Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 December 2014
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuro-education and neuro-rehabilitation View all 17 articles

Engagement in community music classes sparks neuroplasticity and language development in children from disadvantaged backgrounds

  • 1Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
  • 2Department of Otolaryngology, Neurobiology & Physiology and Northwestern University Interdepartmental Neuroscience Program, Northwestern University, Chicago, IL, USA
  • 3Data Sense LLC, Chicago, IL, USA

Children from disadvantaged backgrounds often face impoverished auditory environments, such as greater exposure to ambient noise and fewer opportunities to participate in complex language interactions during development. These circumstances increase their risk for academic failure and dropout. Given the academic and neural benefits associated with musicianship, music training may be one method for providing auditory enrichment to children from disadvantaged backgrounds. We followed a group of primary-school students from gang reduction zones in Los Angeles, CA, USA for 2 years as they participated in Harmony Project. By providing free community music instruction for disadvantaged children, Harmony Project promotes the healthy development of children as learners, the development of children as ambassadors of peace and understanding, and the development of stronger communities. Children who were more engaged in the music program—as defined by better attendance and classroom participation—developed stronger brain encoding of speech after 2 years than their less-engaged peers in the program. Additionally, children who were more engaged in the program showed increases in reading scores, while those less engaged did not show improvements. The neural gains accompanying music engagement were seen in the very measures of neural speech processing that are weaker in children from disadvantaged backgrounds. Our results suggest that community music programs such as Harmony Project provide a form of auditory enrichment that counteracts some of the biological adversities of growing up in poverty, and can further support community-based interventions aimed at improving child health and wellness.

Introduction

Over 16 million children in the US live in families with incomes below the federal poverty level, with a disproportionate number (68%) being members of minority racial and ethnic groups (Jiang et al., 2014). Parental income, occupation, and education are commonly combined to define a child’s socio-economic status [SES (Sirin, 2005)]. A child’s SES can interact with other personal factors (race, ethnicity, gender, etc.) to lead to weaker academic achievement (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Sirin, 2005; Hernandez, 2011; Skoe et al., 2013) and lower high school graduation rates (Ensminger and Slusarcick, 1992; Bradley and Corwyn, 2002), with lower academic achievement persisting even for low SES students who attend college (Walpole, 2003).

Auditory processing skills, known to be important for language development (Benasich et al., 2002, 2008; Boets et al., 2011; Goswami et al., 2011), may contribute to the link between SES and academic achievement. Children from low SES backgrounds have greater daily exposure to noise (Adler and Newman, 2002; Evans and Kantrowitz, 2002) and tend to be less concerned with using hearing protection in excessively noisy contexts like concerts (Vogel et al., 2007). Chronic noise exposure in children has been linked to weaker reading proficiency and cognitive skills (Maxwell and Evans, 2000; Haines et al., 2001; Clark et al., 2005) and can lead to delayed auditory neural development and greater spontaneous neural activity in animals (Chang and Merzenich, 2003; Seki and Eggermont, 2003; Zhu et al., 2014). Additionally, children from low SES backgrounds hear less complex language and fewer words overall during early language development, which contributes to weaker vocabularies when entering school (Bradley and Corwyn, 2002; Hoff, 2003; Cartmill et al., 2013). Similarly, a host of neural systems important for language, memory, and cognition are impacted by SES background (Raizada et al., 2008; Hackman et al., 2010; Noble et al., 2012), including those implicated in auditory attention (D’Angiulli et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2009) and speech encoding (Skoe et al., 2013). Encouragingly though, community and home based interventions may reverse these effects; children from low SES families participating in Head Start and attention training with their parents showed improved language, cognition, and neural measures of auditory attention relative to children in Head Start programs alone (Neville et al., 2013).

Musical training is another avenue of enrichment that may counteract some of the auditory deprivation endemic to low SES environments. A number of studies have revealed that children undergoing music training have stronger cognitive abilities, vocabulary, rhythm perception and production (linked to reading skill), perception of vocal pitch, and perception of speech in noisy backgrounds than non-musician children (Ho et al., 2003; Schellenberg, 2004, 2006; Magne et al., 2006; Forgeard et al., 2008b; Hyde et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009, 2011; Strait et al., 2011; Dege and Schwarzer, 2012; Strait et al., 2012; Slater et al., 2013; Chobert et al., 2014; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Strait and Kraus, 2014). Additionally, musical practice can strengthen children’s auditory encoding of speech (Magne et al., 2006; Besson et al., 2007; Chobert et al., 2011; Strait et al., 2011, 2013; Tierney et al., 2013; Chobert et al., 2014; see Strait and Kraus, 2014 for a review), auditory discrimination and attention (Koelsch et al., 2003; Moreno et al., 2009; Chobert et al., 2011; Putkinen et al., 2013), and lead to structural changes in auditory cortical areas (Hyde et al., 2009; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014). The auditory benefits of music training have direct implications for language skills and academic achievement (Hetland and Winner, 2010; Corrigall and Trainor, 2011; see Tierney and Kraus, 2013 for a review); accordingly, music may serve as an effective training tool for children with learning and attention impairments (Overy, 2003; Bhide et al., 2013; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014).

