A High-Level Aftereffect, and Two Ways to Explain it
Imagine an experiment in which you show someone pictures of a computer-generated face displaying random expressions, ranging from happy, through neutral, to sad. You tell the participant that she must classify each picture as being either “happy” or “sad,” and you note the point on the expression continuum at which she switches from mostly-“happy” classifications to mostly-“sad.” Then, you ask her to repeat the task, but between each picture you have her feel, with her hands but out of sight, the contours of a smiling face mask. You find that her category boundary has shifted: she now classifies more of the pictures as “sad.” This result (reported by Matsumiya, 2013) is an example of an aftereffect, in which adaptation to one input (the mask) has altered responses to subsequent inputs (the images). It is “high-level” in the sense that the adapting and test stimuli have little overlap in their initial sensory encoding (they are presented in separate modalities). There are at least two ways to interpret this finding.
A Decisional Bias?
The presence of the smiling mask may have altered the participant's strategies or criteria for labeling the expression images. For instance, she may now be consciously or unconsciously using the rule: “if in doubt, say the expression was different from that of the mask.” This interpretation places the effect within the extensive catalog of contrast effects in the cognitive and social psychological literature. For example, people judge moderately qualified job applicants as being less qualified after reading the résumé of a highly qualified competitor (Hakel et al., 1970; Wexley et al., 1972).
A Perceptual Bias?
Alternatively, feeling the smiling mask may have changed how the test pictures look to the participant. Although she uses the same strategies and criteria to arrive at her decision, the boundary between “happy” and “sad” expressions falls at a different point because her encoding of the stimuli has changed. According to this interpretation, Matsumiya's (2013) effect is an example of a visual aftereffect, akin to the temporary illusions induced by prolonged exposure to a particular color (Webster, 1996), motion direction (Addams, 1834; Anstis et al., 1998), orientation (Gibson and Radner, 1937), or spatial frequency (Blakemore and Sutton, 1969). Such aftereffects can be visually striking, and have been linked to changes in the responsiveness of neurons selective for the properties of the inducing stimulus (neural adaptation—see, e.g., Kohn, 2007; Webster, 2012).
Matsumiya's (2013) effect joins a growing body of aftereffects between increasingly abstractly-related adapting and test stimuli. For example, people are more likely to report an androgynous face as being male after viewing a female face (or vice versa; Webster et al., 2004), or even after viewing female bodies (Ghuman et al., 2010) or stereotypically female objects (Javadi and Wee, 2012). Analogous effects occur between facial images depicting different identities (Leopold et al., 2001), races, expressions (Webster et al., 2004), ages (Schweinberger et al., 2010), and geometric distortions (Webster and Maclin, 1999). Further examples of high-level aftereffects abound outside of face perception: after receiving downwards-moving tactile stimulation to their hands, people more often judge an oscillating visual grating to be drifting upwards (Konkle et al., 2009); after looking at a looming visual pattern, people more often judge a steady auditory tone to be receding in depth (Kitagawa and Ichihara, 2002); and after seeing a series of urban landscapes, people more often judge semi-rural landscapes to be “natural” (Greene and Oliva, 2010).
In each of these reports, the aftereffect is interpreted as a perceptual bias due to neural adaptation. If this interpretation is correct, high-level aftereffects may provide exciting tools to investigate how complex stimulus properties are encoded, just as “low-level” aftereffects have for simpler stimulus properties (Barlow and Hill, 1963; Blakemore and Campbell, 1969; Mollon, 1974; Thompson and Burr, 2009; Thompson and Burr, although see also Hegde, 2009 for a note of caution). Already, face aftereffects have been widely used to study the encoding of faces (e.g., Leopold et al., 2001; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006; Susilo et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2011; Storrs and Arnold, 2012; McKone et al., 2014). If high-level aftereffects are decisional biases, on the other hand, they may all have a similar origin within amodal cognitive processes and tell us little about the representation of any particular stimulus property. So how can one distinguish perceptual from decisional biases?
