Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 03 February 2014
Sec. Consciousness Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Olfactory Consciousness across Disciplines View all 14 articles

Same same but different: the case of olfactory imagery

  • Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden

In the present work we present an overview of experimental findings corroborating olfactory imagery observations with the visual and auditory modalities. Overall, the results indicate that imagery of olfactory information share many features with those observed in the primary senses although some major differences are evident. One such difference pertains to the considerable individual differences observed, with the majority being unable to reproduce olfactory information in their mind. Here, we highlight factors that are positively related to an olfactory imagery capacity, such as semantic knowledge, perceptual experience, and olfactory interest that may serve as potential moderators of the large individual variation.

Introduction

“We lay no great weight upon these results, though they are evidently in accord with those obtained with vision and audition” (Perky, 1910, p. 441). This statement summarized the opinion stated about the nature of olfactory imagery in one of the first studies targeting mental imagery. However, more than a century later the scientific evidence pertaining to our ability to form olfactory images is yet scarce although the topic has received an upsurge of interest during the past years. The aim of this work is to summarize the current findings from three angles; similarity, difference, and plasticity. First, we show that olfactory imagery shares many of the features known for visual and auditory imagery. Second, we propose that olfactory imagery is radically different in one important aspect; the large individual variation in the capacity to form olfactory images. Finally, we discuss factors that moderate the individual differences, such as semantic knowledge, perceptual experience, and olfactory interest.

Similarities Among Visual, Auditory, and Olfactory Imagery

Although some researchers have declared that we are unable to form olfactory images (Engen, 1982, 1991; Crowder and Schab, 1995; Herz, 2000), support for an olfactory imagery capacity is currently pervasive. The bulk of this work suggests that many features of the olfactory image are shared by visual and auditory imagery. Table 1 provides an overview of some of these features based on experimental observations across the olfactory, visual, and auditory modalities. For example, multidimensional scaling studies have demonstrated a correspondence between visual perception and imagery in judgments of shapes (e.g., Shepard and Chipman, 1970) and within audition, a strong association between perceived and imagined musical timbre has been documented (e.g., Intons-Peterson et al., 1992). In the olfactory domain, correspondences for pleasantness, intensity, and familiarity ratings have been established between olfactory perception and imagery (e.g., Carrasco and Ridout, 1993; Sugiyama et al., 2006). Moreover, studies have demonstrated that both visual (e.g., Craver-Lemley and Reeves, 1992) and auditory (e.g., Segal and Fusella, 1970) imagery can interfere with perceptual thresholds for the same imagery modality. Likewise, Djordjevic et al. (2004) observed that participants that were asked to imagine an odor and later presented with either the same or different odor were less able to detect the latter. A finding that proved modality-specific. In patient studies, clinical manifestations of visual, auditory, and olfactory hallucinations have been observed for a range of conditions, such as epilepsy (visual: Panayiotopoulos, 1999; auditory: Korsnes et al., 2010; olfactory: West and Doty, 1995), and as a result of cocaine abuse (Siegel, 1978). Dream studies have demonstrated sensory specific components included in visual (e.g., MacCarely and Hoffman, 1981), auditory (e.g., Zadra et al., 1998), and olfactory dream reports (e.g., Stevenson and Case, 2005a). Ocular motor activity in visual imagery (e.g., Laeng and Teodorescu, 2002), and subvocalization in auditory imagery (Aleman and Wout, 2004) have been demonstrated as important factors during imagery. Similarly, the peripheral motor act of sniffing have been shown to influence mental imagery in a modality specific manner, as a blocking of the nostrils decrease olfactory (Bensafi et al., 2003) but not visual imagery (Arshamian et al., 2008). Observations promoting the olfactory image as one of our imagery modalities can also be derived from brain research. For example, research indicates that stimulation of sensory specific brain areas can induce modality specific imagery in vision (Diederich and Goetz, 2000), audition (Moriarity et al., 2001) and in olfaction by electrical stimulation of the olfactory bulb and tract (Kumar et al., 2012).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Experimental observations corroborating olfactory imagery observations with the visual and auditory modalities.