Community music programs, such as El Sistema and Harmony Project, provide students from low SES backgrounds with music opportunities that enrich the students and their communities. El Sistema, founded almost 40 years ago, provides over 500,000 Venezuelan children with free musical training in their community (for a review of the program, see Majno, 2012). Children are commonly enrolled as young as 2 or 3 years old and are supported through their teen years. Graduates of the program commonly return to teach at their community music center and parents are continually educated by the organization on how to support and encourage their child’s music as they advance. Since 2009, 62 El Sistema inspired music programs have been started in the United States. Harmony Project (Los Angeles, CA, USA) similarly promotes the development of healthy children and communities by providing free music training to children from low SES backgrounds in gang-reduction zones of Los Angeles, CA, USA (www.harmony-project.org). Children learn basic music skills in a preparatory class, eventually receive a free instrument to participate in group classes, and have opportunities for performance and ensemble playing throughout their enrollment from early grade school to high school. Between 2010 and 2014, 93% of Harmony Project alumni enrolled in post-secondary education, versus 67.6% of students graduating from public schools within Los Angeles County [most recent data from California Department of Education: Educational Demographics Unit (2008–2009) and Harmony Project Whole Notes 2013–2014 (2014)].

Our laboratory has shown that participation in music training through Harmony Project can reinforce literacy skills, enhance the perception of speech in background noise, and strengthen the neural encoding of speech sounds in children from low SES backgrounds (Kraus et al., 2014a,b; Slater et al., 2014a,b; Kraus and Strait, in press). Here we explore how the extent of student engagement in instrumental classes mediates music training’s effects on neural speech processing. Although we do not employ an active control group, our aim is to show within a group undergoing musical training how engagement may influence the benefits seen for students. Music engagement was defined by a student’s percent attendance in class and teacher ratings of classroom participation. We investigated whether greater engagement in music instruction can positively impact the neural encoding of speech. We focused on measures of subcortical brain activity reflecting speech harmonics, the consistency of the response, and spontaneous neural activity, all of which are weaker in teens from low SES backgrounds relative to higher-SES peers (Skoe et al., 2013).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Twenty-six children participated in the study (13 males, ages 6 years 11 months – 9 years 3 months; M = 8 years 5 months). All children were public elementary school students living in the gang reduction zones of Los Angeles, CA, USA. Participants attended schools where ≥90% of students qualify for free or reduced lunch. Families qualify for free or reduced lunch when their income is below 185% of the federal poverty level (U.S. Department of Education, 2014). The average education of the participants’ mothers was 10.7 years (SD = 4.2) and the median and modal maternal educational attainment level was completion of high school/GED. Maternal education is one of the strongest predictors of SES (Hoff et al., 2012), and 76% of children whose parents have a high school degree or less live in low-income families (Jiang et al., 2014). Average maternal education was equivalent across the four Harmony Project sites in which students were enrolled (Kruskal Wallis Test: χ2 = 6.252, p = 0.100).

Participants were excluded if they had a history of neurological disorders, IQ less than 80 [Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (Woerner and Overstreet, 1999)], previous musical training, or failed a hearing screening (air-conduction thresholds above 20 dB HL for octaves 125–8000 Hz). All procedures were approved by the Northwestern University Institutional Review Board and informed parental consent was obtained for all participants.

Music Instruction

Students were followed as they participated in Harmony Project for 2 years. The project curriculum started students in music appreciation class, where they learned pitch-matching and rhythm skills, musical styles and notation, and basic vocal performance and recorder playing. Participants attended these classes twice weekly for 3–10 months (M = 5).

As musical instruments became available and students were judged to be ready, they progressed to instrumental instruction. Students were given their own instruments and participated in a mix of group-based instrumental classes for approximately 4 h per week. Students in this study participated in one of four Harmony Project sites. The description of weekly classes at each site and the number of students at each site is presented in Table 1. Students played a number of different instruments (viola, cello, bass, French horn, clarinet, or trumpet) and the number of students playing each type of instrument is also reported in Table 1. Harmony Project has a standard curriculum for each instrument type (woodwinds, brass, and strings), including rigorous mastery benchmarks required for advancement to the next level of the curriculum. For example, Level 1 benchmarks require students to: demonstrate basic knowledge of music concepts such as rhythm and intonation, exhibit instrument specific skills and knowledge such as proper posture and correct clef identification, and present evidence of commitment through practice at home. As students progress, expectations shift to more instrument specific skills (e.g., proper bowing or breathing techniques) and knowledge and use of dynamics, key signatures and deviations, and articulations (e.g., staccato). Students are required to learn to read music and play from memory throughout the program. All students in the study transitioned to instrument use during their first year, so at the end of their second year in Harmony Project, students had an average of 165 h of school-based instrumental training (SD = 40) and over 210 h of music training overall.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Student instrument experience by site and by instrument.

Teacher ratings of music engagement

At the end of each instrumental class, teachers reported the percent attendance for each student (hours attended/total hours possible) and rated the students on their level of participation in class (1-not at all, 2-little, 3-moderate, 4-fairly good, 5-complete). We averaged percent attendance and teacher ratings of class participation across all the instrumental classes taken by each child over the 2 years. Both attendance and teacher perception of student effort predict ‘behavioral engagement’ in school, reflecting a student’s involvement in the participatory aspects of school (Glanville and Wildhagen, 2007). Because neither percent attendance nor participation were significantly correlated with the number of instrumental classes taken (p-values > 0.310), we are confident that higher levels of class participation or attendance reflect student motivation and are not simply artifacts of being enrolled in more classes. Additionally, percent attendance and participation ratings did not differ across Harmony Project sites (Kruskal Wallis Test: χ2 = 5.366, p = 0.147; χ2 = 4.781, p = 0.189, respectively), suggesting no rating bias among the sites.

Reading Assessment

Before enrolling in Harmony Project and after 2 years of Harmony participation, students were administered the Test of Oral Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE; Torgesen et al., 1999). The TOWRE, a measure of reading fluency, comprises word and non-word reading subtests. Children are required to read a list aloud as quickly as possible and the number of words (or non-words) read correctly in 45 s is tallied and combined to form the Total score.

