To what level are invisible stimuli processed by the brain in the absence of conscious awareness? Taking stock of the evidence to this day, it is widely accepted that simple visual properties of invisible stimuli are processed; however, the existence of higher-level unconscious processing (e.g., involving semantic or executive functions) remains a matter of debate. After several years of research in the field of unconscious processing, we became aware of a number of methodological aspects which need to be controlled carefully to help resolve discrepant findings in the literature. These aspects relate to: (1) when and how visibility is assessed; (2) when and how unconscious processing is assessed; (3) whether spatiotemporal attention is directed or, at least, measured; (4) whether adequate control conditions are used to rule out alternate explanations; (5) whether the studies are sufficiently powered and account for individual differences. Yet even when these aspects are carefully controlled, there may be, and probably are, some inherent differences in the amount of information let through by the different invisibility-inducing techniques (the “psychophysical magic” arsenal, Kim and Blake, 2005). We launched this Research Topic to foster investigations into these inherent differences (note previous attempts, Breitmeyer et al., 2004; Almeida et al., 2008; Kanai et al., 2010; Faivre et al., 2012).
The articles in this issue span various aspects of the research question that we put out to the community.
Perhaps the best starting point for a thorough introduction to the field of unconscious processing is the contribution by Landry et al. (2014): they review the different techniques used to prevent visual awareness, emphasizing the distinction between subliminal vs. preconscious processing, and find difficulty in reconciling the variety of techniques and results in the literature. They advocate the use of hypnosis, a top-down approach that can induce both perceptual and attentional failures, as a way to bridge the existing gap in the literature.
Continuous Flash Suppression (CFS) (Tsuchiya and Koch, 2005; Tsuchiya et al., 2006) consists in presenting a low contrast target stimulus to one eye while flashing a stream of high contrast stimuli to the other eye, resulting in strong interocular suppression. This rather young technique (as compared to masking or binocular rivalry) has boosted the field of unconscious processing in the past 10 years, allowing researchers to render stimuli invisible for seconds at a time in a very robust fashion. A slight but significant twist referred to as “breaking Continuous Flash Suppression” (bCFS) (Jiang et al., 2007), which consists in comparing the time it takes for different stimuli to break suppression, has been in the spotlight recently. Using bCFS, researchers have claimed that many high-level properties of invisible stimuli are processed unconsciously, which clashes with classical results from binocular rivalry and CFS itself. Yang et al. (2014) offer an insightful review of behavioral CFS and bCFS findings. Though they conclude that the emerging picture is that extensive processing can occur under CFS, they caution against many common issues in the use of (b)CFS. Gayet et al. (2014) focus exclusively on bCFS, and review the 30 studies published to date using this technique. They reject most conclusions of high-level processing, arguing that low-level mechanisms could account for the data. In a similar vein (Stein and Sterzer, 2014) argue that bCFS is not a valid measure of consciousness in its current implementation, and propose some modifications to the paradigm. Taking a step back from the controversies about bCFS (Sterzer et al., 2014) review neural processing under interocular suppression. They conclude that the literature currently presents highly heterogeneous findings regarding which structures are involved in processing suppressed stimuli and which stimuli can be processed; they emphasize the importance of controlling the depth of suppression in future studies, and advocate the use of online, continuous measures that capture the functional relevance of brain signals related to the processing of invisible stimuli. Finally (Faivre et al., 2014) compare published findings in the CFS and visual crowding literatures, as two methods designed to induce sustained invisibility. They conclude that the literature does not yet provide a coherent picture on the extent of processing under each of the two methods.
Taken together, these six review and opinion articles paint an accurate picture of the current landscape and controversies around unconscious processing. In addition, we received six original research articles.
A crucial choice in unconscious processing studies is that of the measure to establish invisibility, and there is still considerable debate on what this measure should be. This is illustrated in a contribution by Herzog et al. (2014), in which the very notion of invisibility is challenged, and reformulated in terms of a purposeful interpretation (i.e., spatiotemporal grouping) of incoming stimuli by the brain. In this view, the features of incoming stimuli are always visible and available to the brain, it is just a matter of how they are interpreted. Based on a more traditional definition of invisibility (Sandberg et al., 2014) compare exclusion tasks, in which participants are asked to solve an experimental task without using information from the invisible stimulus, with subjective visibility measures, in which participants are asked to indicate their subjective experience on a perceptual awareness scale (PAS). They find that exclusion tasks may in fact be a less sensitive and exhaustive measure of awareness than the PAS.
Another important choice in unconscious processing studies is what exact parameters should be used, within a given technique, to create the conditions of invisibility that one wishes to work with. Kaunitz et al. (2014) investigate the role of various parameters (onset time of the stimulus with respect to the onset of the masks, mask frequency, number of masks, duration of the stimulus) for the masking of a brief target stimulus with CFS. They notably find that showing a number of masks before the target does not increase suppression depth, and that a higher temporal frequency of Mondrian presentation results in deeper suppression. Their study demonstrates the importance of small, often overlooked, experimental details which can have an impact on the outcome of different studies and prevent comparison of results.