Individual Differences in Olfactory Imagery

Although similar in many respects, the capacity to form olfactory images differs from that observed in visual and auditory imagery. For instance, only a minimal portion of the population is unable to create visual images (Kosslyn et al., 2006), whereas the olfactory modality is documented as the sense with the fewest instances of volitional imagery and with the highest frequency of individuals reporting that mental imagery never has occurred (Stevenson and Case, 2005b). Also, if an odor image is successfully produced it is typically experienced as less vivid than images generated from other modalities (Betts, 1909; Sheehan, 1967; White et al., 1978; Ashton and White, 1980). Also, Olivetti Belardinelli et al. (2009) demonstrated that self-rated reports of olfactory imagery vividness, unlike for example vividness ratings in visual or tactile imagery, did not correlate with modality specific brain activation. In an evolutionary context it is highly likely that the selection pressure for an imagery capacity was stronger for the visual and auditory systems among the early hominoids than for most other mammals. However, this circumstance does not entail that the capacity to form olfactory images reached extinction. A weaker selection pressure more likely resulted in a larger individual variation in the capacity to evoke olfactory images. Hence, the less vibrant olfactory image may be a direct result from an environment favoring proficient imagery abilities in the visual and auditory modalities. In this vein, it is of interest to note that Lawless (1997) reported that the frequencies of olfactory imagery (ranging from never to often) and the image vividness (ranging from 0 to 100%) were more normally distributed than visual and auditory imagery. For example, whereas all study participants had experienced a visual image a significant proportion reported never experiencing olfactory images. Also, the experienced vividness for visual and auditory images were heavily shifted towards vividness ratings over 75%, while more than half of the reported olfactory images had a vividness rating of 25% or less. Hence, an olfactory imagery capacity was probably of little survival value for the anatomically modern human. However, in animals, such as rats, where the olfactory sense is a main percept for survival, the capacity to form olfactory images appears exceptional. For example, April et al. (2013) demonstrated that rodent working memory capacity, as measured by odor span task, was in the magnitude of 72 stimuli, and that its structure more resembled an episodic-like memory. It has been hypothesized that the evolution of working memory, and thus imagery capacity, was partially evolved in the context of planning, recalling, and reasoning appropriately about food caching (see Carruthers, 2013 for a review). Thus, in contrast to rats the ability to evoke olfactory images probable had little, if any use for the modern humans with an evolved visual and auditory imagery capacity. However, as noted below, olfactory imagery may still play an important role in the everyday life.

Factors Moderating Olfactory Imagery Capacity

Most of the arguments raised for the inability to experience smells without external stimuli gain support from studies targeting differences found between olfaction and other sensory modalities. One example speaking to this view is that evidence is yet inconclusive regarding the nature of olfactory working memory in humans (Engen, 1991; Wilson and Stevenson, 2006; Zelano et al., 2009). Other concerns pertain to the well-documented difficulty to name odors, while the corresponding objects to these odors are easy to name when seen (Cain, 1979; Larsson et al., 1999; Olofsson et al., 2013). As a functional working memory capacity and semantic knowledge are considered as prerequisites for an imagery capacity in general, these two factors appear as fundamental for the integrity of olfactory imagery. Hence, activities that may promote the development of these factors, such as perceptual practice and odor-name learning, may contribute positively to the individual variation.

Stevenson et al. (2007) examined the relationship between odor identification and the ability to form odor images. The results showed that odors that were difficult to name also were difficult to imagine and that prior learning of the odor names exerted a positive effect on imagery capacity. Moreover, Tomiczek and Stevenson (2009) reported that odor imagery priming was prevalent only among good odor namers and appeared to be the result of a generic activation of olfactory neural networks when the participants tried to form an odor image. Importantly, Tomiczek and Stevenson (2009) suggested that this could occur in dependently of any consciously reported olfactory image. Thus, the act of trying to imagine an odor could result in a behavioral change that is not accompanied by a consciousness experience of that odor (Stevenson, 2009).