Neurophysiological Measures

Evoked potentials were collected from the auditory brainstem in response to a 40-ms synthesized speech syllable [da], using an Intelligent Hearing Systems SmartEP system equipped with a cABR module (Miami, FL, USA). Stimuli were presented to the right ear at 80 dB SPL at a rate of 10.9 Hz through electromagnetically-shielded earphones (ER-3A, Etymotic Research, Elk Grove Village, IL, USA). Responses to opposing polarities were subtracted and filtered online from 0.1 to 1.5 kHz in the test session before music training began and from 0.05 to 3 kHz in the test session after 2 years of music training. Due to the differences in filter settings, which were expanded as the longitudinal project evolved, within-subject comparisons over the two test dates are not possible.

Neural measures were those previously shown to index SES (Skoe et al., 2013). Measures of response consistency, speech harmonics, and spontaneous neural activity were generated in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA, USA). Response consistency reflects the replicability of the response over the first half and second half of the recording on a scale from -1 to 1. Values were Fisher-transformed for analyses, but Pearson’s r-values are shown in Figures 1 and 2. Speech harmonics comprise the average amplitude of the frequency following response over the first-formant range of the stimulus (264–656 Hz). Spontaneous neural activity reflects the amplitude of brainstem activity in the absence of the stimulus, and likely reflects neural noise. Please see Banai et al. (2009), Hornickel and Kraus (2013), and Skoe et al. (2013) for additional details.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Children who regularly attended instrumental classes had stronger neural encoding of speech after 2 years, particularly for measures of speech harmonics and response consistency. Neural measures before music training began did not predict attendance or level of class participation, suggesting greater engagement in music classes may lead to stronger neural encoding of speech and not vice versa. Speech Harmonics is measured as the average amplitude of harmonic encoding in the frequency following response (μV), Response Consistency is measured as the Pearson’s correlation coefficient between response replications (r), and Spontaneous Neural Activity is measured as the root-mean-square magnitude of the pre-stimulus (μV).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Children who were most engaged during instrument classes had more consistent neural responses to speech after 2 years. (A) (red/maroon), a representative subject who had “complete” participation as rated by multiple teachers. The [da] stimulus is plotted in black in top panel of A for reference, shifted in time to account for neural delay. (B) (black/gray), a representative subject who had “moderate” participation as rated by multiple teachers. Both students completed four instrumental classes over the course of 2 years. Before music training, (top) the participants do not differ greatly in the consistency of their neural response to speech. After 2 years of music training (bottom), however, the child who participated more in class has a more consistent response to speech than the child who participated less. The two traces in each panel represent the two replications of the response, collected as the recording procedure dictates. The time region of the analysis is marked with vertical hashed lines in each panel.

Statistical Analyses

The relationships among instrumental class attendance, class participation, and neural measures were evaluated at each of the two test dates using Spearman’s correlations due to the non-normal distributions of the music engagement variables. Spearman’s correlations are more conservative than Pearson’s correlations because they reduce the impact of outlying data points and are most appropriate for smaller sample sizes. Similar to Pearson’s correlations, Spearman’s rho is a direct measure of effect size, reflecting the proportion of variance in the dependent variable (neural measures) accounted for by the independent variable (music engagement variables). Correlation coefficients with magnitudes of 0.1 are considered small effect sizes, 0.3 medium, and 0.5 large (Cohen, 1992). Reading fluency scores were compared using a paired t-test.

Results

Children who had better attendance in instrumental music classes over 2 years had stronger neural encoding of speech harmonics and better response consistency, two measures previously linked to SES (see Figure 1; Table 2A). Additionally, there was a positive association between class participation and response consistency (see Figure 2 for two representative participants and Table 2A). Interestingly, the least engaged participants in the study still had “moderate” class participation, suggesting that even small variations in student engagement can have implications for success with music training. No relationship was found between music engagement and spontaneous neural activity.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. (A) Children who are more engaged in music classes have stronger neural encoding of speech after music training for measures previously linked to SES (speech harmonics and response consistency). (B) Neural measures before beginning musical training do not predict subsequent engagement in music classes.

Importantly, our results suggest that greater engagement in music classes predicts stronger speech encoding, not vice versa. Neural measures before music training did not predict subsequent attendance or class participation (p-values > 0.126, see Table 2B), which suggests that participants with strong speech encoding to begin with did not necessarily go on to be the most engaged students in the study. Additionally, the small variation in years of maternal education did not predict attendance or class participation (ρ = -0.302, p = 0.142; ρ = 0.126, p = 0.548).

Participants overall showed a subclinical yet statistically-significant decline in reading fluency over the 2 years [M1(SD1) = 109.27(12.76), M2(SD2) = 106.04(15.22), t25 = 2.456, p = 0.021]. However, change in reading fluency scores was strongly correlated with engagement in instrumental classes. Children who participated more in class were more likely to show an improvement in reading fluency, while those who participated less were more likely to show a decrease in reading fluency (ρ = 0.443, p = 0.023).

