Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 October 2013
Sec. Auditory Cognitive Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic What we learn and when we learn it: sensitive periods in development View all 7 articles

Benefits and detriments of unilateral cochlear implant use on bilateral auditory development in children who are deaf

  • 1Archie’s Cochlear Implant Laboratory, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Institute of Medical Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Otolaryngology – Head and Neck surgery, Faculty of Medicine, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

We have explored both the benefits and detriments of providing electrical input through a cochlear implant in one ear to the auditory system of young children. A cochlear implant delivers electrical pulses to stimulate the auditory nerve, providing children who are deaf with access to sound. The goals of implantation are to restrict reorganization of the deprived immature auditory brain and promote development of hearing and spoken language. It is clear that limiting the duration of deprivation is a key factor. Additional considerations are the onset, etiology, and use of residual hearing as each of these can have unique effects on auditory development in the pre-implant period. New findings show that many children receiving unilateral cochlear implants are developing mature-like brainstem and thalamo-cortical responses to sound with long term use despite these sources of variability; however, there remain considerable abnormalities in cortical function. The most apparent, determined by implanting the other ear and measuring responses to acute stimulation, is a loss of normal cortical response from the deprived ear. Recent data reveal that this can be avoided in children by early implantation of both ears simultaneously or with limited delay. We conclude that auditory development requires input early in development and from both ears.

Introduction

A cochlear implant is an auditory prosthesis which is surgically implanted into the cochlea (inner ear), and allows children who are deaf to develop oral speech and language. Because the brain is most susceptible to changes in early life, providing access to sound at a young age is essential to promote auditory development (Papsin and Gordon, 2007; Kral and O’Donoghue, 2010). The implant cannot restore normal hearing. It provides only a crude representation of acoustic sounds, eliminates important cochlear processing, and may not be able to completely reverse the effects of deafness. In addition, cochlear implants were traditionally provided unilaterally (i.e., in only one ear) in children, leaving the opposite pathways deprived of input and susceptible to degeneration and reorganization (O’Neil et al., 2010; Gordon et al., 2013; Kral et al., 2013). Yet, despite these disadvantages, many children achieve excellent listening and oral communication abilities. In the present review, we share findings from studies exploring whether cochlear implantation can limit reorganization of the deprived immature auditory brain and promote appropriate and normal-like development along the auditory pathways.

The Auditory System Reorganizes When Bilaterally Deprived

Prior to cochlear implantation, the absence of auditory input to the auditory system leaves the brain vulnerable to reorganization (Nishimura et al., 1999; Bavelier et al., 2000, 2006; Finney et al., 2001; Lee et al., 2001; Bavelier and Neville, 2002; Merabet and Pascual-Leone, 2010). Secondary and association auditory areas, including parts of the planum temporale, all of which respond to multi-sensory input including hearing, vision and touch (Pandya and Yeterian, 1985; Giard and Peronnet, 1999; Calvert et al., 2001; Calvert and Thesen, 2004), become recruited by the visual (Finney et al., 2001; Lee et al., 2001, 2007b; Lomber et al., 2010; Meredith and Lomber, 2011) and somatosensory (Levänen et al., 1998; Levänen and Hamdorf, 2001; Auer et al., 2007; Meredith and Lomber, 2011) systems to perform non-auditory functions. As a consequence of early auditory deprivation, processing of visual peripheral localization by the posterior auditory field (Lomber et al., 2010), visual motion detection by the dorsal zone of the auditory cortex (Lomber et al., 2010), and somatosensory sensation by the anterior auditory field (Meredith and Lomber, 2011) become enhanced in individuals who are deaf. These changes appear to result from a direct competition for resources in areas which receive multi-sensory input. If governed by principals of Hebbian processing (Hebb, 1949; Abbott and Nelson, 2000; Song et al., 2000), neurons in these areas might preferentially form viable connections with non-auditory inputs to the detriment of inputs carrying auditory information. We must be concerned by the reorganization of the deaf auditory cortex because, depending on how quickly these processes occur, they may be impossible to reverse and could impair outcomes after cochlear implantation. It is also becoming clear that these changes do not occur uniformly in children who are deaf and may be related to the heterogeneity in the onset and cause of pediatric deafness (Gordon et al., 2011a,c).

Limiting the period of bilateral deafness in early life is essential to drive maturation in the auditory pathways (O’Donoghue, 1999; Kral et al., 2001; Ponton and Eggermont, 2001; Sharma et al., 2005; Papsin and Gordon, 2007; Gordon et al., 2008a, 2010; Nikolopoulos et al., 2009), and promote optimal hearing and speech and language development (Beadle et al., 2005; Harrison et al., 2005; Nicholas and Geers, 2007; Geers and Sedey, 2011). Many studies investigating auditory development after cochlear implantation focus on children who are deaf in infancy, but do not examine the larger heterogeneity in etiology, onset and/or degree of deafness. These factors may each have unique effects on auditory activity in the brain prior to implantation. For example, biallelic mutations of the Gap Junction Beta-2 (GJB-2) gene causes deficits in the cochlea at likely very early stages of development with possible consequences for auditory function after implantation (Propst et al., 2006). The GJB-2 gene normally codes for the connexin-26 protein, which creates gap junctions in the cochlea necessary for the appropriate release and maintenance of electrochemical gradients. This in turn, generates action potentials and stimulates the auditory nerve (Kelley et al., 1998; Cohn and Kelley, 1999; Gualandi et al., 2002). Electrophysiological recordings of auditory evoked cortical activity at initial cochlear implant activation in children with severe GJB-2 mutations revealed that responses from the cortex were more homogenous in this cohort compared to those children who did not have such a mutation. Auditory evoked cortical responses in children with GJB-2 mutations were characteristic of earlier stages of cortical development, perhaps reflecting restricted spontaneous activity in the auditory system and more limited access to sound prior to implantation compared to their peers who did not have a GJB-2 related deafness (Gordon et al., 2011c). This was further supported by poorer hearing sensitivity in the low frequencies in the GJB-2 group (Propst et al., 2006).

The degree of residual hearing is another important predictive factor for cochlear implant outcomes. Traditional candidacy criteria for cochlear implantation in children include a diagnosis of permanent severe-to-profound hearing loss bilaterally with little or limited access to acoustic input through hearing aids (Osberger et al., 2002). We recently reported that children who had better hearing at 250 Hz used their hearing aids for longer durations prior to receiving a cochlear implant (Hopyan et al., 2012). Of interest, these children performed significantly better on tests of music perception with their implants, particularly when detecting differences in rhythm, compared to children who did not have acoustical access to these low frequencies prior to implantation (Hopyan et al., 2012). Thus, there are advantages of acoustical input for auditory development which can be capitalized upon after cochlear implantation. In general, we are learning that the cause, onset and degree of deafness in any one child will be important to understand in order to ensure that he/she makes the best possible use of his/her device.

Unilateral Cochlear Implantation Restores Hearing and Promotes Auditory Development

The cochlear implant was made available to children in North America in the early 1990s and works by stimulating the auditory pathways with electrical pulses. The implant contains an array of electrodes which is surgically placed in the scala tympani of the cochlea. These electrodes each deliver electrical pulses to stimulate the auditory nerve. External equipment is worn which takes in acoustic sound through the microphone, extracts frequency and intensity information in a speech processor and sends instructions to an internal device through an FM transmitting coil. The internal receiver-stimulator sends this information to the electrodes which are organized to mimic the normal cochlea; high frequency sounds are allocated to basal electrodes with lower frequencies being allocated to progressively more apical electrodes. In this way, the child receives an electrical representation of the acoustic world and learns to understand sounds including speech.

Auditory brainstem development, measured by decreasing latencies of evoked potential peaks, is largely complete by the first year of cochlear implant use in children with early onset deafness (Gordon et al., 2003, 2006), indicating increasing efficiency of neural conduction and improved neural synchrony with exposure to sound (Gordon et al., 2003). Similar changes have been reported from the auditory brainstems of normal hearing children over a similar time-course (Salamy and McKean, 1976; Starr et al., 1977; Jerger and Hall, 1980; Salamy, 1984; Hecox and Burkard, 2006). Data from Gordon et al. (2006) is shown in Figure 1A; on the left is an example of an electrically evoked auditory brainstem response. The stimulus artifact is shown at time 0 ms followed by waves eII, eIII and eV, and on the right, the latency values of wave eV are plotted at initial device activation and over the first year following cochlear implant use in 44 children who had early onset deafness and were implanted unilaterally (Gordon et al., 2006). Recently, we recorded these same responses in two children who were in the original study once they had over a decade of unilateral cochlear implant experience. Their responses are shown in Figures 1B,C (Jiwani et al., 2011). In both cases, wave eV latency clearly decreases over the first year of cochlear implant use, with no further changes thereafter. This suggests that activity in auditory brainstem is largely complete by the first year (Gordon et al., 2006).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A) Example of an electrically evoked auditory brainstem response waveform is shown on the left. The onset of the cochlear implant artifact is shown at time 0 ms, followed by peaks eII, eIII and eV. Data from Gordon et al. (2006) are plotted on the right and show the mean wave eV latency values of 44 children recorded at initial activation of the implant, and at months 2, 6 and 12 following unilateral cochlear implantation. (B,C) on the right show the changes in the brainstem responses of two children who were in the original study (Gordon et al., 2006), recorded from initial activation of the device to different intervals over the first year of cochlear implantation use. New responses recorded after 10 years of unilateral cochlear implant experience are also shown (Jiwani et al., 2011), further confirming that little change in the eV latency occurs beyond the first year of implant use. The wave eV latencies at each time-point are represented on the right for each child.

