015 Bob and Louchakova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Petr Bob, Center for Neuropsychiatric Research of Traumatic Stress, Department of Psychiatry and UHSL, First Faculty of Medicine, Charles University, Ke Karlovu 11, 128 00 Prague, Czech Republic, petrbob@netscape.net

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.