Discussion

Here we found that greater engagement in group music instruction by children from low SES backgrounds predicted stronger neural encoding of speech for measures negatively influenced by low SES. Children who attended class more regularly and had better classroom participation had stronger neural encoding of speech after 2 years of music training than did their less-engaged peers. These results were found within a group of children undergoing music training, and without an active control group. It is likely that greater engagement in other extracurricular activities could also yield stronger neural outcomes. Perhaps the novel experience of participating in an extracurricular activity, interacting with the researchers, etc., influenced our findings. However, previous studies that have employed active control groups indicate that music training but not art training strengthens auditory skills (Moreno et al., 2009, 2011). Moreover, child musicians differ from non-musician children on auditory working memory and attention but not visual analogs (Strait et al., 2014). Similar to our analyses within a group of children participating in music, Hyde et al. (2009) found that the quality of musical training is critical for neuroplasticity. Children participating in private keyboard lessons for 15 months showed structural growth in primary auditory cortex, correlated with their improvement in melody and rhythm perception tasks; however, a second group of children who participated in school music classes each week learning basic singing and drumming skills did not show the same neural growth (Hyde et al., 2009). Our results suggest that engagement is an important factor mediating the benefits seen from musical training. It’s important to note that we found these relationships between engagement and neural function within a group of students whose attendance was relatively high on average (88%), who were rated as having ‘moderate’ or better class participation, and whose families were highly motivated for them to participate. The limited variance in music engagement and probable influence of other factors such as personality on student engagement may have contributed to the modest statistical significance of our tests. Nevertheless, we do see moderately strong relationships between engagement and neural measures, suggesting that even if well-motivated to begin music training, students may not make gains unless they are actively engaged in the process. In the same vein, previous studies of musicians have revealed the important role of continued practice in maintaining the benefits of musicianship (Pantev et al., 1998; Ho et al., 2003; Norton et al., 2005; Forgeard et al., 2008b; see Strait and Kraus, 2014 for a review).

Greater attendance and class participation were not predicted by neural measures before musical training. This suggests that children with the strongest neural function before beginning musical practice did not go on to be those most engaged in music classes. Instead, it appears that greater engagement in music may have resulted in stronger neural encoding. Cohort research revealing neural differences between musicians and non-musicians is unable to definitively say whether those differences were pre-existing, although the oft observed relationship between length of practice and benefits of musicianship supports music experience as the cause (Pantev et al., 1998; Ho et al., 2003; Norton et al., 2005; Forgeard et al., 2008b; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; see Strait and Kraus, 2014 for a review). A host of recent longitudinal studies report that musical training can selectively enhance auditory function in children without pre-existing differences. When children are randomly assigned to music or art training, only those in musical training show enhancements in auditory neurophysiology and attention (Moreno et al., 2009, 2011; Chobert et al., 2014). Additionally, children who elect to participate in music do not have inherent differences in neural structure or function from their peers (Norton et al., 2005; Hyde et al., 2009; Tierney et al., 2013), but show enhancements in auditory system structure and function after 1–2 years of musical training (Hyde et al., 2009; Tierney et al., 2013; Kraus et al., 2014b). We were not able to conduct paired pre-post comparisons due to alterations to our recording scheme as the study progressed. However, the lack of relationship between pre-test neural measures and subsequent classroom engagement in our study suggests our results are independent of pre-existing differences in neural function.

Although some studies report no differences in cognition for children who elect to participate in music versus those who do not (Schlaug et al., 2005; Hyde et al., 2009), personality and pre-existing skill may nevertheless influence a student’s engagement and persistence in music. For example, using duration of music lessons as the outcome variable, Corrigall et al. (2013) showed that cognitive skills (along with parental income) predicted how long students continued with music. They additionally showed that children exhibiting high Openness to experience (part of the Big Five personality traits) were more likely to continue with music lessons after taking into account cognition and parental income (Corrigall et al., 2013). In our group of students, personality factors may have influenced attendance and participation in music classes. Those who were highly engaged in music classes may have been highly engaged throughout their school day, had a more encouraging home environment, may have been more motivated individuals, etc. Indeed, Openness as a personality trait is linked to factors reflecting academic engagement and achievement in college students, such as enjoying thinking and analyzing, enjoying connecting with others in class, and enjoying working hard (Komarraju and Karau, 2005). One limitation of our study is the lack of measures to investigate which of these personality factors may predict engagement in music. Importantly though, duration of musical experience also predicts cognition and language skills (Schellenberg, 2006; Forgeard et al., 2008b), suggesting the presence of a reinforcing loop. Children more open to new experiences may elect to begin music lessons and continue to participate longer, which leads to larger cognitive benefits, predicting continued retention in music, etc.

In addition to enhancing cognitive skills, music training can lead to stronger reading, language, and academic skills in children (Schellenberg, 2004, 2006; Hetland and Winner, 2010; Corrigall and Trainor, 2011; Dege and Schwarzer, 2012; Francois et al., 2013; Seither-Preisler et al., 2014; Slater et al., 2014a; see Tierney and Kraus, 2013 for a review), including those with reading impairments (Overy, 2003; Bhide et al., 2013). As a group our participants showed a small decline in reading fluency scores, which likely reflects a trend for the achievement gap to widen for children from low SES backgrounds as they age (Sirin, 2005). However, the change in reading scores varied with music engagement; reading fluency improved in children with greater class participation. That this relationship was seen despite the small variance in class participation ratings speaks to the importance of active engagement in music for engendering benefits. Students who had the highest class participation rankings were more likely to show an increase in reading score after 2 years of participation in Harmony Project. On the other hand, participants who had ‘moderate’ class participation on average were likely to show a decrease in reading score. As the consistency of the auditory brainstem response to speech and the strength of representations of the speech harmonics have been repeatedly linked to reading ability (Banai et al., 2009; Hornickel et al., 2011, 2012a; Hornickel and Kraus, 2013; White-Schwoch and Kraus, 2013), and musical aptitude can predict reading ability (Ho et al., 2003; Forgeard et al., 2008a; Huss et al., 2011; Strait et al., 2011), it is possible that stronger speech encoding arising from greater participation in music classes contributed to improved reading scores.