Further studies concentrated on the development of cortical auditory activity in children with time after cochlear implantation. Cochlear implants provided to children who are congenitally deaf within 3.5 years of bilateral deafness promote age-appropriate cortical responses over the first 3–6 months of implant use (Sharma et al., 2002a). After this initial period, these responses change at a rate which is similar to normal (Eggermont et al., 1997; Eggermont and Ponton, 2003). We recently assessed changes in cortical responses after longer term unilateral cochlear implant use in children who were implanted early (Jiwani et al., 2013a). Grand mean cortical evoked responses from 79 unilateral cochlear implant users (red waveforms) are plotted in Figure 2 along with the grand mean responses from 58 normal hearing peers (black waveforms) for different intervals of hearing experience. Figures 2AC show grand mean cortical evoked waveforms from children who have between 0 and 7 years (40 cochlear implant users; 11 normal hearing), 7 to 12 years (21 cochlear implant users; 18 normal hearing) and over 12 years (18 cochlear implant users; 29 normal hearing) of hearing experience, respectively. Cochlear implant users represented in these Figures had limited durations of bilateral deafness prior to implantation (2.03 ± 1.36 years) with typical heterogeneity in their etiologies of deafness.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Grand mean cortical evoked responses from 79 cochlear implant users (red waveform; Jiwani et al., 2013a) are plotted for children who have (A) under 7 years of hearing experience (4.3 ± 1.7 years; n = 40), (B) between 7 and 12 years of hearing experience (9.4 ± 1.6 years; n = 21), and (C) those who have more than 12 years of hearing experience (13.8 ± 0.9 years; n = 18). Mean responses for each range of hearing experience are compared to a normal and mature cortical waveform (black waveform; Jiwani et al., 2013a), recorded from normal hearing peers who are matched for hearing age (n = 58).

As shown in Figure 2A, responses from children with up to 7 years of hearing experience with an implant or with normal bilateral hearing are dominated by a large and broad positive amplitude peak, labeled P1/P2. Comparison of peak latencies (t(47.3) = -1.63; p > 0.05) and amplitudes (t(42.1) = -0.64; p > 0.05) reveal no significant differences between the two groups. This positive-peaked response is believed to reflect either excitatory auditory activity from the thalamus to deep layers of the auditory cortex (Liegeois-Chauvel et al., 1994), or auditory driven activity from association auditory areas to the reticular activating system in the non-lemniscal auditory pathways (Kraus et al., 1992; Ponton et al., 2000; Ponton and Eggermont, 2001). As thalamo-cortical and cortico-cortical connections develop around 9 to 12 years of age in superficial layers of the auditory cortex, a small negative amplitude peak, labeled N1, develops in the cortical evoked response and bifurcates the large P1/P2 response into three peaks: P1, N1 and P2. Similar developmental changes to the cortical response are observed in early implanted cochlear implant users who have equal durations of hearing experience. Indeed, as shown in Figure 2B, with 7 to 12 years of auditory experience (9.38 ± 1.57 years in cochlear implant users; 9.92 ± 1.57 years in normal hearing individuals), the cortical response in both groups begins to develop into a polyphasic waveform. The grand mean response from all 21 unilaterally implanted children begins to bifurcate into a 3-peaked cortical response at this stage of implant use (Figure 2B). Differences in the wavepeak latencies (P1: t(10) = -0.88, p > 0.05; N1: t(10.18) = -1.3, p > 0.05; P2: t(10.77) = 1.43, p > 0.05) and peak-to-peak amplitudes (P1-N1: t(6.87) = 1.75, p > 0.05; N1-P2: t(10.67) = 2.2, p > 0.05) between both groups were not significant. This response continues to develop with time. As auditory pathways mature in the auditory cortex, peaks P1-N1-P2-N2 become clearly present (Figure 2C) when auditory experience exceeds 12 years in all 18 cochlear implant users (13.81 ± 0.92 years of unilateral implant experience) and 29 normal hearing peers (15.30 ± 1.81 years of age and hearing) (Jiwani et al., 2013a).

The data from individuals with normal hearing shown in Figure 2 is consistent with findings by Ponton, Eggermont and colleagues who suggested that peak N1 normally emerges around 9 to 12 years of age reflecting maturation of thalamo-cortical and cortico-cortical loops in superficial layers of the auditory cortex (Ponton et al., 2000; Eggermont and Ponton, 2003). These pathways mediate the transfer of primary auditory and multi-sensory input from the thalamus to various regions of the ipsilateral and contralateral auditory cortices (Winer et al., 2001, 2005; Razak et al., 2009), and the transmission of information from the auditory cortex to primary and secondary sensory areas in both hemispheres (Read et al., 2002; Lee and Winer, 2005; Klinge et al., 2010). The developmental trajectory of the electrically evoked cortical waveform suggests that similar development is taking place in children using cochlear implant (Jiwani et al., 2013a), perhaps establishing: (1) appropriate relay of auditory input from the ear to the cortex, via the thalamus; (2) communication between the two cortical hemispheres; and/or (3) connectivity between different sensory areas (Jiwani et al., 2013a). These normal-like developmental changes to the auditory cortex may underlie the impressive improvements in auditory function observed with cochlear implant use over time (Beadle et al., 2005; Nicholas and Geers, 2007; Geers and Sedey, 2011).

Differences From Normal Persist in Auditory Processing Despite Long Durations of Unilateral Cochlear Implant Use

Although early implantation of young children results in normal-like cortical response peaks, as shown in Figure 2C, the waveform has at least one abnormality. Specifically, the amplitude of the P2 peak in cochlear implant users is larger than in normal hearing peers (t(14.51) = 2.49, p < 0.05) (Jiwani et al., 2013a). The importance of this recent finding is that it suggests that deviations from normal cortical processing remain in these young people despite long-term unilateral implant use. Enhanced P2 peak amplitudes in normal hearing adults are known to reflect increases in selective attention (Picton and Hillyard, 1974; Hocherman et al., 1976; Rif et al., 1991; García-Larrea et al., 1992; Posner and Dehaene, 1994; Grady et al., 1997; Fujiwara et al., 1998; Tremblay et al., 2009) and increases in multi-sensory integration during auditory processing (Hari, 1990; García-Larrea et al., 1992; Levänen et al., 1998; Webster and Colrain, 2000; Moller and Rollins, 2002; Crowley and Colrain, 2004; Johnson and Zatorre, 2005). These processes cause a reduction in the primary network which becomes supplemented by the frontal and parietal areas through increased neural recruitment and synchrony (Tremblay et al., 2001, 2009; Tremblay and Kraus, 2002; Tremblay, 2007) from the non-primary and association auditory pathways (Hocherman et al., 1976; Kraus and McGee, 1993; Kraus et al., 1994; Grady et al., 1997; Busse et al., 2005). It is therefore possible that the larger than normal amplitude of peak P2 observed in children with long-term cochlear implant experience reflects increased cognitive demands for attention and multi-sensory system integration during hearing (Jiwani et al., 2013a). This may reflect compensatory mechanisms to offset: (1) the reorganization in the auditory brain potentially occurring during the period of deafness prior to implantation; (2) the abnormal auditory input provided by the cochlear implant; and/or, (3) the absence of sound to the un-implanted ear which may lead to reorganization in the deprived pathways.

Cochlear implant users compensate for the abnormal input they receive through the device (Doucet et al., 2006; Giraud and Lee, 2007; Lee et al., 2007a,b; Hopyan-Misakyan et al., 2009; Strelnikov et al., 2010; Hopyan et al., 2011; Kral and Sharma, 2011; Lazard et al., 2011, 2012; Hopyan et al., 2012; Sandmann et al., 2012). We found that children using cochlear implants depend on visual cues more heavily than normal to listen for complex information embedded in speech (Hopyan-Misakyan et al., 2009). Emotion perception was tested using 2 subtests of the standardized Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Behavior-2 (DANVA-2) in 18 cochlear implant users who received one implant by 2.9 ± 0.9 years, had 7.2 ± 1.3 years of cochlear implant experience at the time of the test, and had good speech perception skills. In the first test, children listened to the spoken sentence: “I’m going out of the room now and I’ll be back later” (24 trials), and had to decide which 1 of 4 emotions (happy, sad, angry or fearful) was conveyed by the voice. In the second test, children watched pictures of other children’s faces, each depicting one of the same four emotions, and had to decide which emotion was conveyed by the photographs. Performance accuracy was assessed for each task, and compared to 18 normal hearing controls who were matched for age (10.3 ± 1.5 years of age) (Hopyan-Misakyan et al., 2009).