Other factors that have been linked to olfactory imagery are olfactory dreams and interest. For example, Stevenson and Case (2005a) explored factors such as odor interest, prevalence of odor dreams, and self-rated olfactory imagery in relation to olfactory performance. The results revealed that individuals who experienced olfactory dream content identified more odors correctly than non-olfactory dreamers. Concomitantly, prevalence of olfactory dreams was positively related to olfactory imagery capacity and a higher interest of odors in general. Moreover, Arshamian et al. (2011) selected individuals with either high or low olfactory awareness as indexed by rated imagery ability, prevalence of olfactory dreams, and odor interest. The results replicated and extended Stevenson and Case (2005a) by showing that high olfactory awareness not only was related to a more proficient spontaneous odor identification but also to a better retention of olfactory information as compared to the group with low awareness. Notably, the better episodic memory performance was not driven by a higher proficiency to verbalize information (Larsson, 1997; Larsson and Bäckman, 1997). Hence it is possible that persons experiencing olfactory dreams and have high olfactory interest may be less dependent on semantic processes when remembering odors. Moreover, the individual variation in interest may partially be attributed to differences in attraction and attention towards odors. For example, Bensafi and Rouby (2007) showed that individuals who scored high in olfactory imagery also had a higher ability to experience pleasure, and perceived pleasant odors as more pleasant and familiar than poor olfactory imagers.

Plasticity in Olfactory Imagery Capacity Among Novices and Experts

Studies indicate that indirect and moderate opportunities to stimulate olfactory imagery through perceptual exposure are effective. Recently, Bensafi et al. (2013) compared olfactory and auditory imagery in individuals that cooked on a daily basis with a group that played music and was musically trained with a group of control participants who neither cooked nor played any instruments. The results showed that individuals that cooked had shorter response times than musical and controls in judgments associated with olfactory imagery, but not auditory imagery, whereas response times in auditory imagery were shorter for the musical group. Hence, this observation suggests that indirect and moderate perceptual practice may exert positive effects on modality specific behavior.

Research focusing on training the sense of smell has mainly focused on wine experts and perfumers (e.g., Lawless, 1984; Melcher and Schooler, 1996; Parr et al., 2002; Plailly et al., 2012). One observation is that olfactory experts, such as perfumers, exhibit a higher volitional olfactory imagery capacity than novices (Gilbert et al., 1998) and that the skills primarily result from a higher conceptual knowledge, rather than an inherent higher chemosensory sensitivity (De Beni et al., 2007). For example, Melcher and Schooler (1996) reported that wine experts compared to novices performed better in a “triangle test” where one target wine had to be picked out from a group of three. Experts and novices had to verbally describe the target wine before picking it out after a 4-min retention interval. Whereas verbalization did not affect wine experts in recognition, the novices showed impaired wine recognition. Similarly, it has been shown that wine experts are less susceptible to verbal overshadowing than novices (Parr et al., 2002). Several studies report that the superior performance of wine experts is largely determined by their ability to form appropriate verbal descriptors that focuses on the sensory quality (Lawless, 1984). In this vein, Engen and Ross (1973) reported that odor memory decreased if participants gave loosely related verbal labels to the odors compared to odors that were not labeled. In line with this idea, Fiore et al. (2012) tested if short-term memory for flavors could be influenced by olfactory imagery and the usage of appropriate verbal labels in amateurs. The results showed that imagination of a wine flavor with descriptive oenological adjectives, enhanced memory for the specific wine. In contrast, Parr et al. (2002) observed that wine experts performed better in odor recognition memory, although there were no group differences in odor identification and verbal memory. Hence, verbal codes were not necessary for a better recognition among experts suggesting the use of other strategies (cf. Arshamian et al., 2011).