Methods to Distinguish Perceptual from Decisional Biases
The Bias Manifests in a “Criterion Free” Task
With the exception of Webster and Maclin (1999), each of the high-level aftereffects above was demonstrated using a “method of single stimuli” (MSS). In an MSS task, a single test stimulus is shown on each trial and the observer classifies it as belonging to one of two categories. The placement of the category boundary is determined both by the participant's sensory evidence and by her criteria for applying each of the response labels to that evidence (see Green and Swets, 1966; Farell and Pelli, 1999; Kingdom and Prins, 2010). Changes in criteria can therefore produce exactly the same pattern of response shifts as changes in perception, making MSS data ambiguous (Green and Swets, 1966; Gescheider et al., 1970; Morgan et al., 2011, 2013; Yarrow et al., 2011). Why then have many papers in recent years claimed to report novel perceptual aftereffects on the basis only of MSS data?
Unfortunately, the best psychophysical methods to measure perceptual experience are unsuited to high-level aftereffects. Visual appearance can be measured without relying on semantic labels or remembered reference stimuli only if there exists an unadapted (or differently adapted) location in the visual field. Adaptation to simple properties, such as orientation, contrast, and spatial frequency, produces aftereffects localized to within a few degrees of the adaptor (Gibson, 1937; Williams et al., 1982; Ejima and Takahashi, 1985). A test stimulus can then be shown within the affected region while a reference stimulus is shown in an unaffected region. The point of subjective equality (PSE) between adapted and unadapted locations is quantified by having the observer adjust the test to match the reference, indicate whether or not the two appear the same, or decide which location contains the “stronger” signal along some dimension (see Kingdom and Prins, 2010). This last task is known as a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC, or more generally, nAFC).
Isolating perceptual bias is still not straightforward. If one shows the same tilted grating in an adapted and unadapted location and asks “which is tilted further clockwise?” (a simple 2AFC), a strategy of picking the stimulus in the adapted location when unsure could produce a shift in PSE between baseline and adaptation trials (Schneider and Komlos, 2008; Morgan, 2013, 2014; Jogan and Stocker, 2014). Such problems can be alleviated by elaborations to the nAFC task, such as varying the reference stimulus from trial-to-trial so that a perceptual bias predicts opposite PSE shifts for different reference stimuli (Morgan, 2013, 2014; Morgan et al., 2013) and presenting two reference stimuli in unadapted locations, from which the participant selects the one most similar to a test shown in the adapted location (Jogan and Stocker, 2014).
While there is no objective way to measure a subjective perceptual bias, nAFC methods with multiple reference stimuli come closest to providing a measure uncontaminated by decisional criteria. Unfortunately they are only practical when the aftereffect is localized to the adapted location. The position-dependence of most high-level aftereffects is unknown, but it seems likely that some (e.g., cross-modal aftereffects) are spatially global.
The Bias is Mediated by Properties of Early Sensory Neurons
The magnitude of a perceptual bias is often mediated by the receptive field properties of early visual neurons. Adaptation at one retinal location may not affect tests elsewhere (see above), or adaptation in one eye may not affect tests seen with the other (e.g., McCollough, 1965). The bias may even occur when the adapting stimulus is suppressed from awareness (e.g., Blake and Fox, 1974). These indicators are of limited use in the present case, though, as high-level adaptation may not be mediated by properties of early visual neurons.
The Bias is Accompanied by Objectively-Measured Sensitivity Changes
“Low-level” visual aftereffects are often accompanied by reduced sensitivity to detect the adapted properties. For example, the image contrast required to detect a grating pattern is selectively raised for patterns with a similar orientation and spatial frequency to the adaptor (Blakemore and Campbell, 1969; see also Levinson and Sekuler, 1980; Krauskopf et al., 1982). Changes may also be found in discrimination sensitivity (e.g., Regan and Beverley, 1985; Clifford et al., 2001).
Selective changes in sensitivity near an adapted value constitute reasonable evidence for changes in sensory encoding—they often accompany low-level aftereffects, are predicted by models based on neural adaptation (e.g., Blakemore and Campbell, 1969; Clifford et al., 2001; Kohn, 2007), and not easily explained in terms of decisional bias. nAFC tasks can provide objective measures of sensitivity (Green and Swets, 1966; Farell and Pelli, 1999) even when there is no unadapted visual field location. In the domain of face aftereffects, there is some evidence for improved discrimination near an adapted face (Rhodes et al., 2010; Oruc and Barton, 2011), although other researchers have found no changes in sensitivity (Rhodes et al., 2007; Ng et al., 2008).