Children using cochlear implants showed significantly poorer than normal performance on the emotion identification task in the auditory subtest (F(1,34) = 43.7, p > 0.01). This deficit does not reflect a general failure to identify emotions, however, since they performed as well as their peers with normal hearing when the emotions were presented in the visual modality (F(1,34) = 0.1, p > 0.05) (Hopyan-Misakyan et al., 2009). The inability of these children to perceive emotions in speech might reflect abnormal development of cortical representation of emotional prosody in speech without normal hearing (Nishimura et al., 1999; Lee et al., 2001, 2007b; Doucet et al., 2006; Meredith and Lomber, 2011; Sandmann, 2012; Sandmann et al., 2012).

In sum, unilateral cochlear implantation promotes the development of normal-like activity in the auditory pathways over the long-term, but functional abnormalities persist. These could reflect: (1) deleterious or irreversible changes to neural reorganization which occurred during the period of auditory deprivation in early life, (2) abnormal representation of sound through electrical pulses stimulation of the auditory system, and/or (3) abnormal cortical development driven by the absence of auditory input to the deprived pathways from the opposite un-implanted ear. We have been studying effects of the latter issue in children.

Binaural Hearing is not Available to Traditional Unilateral Cochlear Implant Users

Hearing through only one cochlear implant eliminates access to binaural hearing, which is the ability of the auditory system to process and integrate auditory input from both ears. Binaural hearing is especially important for children because they are rarely in one place and listening to a single speaker at a time. Children need to attend to and discriminate between several sound sources when playing and learning. The noise, reverberation and distance, predominant in most situations including typical classrooms, make it challenging for children to listen and learn when binaural cues are not accessible. For children who are deaf in both ears, binaural hearing might be achieved with bilateral cochlear implantation (i.e., cochlear implants in both ears) (van Hoesel and Tyler, 2003; Litovsky et al., 2004, 2006; Brown and Balkany, 2007; Steffens et al., 2008b; Basura et al., 2009; Eapen and Buchman, 2009; Gordon et al., 2010, 2011b; Salloum et al., 2010; Chadha et al., 2011). Bilateral cochlear implantation is now being increasingly provided to children either in the same surgery (simultaneously) or in two different surgeries following a period of unilateral implant use (sequentially).

Bilateral cochlear implants attempt to restore binaural hearing by providing information to both ears. Normally, the auditory system compares, processes and integrates subtle differences between level and timing of sounds reaching each ear. In this way, binaural hearing allows: (1) the identification/localization of sound sources in space (Batteau, 1967; Lorenzi et al., 1999; Van Deun et al., 2009b; Grothe et al., 2010); (2) increased perception of loudness through binaural summation (Bocca, 1955; Blegvad, 1975); and (3) improved hearing in quiet and in noisy environments through the head shadow and squelch effects (Hawley et al., 2004; Van Wanrooij and Van Opstal, 2004). Binaural hearing also makes communication less tiring which enables listening and communication to be a more pleasant experience. Although restoring binaural hearing is the goal of bilateral implantation, this has not been completely realized in either adults or children (van Hoesel and Tyler, 2003; Seeber and Fastl, 2008; Grieco-Calub and Litovsky, 2010; Salloum et al., 2010).

Children who are deaf in both ears hear speech better with bilateral cochlear implants than unilateral implants (Litovsky et al., 2004; Brown and Balkany, 2007; Ching et al., 2007; Galvin et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2007; Seeber and Fastl, 2008; Steffens et al., 2008a; Basura et al., 2009; Eapen and Buchman, 2009; Gordon and Papsin, 2009; Van Deun et al., 2009a; Salloum et al., 2010; Chadha et al., 2011), but do not hear binaural cues normally (Grieco-Calub and Litovsky, 2010; Salloum et al., 2010). Outcomes improve when both implants are provided with limited delays and at young ages (van Hoesel and Tyler, 2003; Gordon and Papsin, 2009; Van Deun et al., 2009a; Gordon et al., 2010; Chadha et al., 2011). As the duration of inter-implant delay decreases, the two ears develop more symmetric speech perception abilities and children show increasing advantages of bilateral over unilateral implantation (Gordon and Papsin, 2009). Significant improvements on standardized speech perception tests are seen as early as 6 months following bilateral cochlear implant stimulation in children who receive their second implant simultaneously or within short delays (Gordon and Papsin, 2009). Furthermore, children implanted with both cochlear implants simultaneously derive significantly more benefit from spatial separation of noise compared to children who have longer delays between implants (Chadha et al., 2011). Sound localization improves in children who are provided access to sound early and in both ears (Van Deun et al., 2009a). By contrast, children who receive both cochlear implants sequentially after long inter-implant delays (>2 years) have persistent asymmetries in auditory function and compromised bilateral benefits for speech perception, even after 36 months of bilateral cochlear implant use (Gordon and Papsin, 2009). Sequentially implanted children also seem to depend more on their first implanted ear than their second for speech perception, and show less bilateral improvement (relative to unilateral implant use) on speech outcomes than children implanted simultaneously or with limited delay (Gordon and Papsin, 2009). These children localize sound inaccurately and rely heavily on level cues to do so (Grieco-Calub and Litovsky, 2010). The negative effect of inter-implant delay might be explained by underlying changes to the developing auditory pathways before and after unilateral and bilateral implantation.

Evidence of a Short Sensitive Period for Bilateral Input in Human Auditory Development

Data presented in Figures 1 and 2 show that unilateral stimulation promotes development of the auditory pathways (Jiwani et al., 2013a), thus limiting effects of deafness. At the same time, this development might occur at the expense of pathways from the opposite and deprived ear. This might be explained by the absence of inhibition which would normally have come from input from the opposite ear during binaural hearing (Grothe et al., 2010). Without this inhibition, ascending projections from the stimulated ear may be abnormally strengthened in children who are deaf and use unilateral cochlear implants.

We studied bilateral auditory function in children who had different durations of unilateral exposure. We hypothesized that the stage of unilaterally driven brainstem development would be an important factor to consider. Perhaps changes occurring in the brainstem at earlier stages of unilaterally driven development would have less long lasting consequences on the bilateral pathways than after the unilaterally stimulated brainstem reached maturity. Development in the auditory brainstem is largely complete by 1 year of unilateral implant use (Gordon et al., 2006). Thus, children with >2 years of unilateral experience were categorized as having mature auditory brainstem function and long unilateral use. Children with <1 year of unilateral experience were considered to have short-term use with continuing auditory brainstem development. Auditory development in these children was compared to that of children who were deaf and had not yet used cochlear implants (i.e., limited to no auditory brainstem development). All children were implanted bilaterally, allowing us to assess auditory brainstem function evoked by stimulation from each ear. All children receiving bilateral implants sequentially showed brainstem responses which were faster when evoked by the experienced ear compared to the newly implanted ear at initial bilateral implant use (Gordon et al., 2008b). This was expected and confirmed earlier findings that the first implant promoted improved neural conduction through the brainstem. Repeated tests completed after 1.7 ± 1.65 year of bilateral implant use indicated that mismatches in response latencies persisted in a group of children receiving the second implant after a long delay (>2 years) (Gordon et al., 2012). Increased response latencies in response to sound from the second implanted side could reflect decreased axonal myelination, longer neural conduction times, slower or weaker synapses or more asynchronous neural activity – all signs of more limited brainstem development. Abnormal mismatches between brainstem response latencies were never present in children receiving bilateral implants simultaneously and resolved with bilateral implant use in children who received both implants after a short inter-implant delay (<1 year) (Gordon et al., 2007, 2008b, 2011b, 2012). Thus, allowing the brainstem to develop unilaterally for >2 years compromises the later promotion of symmetrically functioning bilateral auditory brainstem pathways.

Mismatched bilateral auditory development in sequentially implanted children was not restricted to the brainstem. Effects of asymmetric activity in the pathways from the first stimulated ear were also found in the auditory cortex. Consistent with the brainstem findings, cortical abnormalities were not resolved by chronic bilateral implant use (3.57 ± 0.74 years) when unilateral experience exceeded 1.5 years in children who were implanted early (1.87 ± 1.25 years of age). These findings were recently reported by Gordon et al. (2013) and are shown in Figure 3 (re-printed from that paper). We used a unique and validated “Time Restricted Artifact and Coherent Suppression” (TRACS) beamformer method (Wong and Gordon, 2009) to suppress the electrical artifact from the cochlear implant device and spatially localize areas of cortical activity in hemispheres ipsilateral and contralateral to stimulation. Like many imaging methods, the brain was divided into thousands of 3-dimensional coordinate spaces (voxels). Responses were recorded at 64-cephalic surface electrodes and the contribution of the dipole centered in each voxel to the measured field was assessed by the adaptive spatial filter of the TRACS beamformer. Dipole moments for a given voxel were calculated across latency (virtual sensor) and peak values were used for analyses.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Re-printed with permission from Gordon et al. (2013).(A) Per cent cortical lateralization (mean ± 1 standard error) is plotted for each participant group. Greater than normal contralateral lateralization to right/CI-1 stimuli was found in long delay and unilateral cochlear implant users (p < 0.05 and <0.0001, respectively) but not in short delay and simultaneous groups (p > 0.05). The long delay group showed a decrease in contralateral lateralization/increase in ipsilateral lateralization relative to those with normal hearing in response to left/CI-2 stimulation. This did not occur in the short delay and simultaneous groups. (B) Grand mean virtual sensor data for left and right hemispheric sources of P1 (normal hearing) and P1ci (cochlear implant users for stimulation from right/CI-1 and left/CI-2). Large peaks in responses to CI-1 (right) stimulation can be seen in the long delay and unilateral group data. (C) Left and right hemispheric dipole moments (mean ± 1 SE) for P1/P1ci in each group in response to right/CI-1 and left/CI-2 stimulation. In response to CI-1 (right) stimulation, there is a marked increase in left hemispheric dipole moments in participant groups with >2 years of unilateral hearing experience (long delay and unilateral; p < 0.05).” (Gordon et al., 2013; Brain, Figure 7, p. 11)