Animal models of auditory deprivation and enrichment yield insight into the potential mechanisms involved in the neuroplasticity observed here. Similar to humans exposed to chronic noise, animals reared in noisy environments show detrimental effects on auditory processing, such as broader neural frequency selectivity (Zhou et al., 2011; Zhu et al., 2014), greater spontaneous neural activity (Seki and Eggermont, 2003), and weaker neural speech processing (Reed et al., 2014). Additionally, rats with a knock-down of a dyslexia-linked gene show more variable cortical responses to speech and weaker neural differentiation of speech (Centanni et al., 2014a). These deficits are thought to be due to imbalances in excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters, also shown to be negatively affected by noise exposure during development (Guo et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2014). Auditory enrichment and behavioral training can reverse these effects, yielding better neural frequency selectivity, more consistent neural responses to speech, better neural differentiation of speech sounds, and can re-establish excitatory/inhibitory balances in rats with early auditory deprivation (Guo et al., 2012; Centanni et al., 2014b; Zhu et al., 2014). Although these exact mechanisms have not been investigated in humans, it is very likely that similar ones are at play. Like the auditory enrichment paradigms in animal studies, music training is a social, multi-sensory process that activates multiple neural networks during repeated exposure to the enriching “environment” [please see Patel (2011) for a review of the multi-faceted nature of music training].

We failed to find significant relationships between spontaneous neural activity and musical engagement. Previous studies of auditory training in humans, such as music training, computer-based training games, foreign language learning, and classroom acoustic modification, have shown improvements in the evoked response to speech, but none have reported changes in spontaneous neural activity (Besson et al., 2007; Song et al., 2008; Stevens et al., 2008; Russo et al., 2010; Hornickel et al., 2012b; Strait et al., 2013). Animals that are subjected to noise during development have increased spontaneous neural activity, likely due to decreased neural inhibition as a result of abnormal excitatory/inhibitory neurotransmitter ratios (Seki and Eggermont, 2003; Zhu et al., 2014). However, auditory enrichment can alleviate imbalances in excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters (Guo et al., 2012; Zhu et al., 2014) and possibly reduce noise-induced spontaneous neural activity. Spontaneous cortical activity thought to reflect a functional “resting-state network” can be altered with training in humans (Lewis et al., 2009; Taubert et al., 2011). The present findings show that the extent of music engagement tracks with changes in stimulus-evoked, but not spontaneous background activity; training-related influences on spontaneous activity should be explored further.

Our results support the importance of active experience and meaningful engagement with sound to engender neural changes. Even in a group of highly motivated students, small variations in music engagement (attendance and class participation) predicted the strength of speech encoding after music training. These measures of neural encoding are known to be weaker in children from low SES backgrounds. Although personality features likely played a large role in students’ levels of engagement in class, our results do support that greater levels of engagement are not prompted by pre-existing differences in neural encoding. In our group of participants from low SES backgrounds, those who were the most engaged in music classes had the strongest neural responses and were most likely to show improvements in reading ability. Community music programs such as Harmony Project and El Sistema have proven success in bolstering academic achievement in children from disadvantaged backgrounds. We have also shown that participation in Harmony Project reinforces literacy skills and enhances the neural encoding of speech cues important for reading and the perception of speech in noisy backgrounds (Kraus et al., 2014a,b; Slater et al., 2014a; Kraus and Strait, in press). Taken together, we suggest that motivated engagement in community music programs may counteract some of the auditory impoverishment that children from low SES backgrounds commonly experience.

Author Contributions

Nina Kraus and Dana L. Strait designed the study; Dana L. Strait, Jessica Slater, and Elaine Thompson collected the data; Jane Hornickel analyzed the data; Jane Hornickel and Nina Kraus prepared the manuscript; all authors contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Samantha R. O’Connell, Shivani Bhatia, Jason V. Thompson, Emily Spitzer, Erika Skoe, Jen Krizman, Travis White-Schwoch, and Trent Nicol for their contributions throughout the project. We are grateful to Harmony Project (www.harmony-project.org) administrators and staff for facilitating this research, especially Harmony Project founder Dr. Margaret Martin, Executive Director Myka Miller, Monk Turner, Sara Flores, and Jeremy Drake. We thank the children and their families for participating. This work was supported by the National Association of Music Merchants, the GRAMMY Foundation, and the Knowles Hearing Center of Northwestern University.

References

Adler, N. E., and Newman, K. (2002). Socioeconomic disparities in health: pathways and policies. Health Aff. 21, 60–76. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.21.2.60

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banai, K., Hornickel, J., Skoe, E., Nicol, T., Zecker, S. G., and Kraus, N. (2009). Reading and subcortical auditory function. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2699–2707. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benasich, A. A., Gou, Z., Choudhury, N., and Harris, K. (2008). Early cognitive and language skills are linked to resting frontal gamma power across the first 3 years. Behav. Brain Res. 195, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.08.049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benasich, A. A., Thomas, J. J., Choudhury, N., and Leppanen, P. H. T. (2002). The importance of rapid auditory processing abilities to early language development: evidence from converging methodologies. Dev. Psychobiol. 40, 278–292. doi: 10.1002/dev.10032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besson, M., Schon, D., Moreno, S., Santos, A., and Magne, C. (2007). Influence of musical expertise and musical training on pitch processing in music and language. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 399–410.

