- 1Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy, and Assessment, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Braunschweig, Braunschweig, Germany
- 2Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
A multitude of factors contribute to why and how romantic relationships are formed as well as whether they ultimately succeed or fail. Drawing on evolutionary models of attraction and speech production as well as integrative models of relationship functioning, this review argues that paralinguistic cues (more specifically the fundamental frequency of the voice) that are initially a strong source of attraction also increase couples’ risk for relationship failure. Conceptual similarities and differences between the multiple operationalizations and interpretations of vocal fundamental frequency are discussed and guidelines are presented for understanding both convergent and non-convergent findings. Implications for clinical practice and future research are discussed.
Introduction
Across cultures, adults rate marriage and long-term monogamous relationships as the most important ones in their lives (Buss, 2005). Despite this, divorce rates in industrialized countries rank between 40 to 55% (Hahlweg et al., 2010). A tremendous amount of effort has been accordingly devoted to identifying and understanding risk factors for relationship dissolution (Gottman and Notarius, 2002) with a particular emphasis on identifying risk factors that are present from the outset of a relationship (Caughlin and Huston, 2002). The polarization model of romantic relationships (Jacobson and Christensen, 1998; Baucom and Atkins, 2013) suggests that one of the most important kinds of risk factors originates in variables that are initially associated with attraction and desire that later become sources of distress and interpersonal friction. A growing body of empirical evidence suggests that the fundamental frequency of romantic partner’s voices may represent precisely this kind of risk. Vocal qualities that are found to be attractive during the early stages of relationship formation are also associated with increased levels of dysfunction, increased risk for divorce, and decreased likelihood of benefitting from couple therapy in later stages of a relationship.
Evolutionary models suggest that perceptions of health status and likelihood of reproductive success are one of the primary determinants of attractiveness and mate selection. Physical aspects of attractiveness like facial symmetry, body height, and the voice are indicative of levels of sexual hormones, and thus of the likelihood of producing healthy offspring. Generally, indicators of higher levels of masculinity in males (based on higher levels of testosterone) and higher levels of femininity in females (based on higher levels of estrogen) are perceived as more attractive and are linked to better reproductive success. This leads to a dimorphism in attractiveness ratings: males tend to prefer female partners with higher f0 voices and smaller body sizes while female tend to prefer males with lower f0 voices and larger body sizes (Puts et al., 2012). Though these sex-related differences are important determinants of attraction, the polarization model of relationship distress suggests that they may become associated with dysfunction and increased risk for divorce as relationships mature. A change from attraction to distress is often accompanied by a change in attribution for the difference itself. Differences that are seen as enriching and complimentary are often experienced as desirable (Aron and Aron, 1997) while differences that are seen as short-comings and faults in the other commonly contribute to a cycle of distress. When differences are seen as faults in the other, spouses typically alternate between criticizing and blaming one another for the problems in their relationship and defending themselves from their partner’s attacks (i.e., they engage in a strong demand/withdraw cycle of conflict). As this process unfolds, the relationship becomes polarized, and the three core aspects of interaction, communication, perception, and physiology, become imbalanced (Burman and Margolin, 1992; Gottman, 1993), conflict ensues, and even routine interaction with the spouse becomes highly aversive. The aversive nature of this cycle typically results in increasing polarization over time and makes it very difficult for spouses to resolve even minor problems. Being stuck in the polarization cycle is highly distressing for spouses, and high levels of aversive arousal during interaction are one of the most robust predictors of risk for relationship distress and dissolution (Gottman and Levenson, 1992). Aversive arousal has most often been captured via well-established physiological indices like heart rate (HR), blood pressure (BP), or skin conductance (Larsen et al., 2008) as well as via endocrine measures like cortisol, or epinephrine (Robles and Kiecolt-Glaser, 2003; Ditzen et al., 2011). In addition to well-replicated findings based on physiological measures, a growing body of evidence shows that higher levels of vocally encoded emotional arousal, which are reflected in higher levels of f0, are similarly related to increased risk for a wide range of negative relationship outcomes.