Cortical responses were evoked by unilateral electrical pulse trains delivered from one implant electrode in seven children with normal hearing, eight children who were implanted unilaterally in the right ear (2.32 ± 1.61 years) and had 7.21 ± 2.48 years of hearing experience and 26 children who used bilateral cochlear implants for 3.42 ± 0.59 years. Of the bilateral implant users, 10 children received both cochlear implants simultaneously and 16 were sequentially implanted (right ear implanted first with no hearing aid in the left ear). Bilateral deafness prior to implantation was limited (1.74 ± 0.90 years) in all children. The children in this study had less than 12 years of hearing experience, and therefore all produced a cortical evoked response which was dominated by an immature large amplitude positive peak, similar to the one shown in Figure 2A. The differences between the dipoles from the left and right auditory cortices were normalized as a percent lateralization [% lateralization = (dipole right - dipole left)/(dipole right + dipole left) × 100].

A larger than normal variability in the lateralization of cortical dipoles was found in children receiving bilateral cochlear implants sequentially. A factor analysis of multiple demographic variables identified the duration of unilateral implant use as the factor which best accounted for the spread of cortical responses. We thus further analyzed the cortical lateralization data for effects of duration of unilateral implant use occurring prior to bilateral implantation. When responses were evoked by the first (i.e., right) implant, there was an increase in lateralization of activity to the contralateral left auditory cortex with unilateral implant use. This became significantly larger than the percent of cortical lateralization in the simultaneously implanted group at 1.48 years of unilateral implant use. Consistent results were obtained in data evoked by the second (i.e., left) implant but, in this case, cortical lateralization changed from the normally expected contralateral direction to ipsilateral lateralization with unilateral implant use. This abnormal switch to larger activity in the ipsilateral auditory cortex became significantly different from responses in the simultaneously implanted group by 1.37 years of unilateral implant use. These analyses indicated that children with longer than approximately 1.5 years of unilateral implant use had experienced an abnormal strengthening of pathways from their first implanted right ear through the auditory brainstem (Gordon et al., 2008b, 2012) to their left contralateral cortex. This was not resolved by several years of bilateral implant use and was associated with poorer speech perception in the second than first implanted ear (Gordon et al., 2013).

The importance of restricting unilateral implant use to less than 1.5 years is further evident in Figure 3 (reprinted from Gordon et al., 2013). Here, the grand mean lateralization of cortical activity are shown (Figure 3A), as well as the grand mean dipole moments identified from the virtual sensors in each hemisphere (Figure 3B). The group of 16 sequentially implanted children have been divided into two groups based on the cut off of 1.5 years of unilateral implant use. The Short Delay group includes seven children who had 0.86 ± 0.1 years of unilateral implant experience at the time of testing. The other nine children, the Long Delay group, had more than 2 years of unilateral implant use (3.44 ± 1.27 years). The single positive peaked response is clear in all of the group averaged waveforms shown in Figure 3B. The maximum dipoles were marked and analyzed in each child. The left plot of Figure 3C shows that dipoles evoked by stimulation from the first/right implanted ear resulted in significantly higher dipoles in the left auditory cortex (blue bars) of children who had >1.5 years of unilateral implant use (Unilateral and Long Delay groups) than other groups of children (F(4,36) = 3.52, p < 0.05). The similar findings for these two groups confirm that unilaterally driven strengthening of projections to the contralateral left auditory cortex was not reversed by the addition of a second cochlear implant. This was true despite the children in the Long Delay group having had several years of bilateral implant experience at the time of the test. The right plot in Figure 3C shows mean dipoles for each auditory cortex in response to left/second cochlear implant stimulation. The Long Delay group shows significantly higher dipole moments in the left auditory cortex than the other groups of children (F(3,29) = 5.31, p < 0.01). Thus, regardless of which ear was stimulated, the left auditory cortex (contralateral to the first/right implanted ear) was the more active side of the brain in children who had used one implant for >1.5 years. One explanation for this finding is that the specialized processing of language in left auditory cortex (Zatorre and Belin, 2001; Zatorre et al., 2002; Tervaniemi and Hugdahl, 2003; Firszt et al., 2006) is abnormally increased in unilateral cochlear implant users. It is not clear, however, how such a network would have been recruited by the simple non-speech stimuli used in the present experiment. An alternate explanation is that unilateral stimulation allowed abnormal strengthening of pathways from that ear.

Further evidence that the cortical changes were due to unilaterally driven strengthening was found by assessing activity in the ipsilateral/right auditory cortex. We assessed which ear preferentially activated the hemisphere contralateral to the ear deprived during the period of unilateral implant use (i.e., the right auditory cortex). The right auditory cortex was expected to respond more strongly to input from the left than right ear because the majority of neurons from one ear normally cross to the contralateral brainstem and ascend ipsilaterally from there. This was confirmed in the group of children with normal hearing and children with limited unilateral implant use prior to bilateral implantation (short delay and simultaneous). By contrast, this pattern was reversed in children in the Long Delay group. This meant that this group of children had experienced a strengthening of pathways from their hearing ear to both the ipsilateral (right) cortex, as shown by the reversal of aural preference, as well as the contralateral (left) cortex as shown by the data in Figure 3. The same reversal of aural preference in the cortex ipsilateral to the hearing ear has recently been reported in congenitally deaf white cats (Kral et al., 2013).

The abnormal strengthening of pathways from the unilaterally hearing ear to the immature brain seems to initially occur at the level of the brainstem. This is supported by evidence of mismatched brainstem latencies observed from children with long (>2 years) unilateral hearing experience (Gordon et al., 2012). The shorter wave eV latencies evoked from the more experienced ear suggest an increasing efficiency of activity from this side and a weakening of pathways from the opposite ear, as reflected by slower peak latencies on the second implanted side. This could result from a lack of inhibitory processes in the brainstem which are normally present during binaural hearing (Grothe et al., 2010). Listening from one side would allow auditory input from the first right implanted side to be projected to the cortex with abnormally high excitation during development thus strengthening pathways to the contralateral cortex. It appears that if this is allowed to occur until the brainstem is largely developed (i.e., >1 year of unilateral implant use), it establishes asymmetric activity in the auditory pathways which is not easily reversed by providing a second implant in the deprived ear. Limiting the period of unilateral hearing in children by providing bilateral cochlear implants with little or no delay appears to protect the bilateral pathways from this abnormal development. These findings thus suggest that there is a sensitive period of 1.5 years for binaural auditory development in children.

Long-Term Unilateral Implant Use in Older Children Causes Lasting Asymmetry in the Bilateral Auditory Pathways

We make the case above that unilateral implant use in children who have been deaf since infancy should be limited to less than 1.5 years to promote normal-like symmetrical development of the auditory pathways from both ears. However, providing bilateral implants within this time frame may not always be possible. For example, many adolescents/young adults who were implanted as babies and have already had many years of unilateral hearing experience are now seeking a cochlear implant for their opposite ear in hopes of deriving benefits of bilateral implantation. These children are different in several ways from our previous research cohorts of sequentially implanted children. They have had very long periods of unilateral cochlear implant use concurrently with long durations of deprivation in their non-implanted ear, and they are no longer children. We thus expect unique cortical development in this new group of bilateral implant users, relative to our previous study groups.