Google Scholar

Bhide, A., Power, A., and Goswami, U. (2013). A rhythmic musical intervention for poor readers: a comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention. Mind Brain Educ. 7, 113–123. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boets, B., Vandermosten, M., Poelmans, H., Luts, H., Wouters, J., and Ghesquiere, P. (2011). Preschool impairments in auditory processing and speech perception uniquely predict future reading problems. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 560–570. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 371–399. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

California Department of Education: Educational Demographics Unit. (2008–2009).High School Graduates’ College Enrollment. Available at: http://dq.cde.ca.gov/dataquest/sfsf/PostsecondaryIndicatorC11.aspx?cChoice=C11Cnty&cYear=2008-09&TheCounty = 19%2CLOSÂNGELES&cLevel=County&cTopic=C11&myTimeFrame=S&submit1=Submit

Cartmill, E. A., Amstrong, B. F. I., Gleitman, L. R., Goldin-Meadow, S., Medina, T. N., and Trueswell, J. C. (2013). Quality of early parent input predicts child vocabulary 3 years later. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 11278–11283. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1309518110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centanni, T. M., Booker, A. M., Sloan, A. M., Chen, F., Maher, B. J., Carraway, R. S.,et al. (2014a). Knockdown of the dyslexia-associated gene Kiaa0319 impairs temporal responses to speech stimuli in rat primary auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1753–1766. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht028

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Centanni, T. M., Chen, F., Booker, A. M., Engineer, C., Sloan, A. M., Rennaker, R. L.,et al. (2014b). Speech sound processing deficits and training-induced neural plasticity in rats with a dyslexia gene knockdown. PLoS ONE 9:e98439. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098439

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, E., and Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Environmental noise retards auditory cortical development. Science 300, 498–502. doi: 10.1126/science.1082163

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chobert, J., Francois, C., Velay, J.-L., and Besson, M. (2014). Twelve months of active musical training in 8-to10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cereb. Cortex 24, 956–967. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs377

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chobert, J., Marie, C., Francois, C., Schon, D., and Besson, M. (2011). Enhanced passive and active processing of syllables in musician children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3874–3887. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00088

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C., Martin, R., Van Kempen, E., Alfred, T., Head, J., Davies, H. W.,et al. (2005). Exposure-effect relations between aircraft and road traffic noise exposure at school and reading comprehension. Am. J. Epidemiol. 163, 27–37. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwj001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Q. Methods Psychol. 112, 155–159.

Google Scholar

Corrigall, K. A., Schellenberg, E. G., and Misura, N. M. (2013). Music training, cognition, and personality. Front. Psychol. 4:222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corrigall, K. A., and Trainor, L. J. (2011). Associations between length of music training and reading skills in children. Music Percept. 29, 147–155. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Angiulli, A., Herdman, A., Stapells, D., and Hertzman, C. (2008). Children’s event-related potentials of auditory selective attention vary with their socioeconomic status. Neuropsychology 22, 293–300. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.293

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dege, F., and Schwarzer, G. (2012). The effect of a music program on phonological awareness in preschoolers. Front. Psychol. 2:124. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00124

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ensminger, M. E., and Slusarcick, A. L. (1992). Paths to high school graduation or dropout: a longitudinal study of a first-grade cohort. Sociol. Educ. 65, 95–113. doi: 10.2307/2112677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, G. W., and Kantrowitz, E. (2002). Socioeconomic status and health: the potential role of environmental risk exposure. Annu. Rev. Public Health 23, 303–331. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.23.112001.112349

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgeard, M., Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Rosam, C., and Iyengar, U. (2008a). The relation betwen music and phonological processing in normal-reading children and children with dyslexia. Music Percept. 25, 383–390. doi: 10.1525/mp.2008.25.4.383

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Norton, A., and Schlaug, G. (2008b). Practicing a musical instrument in childhood is associated with enhanced verbal ability and nonverbal reasoning. PLoS ONE 3:e3566. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0003566

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Francois, C., Chobert, J., Besson, M., and Schon, D. (2013). Music training for the development of speech segmentation. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2038–2043. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs180

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glanville, J. L., and Wildhagen, T. (2007). The measurement of school engagement: assessing dimensionality and measurement invariance across race and ethnicity. J. Psychol. Meas. 67, 1019–1041. doi: 10.1177/0013164406299126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., Wang, H. L., Cruz, A., Fosker, T., Mead, N., and Huss, M. (2011). Language-universal sensory deficits in developmental dyslexia: English, Spanish, and Chinese. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 325–327. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21453

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, F., Zhang, Y., Zhu, X., Cai, R., Zhou, X., and Sun, X. (2012). Auditory discrimination training rescues developmentally degraded directional selectivity and restores mature expression of GABAA and AMPA receptor subunits in rat auditory cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 229, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.12.041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., and Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 651–659. doi: 10.1038/nrn2897

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haines, M. M., Stansfeld, S. A., Job, R. F. S., and Head, J. (2001). Chronic aircraft noise exposure, stress responses, mental health and cognitive performance in school children. Psychol. Med. 31, 265–277. doi: org/10.1017/S0033291701003282

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmony Project Whole Notes 2013–2014. (2014). Available at: http://www.harmony-project.org/about-us/harmony-project-current-newsletter/

Hernandez, D. J. (2011). Double Jeopardy: How Third-Grade Reading Skills and Povery Influence High School Graduation. Baltimore, MD: The Annie E. Casey Foundation.

Google Scholar

Hetland, L., and Winner, E. (2010). The arts and academic achievement: what the evidence shows. Arts Educ. Policy Rev. 102, 3–6. doi: 10.1080/10632910109600008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, Y.-C., Cheung, M.-C., and Chan, A. S. (2003). Music training improves verbal but not visual memory: cross-sectional and longitudinal explorations in children. Neuropsychology 17, 439–450. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.17.3.439

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E. (2003). The specificity of environmental influence: socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Dev. 74, 1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00612

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoff, E., Laursen, B., and Bridges, K. (2012). “Measurement and modeling in studying the influence of socioeconomic status on child development,” in The Cambridge Handbook of Environment in Human Development. eds L Mayes and M Lewis (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 590–606.