The Bias Fails to Manifest in an “El Greco Task”
Since the opthalmologist Beritens proposed that an astigmatism was to blame for the oddly elongated figures painted by the artist El Greco, many have pointed out the fallacy in his theory: any optical distortion El Greco experienced must have applied equally to both his subjects and his own paintings (Rock, 1966; Anstis, 2002; Firestone, 2013). Likewise, if an adaptation-induced bias is a literal change in how things look, it should apply equally to the test and to any reference against which the observer judges it (Firestone and Scholl, 2013). After adaptation, an observer could be shown a test stimulus, then asked to adjust or select a reference stimulus to match it. Any bias shown in this “El Greco task” is likely of a cognitive rather than perceptual origin.
Questionable Methods to Distinguish Perceptual from Decisional Biases
The Bias has Similar Temporal Dynamics to a Perceptual One
Several authors (Leopold et al., 2005; Ghuman et al., 2010; Matsumiya, 2013) show that the magnitudes of their respective aftereffects increase logarithmically with the duration of the adapting stimulus. This is similar to the temporal dynamics of tilt (Magnussen and Johnsen, 1986) and motion (Hershenson, 1989) aftereffects, and is presented as evidence that the high-level aftereffects in question share a common mechanism with low-level aftereffects. A priori, the fact that two pairs of variables are related to one another by similar functions is poor evidence that they are subserved by similar mechanisms (a sum of money accumulating compound interest also increases logarithmically with time). Temporal dynamics may turn out to have diagnostic value, but only if effects deemed perceptual on other grounds have reliably different temporal dynamics from those deemed decisional. These data do not yet exist.
The Bias Manifests in Some Conditions but not Others
Kitagawa and Ichihara (2002) find that although viewing a looming visual pattern causes participants to judge a steady auditory tone as receding in depth, hearing a tone increasing in volume has no effect on visual judgements. The authors argue that this selectivity for particular adaptor-test pairings indicates a perceptual origin (Van der Burg et al. (2013) present a similar argument). This relies on the assumption that all adaptor-test pairings should be equally effective in inducing shifts in decisional criteria—it is not obvious why this should be the case.
Conclusion
Much of the interest in high-level aftereffects depends on claims that, like colored afterimages or motion aftereffects, they involve literal changes in how the world looks, feels or sounds. Such changes in sensory encoding may help us understand how the brain represents complex stimulus properties and integrates information across modalities. However, most high-level aftereffects have so far been demonstrated only as biases in how people classify stimuli during method-of-single-stimulus tasks, and are therefore equally consistent with changes in amodal decision-making processes.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
I am grateful to Derek Arnold, Michael Morgan, Josh Solomon and Kielan Yarrow for conversations on these subjects, and for helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.
References
Addams, R. (1834). An account of a peculiar optical phenomenon seen after having looked at a moving body. Lond. Edinb. Philos. Mag. J. Sci. 5, 373–374.
Anstis, S., Verstraten, F. A., and Mather, G. (1998). The motion aftereffect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 111–117. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01142-5
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Barlow, H. B., and Hill, R. M. (1963). Evidence for a physiological explanation of the waterfall phenomenon and figural after-effects. Nature 200, 134–1347. doi: 10.1038/2001345a0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Blake, R., and Fox, R. (1974). Adaptation to invisible gratings and the site of binocular rivalry suppression. Nature 249, 488–490. doi: 10.1038/249488a0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Blakemore, C., and Sutton, P. (1969). Size adaptation: a new aftereffect. Science 166, 245–247. doi: 10.1126/science.166.3902.245
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Blakemore, C. T., and Campbell, F. W. (1969). On the existence of neurones in the human visual system selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images. J. Physiol. 203, 237–260. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1969.sp008862
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Clifford, C. W., Wyatt, A. M., Arnold, D. H., Smith, S. T., and Wenderoth, P. (2001). Orthogonal adaptation improves orientation discrimination. Vision Res. 41, 151–159. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00248-0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ejima, Y., and Takahashi, S. (1985). Effect of localized grating adaptation as a function of separation along the length axis between test and adaptation areas. Vision Res. 25, 1701–1707. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90142-7
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Farell, B., and Pelli, D. G. (1999). “Psychophysical methods, or how to measure a threshold and why,” in Vision Research: A Practical Guide to Laboratory Methods, eds R. H. S. Carpenter and J. G. Robson (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 129–137.