Figure 4 shows the cortical responses recorded at a midline cephalic location on the head (Cz) and evoked by cochlear implant stimulation from each ear on the first day of activation of the second implant in a child who had 15.95 years of hearing experience on the right side and was deprived of auditory input in the left ear. These measures were repeated after 1 month of bilateral implant use and then again after 9 months (Jiwani et al., 2013b,c). Responses from the latter two time points are shown in Figures 4B,C, respectively. The red waveform shows the grand mean response recorded from the side with long-term unilateral cochlear implant experience, and the blue is the cortical waveform evoked by stimulation of the newly implanted side (naïve side). The two responses are very different from one another at all time points. Consistent with previous findings, the cortical responses from the experienced side (red waveform) in Figure 4 were dominated by a mature-like morphology, comprised of the obligatory peaks P1-N1-P2-N2, similar to those expected in same aged peers with normal hearing (Jiwani et al., 2013a). By contrast, responses recorded from the newly implanted ear (blue waveform) were characterized by different peaks occurring with much larger amplitudes than the responses from the side with long-term hearing experience; a large negative peak (N(ci)), followed by a large positive peak (P(ci)) can be seen (Jiwani et al., 2013b,c). Little changes to either response occurred over the first months of bilateral implant use. Slight decreases in the latencies and amplitudes of the peaks evoked by the newly implanted ear were found after one month (Figure 4B), with almost no change in latency, amplitude or waveform morphology thereafter. This is shown by the response recorded at 9 months following activation of the second implant in Figure 4C (Jiwani et al., 2013c).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Example of cortical evoked responses from an adolescent in the Jiwani et al. (2013b,c) study cohorts. She received a right unilateral cochlear implant (red waveform) within limited durations of bilateral deafness (3 years of age) and used it unilaterally to hear for 15.95 years. She then received a second implant in the opposite and deprived left side (naïve side; blue waveform). Cortical responses evoked from both implants are shown at: (A) the first day of activation of the second implanted ear (Jiwani et al., 2013b), (B) one month after bilateral implantation (Jiwani et al., 2013c) and (C) 9 months following bilateral cochlear implant experience (Jiwani et al., 2013c).

The lack of cortical development evoked by stimulation of the second implanted side is in contrast to the rapid developmental change expected to occur at early stages of unilateral cochlear implant use in young children (Sharma et al., 2002a; Sharma and Dorman, 2006), and, rather, more similar to the limited change reported in older children implanted after long durations of bilateral deafness (Sharma et al., 2002b; Gordon et al., 2005, 2008a). This might reflect immaturity or abnormalities in auditory development from the second implanted side, driven by either long duration of auditory deprivation or by maturation of the auditory cortex from unilateral cochlear implant use. Providing a second implant to children after this period has passed may prevent the naïve cortical pathways from developing after an important period in cortical auditory development has been missed. The findings from our previous study (Gordon et al., 2013) (discussed above and shown in Figure 3) suggest that there is an early sensitive period for bilateral brainstem development (exceeded after 1.5 years of unilateral implant use) and a later cortical maturation promoted by unilateral use of over 10 years (Jiwani et al., 2013a), as shown by the data in Figure 4 (Jiwani et al., 2013b, c). Together, these results suggest that there are multiple sensitive periods in the developing auditory system.

Bilateral Implantation within a Sensitive Period Improves Perception of Binaural Timing Cues

As reviewed above, several lines of investigation suggest that the potential for promoting binaural hearing in children who are deaf will be best realized by limiting the period of bilateral deafness and providing bilateral implants with little delay. We have been studying the perception of binaural level and timing cues in children who received bilateral cochlear implants because these cues are important for binaural hearing. Interaural level and timing cues arise because sounds coming from one side of the head reach the closer ear at higher intensities and/or faster than the other ear. Level and timing differences are coded in the auditory brainstem by the degree of inhibition (Grothe et al., 2010).

We found that 19 children receiving one implant at 2.1 ± 1.1 years of age and the second after 4.9 ± 2.8 years of unilateral implant use can hear changes in interaural level differences but have particularly poor abilities to detect interaural timing cues even after several years of bilateral cochlear implant use (Salloum et al., 2010). Poor detection of binaural timing cues by sequentially implanted children was surprising given evidence from a similar group showing that the auditory brainstem integrates input from both implants as measured by the electrophysiological binaural interaction component (Gordon et al., 2012). This measure is a calculated difference between the sum of the left and right evoked auditory brainstem responses and the bilaterally evoked brainstem response. Peaks in the difference response reflect inhibition occurring with binaural processing (Dobie and Berlin, 1979; Dobie and Norton, 1980; Brantberg et al., 1999). Using this difference measure, we found that tonotopic organization is maintained in the bilateral brainstem of children who are deaf and that the pathways continue to code interaural level cues despite development driven from one ear before the other. There are consequences of the mismatches in development resulting from unilateral implant use. Although the auditory brainstem codes interaural timing differences, this does not occur normally (Gordon et al., 2008b). A miscalculation of binaural brainstem interactions results from the mismatch in neural conduction (measured by shorter peak latencies responses from the more experienced ear). More recent findings show that a sound arriving first to the more experienced ear by 1ms, for example, reduces the binaural interaural component more than when it arrives first by the same amount to the second implanted ear (Gordon, et al., in preparation). Nonetheless, coding of interaural timing remains (albeit abnormally calibrated); thus abnormal brainstem processing cannot account for the profound difficulties these children have detecting timing differences sent by their bilateral implants. This suggests a deficit for interaural timing processing in more central areas of the auditory system which likely occurred during the period before bilateral implantation. In support, the numbers of cortical neurons specialized to respond to interaural timing cues are reduced in congenitally deaf white cats (Tillein et al., 2010) as are numbers of neurons in auditory cortices responsible for sound localization (Malhotra et al., 2008). In more recent work, we are asking whether binaural timing cues are better heard by children who received bilateral cochlear implants simultaneously. Preliminary findings suggest good potential for development of binaural hearing in children who have limited durations of bilateral and unilateral deafness, but is compromised in children with long unilateral cochlear implants experience (>1.5 years).

Conclusion

We have reviewed evidence showing that access to sound within limited durations of bilateral deafness in early life promotes normal-like development of activity along the auditory pathways in children who have many years of hearing experience with a unilateral cochlear implant. At the same time, however, the unilaterally driven stimulation leaves the opposite pathways deprived of input and susceptible to reorganization. We find that providing bilateral cochlear implants to children after a period of unilateral deafness of longer than 1.5 years drives abnormal mismatches in activity at the level of the brainstem and cortex. This is characterized by abnormal strengthening of activity to both the contralateral and ipsilateral auditory cortices from the first implanted ear. These abnormalities in auditory development are associated with more asymmetric speech perception, poorer hearing in noise, abnormal sound localization, and an inability to identify inter-aural timing cues. These skills are important for normal integration and processing of auditory input. We therefore suggest that binaural hearing is compromised in children who receive bilateral cochlear implants after a period of unilateral implant use exceeding 1.5 years. With that in mind, cochlear implants should be provided to children early as well as bilaterally within very limited or no delays between implants (i.e., simultaneously). Our current studies are now examining how much residual hearing is needed in the un-implanted ear to provide a potential protective effect against unilaterally driven reorganization and whether bimodal hearing (acoustic and electrical input) can be used to restore binaural hearing. Further, we are asking whether the sensitive period for bilateral input can be “reopened” by attempting to strengthen pathways from the second implanted ear to restore symmetric bilateral pathways and binaural hearing. Our findings suggest that both bilateral and unilateral deprivation should be limited to promote optimal binaural hearing in children who use cochlear implants, and enable them to function better and more naturally in challenging listening situations such as the playground or classroom environments.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The Authors of this manuscript wish to thank all of the families who participated in our studies, as well as all of the members of the clinical and research teams of the Cochlear Implant Program at the Hospital for Sick Children.

References

Abbott, L. F., and Nelson, S. B. (2000). Synaptic plasticity: taming the beast. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1178–1183. doi: 10.1038/81453

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Auer, E. T. Jr., Bernstein, L. E., Sungkarat, W., and Singh, M. (2007). Vibrotactile activation of the auditory cortices in deaf versus hearing adults. Neuroreport 18, 645. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280d943b9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Basura, G. J., Eapen, R., and Buchman, C. A. (2009). Bilateral cochlear implantation: current concepts, indications, and results. Laryngoscope 119, 2395–2401. doi: 10.1002/lary.20751

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Batteau, D. W. (1967). The role of the pinna in human localization. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 168, 158–180. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1967.0058

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bavelier, D., Dye, M. W. G., and Hauser, P. C. (2006). Do deaf individuals see better? Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 512–518. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bavelier, D., and Neville, H. J. (2002). Cross-modal plasticity: where and how? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 443–452. doi: 10.1038/nrn848

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bavelier, D., Tomann, A., Hutton, C., Mitchell, T., Corina, D., Liu, G., et al. (2000). Visual attention to the periphery is enhanced in congenitally deaf individuals. J. Neurosci. 20, 1–6.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Beadle, E. A. R., Mckinley, D. J., Nikolopoulos, T. P., Brough, J., O’Donoghue, G. M., and Archbold, S. M. (2005). Long-term functional outcomes and academic-occupational status in implanted children after 10 to 14 years of cochlear implant use. Otol. Neurotol. 26, 1152. doi: 10.1097/01.mao.0000180483.16619.8f

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Blegvad, B. (1975). Binaural summation of surface-recorded electrocochleographic responses normal-hearing subjects. Scand. Audiol. 4, 233–238. doi: 10.3109/01050397509043087

CrossRef Full Text

Bocca, E. (1955). Binaural hearing: another approach. Laryngoscope 65, 1164–1171. doi: 10.1288/00005537-195512000-00005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brantberg, K., Fransson, P.-A., Hansson, H., and Rosenhall, U. (1999). Measures of the binaural interaction component in human auditory brainstem response using objective detection criteria. Scand. Audiol. 28, 15–26. doi: 10.1080/010503999424879

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Brown, K. D., and Balkany, T. J. (2007). Benefits of bilateral cochlear implantation: a review. Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head. Neck. Surg. 15, 315. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e3282ef3d3e