Hornickel, J., Anderson, S., Skoe, E., Yi, H., and Kraus, N. (2012a). Subcortical representation of speech fine structure relates to reading ability. Neuroreport 23, 6–9. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834d2ffd

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornickel, J., Zecker, S., Bradlow, A. R., and Kraus, N. (2012b). Assistive listening devices drive neuroplasticity in children with dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 16731–16736. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1206628109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornickel, J., Chandrasekaran, B., Zecker, S. G., and Kraus, N. (2011). Auditory brainstem measures predict reading and speech-in-noise perception in school-aged children. Behav. Brain Res. 216, 597–605. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.08.051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hornickel, J., and Kraus, N. (2013). Unstable representation of sound: a biological marker of dyslexia. J. Neurosci. 33, 3500–3504. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4205-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huss, M., Verney, J. P., Fosker, T., Mead, N., and Goswami, U. (2011). Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex 47, 674–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C.,et al. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. J. Neurosci. 29, 3019–3025. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5118-08.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, Y., Ekono, M., and Skinner, C. (2014). Basic Facts About Low-Income Children: Children Under 18, 2012. New York, NY: National Center for Children in Poverty. Available at: www.nccp.org/publications/pub_1047.html

Google Scholar

Koelsch, S., Grossmann, T., Gunter, T. C., Hahne, A., Schroger, E., and Friederici, A. D. (2003). Children processing music: electric brain responses reveal musical competence and gender differences. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 683–693. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2003.15.5.683

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Komarraju, M., and Karau, S. J. (2005). The relationship between the big five personality traits and academic motivation. Pers. Individ. Differ. 39, 557–567. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.02.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, N., Slater, J., Thompson, E., Hornickel, J., Strait, D. L., Nicol, T.,et al. (2014a). Auditory learning through active engagement with sound: biological impact of community music lessons in at-risk children. Front. Neurosci. 8:351. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00351

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, N., Slater, J., Thompson, E., Hornickel, J., Strait, D. L., Nicol, T.,et al. (2014b). Music enrichment programs improve the neural encoding of speech in at-risk children. J. Neurosci. 34, 11913–11918. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1881-14.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, N., and Strait, D. L. (in press). Emergence of biological markers of musicianship with school-based music instruction. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

Lewis, C. M., Baldassarre, A., Committeri, G., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2009). Learning sculpts spontaneous activity of the resting human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 17558–17563. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902455106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magne, C., Schon, D., and Besson, M. (2006). Musician children detect pitch violations in both music and language better than nonmusician children: behavioral and electrophysiological approaches. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 199–211. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.2.199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Majno, M. (2012). From the model of El Sistema in Venezuela to current applications: learning and integration through collective music education. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1252, 56–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06498.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maxwell, L. E., and Evans, G. W. (2000). The effects of noise on pre-school children’s pre-reading skills. J. Environ. Psychol. 20, 91–97. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1999.0144

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Bialystok, E., Barac, R., Schellenberg, E. G., Cepeda, N. J., and Chau, T. (2011). Short-term musical training enhances verbal intelligence and executive function. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1425–1433. doi: 10.1177/0956797611416999

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., and Besson, M. (2009). Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: more evidence for brain plasticity. Cereb. Cortex 19, 712–723. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn120

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neville, H., Stevens, C., Pakulak, E., Bell, T. A., Fanning, J., Klein, S.,et al. (2013). Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status preschoolers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 12138–12143. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1304437110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., and Sowell, E. R. (2012). Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 15, 516–527. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, A., Winner, E., Cronin, K., Overy, K., Lee, D. J., and Schlaug, G. (2005). Are there pre-existing neural, cognitive, or motoric markers for musical ability? Brain Cogn. 59, 124–134. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.05.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overy, K. (2003). Dyslexia and music: from timing deficits to musical intervention. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 999, 497–505. doi: 10.1196/annals.1284.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pantev, C., Oostenveld, R., Engelien, A., Ross, B., Roberts, L. E., and Hoke, M. (1998). Increased auditory cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392, 811–814. doi: 10.1038/33918

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patel, A. D. (2011). Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech? The OPERA hypothesis. Front. Psychol. 2:142. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00142

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Putkinen, V., Tervaniemi, M., and Huotilainen, M. (2013). Informal musical activities are linked to auditory discrimination and attenction in 2-3 year old children: an event-related potential study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 37, 654–661. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12049

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raizada, R. D. S., Richards, T. L., Meltzoff, A., and Kuhl, P. K. (2008). Socioeconomic status predicts hemispheric specialisation of the left inferior frontal gyrus in young children. Neuroimage 40, 1392–1401. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, A. C., Centanni, T. M., Borland, M. S., Matney, C. J., Engineer, C., and Kilgard, M. P. (2014). Behavioral and neural discrimination of speech sounds after moderate or intense noise exposure in rats. Ear Hear. 35:e248–e261. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000062

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russo, N. M., Hornickel, J., Nicol, T., Zecker, S., and Kraus, N. (2010). Biological changes in auditory function following training in children with autism spectrum disorders. Behav. Brain Funct. 6, 60. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-6-60

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G. (2004). Music lessons enhance IQ. Psychol. Sci. 15, 511–514. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00711.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Long-term positive associations between music lessons and IQ. J. Educ. Psychol. 98, 457–468. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Overy, K., and Winner, E. (2005). Effects of music training on the child’s brain and cognitive development. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1060, 219–230. doi: 10.1196/annals.1360.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seither-Preisler, A., Parncutt, R., and Schneider, P. (2014). Size and synchronization of auditory cortex promotes musical, literacy, and attentional skills in children. J. Neurosci. 34, 10937–10949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5315-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seki, S., and Eggermont, J. J. (2003). Changes in spontaneous firing rate and neural synchrony in cat primary auditory cortex after localized tone-induced hearing loss. Hear. Res. 180, 28–38. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00074-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sirin, S. R. (2005). Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: a meta-analytic review of research. Rev. Educ. Res. 75, 417–453. doi: 10.3102/00346543075003417