However, not only conceptual odor knowledge shows positive benefits from training. Plailly et al. (2012) used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to study changes in functional activity as a function of extensive olfactory training. Student and professional perfumers were presented with odor names and were asked to create an olfactory image for each odor name. In general, the anterior part of the piriform cortex appeared as a crucial area for olfactory imagery, although students showed more activation in the posterior part of the piriform cortex. This indicated that that the two groups used different strategies when generating odor images. Interestingly, the duration of work experience in perfumers also modified the neural activity. A longer work experience was related to less brain activity in areas associated with olfactory imagery and perception (i.e., piriform cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and the hippocampus). This type of experience-induced decrease in functional brain activity has been reported for other modalities, such as vision (Maguire et al., 2002) and audition (Ohnishi et al., 2001). However, caution should be made when drawing conclusions from olfactory cortex activity alone as several other factors, such as sniffing (Sobel et al., 1998), semantic labels denoting odors (González et al., 2006), cross-modal reactivation (Gottfried et al., 2002, 2004), and attention towards odors (Zelano et al., 2005, 2011) may activate olfactory cortex. Hence, activity in olfactory cortex may be conceived as a necessary, but not a sufficient condition for the integrity of olfactory imagery (see Royet et al., 2013, for a review). Plailly et al. (2012) also reported that the inferior temporal gyrus, an area involved in semantic memory processing (Irish et al., 2012), decreased its activity with increasing expertise. This observation may reflect that generation of an olfactory image is subserved by semantic memory, but that with more extensive olfactory knowledge the retrieval gets less dependent on semantic feedback. A follow-up study also demonstrated that the structural brain images were modified with olfactory expertise”(Delon-Martin et al., 2013). Specifically, perfumers had larger gray-matter volumes in areas associated with olfactory processing, which included the bilateral gyrus rectus/medial orbital gyrus and the anterior cingulate. Further, the gray-matter volume increased with experience in the primary olfactory cortex and in the left rectus/medial orbital gyrus. No differences in areas involved in semantic processing were reported suggesting that structural changes following extensive perceptual experience, and to some extent olfactory imagery training, were restricted to modality-specific areas, such as primary and secondary olfactory cortices.

Odor Imagery in Persons with Smell Loss

Flohr et al. (Submitted) investigated the relationship between olfactory loss and the capacity to form olfactory images. Patients with olfactory loss and a control group with a normal sense of smell performed odor imagery tasks in the fMRI whilst also factors that could potentially activate olfactory cortex (e.g., sniffing) were controlled for. The study took advantage of results from studies indicating that odor imagery mimics that of olfactory perception. Specifically, both unpleasant odors and their mental images induce stronger activity in the piriform cortex and insula as compared to activity related to pleasant odors and their respective images (Bensafi et al., 2007). The results from Flohr et al. (submitted) showed that although patients with olfactory loss showed activity in areas associated with olfactory imagery, it was, unlike the control group, not related to the hedonic quality to-be-imagined. Also, the longer the duration of the smell loss the more activity in regions associated with olfactory imagery was observed. Thus, olfactory loss shows a reverse activation pattern than that observed among perfumers, which showed less activity with increasing experience (Plailly et al., 2012). The conclusion was that patients with olfactory loss were unable to evoke olfactory images similar to controls and that a regular exposure to olfactory information is crucial for successful imagery and that there may be a gradual memory loss of olfactory representations over time.

Concluding Remarks

The capacity to form olfactory images in the normal population should be regarded as a continuous factor. At the opposite ends, individuals with anosmia and olfactory experts are located. Severe olfactory impairment and anosmia are associated with reductions in accessing conscious odor information whereas olfactory expertise is linked to a fluent and conscious retrieval of olfactory information (Flohr et al., submitted; Plailly et al., 2012). The majority of the population is, however, located at an intermediate position, where difficulties in experiencing and recreating an odor into a conscious image are typical. However, a continuous perceptual stimulation and exposure to olfactory information may eventually increase the likelihood to be able to recreate conscious olfactory percepts in the mind.

In conclusion, this overview suggests that the olfactory image shares many features with visual and auditory imagery although some major differences are evident. The most prominent discrepancy concerns the large individual differences reported for our capacity to reproduce a smell with our inner nose. Here, factors such as the identity of the odor, odor interest, and perceptual experience were discussed as potential moderators of the individual variation.

Author Contributions

Artin Arshamian and Maria Larsson jointly wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

This work was supported by a grant from the Swedish Research Council (421-2011-1792) to Maria Larsson.

References

Aleman, A., and Wout, M. V. T. (2004). Subvocalization in auditory-verbal imagery: just a form of motor imagery? Cogn. Process. 5, 228–231. doi: 10.1007/s10339-004-0034-y

CrossRef Full Text

Algom, D., and Cain, W. S. (1991). Remembered odors and mental mixtures: tapping reservoirs of olfactory knowledge. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 17, 1104–1119. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.17.4.1104

CrossRef Full Text

April, L. B., Bruce, K., and Galizio, M. (2013). The magic number 70 (plus or minus 20): variables determining performance in the rodent odor span task. Learn. Motiv. 44, 143–158. doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2013.03.00