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Busse, L., Roberts, K. C., Crist, R. E., Weissman, D. H., and Woldorff, M. G. (2005). The spread of attention across modalities and space in a multisensory object. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102, 18751–18756. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0507704102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calvert, G. A., Hansen, P. C., Iversen, S. D., and Brammer, M. J. (2001). Detection of audio-visual integration sites in humans by application of electrophysiological criteria to the BOLD effect. Neuroimage 14, 427–438. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0812

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Calvert, G. A., and Thesen, T. (2004). Multisensory integration: methodological approaches and emerging principles in the human brain. J. Physiol. Paris 98, 191–205. doi: 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.03.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Chadha, N. K., Papsin, B. C., Jiwani, S., and Gordon, K. A. (2011). Speech detection in noise and spatial unmasking in children with simultaneous versus sequential bilateral cochlear implants. Otol. Neurotol. 32, 1057. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0b013e3182267de7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ching, T. Y., Van Wanrooy, E., and Dillon, H. (2007). Binaural-bimodal fitting or bilateral implantation for managing severe to profound deafness: a review. Trends Amplif. 11, 161–192. doi: 10.1177/1084713807304357

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cohn, E. S., and Kelley, P. M. (1999). Clinical phenotype and mutations in connexin 26 (DFNB1/GJB2), the most common cause of childhood hearing loss. Am. J. Med. Genet. 89, 130–136. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19990924)89:3<130::AID-AJMG3>3.0.CO;2-M

CrossRef Full Text

Crowley, K. E., and Colrain, I. M. (2004). A review of the evidence for P2 being an independent component process: age, sleep and modality. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 732–744. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.11.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobie, R., and Norton, S. (1980). Binaural interaction in human auditory evoked potentials. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 49, 303–313. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(80)90224-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dobie, R. A., and Berlin, C. I. (1979). Binaural interaction in brainstem-evoked responses. Arch. Otolaryngol. 105, 391. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1979.00790190017004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Doucet, M., Bergeron, F., Lassonde, M., Ferron, P., and Lepore, F. (2006). Cross-modal reorganization and speech perception in cochlear implant users. Brain 129, 3376–3383. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl264

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eapen, R. J., and Buchman, C. A. (2009). Bilateral cochlear implantation: current concepts. Curr. Opin. Otolaryngol. Head Neck. Surg. 17, 351–355. doi: 10.1097/MOO.0b013e3283301702

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eggermont, J. J., and Ponton, C. W. (2003). Auditory-evoked potential studies of cortical maturation in normal hearing and implanted children: correlations with changes in structure and speech perception. Acta Otolaryngol. 123, 249–252. doi: 10.1080/0036554021000028098

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Eggermont, J. J., Ponton, C. W., Don, M., Waring, M. D., and Kwong, B. (1997). Maturational delays in cortical evoked potentials in cochlear implant users. Acta Otolaryngol. 117, 161–163. doi: 10.3109/00016489709117760

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Finney, E. M., Fine, I., and Dobkins, K. R. (2001). Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 1171–1174. doi: 10.1038/nn763

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Firszt, J. B., Ulmer, J. L., and Gaggl, W. (2006). Differential representation of speech sounds in the human cerebral hemispheres. Anat. Rec. A Discov. Mol. Cell. Evol. Biol. 288, 345–357. doi: 10.1002/ar.a.20295

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fujiwara, N., Nagamine, T., Imai, M., Tanaka, T., and Shibasaki, H. (1998). Role of the primary auditory cortex in auditory selective attention studied by whole-head neuromagnetometer. Cogn. Brain Res. 7, 99–109. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(98)00014-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Galvin, K. L., Mok, M., and Dowell, R. C. (2007). Perceptual benefit and functional outcomes for children using sequential bilateral cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 28, 470. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31806dc194

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

García-Larrea, L., Lukaszewicz, A. C., and Mauguiére, F. (1992). Revisiting the oddball paradigm. Non-target vs neutral stimuli and the evaluation of ERP attentional effects. Neuropsychologia 30, 723–741. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(92)90042-K

CrossRef Full Text

Geers, A. E., and Sedey, A. L. (2011). Language and verbal reasoning skills in adolescents with 10 or more years of cochlear implant experience. Ear Hear. 32, 39S. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181fa41dc

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giard, M., and Peronnet, F. (1999). Auditory-visual integration during multimodal object recognition in humans: a behavioral and electrophysiological study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 11, 473–490. doi: 10.1162/089892999563544

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Giraud, A. L., and Lee, H. J. (2007). Predicting cochlear implant outcome from brain organisation in the deaf. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 25, 381–390.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Gordon, K., Wong, D. D., Valero, J., Jewell, S., Yoo, P., and Papsin, B. (2011a). Use it or lose it? Lessons learned from the developing brains of children who are deaf and use cochlear implants to hear. Brain Topogr. 24, 204–219. doi: 10.1007/s10548-011-0181-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Jiwani, S., and Papsin, B. C. (2011b). What is the optimal timing for bilateral cochlear implantation in children? Cochlear Implants Int. 12(Suppl. 2), S8–S14. doi: 10.1179/146701011X13074645127199

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Tanaka, S., Wong, D. D., Stockley, T., Ramsden, J. D., Brown, T., et al. (2011c). Multiple effects of childhood deafness on cortical activity in children receiving bilateral cochlear implants simultaneously. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 823–833. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.10.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., and Papsin, B. C. (2009). Benefits of short interimplant delays in children receiving bilateral cochlear implants. Otol. Neurotol. 30, 319. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0b013e31819a8f4c

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Papsin, B. C., and Harrison, R. V. (2003). Activity-dependent developmental plasticity of the auditory brain stem in children who use cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 24, 485–500. doi: 10.1097/01.AUD.0000100203.65990.D4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Papsin, B. C., and Harrison, R. V. (2006). An evoked potential study of the developmental time course of the auditory nerve and brainstem in children using cochlear implants. Audiol. Neurotol. 11, 7–23. doi: 10.1159/000088851

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Valero, J., and Papsin, B. C. (2007). Auditory brainstem activity in children with 9-30 months of bilateral cochlear implant use. Hear. Res. 233, 97–107. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2007.08.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Salloum, C., Toor, G. S., Van Hoesel, R., and Papsin, B. C. (2012). Binaural interactions develop in the auditory brainstem of children who aAre deaf: effects of place and level of bilateral electrical stimulation. J. Neurosci. 32, 4212–4223. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5741-11.2012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Tanaka, S., and Papsin, B. C. (2005). Atypical cortical responses underlie poor speech perception in children using cochlear implants. Neuroreport 16, 2041. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200512190-00015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Tanaka, S., Wong, D. D., and Papsin, B. C. (2008a). Characterizing responses from auditory cortex in young people with several years of cochlear implant experience. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 2347–2362. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.06.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Valero, J., Van Hoesel, R., and Papsin, B. C. (2008b). Abnormal timing delays in auditory brainstem responses evoked by bilateral cochlear implant use in children. Otol. Neurotol. 29, 193. doi: 10.1097/mao.0b013e318162514c

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Wong, D. D., and Papsin, B. C. (2013). Bilateral input protects the cortex from unilaterally-driven reorganization in children who are deaf. Brain 136, 1609–1625. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gordon, K. A., Wong, D. D. E., and Papsin, B. C. (2010). Cortical function in children receiving bilateral cochlear implants simultaneously or after a period of interimplant delay. Otol. Neurotol. 31, 1293. doi: 10.1097/MAO.0b013e3181e8f965

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grady, C. L., Van Meter, J. W., Maisog, J. M., Pietrini, P., Krasuski, J., and Rauschecker, J. P. (1997). Attention-related modulation of activity in primary and secondary auditory cortex. Neuroreport 8, 2511. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199707280-00019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grieco-Calub, T. M., and Litovsky, R. Y. (2010). Sound localization skills in children who use bilateral cochlear implants and in children with normal acoustic hearing. Ear Hear. 31, 645. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181e50a1d

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Grothe, B., Pecka, M., and Mcalpine, D. (2010). Mechanisms of sound localization in mammals. Physiol. Rev. 90, 983–1012. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00026.2009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gualandi, F., Ravani, A., Berto, A., Sensi, A., Trabanelli, C., Falciano, F., et al. (2002). Exploring the clinical and epidemiological complexity of GJB2-linked deafness. Am. J. Med. Genet. 112, 38–45. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.10621

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hari, R. (1990). The neuromagnetic method in the study of the human auditory cortex. Adv. Audiol. 6, 222–282.

Harrison, R. V., Gordon, K. A., and Mount, R. J. (2005). Is there a critical period for cochlear implantation in congenitally deaf children? Analyses of hearing and speech perception performance after implantation. Dev. Psychobiol. 46, 252–261. doi: 10.1002/dev.20052

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hawley, M. L., Litovsky, R. Y., and Culling, J. F. (2004). The benefit of binaural hearing in a cocktail party: effect of location and type of interferer. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 115, 833. doi: 10.1121/1.1639908

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior. New York: Wiley.