Firestone, C. (2013). On the origin and status of the “El Greco fallacy.” Perception 42, 672–674. doi: 10.1068/p7488
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Firestone, C., and Scholl, B. (2013). “Top-down” effects where none should be found: the El Greco fallacy in perception research. Psychol. Sci. 25, 38–46. doi: 10.1177/0956797613485092
Gescheider, G. A., Herman, D. D., and Phillips, J. N. (1970). Criterion shifts in the measurement of tactile masking. Percept. Psychophys. 8, 433–436. doi: 10.3758/BF03207041
Ghuman, A. S., McDaniel, J. R., and Martin, A. (2010). Face adaptation without a face. Curr. Biol. 20, 32–36. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.077
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect, and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. II. Simultaneous contrast and the areal restriction of the after-effect. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 553. doi: 10.1037/h0057585
Gibson, J. J., and Radner, M. (1937). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of tilted lines. I. Quantitative studies. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 453.
Green, D. M., and Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York, NY: Wiley.
Greene, M. R., and Oliva, A. (2010). High-level aftereffects to global scene properties. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 36, 1430–1442. doi: 10.1037/a0019058
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hakel, M. D., Ohnesorge, J. P., and Dunnette, M. D. (1970). Interviewer evaluations of job applicants' resumes as a function of the qualifications of the immediately preceding applicants: an examination of contrast effects. J. Appl. Psychol. 54, 27–30. doi: 10.1037/h0028650
Hegde, J. (2009). How reliable is the pattern adaptation technique? A modeling study.J. Neurophysiol. 102, 2245–2252. doi: 10.1152/jn.00216.2009
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hershenson, M. (1989). Duration, time constant, and decay of the linear motion aftereffect as a function of inspection duration. Percept. psychophys. 45, 251–257. doi: 10.3758/BF03210704
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Javadi, A. H., and Wee, N. (2012). Cross-category adaptation: objects produce gender adaptation in the perception of faces. PLoS ONE 7:e46079. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046079
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jogan, M., and Stocker, A. A. (2014). A new two-alternative forced choice method for the unbiased characterization of perceptual bias and discriminability. J. Vis. 14, 1–18. doi: 10.1167/14.3.20
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kingdom, F. A. A., and Prins, N. (2010). Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction. London: Academic Press.
Kitagawa, N., and Ichihara, S. (2002). Hearing visual motion in depth. Nature 416, 172–174. doi: 10.1038/416172a
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kohn, A. (2007). Visual adaptation: physiology, mechanisms, and functional benefits. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 3155–3164. doi: 10.1152/jn.00086.2007
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Konkle, T., Wang, Q., Hayward, V., and Moore, C. I. (2009). Motion aftereffects transfer between touch and vision. Curr. Biol. 19, 745–750. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.035
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Krauskopf, J., Williams, D. R., and Heeley, D. W. (1982). Cardinal directions of color space. Vision Res. 22, 1123–1131. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90077-3
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Leopold, D. A., Rhodes, G., Muller, K. M., and Jeffery, L. (2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 272, 897–904 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2004.3022
Leopold, D., O'Toole, A., Vetter, T., and Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level after effects. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 40–46. doi: 10.1038/82947
Levinson, E., and Sekuler, R. (1980). A two-dimensional analysis of direction-specific adaptation. Vision Res. 20, 103–107. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(80)90151-0
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Magnussen, S., and Johnsen, T. (1986). Temporal aspects of spatial adaptation. A study of the tilt aftereffect. Vision Res. 26, 661–672. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(86)90014-3
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Matsumiya, K. (2013). Seeing a haptically explored face: visual facial-expression aftereffect from haptic adaptation to a face. Psychol. Sci. 24, 2088–2098. doi: 10.1177/0956797613486981
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
McCollough, C. (1965). Color adaptation of edge detectors in the human visual system. Science 149, 1115–1116. doi: 10.1126/science.149.3688.1115
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
McKone, E., Jeffery, L., Boeing, A., Clifford, C. W., and Rhodes, G. (2014). Face identity aftereffects increase monotonically with adaptor extremity over, but not beyond, the range of natural faces. Vision Res. 98, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2014.01.007
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Morgan, M., Dillenburger, B., Raphael, S., and Solomon, J. A. (2011). Observers can voluntarily shift their psychometric functions without losing sensitivity. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 74, 185–193. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0222-7
Morgan, M. J. (2013). Sustained attention is not necessary for velocity adaptation. J. Vis. 13, 1–11. doi: 10.1167/13.8.26
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Morgan, M. J. (2014). A bias-free measure of retinotopic tilt adaptation. J. Vis. 14, 1–9. doi: 10.1167/4.1.7