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Arshamian, A., Olofsson, J. K., Jönsson, F. U., and Larsson, M. (2008). Sniff your way to clarity: the case of olfactory imagery. Chemosens. Percept. 1, 242–246. doi: 10.1007/s12078-008-9035-z

CrossRef Full Text

Arshamian, A., Willander, J., and Larsson, M. (2011). Olfactory awareness is positively associated to odour memory. J. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 220–226. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.483226

CrossRef Full Text

Ashton, R., and White, K. D. (1980). Sex differences in imagery vividness: an artifact of the test. Br. J. Psychol. 71, 35–38. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb02726.x

CrossRef Full Text

Bayard, M., Mcintyre, J., Hill, K. R., and Woodsidem, J. Jr. (2004). Alcohol withdrawal syndrome. Am. Fam. Phys.69, 1443–1450.

Bensafi, M., Porter, J., Pouliot, S., Mainland, J., Johnson, B., Zelano, C., et al. (2003). Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1142–1144. doi: 10.1038/nn1145

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., and Rouby, C. (2007). Individual differences in odor imaging ability reflect differences in olfactory and emotional perception. Chem. Senses 32, 237–244. doi: 10.1093/chemse/bjl051

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., Sobel, N., and Khan, R. M. (2007). Hedonic-specific activity in piriform cortex during odor imagery mimics that during odor perception. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 3254–3262. doi: 10.1152/jn.00349.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bensafi, M., Tillmann, B., Poncelet, J., Przybylski, L., and Rouby, C. (2013). “Olfactory and gustatory mental imagery: modulation by sensory experience and comparison to auditory mental imagery”, in Multisensory Imagery, eds S. Lacey and R. Lawson (New York: Springer), 77–91. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-5879-1_5

CrossRef Full Text

Betts, G. H. (1909). The Distribution and Functions of Mental Imagery. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.

Bracha, H. S., Wolkowitz, O. M., Lohr, J. B., Karson, C. N., and Bigelow, L. B. (1989). High prevalence of visual hallucinations in research subjects with chronic schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 146, 526–528.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Brandt, S. A., and Stark, L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 27–38. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.1.27

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cain, W. S. (1979). To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science 203, 467–470. doi: 0.1126/science.760202

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carrasco, M., and Ridout, J. B. (1993). Olfactory perception and olfactory imagery: a multidimensional analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 19, 287–301. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.19.2.287

CrossRef Full Text

Carruthers, P. (2013). Evolution of working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 10371–10378. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301195110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Craver-Lemley, C., and Reeves, A. (1992). How visual imagery interferes with vision. Psychol. Rev. 99, 633–649. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.633

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Crowder, R. G., and Schab, F. R. (1995). “Imagery for odors”, in Memory for Odors, eds R. G. Crowder and F. R. Schab (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 93–107.

De Beni, R., Cornoldi, C., Larsson, M., Magnussen, S., and Rönnberg, J. (2007). “Memory in experts: visual learning, wine tasting, orienteering, and speech-reading,” in Everyday Memory, eds T. Helstrup and S. Magnussen (Hove: Psychological Press), 201–228.

Delon-Martin, C., Plailly, J., Fonlupt, P., Veyrac, A., and Royet, J. P. (2013). Perfumers’ expertise induces structural reorganization in olfactory brain regions. Neuroimage 68, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.044

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Diederich, N. J., and Goetz, C. G. (2000). Visual hallucinations induced by deep brain stimulation in Parkinson’s disease. Clin. Neuropharmacol. 23, 287–289. doi: 10.1097/00002826-200009000-00010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Djordjevic, J., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M., Boyle, J. A., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2005). Functional neuroimaging of odor imagery. Neuroimage 24, 791–801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Djordjevic, J., Zatorre, R. J., Petrides, M., and Jones-Gotman, M. (2004). The mind’s nose effects of odor and visual imagery on odor detection. Psychol. Sci. 15, 143–148. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503001.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Engen, T. (1982). The Perception of Odors. New York: Academic Press.

Engen, T. (1991). Odor Sensation and Memory. Santa Barbara, CA: Greenwood Publishing Group.