Hecox, K., and Burkard, R. (2006). Developmental dependencies of the human brainstem auditory evoked response. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 338, 538–556. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1980.tb19380.x

CrossRef Full Text

Hocherman, S., Benson, D., Goldstein, M., Heffner, H., and Hienz, R. (1976). Evoked unit activity in auditory cortex of monkeys performing a selective attention task. Brain Res. 117, 51–68. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(76)90555-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hopyan-Misakyan, T. M., Gordon, K. A., Dennis, M., and Papsin, B. C. (2009). Recognition of affective speech prosody and facial affect in deaf children with unilateral right cochlear implants. Child Neuropsychol. 15, 136–146. doi: 10.1080/09297040802403682

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hopyan, T., Gordon, K., and Papsin, B. (2011). Identifying emotions in music through electrical hearing in deaf children using cochlear implants. Cochlear Implants Int. 12, 21–26. doi: 10.1179/146701010X12677899497399

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hopyan, T., Peretz, I., Chan, L. P., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2012). Children using cochlear implants capitalize on acoustical hearing for music perception. Front. Psychol. 3:425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00425

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jerger, J., and Hall, J. (1980). Effects of age and sex on auditory brainstem response. Arch. Otolaryngol. 106, 387. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1980.00790310011003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jiwani, S., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2011). Auditory development in long-term cochlear implant users [Abstract]. 13th International Symposium on Cochlear Implants in Children, Chicago, IL.

Jiwani, S., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2013a). Central auditory development after long-term cochlear implant use. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1268–1280. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.03.023

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jiwani, S., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2013b). Extensive areas of the cortex are evoked by stimulation from the newly implanted ear in children who were long-term unilateral cochlear implant users [Abstract]. 36th Annual MidWinter Meeting of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology, Baltimore, USA. Available at: http://www.aro.org/documents/ARO2013FinalAbstractBook_000.pdf

Jiwani, S., Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2013c). Long durations of unilateral cochlear implant use do not protect the un-stimulated pathays from effects of auditory deprivation [Abstract]. 2013 Conference on Implantable Auditory Prosthesis, Lake Tahoe, California. Available at: http://www.ciap2013.org/PDFs/CIAP2013%20Program%20Book%20draft.pdf

Johnson, J. A., and Zatorre, R. J. (2005). Attention to simultaneous unrelated auditory and visual events: behavioral and neural correlates. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1609–1620. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kelley, P., Harris, D., Comer, B., Askew, J., Fowler, T., Smith, S., et al. (1998). Novel mutations in the connexin 26 gene (GJB2) that cause autosomal recessive (DFNB1) hearing loss. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 62, 792–799. doi: 10.1086/301807

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Klinge, C., Eippert, F., Roder, B., and Buchel, C. (2010). Corticocortical connections mediate primary visual cortex responses to auditory stimulation in the blind. J. Neurosci. 30, 12798–12805. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2384-10.2010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kral, A., Hartmann, R., Tillein, J., Heid, S., and Klinke, R. (2001). Delayed maturation and sensitive periods in the auditory cortex. Audiol. Neurootol. 6, 346–362. doi: 10.1159/000046845

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kral, A., Hubka, P., Heid, S., and Tillein, J. (2013). Single-sided deafness leads to unilateral aural preference within an early sensitive period. Brain 136, 180–193. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws305

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kral, A., and O’Donoghue, G. M. (2010). Profound deafness in childhood. N. Engl. J. Med. 363, 1438–1450. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra0911225

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kral, A., and Sharma, A. (2011). Developmental neuroplasticity after cochlear implantation. Trends Neurosci. 35, 111–122. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kraus, N., and McGee, T. (1993). Clinical implications of primary and nonprimary pathway contributions to the middle latency response generating system. Ear Hear. 14, 36. doi: 10.1097/00003446-199302000-00006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kraus, N., Mcgee, T., Littman, T., and Nicol, T. (1992). Reticular formation influences on primary and non-primary auditory pathways as reflected by the middle latency response. Brain Res. 587, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(92)90996-M

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kraus, N., Mcgee, T., Littman, T., Nicol, T., and King, C. (1994). Nonprimary auditory thalamic representation of acoustic change. J. Neurophysiol. 72, 1270–1277.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Lazard, D., Giraud, A. -L., Gnansia, D., Meyer, B., and Sterkers, O. (2011). Understanding the deafened brain: implications for cochlear implant rehabilitation. Eur. Ann. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Dis. 129, 98–103/ doi: 10.1016/j.anorl.2011.06.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lazard, D. S., Lee, H. J., Truy, E., and Giraud, A. L. (2012). Bilateral reorganization of posterior temporal cortices in post-lingual deafness and its relation to cochlear implant outcome. Hum. Brain Mapp. 34, 1208–1219. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, C. C., and Winer, J. A. (2005). Principles governing auditory cortex connections. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1804–1814. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi057

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, D. S., Lee, J. S., Oh, S. H., Kim, S. K., Kim, J. W., Chung, J. K., et al. (2001). Deafness: cross-modal plasticity and cochlear implants. Nature 409, 149–150. doi: 10.1038/35051653

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, H. J., Giraud, A. L., Kang, E., Oh, S. H., Kang, H., Kim, C. S., et al. (2007a). Cortical activity at rest predicts cochlear implantation outcome. Cereb. Cortex 17, 909–917. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lee, H. J., Truy, E., Mamou, G., Sappey-Marinier, D., and Giraud, A. L. (2007b). Visual speech circuits in profound acquired deafness: a possible role for latent multimodal connectivity. Brain 130, 2929–2941. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm230

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levänen, S., and Hamdorf, D. (2001). Feeling vibrations: enhanced tactile sensitivity in congenitally deaf humans. Neurosci. Lett. 301, 75–77. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01597-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Levänen, S., Jousmäki, V., and Hari, R. (1998). Vibration-induced auditory-cortex activation in a congenitally deaf adult. Curr. Biol. 8, 869–872. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(07)00348-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Liegeois-Chauvel, C., Musolino, A., Badier, J., Marquis, P., and Chauvel, P. (1994). Evoked potentials recorded from the auditory cortex in man: evaluation and topography of the middle latency components. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 92, 204–214. doi: 10.1016/0168-5597(94)90064-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Litovsky, R. Y., Johnstone, P. M., and Godar, S. P. (2006). Benefits of bilateral cochlear implants and/or hearing aids in children. Int. J. Audiol. 45, 78–91. doi: 10.1080/14992020600782956

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Litovsky, R. Y., Parkinson, A., Arcaroli, J., Peters, R., Lake, J., Johnstone, P., et al. (2004). Bilateral cochlear implants in adults and children. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 130, 648. doi: 10.1001/archotol.130.5.648

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lomber, S. G., Meredith, M. A., and Kral, A. (2010). Cross-modal plasticity in specific auditory cortices underlies visual compensations in the deaf. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 1421–1427. doi: 10.1038/nn.2653

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lorenzi, C., Gatehouse, S., and Lever, C. (1999). Sound localization in noise in normal-hearing listeners. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 1810. doi: 10.1121/1.426719

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Malhotra, S., Stecker, G. C., Middlebrooks, J. C., and Lomber, S. G. (2008). Sound localization deficits during reversible deactivation of primary auditory cortex and/or the dorsal zone. J. Neurophysiol. 99, 1628–1642. doi: 10.1152/jn.01228.2007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Merabet, L. B., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2010). Neural reorganization following sensory loss: the opportunity of change. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 44–52. doi: 10.1038/nrn2758

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Meredith, M. A., and Lomber, S. G. (2011). Somatosensory and visual crossmodal plasticity in the anterior auditory field of early-deaf cats. Hear. Res. 280, 38–47. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2011.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moller, A. R., and Rollins, P. R. (2002). The non-classical auditory pathways are involved in hearing in children but not in adults. Neurosci. Lett. 319, 41–44. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02516-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nicholas, J. G., and Geers, A. E. (2007). Will they catch up? The role of age at cochlear implantation in the spoken language development of children with severe to profound hearing loss. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 50, 1048. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/073)

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nikolopoulos, T. P., O’Donoghue, G. M., and Archbold, S. (2009). Age at implantation: its importance in pediatric cochlear implantation. Laryngoscope 109, 595–599. doi: 10.1097/00005537-199904000-00014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nishimura, H., Hashikawa, K., Doi, K., Iwaki, T., Watanabe, Y., Kusuoka, H., et al. (1999). Sign language ‘heard’in the auditory cortex. Nature 397, 116–116. doi: 10.1038/16376

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Donoghue, G. M. (1999). Hearing without ears: do cochlear implants work in children?: Yes, so long as they are given to the right children early enough. BMJ 318, 72. doi: 10.1136/bmj.318.7176.72

CrossRef Full Text

O’Neil, J. N., Limb, C. J., Baker, C. A., and Ryugo, D. K. (2010). Bilateral effects of unilateral cochlear implantation in congenitally deaf cats. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 2382–2404. doi: 10.1002/cne.22339

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Osberger, M. J., Zimmerman-Phillips, S., and Koch, D. B. (2002). Cochlear implant candidacy and performance trends in children. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. Suppl. 189, 62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Pandya, D. N., and Yeterian, E. H. (1985). Architecture and connections of cortical association areas. Cereb. Cortex 4, 3–61.