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skoe, E., Krizman, J., and Kraus, N. (2013). The impoverished brain: disparities in maternal education affect the neural response to sound. J. Neurosci. 33, 17221–17231. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2102-13.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slater, J., Strait, D. L., Skoe, E., O’connell, S., Thompson, E., and Kraus, N. (2014a). Longitudinal effects of group music instruction on literacy skills in low-income children. PLoS ONE. 9:e113383. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113383

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Slater, J., Strait, D. L., Thompson, E., Hornickel, J., and Kraus, N. (2014b). “Longitudinal effects of group music instruction on speech and rhythm processing: cognitive, perceptual, and neural evidence,” in Neurosciences and Music-V, Dijon.

Google Scholar

Slater, J., Tierney, A., and Kraus, N. (2013). At-risk elementary school children with one year of classroom music instruction are better at keeping a beat. PLoS ONE 8:e77250. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077250

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, J. H., Skoe, E., Wong, P. C. M., and Kraus, N. (2008). Plasticity in the adult human auditory brainstem following short-term linguistic training. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1892–1902. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20131

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C., Fanning, J., Coch, D., Sanders, L., and Neville, H. (2008). Neural mechanisms of selective auditory attention are enhanced by computerized training: electrophysiological evidence from language-impaired and typically developing children. Brain Res. 1205, 55–69. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.10.108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., and Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an event-related brain potential study. Dev. Sci. 12, 634–646. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00807.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strait, D. L., Hornickel, J., and Kraus, N. (2011). Subcortical processing of speech regularities predicts reading and music aptitude in children. Behav. Brain Funct. 7, 44. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-44

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strait, D. L., and Kraus, N. (2014). Biological impact of auditory expertise across the life span: musicians as a model of auditory learning. Hear. Res. 308, 109–121. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strait, D. L., O’connell, S., Parbery-Clark, A., and Kraus, N. (2014). Musicians’ enhanced neural differentiation of speech sounds arises early in life: developmental evidence from ages 3 to 30. Cereb. Cortex 24, 2512–2521. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strait, D. L., Parbery-Clark, A., Hittner, E., and Kraus, N. (2012). Musical training during early childhood enhances the neural encoding of speech in noise. Brain Lang. 123, 191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.09.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strait, D. L., Parbery-Clark, A., O’connell, S., and Kraus, N. (2013). Biological impact of preschool music classes on processing speech in noise. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.06.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taubert, M., Lohman, G., Margulies, D. S., Villringer, A., and Ragert, P. (2011). Long-term effects of motor training on resting-state networks and underlying brain structure. Neuroimage 57, 1492–1498. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, A., and Kraus, N. (2013). “Music training for the development of reading skills,” in Progress in Brain Research: Changing Brains, Applying Brain Plasticity to Advance and Recovery Human Ability, eds M. M. Merzenich, M. Nahum, and T. M. Van Vleet. (Burlington, VT: Academic Press), 209–241.

Google Scholar

Tierney, A., Krizman, J., Skoe, E., Johnston, K., and Kraus, N. (2013). High school music classes enhance the neural processing of speech. Front. Psychol. 4:855. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00855

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., and Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of Word Reading Efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Google Scholar

U.S. Department of Education (2014). Children and Youth with Disabilities. Available at: http://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_cgg.asp

Vogel, I., Brug, J., Van Der Ploeg, C. P. B., and Raat, H. (2007). Young people’s exposure to loud music. Am. J. Prev. Med. 33, 124–133. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.03.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walpole, M. (2003). Socioeconomic status and college: how SES affects college experiences and outcomes. Rev. Higher Educ. 27, 45–73. doi: 10.1353/rhe.2003.0044

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

White-Schwoch, T., and Kraus, N. (2013). Physiological discrimination of stop consonantsrelates to phonological skills in pre-readers: a biomarker forsubsequent reading ability? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:899. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00899

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woerner, C., and Overstreet, K. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Zhou, X., Panizzutti, R., De Villers-Sidani, E., Madiera, C., and Merzenich, M. M. (2011). Natural restoration of critical period plasticity in the juvenile and adult primary auditory cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 5625–5634. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6470-10.2011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Wang, F., Hu, H., Sun, X., Kilgard, M. P., Merzenich, M. M.,et al. (2014). Environmental acoustic enrichment promotes recovery from developmentally degraded auditory cortical processing. J. Neurosci. 34, 5406–5415. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5310-13.2014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: low socioeconomic status/poverty, community music training, electrophysiology, reading, speech, auditory training

Citation: Kraus N, Hornickel J, Strait DL, Slater J and Thompson E (2014) Engagement in community music classes sparks neuroplasticity and language development in children from disadvantaged backgrounds. Front. Psychol. 5:1403. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01403

Received: 21 August 2014; Accepted: 17 November 2014;
Published online: 16 December 2014.

Edited by:

Mireille Besson, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique – Institut de Neurosciences Cognitives de la Méditerranée, France

Reviewed by:

Lutz Jäncke, University of Zurich, Switzerland
Johannes Ziegler, Université Aix-Marseille, France
Sumitava Mukherjee, Indian Institute of Management, India

Copyright © 2014 Kraus, Hornickel, Strait, Slater and Thompson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Nina Kraus, Auditory Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, 2240 Campus Drive, Evanston, IL 60208, USA e-mail: nkraus@northwestern.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.