Fundamental Frequency (f0)
As a mathematical quantity, fundamental frequency (f0) refers to the lowest frequency harmonic of the speech sound wave. This frequency is biologically determined by the pattern of vibration created by the vocal folds during phonation, the phase of speech production where the outward flow of air from the lungs is regulated by the larynx. Higher rates of opening and closing of the vocal folds across the glottis are associated with higher f0 values (measured in cycles per second or Hertz [Hz]). Perceptually, f0 is highly correlated with pitch where higher f0 values correspond to higher pitch (Juslin and Scherer, 2005). F0 can be easily calculated using Praat, a freely available, Windows-based software package (Boersma and Weenink, 2013; www.praat.org), to analyze existing audio recordings of speech in adequate quality. F0 is assessed continuously during human speech and can change rapidly. Different parameters like mean f0, f0 range, minimum f0, or maximum f0 can be calculated and used in empirical research. All of those can be calculated either at a very small scale (such as for each talk turn) or averaged across over the entirety of a conversation. As is true of much behavioral research, there is greater agreement about how to calculate f0 than there is about how to understand what f0 represents. F0 has been variously interpreted as an index of vocally encoded emotional arousal (see Juslin and Scherer, 2005 for a review; Weusthoff et al., 2013) and dominance (Puts et al., 2006; Borkowska and Pawlowski, 2011), and additional work demonstrates that f0 correlates with other factors such as age and pubertal (Hollien and Shipp, 1972; Brown et al., 1991; Hollien et al., 1994, 1997), phonemic and syntactic structure of speech (Whalen and Levitt, 1995). Recent work examining f0 during social interaction provides a framework for integrating the various interpretations of f0. In Weusthoff et al.’s 2013 examination of simultaneous associations between f0, biological sex, physiological indices of arousal, and social behavior, biological sex was the largest predictor of individual differences in f0 while physiological arousal and social behaviors were the best predictors of variance in f0 attributable to a specific social interaction. These results suggest that f0 can be understood as conveying information about both traits (such as biological sex) and states (momentary physiological arousal and social behavior) and highlights the need for careful analysis of f0 to allow for specific interpretations.
Method of Review
In contrast to the nascent body of research examining f0 during marital interactions,f0 has been intensively studied using different research paradigms across multiple disciplines (e.g., single conversations recorded during naturally occurring stressful events like the New York City blackout in 1977, Streeter et al., 1982, or emotion portrayals by professional actors, Banse and Scherer, 1996; see Scherer, 2003 for a review). For example, a key-word based search on SCOPUS (using the search terms “social interaction” AND “verbal” OR “non-verbal” in “Keywords, Abstract, or Title”) yielded more than 2000 results. Here, we focus on research with a specific focus on relationship formation and maintenance. The body of work on f0 and attraction is robust and well-developed. Thus, we seek to provide a representative sampling of the main findings in this area. In contrast, there are many fewer studies of naturalistic social interaction in general much less about marital interaction specifically. As others have noted, studies using naturalistic vocal expressions of affect with sufficient intensity of the emotions displayed are needed in vocal expression research in order to obtain speech that offers sufficient levels of “ecological validity” (Juslin and Scherer, 2005). Communication in close relationships seems to fulfill these criterions but research has only recently begun to take f0 into account here.
Research with human participants can be split up in two further groups: whether interlocutors had some form of relationship with each other before interacting, or not. In the case of pre-existing relationship, four main areas of relationships have been covered: the ones between parents and infants (see Irwin, 2003 for a review), between psychotherapists and clients (see Greenberg and Pascual-Leone, 2006 for a review), between physicians and patients (see Hassan et al., 2007; Hulsman et al., 2011 for a review), and between spouses in intimate relationship. Independent of targets, f0 has been found to be associated with arousal. As it it beyond the scope of this review to cover this all, the interested reader is referred to the according reviews. In couples interactions, however, the role of vocally encoded emotional arousal has not been investigated closely. This review aims at looking into f0 and its associations with couple functioning more closely. The authors identified a total number of N = 5 studies for this task. All analyses were based on dyadic communication settings (conflict discussions) between spouses from long-term or married heterosexual couples. Instructions for the tasks were standardized, and recordings of the interactions were videotaped during assessment sessions conducted in research laboratories. Participants were either English (Baucom et al., 2009, 2011), or German (Baucom et al., 2012b; Kliem et al., 2012; Weusthoff et al., 2013) native speakers (for a detailed description of the studies included in this review, please see Table 1).