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Morgan, M., Melmoth, D., and Solomon, J. A. (2013). Linking hypotheses underlying Class A and Class B methods. Vis. Neurosci. 30, 197–206. doi: 10.1017/S095252381300045X
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ng, M., Boynton, G. M., and Fine, I. (2008). Face adaptation does not improve performance on search or discrimination tasks. J. Vis. 8, 1–20. doi: 10.1167/8.1.1
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Oruc, I., and Barton, J. J. S. (2011). Adaptation improves discrimination of face identity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2591–2597. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2480
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Regan, D., and Beverley, K. I. (1985). Postadaptation orientation discrimination. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 147–155. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.2.000147
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rhodes, G., and Jeffery, L. (2006). Adaptive norm-based coding of facial identity. Vision Res. 46, 2977–2987. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.002
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rhodes, G., Maloney, L. T., Turner, J., and Ewing, L. (2007). Adaptive face coding and discrimination around the average face. Vision Res. 47, 974–989. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.12.010
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Rhodes, G., Watson, T. L., Jeffery, L., and Clifford, C. W. G. (2010). Perceptual adaptation helps us identify faces. Vision Res. 50, 963–968. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.003
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schneider, K. A., and Komlos, M. (2008). Attention biases decisions but does not alter appearance. J. Vis. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1167/8.15.3
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Schweinberger, S. R., Zäske, R., Walther, C., Golle, J., Kovács, G., and Wiese, H. (2010). Young without plastic surgery: perceptual adaptation to the age of female and male faces. Vision Res. 50, 2570–2576. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.017
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Storrs, K. R., and Arnold, D. H. (2012). Not all face aftereffects are equal. Vision Res. 64, 7–16. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2012.04.020
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Susilo, T., McKone, E., and Edwards, M. (2010). What shape are the neural response functions underlying opponent coding in face space? A psychophysical investigation. Vision Res. 50, 300–314. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.11.016
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thompson, P., and Burr, D. (2009). Visual aftereffects. Curr. Biol. 19, R11–R14. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.10.014
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van der Burg, E., Alais, D., and Cass, J. (2013). Rapid recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony. J. Neurosci. 33, 14633–14637. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1182-13.2013
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Webster, M. (2012). Evolving concepts of sensory adaptation. F1000 Biol. Rep. 4:21. doi: 10.3410/B4-21
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Webster, M. A. (1996). Human colour perception and its adaptation. Netw. Comput. Neural Syst. 7, 587–634. doi: 10.1088/0954-898X/7/4/002
Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., and Duhamel, P. (2004). Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature 428, 557–561. doi: 10.1038/nature02420
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Webster, M. A., and Maclin, O. H. (1999). Figural aftereffects in the perception of faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 647–653. doi: 10.3758/BF03212974
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wexley, K. N., Yukl, G. A., Kovacs, S. Z., and Sanders, R. E. (1972). Importance of contrast effects in employment interviews. J. Appl. Psychol. 56, 45. doi: 10.1037/h0032132
Williams, D. W., Wilson, H. R., and Cowan, J. D. (1982). Localized effects of spatial frequency adaptation. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 72, 878–887. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.72.000878
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yarrow, K., Jahn, N., Durant, S., and Arnold, D. H. (2011). Shifts of criteria or neural timing? The assumptions underlying timing perception studies. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1518–1531. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.07.003
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zhao, C., Seriès, P., Hancock, P. J. B., and Bednar, J. A. (2011). Similar neural adaptation mechanisms underlying face gender and tilt aftereffects. Vision Res. 51, 2021–2030. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.014
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: psychophysical methods, aftereffects, anchoring effect, response bias, contrast effect, neural adaptation, adaptation, psychological
Citation: Storrs KR (2015) Are high-level aftereffects perceptual? Front. Psychol. 6:157. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00157
Received: 11 October 2014; Paper pending published: 12 January 2015;
Accepted: 31 January 2015; Published online: 19 February 2015.
Edited by:
Frédéric Gosselin, University of Montreal, CanadaReviewed by:
Carl M. Gaspar, Hangzhou Normal University, ChinaGregory West, University of Montreal, Canada
Copyright © 2015 Storrs. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: k.storrs@uq.edu.au