Papsin, B. C., and Gordon, K. A. (2007). Cochlear implants for children with severe-to-profound hearing loss. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 2380–2387. doi: 10.1056/NEJMct0706268

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Peters, B. R., Litovsky, R., Parkinson, A., and Lake, J. (2007). Importance of age and postimplantation experience on speech perception measures in children with sequential bilateral cochlear implants. Otol. Neurotol. 28, 649. doi: 10.1097/01.mao.0000281807.89938.60

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Picton, T., and Hillyard, S. (1974). Human auditory evoked potentials. II: effects of attention. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 36, 191–200. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(74)90156-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ponton, C. W., and Eggermont, J. J. (2001). Of kittens and kids: altered cortical maturation following profound deafness and cochlear implant use. Audiol. Neurotol. 6, 363–380. doi: 10.1159/000046846

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ponton, C. W., Eggermont, J. J., Kwong, B., and Don, M. (2000). Maturation of human central auditory system activity: evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 111, 220–236. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00236-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Posner, M. I., and Dehaene, S. (1994). Attentional networks. Trends Neurosci. 17, 75–79. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90078-7

CrossRef Full Text

Propst, E. J., Papsin, B. C., Stockley, T. L., Harrison, R. V., and Gordon, K. A. (2006). Auditory responses in cochlear implant users with and without GJB2 deafness. Laryngoscope 116, 317–327. doi: 10.1097/01.mlg.0000199401.26626.4b

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Razak, K. A., Zumsteg, T., and Fuzessery, Z. M. (2009). Development of auditory thalamocortical connections in the pallid bat, Antrozous pallidus. J. Comp. Neurol. 515, 231–242. doi: 10.1002/cne.22050

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Read, H. L., Winer, J. A., and Schreiner, C. E. (2002). Functional architecture of auditory cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12, 433–440. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00342-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rif, J., Hari, R., Hämäläinen, M. S., and Sams, M. (1991). Auditory attention affects two different areas in the human supratemporal cortex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 79, 464–472. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(91)90166-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salamy, A. (1984). Maturation of the auditory brainstem response from birth through early childhood. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1, 293–329. doi: 10.1097/00004691-198407000-00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salamy, A., and McKean, C. M. (1976). Postnatal development of human brainstem potentials during the first year of life. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 40, 418–426. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(76)90193-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Salloum, C. A. M., Valero, J., Wong, D. D. E., Papsin, B. C., Van Hoesel, R., and Gordon, K. A. (2010). Lateralization of interimplant timing and level differences in children who use bilateral cochlear implants. Ear Hear. 31, 441. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181d4f228

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sandmann, P. (2012). User Research Visual processing in the auditory cortex of cochlear-implant users. Brain 42. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr329

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sandmann, P., Dillier, N., Eichele, T., Meyer, M., Kegel, A., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., et al. (2012). Visual activation of auditory cortex reflects maladaptive plasticity in cochlear implant users. Brain 135, 555–568. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr329

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Seeber, B. U., and Fastl, H. (2008). Localization cues with bilateral cochlear implants. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 123, 1030. doi: 10.1121/1.2821965

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sharma, A., and Dorman, M. (2006). Central auditory development in children with cochlear implants: clinical implications. Adv. Otorhinolaryngol. 64, 66–88. doi: 10.1159/000094646

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., and Kral, A. (2005). The influence of a sensitive period on central auditory development in children with unilateral and bilateral cochlear implants. Hear. Res. 203, 134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2004.12.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., and Spahr, A. J. (2002a). Rapid development of cortical auditory evoked potentials after early cochlear implantation. Neuroreport 13, 1365. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200207190-00030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sharma, A., Dorman, M. F., and Spahr, A. J. (2002b). A sensitive period for the development of the central auditory system in children with cochlear implants: implications for age of implantation. Ear Hear. 23, 532–539. doi: 10.1097/00003446-200212000-00004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Song, S., Miller, K. D., and Abbott, L. F. (2000). Competitive Hebbian learning through spike-timing-dependent synaptic plasticity. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 919–926. doi: 10.1038/78829

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Starr, A., Amlie, R., Martin, W., and Sanders, S. (1977). Development of auditory function in newborn infants revealed by auditory brainstem potentials. Pediatrics 60, 831–839.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Steffens, T., Lesinski-Schiedat, A., Strutz, J., Aschendorff, A., Klenzner, T., Ruhl, S., et al. (2008a). The benefits of sequential bilateral cochlear implantation for hearing-impaired children. Acta Otolaryngol. 128, 164–176.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Steffens, T., Lesinski-Schiedat, A., Strutz, J., Aschendorff, A., Klenzner, T., Rühl, S., et al. (2008b). The benefits of sequential bilateral cochlear implantation for hearing-impaired children. Acta Otolaryngol. 128, 164–176.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strelnikov, K., Rouger, J., Demonet, J.-F., Lagleyre, S., Fraysse, B., Deguine, O., et al. (2010). Does brain activity at rest reflect adaptive strategies? Evidence from speech processing after cochlear implantation. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1217–1222. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp183

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tervaniemi, M., and Hugdahl, K. (2003). Lateralization of auditory-cortex functions. Brain Res. Rev. 43, 231–246. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tillein, J., Hubka, P., Syed, E., Hartmann, R., Engel, A. K., and Kral, A. (2010). Cortical representation of interaural time difference in congenital deafness. Cereb. Cortex 20, 492–506. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp222

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremblay, K., Kraus, N., Mcgee, T., Ponton, C., and Otis, B. (2001). Central auditory plasticity: changes in the N1-P2 complex after speech-sound training. Ear Hear. 22, 79–90. doi: 10.1097/00003446-200104000-00001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremblay, K. L. (2007). Training-related changes in the brain: evidence from human auditory-evoked potentials. 28, 120–132. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-973438

Tremblay, K. L., and Kraus, N. (2002). Auditory training induces asymmetrical changes in cortical neural activity. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 45, 564. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/045)

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tremblay, K. L., Shahin, A. J., Picton, T., and Ross, B. (2009). Auditory training alters the physiological detection of stimulus-specific cues in humans. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 128. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Deun, L., Van Wieringen, A., Scherf, F., Deggouj, N., Desloovere, C., Offeciers, F. E., et al. (2009a). Earlier intervention leads to better sound localization in children with bilateral cochlear implants. Audiol. Neurotol. 15, 7–17. doi: 10.1159/000218358

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Deun, L., Van Wieringen, A., Van Den Bogaert, T., Scherf, F., Offeciers, F. E., Van De Heyning, P. H., et al. (2009b). Sound localization, sound lateralization, and binaural masking level differences in young children with normal hearing. Ear Hear. 30, 178–190. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e318194256b

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

van Hoesel, R. J., and Tyler, R. S. (2003). Speech perception, localization, and lateralization with bilateral cochlear implants. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 113, 1617. doi: 10.1121/1.1539520

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Wanrooij, M. M., and Van Opstal, A. J. (2004). Contribution of head shadow and pinna cues to chronic monaural sound localization. J. Neurosci. 24, 4163–4171. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0048-04.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Webster, K. E., and Colrain, I. M. (2000). The respiratory-related evoked potential: effects of attention and occlusion duration. Psychophysiology 37, 310–318. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3730310

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winer, J. A., Diehl, J. J., and Larue, D. T. (2001). Projections of auditory cortex to the medial geniculate body of the cat. J. Comp. Neurol. 430, 27–55. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20010129)430:1<27::AID-CNE1013>3.0.CO;2-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Winer, J. A., Miller, L. M., Lee, C. C., and Schreiner, C. E. (2005). Auditory thalamocortical transformation: structure and function. Trends Neurosci. 28, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wong, D. D. E., and Gordon, K. A. (2009). Beamformer suppression of cochlear implant artifacts in an electroencephalography dataset. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 56, 2851–2857. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2009.2029239

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., and Belin, P. (2001). Spectral and temporal processing in human auditory cortex. Cereb. Cortex 11, 946–953. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.10.946

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zatorre, R. J., Belin, P., and Penhune, V. B. (2002). Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends Cogn. Sci. 6, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01816-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: deafness, cochlear implantation, unilateral hearing, auditory brainstem responses, auditory evoked cortical potentials, auditory development, plasticity, binaural hearing

Citation: Gordon KA, Jiwani S and Papsin BC (2013) Benefits and detriments of unilateral cochlear implant use on bilateral auditory development in children who are deaf. Front. Psychol. 4:719. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00719

Received: 01 May 2013; Accepted: 18 September 2013;
Published online: 16 October 2013.

Edited by:

Virginia Penhune, Concordia University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Minna Huotilainen, University of Helsinki, Finland
Heikki Lyytinen, University of Jyväskylä, Finland

Copyright © 2013 Gordon, Jiwani and Papsin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Karen A. Gordon, Archie’s Cochlear Implant Laboratory, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Room 6D06, Toronto, ON M5G 1X8, Canada e-mail: Karen.gordon@utoronto.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.