Engen, T., and Ross, B. M. (1973). Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal descriptions. J. Exp. Psychol. 100, 221–227. doi: 10.1037/h0035492

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fiore, F., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., D’Urso, V., Eilertsen, D. E., and Magnussen, S. (2012). Short-term memory for flavour. J. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 134–139. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2011.598854

CrossRef Full Text

Fuller, G. N., and Guiloff, R. J. (1987). Migrainous olfactory hallucinations. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 50, 1688–1690. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.50.12.1688

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ganis, G., Thompson, W. L., and Kosslyn, S. M. (2004). Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. Cogn. Brain Res. 20, 226–241. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.02.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

George, L., and Neufeld, R. W. J. (1985). Cognition and symptomatology in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 11, 264–284. doi: 10.1093/schbul/11.2.264

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gilbert, A. N., Crouch, M., and Kemp, S. E. (1998). Olfactory and visual mental imagery. J. Ment. Imag. 22, 137–146.

Gobet, F., and Simon, H. A. (1996). Recall of rapidly presented random chess positions is a function of skill. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 3, 159–163. doi: 10.3758/BF03200937

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

González, J., Barros-Loscertales, A., Pulvermüller, F., Meseguer, V., Sanjuán, A., Belloch, V., et al. (2006). Reading cinnamon activates olfactory brain regions. Neuroimage 32, 906–912. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.03.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottfried, J. A., O’Doherty J., and Dolan R. J. (2002). Appetitive and aversive olfactory learning in humans studied using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. J. Neurosci. 22, 10829–10837.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gottfried, J. A., Smith, A. P., Rugg, M. D., and Dolan, R. J. (2004). Remembrance of odors past: human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron 42, 687–695.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Groussard, M., La Joie, R., Rauchs, G., Landeau, B., Chételat, G., Viader, F., et al. (2010). When music and long-term memory interact: effects of musical expertise on functional and structural plasticity in the hippocampus. PLoS ONE 5:e13225.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halpern, A. R., Zatorre, R. J., Bouffard, M., and Johnson, J. A. (2004). Behavioral and neural correlates of perceived and imagined musical timbre. Neuropsychologia 42, 1281–1292.doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Herz, R. S. (2000). Verbal coding in olfactory versus nonolfactory cognition. Mem. Cognit. 28, 957–964. doi: 10.3758/BF03209343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory imagery: empirical findings. Psychol. Bull. 136, 302–329. doi: 10.1037/a0018436

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Intons-Peterson, M. J., Russell, W., and Dressel, S. (1992). The role of pitch in auditory imagery. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 18, 233–240. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.1.233

CrossRef Full Text

Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R., and Piguet, O. (2012). Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: evidence from semantic dementia. Brain 135, 2178–2191. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws119

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Janata, P., and Paroo, K. (2006). Acuity of auditory images in pitch and time. Percept. Psychophys. 68, 829–844. doi: 10.3758/BF03193705

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Korsnes, M. S., Hugdahl, K., Nygård, M., and Bjørnæs, H. (2010). An fMRI study of auditory hallucinations in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsia 51, 610–617. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2009.02338.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., and Ganis, G. (2006). The case for mental imagery. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195179088.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text

Kumar, G., Juhász, C., Sood, S., and Asano, E. (2012). Olfactory hallucinations elicited by electrical stimulation via subdural electrodes: effects of direct stimulation of olfactory bulb and tract. Epilepsy Behav. 24, 264–268. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.03.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laeng, B., and Teodorescu, D. S. (2002). Eye scan paths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cogn. Sci. 26, 207–231. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2602_3

CrossRef Full Text

Larsson, M. (1997). Semantic factors in episodic recognition of common odors: a review. Chem. Senses 22, 623–633. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.6.623

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Larsson, M., and Bäckman, L. (1997). Age-related differences in episodic odour recognition: The role of access to specific odour names. Memory 5, 361–378. doi: 10.1080/741941391

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Larsson, M., Semb, H., Winblad, B., Amberla, K., Wahlund, L.-O., and Bäckman, L. (1999). Odor identification in normal aging and early Alzheimer’s disease: effects of retrieval support. Neuropsychology 13, 1–7. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.13.1.47

CrossRef Full Text

Lawless, H. T. (1984). Flavor description of white wine by “expert” and nonexpert wine consumers. J. Food Sci. 49, 120–123. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2621.1984.tb13686.x