Finally, we received three studies comparing the extent of unconscious processing under different suppression techniques, either at a behavioral or at a neural level. Peremen and Lamy (2014) compare priming for directional arrow stimuli under metacontrast masking and CFS, in two separate experiments. They find that priming occurs with metacontrast masking but is abolished when stimuli are rendered invisible with CFS. They use each technique as it is optimally implemented, meaning that a number of experimental factors differ between the two experiments (as in Almeida et al., 2008, 2010). Izatt et al. (2014) also chose to compare masking and interocular suppression, but with a fame priming paradigm using invisible faces. The authors take great care to equate most experimental parameters and randomly present both techniques within the same experiment, such that the subjects are completely unaware of which is used on any given trial. Under these conditions, the authors do not find significant differences in the processing of faces rendered invisible by the two techniques, but they generally observe that priming effects are larger with masking than interocular suppression. Last (Fogelson et al., 2014) render a small set of four faces and four tools invisible using two techniques that allow sustained invisibility: CFS, and the less common chromatic flicker fusion (CFF). They concurrently record fMRI and, under the same condition of invisibility, attempt to decode the category of the invisible stimuli throughout the brain. They find that informative regions differ between the two techniques.
These contributions bring valuable insights into the question that we set out to address when we launched this Research Topic. This is a strong starting point to an in-depth systematic comparison of measures and techniques in the study of unconscious vision. We sincerely hope that the readers will find this collection of articles as stimulating and thought-provoking as we do, and that they will apply some of the many wise recommendations interspersed throughout to their own line of research.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgment
We thank Liad Mudrik for helpful comments on this manuscript.
References
Almeida, J., Mahon, B. Z., and Caramazza, A. (2010). The role of the dorsal visual processing stream in tool identification. Psychol. Sci. 21, 772–778. doi: 10.1177/0956797610371343
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Almeida, J., Mahon, B. Z., Nakayama, K., and Caramazza, A. (2008). Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 15214–15218. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805867105
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Breitmeyer, B. G., Ro, T., and Ogmen, H. (2004). A comparison of masking by visual and transcranial magnetic stimulation: implications for the study of conscious and unconscious visual processing. Conscious. Cogn. 13, 829–843. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2004.08.007
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Faivre, N., Berthet, V., and Kouider, S. (2012). Nonconscious influences from emotional faces: a comparison of visual crowding, masking, and continuous flash suppression. Front. Psychol. 3:129. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00129
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Faivre, N., Berthet, V., and Kouider, S. (2014). Sustained invisibility through crowding and continuous flash suppression: a comparative review. Front. Psychol. 5:475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00475
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Fogelson, S. V., Kohler, P. J., Miller, K. J., Granger, R., and Tse, P. U. (2014). Unconscious neural processing differs with method used to render stimuli invisible. Front. Psychol. 5:601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00601
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gayet, S., Van der Stigchel, S., and Paffen, C. L. E. (2014). Breaking continuous flash suppression: competing for consciousness on the pre-semantic battlefield. Front. Psychol. 5:460. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00460
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Herzog, M., Hermens, F., and Ogmen, H. (2014). Invisibility & Interpretation. Front. Psychol. 5:975. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00975
Izatt, G., Dubois, J., Faivre, N., and Koch, C. (2014). A direct comparison of unconscious face processing under masking and interocular suppression. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00659
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., and He, S. (2007). Processing of invisible stimuli: advantage of upright faces and recognizable words in overcoming interocular suppression. Psychol. Sci. 18, 349–355. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01902.x
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kanai, R., Walsh, V., and Tseng, C. (2010). Subjective discriminability of invisibility: a framework for distinguishing perceptual and attentional failures of awareness. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 1045–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.003
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaunitz, L. N., Fracasso, A., Skujevskis, M., and Melcher, D. (2014). Waves of visibility: probing the depth of inter-ocular suppression with transient and sustained targets. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00804
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kim, C.-Y., and Blake, R. (2005). Psychophysical magic: rendering the visible “invisible”. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.012
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Landry, M., Appourchaux, K., and Raz, A. (2014). Elucidating unconscious processing with instrumental hypnosis. Front. Psychol. 5, 1–18. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00785
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Peremen, Z., and Lamy, D. (2014). Comparing unconscious processing during continuous flash suppression and meta-contrast masking just under the limen of consciousness. Front. Psychol. 5:969. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00969
Sandberg, K., Del Pin, S. H., Bibby, B. M., and Overgaard, M. (2014). Evidence of weak conscious experiences in the exclusion task. Front. Psychol. 5, 1080. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01080
Stein, T., and Sterzer, P. (2014). Unconscious processing under interocular suppression: getting the right measure. Front. Psychol. 5:387. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00387
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sterzer, P., Stein, T., Ludwig, K., Rothkirch, M., and Hesselmann, G. (2014). Neural processing of visual information under interocular suppression: a critical review. Front. Psychol. 5:453. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00453
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., and Koch, C. (2005). Continuous flash suppression reduces negative afterimages. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1096–1101. doi: 10.1038/nn1500
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N., Koch, C., Gilroy, L. A., and Blake, R. (2006). Depth of interocular suppression associated with continuous flash suppression, flash suppression, and binocular rivalry. J. Vis. 6, 1068–1078. doi: 10.1167/6.10.6
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yang, E., Brascamp, J., Kang, M.-S., and Blake, R. (2014). On the use of continuous flash suppression for the study of visual processing outside of awareness. Front. Psychol. 5:724. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00724
Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: unconscious processing, Continuous Flash Suppression (CFS), backward masking, visual crowding, invisibility, measures of consciousness, interocular suppression, psychophysical magic
Citation: Dubois J and Faivre N (2014) Invisible, but how? The depth of unconscious processing as inferred from different suppression techniques. Front. Psychol. 5:1117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01117
Received: 08 September 2014; Accepted: 15 September 2014;
Published online: 01 October 2014.
Edited and reviewed by: Anil K. Seth, University of Sussex, UK
Copyright © 2014 Dubois and Faivre. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence:amNyZHVib2lzQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==