CrossRef Full Text

Lawless, H. T. (1997). “Olfactory psychophysics,” in Tasting and Smelling, eds G. K. Beauchamp and L. Bartoshuk (San Diego: Academic Press), 125–175. doi: 10.1016/B978-012161958-9/50005-1

CrossRef Full Text

Lee, B., Park, J. Y., Jung, W. H., Kim, H. S., Oh, J. S., Choi, C. H., et al. (2010). White matter neuroplastic changes in long-term trained players of the game of “Baduk”(GO): a voxel-based diffusion-tensor imaging study. Neuroimage 52, 9–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacCarely, R. W., and Hoffman, E. (1981). REM sleep dreams and activation-synthesis hypothesis. Am. J. Psychol. 138, 904–912.

Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., and Kapur, N. (2002). Routes to remembering: the brains behind superior memory. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 90–95. doi: 10.1038/nn988

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marks, D. F. (1973). Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br. J. Psychol. 64, 17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Melcher, J. M., and Schooler, J. W. (1996). The misremembrance of wines past: verbal and perceptual expertise differentially mediate verbal overshadowing of taste memory. J. Mem. Lang. 35, 231–245. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0013.

CrossRef Full Text

Moriarity, J. L., Boatman, D., Krauss, G. L., Storm, P. B., and Lenz, F. A. (2001). Human “memories” can be evoked by stimulation of the lateral temporal cortex after ipsilateral medial temporal lobe resection. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 71, 549–551. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.71.4.549

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Asada, T., Aruga, M., Hirakata, M., Nishikawa, M., et al.(2001). Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cereb. Cortex 11, 754-760. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.8.754

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olivetti Belardinelli, M., Palmiero, M., Sestieri, C., Nardo, D., Di Matteo, R., Londei, A., et al. (2009). An fMRI investigation on image generation in different sensory modalities: the influence of vividness. Acta Psychol. 132, 190–200. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Olofsson, J. K., Rogalski, E., Harrison, T., Mesulam, M. M., and Gottfried, J. A. (2013). A cortical pathway to olfactory naming: evidence from primary progressive aphasia. Brain 136, 1245–1259. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Panayiotopoulos, C. P. (1999). Elementary visual hallucinations, blindness, and headache in idiopathic occipital epilepsy: differentiation from migraine. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 66, 536–540. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.66.4.536

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Parr, W. V., Heatherbell, D., and White, K. G. (2002). Demystifying wine expertise: olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine judges. Chem. Senses 27, 747–755. doi: 10.1093/chemse/27.8.747

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Perky, C. W. (1910). An experimental study of imagination. Am. J. Psychol. 21, 422–452. doi: 10.2307/1413350

CrossRef Full Text

Plailly, J., Delon-Martin, C., and Royet, J. P. (2012). Experience induces functional reorganization in brain regions involved in odor imagery in perfumers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 224–234. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21207

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Royet, J. P., Delon-Martin, C., and Plailly, J. (2013). Odor mental imagery in non-experts in odors: a paradox? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:87. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rubin, D., McAbee, G. N., and Feldman-Winter, L. B. (2002). Auditory hallucinations associated with migraine. Headache 42, 646–648. doi: 10.1046/j.1526-4610.2002.02153.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Saravay, S. M., and Pardes, H. (1967). Auditory elementary hallucinations in alcohol withdrawal psychosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 16, 652–658. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1967.01730240008002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schott, G. D. (2007). Exploring the visual hallucinations of migraine aura: the tacit contribution of illustration. Brain 130, 1690–1703. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl348

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Segal, S. J., and Fusella, V. (1970). Influence of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals. J. Exp. Psychol. 83, 458–464. doi: 10.1037/h0028840

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sheehan, P. W. (1967). A shortened form of betts’ questionnaire upon mental imagery. J. Clin. Psychol. 23, 386–389. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(196707)23:3 < 386::AID-JCLP2270230328 > 3.0.CO;2-S

CrossRef Full Text

Shepard, R. N., and Chipman, S. (1970). Second-order isomorphism of internal representations: shapes of states. Cogn. Psychol. 1, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(70)90002-2

CrossRef Full Text

Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171, 701–703. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Siegel, R. K. (1978). Cocaine hallucinations. Am. J. Psychiatry 135, 309–314. doi: 10.1126/science.ns-14.354.332-c

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sobel, N., Prabhakaran, V., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., Goode, R. L., Sullivan, E. V., et al. (1998). Sniffing and smelling: separate subsystems in the human olfactory cortex. Nature 392, 282–286. doi: 10.1038/32654

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stedman, T. J., and Clair, A. L. (1998). Neuropsychological, neurological and symptom correlates of impaired olfactory identification in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 32, 23–30. doi. 10.1016/S0920-9964(98)00021-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevenson, R. J. (2009). Phenomenal and access consciousness in olfaction. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 1004–1017. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2005a). Olfactory dreams: phenomenology, relationship to volitional imagery and odor identification. Imagin. Cogn. Pers. 24, 69–90. doi: 10.2190/R97G-RM98-B58N-DUYA

CrossRef Full Text

Stevenson, R. J., and Case, T. I. (2005b). Olfactory imagery: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 244–264. doi: 10.3758/BF03196369

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevenson, R. J., Case, T. I., and Mahmut, M. (2007). Difficulty in evoking odor images: the role of odor naming. Mem. Cognit. 35, 578–589. doi: 10.3758/BF03193296

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stevenson, R. J., and Langdon, R. (2012). “Olfactory and gustatory hallucinations,” in Hallucinations: Research and Practice, eds J. D. Blom and I. E. C. Sommer’ (Amsterdam: Springer), 143–155. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-0959-5_12

Sugiyama, H., Ayabe-Kanamura, S., and Kikuchi, T. (2006). Are olfactory images sensory in nature? Perception 35, 1699–1708. doi: 10.1068/p5453

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tomiczek, C., and Stevenson, R. J. (2009). Olfactory imagery and repetition priming: the effect of odor naming and imagery ability. Exp. Psychol. 56, 397–408. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.397

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

West, S. E., and Doty, R. L. (1995). Influence of epilepsy and temporal lobe resection on olfactory function. Epilepsia 36, 531–542. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1995.tb02565.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

White, K. D., Ashton, R., and Brown, R. M. D. (1977). The measurement of imagery vividness: normative data and their relationship to sex, age, and modality differences. Br. J. Psychol. 68, 203–211. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1977.tb01576.x

CrossRef Full Text

White, K. D., Ashton, R., and Law, H. (1978). The measurement of imagery vividness: effects of format and order on the Betts’ Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 10, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/h0081537

CrossRef Full Text

Willander, J., and Baraldi, S. (2010). Development of a new clarity of auditory imagery scale. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 785–790. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.785

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wilson, D. A., and Stevenson, R. J. (2006). Learning to Smell: Olfactory Perception from Neurobiology to Behavior. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

Zadra, A. L., Nielsen, T. A., and Donderi, D. C. (1998). Prevalence of auditory, olfactory, and gustatory experiences in home dreams. Percept. Mot. Skills 87, 819–826. doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.87.3.819

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zelano, C., Bensafi, M., Porter, J., Mainland, J., Johnson, B., Bremner, E., et al. (2005). Attentional modulation in human primary olfactory cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 114–120. doi: 10.1038/nn1368

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zelano, C., Mohanty, A., and Gottfried, J. A. (2011). Olfactory predictive codes and stimulus templates in piriform cortex. Neuron 72, 178–187. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zelano, C., Montag, J., Khan, R., and Sobel, N. (2009). A specialized odor memory buffer in primary olfactory cortex. PloS ONE 4:e4965. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004965

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: imagery, olfaction, consciousness, comparative, expertise

Citation: Arshamian A and Larsson M (2014) Same same but different: the case of olfactory imagery. Front. Psychol. 5:34. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00034

Received: 11 September 2013; Accepted: 10 January 2014;
Published online: 03 February 2014.

Edited by:

Andreas Keller, Rockefeller University, USA

Reviewed by:

Moustafa Bensafi, Université de Lyon, France
Jean-Pierre Royet, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, France

Copyright © 2014 Arshamian and Larsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Artin Arshamian, Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Frescati Hagväg 9A, 106 91 Stockholm, Sweden e-mail: artin.arshamian@psychology.su.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.