As also described in Weusthoff et al. (2013), f0 was obtained continuously from the speech of each person during the problem discussion of the assessment point(s). Conversations were segmented per speaker using audio editing programs like Adobe Premier Pro, resulting in data per speaker containing only conversation parts during which he or she talked separately without any other human, or background sound being present. Bandpass filtering was applied to all voice samples prior to further analyses (calculating f0 values) in order to restrict f0 values to the normal range of emotional adult speech. The typical range for male speakers is 75–150Hz, for female speakers 150–300Hz, though wider limits can be observed during highly aroused emotional states (Owren and Bachorowski, 2007). Common f0 mean values are around 225 Hz for women, and about 120 Hz for men. Across the lifespan, women’s mean f0 decreases while male speakers’ mean f0 scores also decrease first, but start to rise again. Changes in adults are most prominent for both sexes between age 50 and 60, with women additionally experiencing hormonal influences associated with the onset of the menopause (Hollien and Shipp, 1972; Brown et al., 1991; Hollien et al., 1997). Though it is possible for human speakers to generate sound outside the typical filtering limits of 75 and 300 Hz, f0 scores outside of this range are very likely resulting from background machine or electronic noise. Minimum, maximum, and mean f0 values were generated separately for each spouse by analyzing their respective segmented audio recordings using Praat, a free multiple platform program (Boersma and Weenink, 2013). F0 range was calculated separately for each partner at each assessment by subtracting the partner’s averaged minimum f0 from the partner’s averaged maximum f0 across the whole conversation. In Baucom et al.’s (2011) study, f0 mean was derived from raw f0 scores across the whole conversation. For a more detailed description of potential f0 range indices, please see Table 1 in Baucom et al. (2012a).
f0 in Intimate Relationship Research
Due to three reasons, it is important to further investigate the role of f0 in intimate relationships. One, relationship deterioration and/or separation are associated with tremendous costs for affected individuals and society as a whole, both concurrently and in the long run. Dysfunctional relationships lead to poorer mental and physical health in spouses (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001), a higher likelihood for divorce and reduced levels of social support (Amato, 2010). Due to higher levels of unhealthy behaviors like smoking, even a decreased life expectancy (Larson and Halfon, 2013) in affected children has been observed. Second, couple-relationship education programs (CRE) aim at reducing the risk of relationship dissolution by teaching communication skills and ways of dealing with aspects of couple’s emotional lives, e.g., EPL – Ein Partnerschaftliches Lernprogramm (“A Learning Program for Couples”; Kaiser et al., 1998), especially during conflict discussions. These programs are known to be highly effective in doing so both in the short and in the long run (Hawkins et al., 2008; Hahlweg and Richter, 2010). However, the mechanisms of change in CRE and how emotional arousal affects the work in and outcome of CRE are still unknown. Communication is assumed to be the central mechanism of change, however, empirical studies using the paradigm of couples having videotaped conflict discussions that are later rated and / or analyzed with regard to different aspects of behavior have not consistently supported this supposition. One reason for this might be the rather simplistic ways of operationalizing a complex behavior like human communication as single elements like positive, or negative utterances (Christensen, 2010). Therefore, research should consider several aspects of communication simultaneously in a multi-channel approach (Gottman and Notarius, 2002). Third, though emotional arousal is known to play a crucial role in relationship dissolution (Gottman and Levenson, 1992), the specific pathways and interactions with other important variables like communication behavior, or relationship satisfaction are still unknown (Weusthoff et al., 2013). As future research in this field should focus on how to “modify emotion-driven, dysfunctional, and destructive interactional behavior,” and to “elicit avoided, emotion-based” behavior (Christensen, 2010, pp. 36-37), there is additional need for an objective tool for detecting and explaining emotional arousal in the context of couple interactions and intimate relationships.
During courtship, f0 is one of the evolutionary most important signals for non-visual gender discrimination (which is important for successful identification of potential mates; Junger et al., 2013), and for judging the attractiveness of a potential spouse (Borkowska and Pawlowski, 2011). Very masculine voices in males (perceived as low voice pitch, and thus low f0), and very feminine voices in females (perceived as high voice pitch, and thus high f0) are perceived as particularly attractive (DeBruine et al., 2006; Feinberg et al., 2006, 2012; Borkowska and Pawlowski, 2011; O’Connor et al., 2012). These perceptions are attenuated in both men and women during high-fertile phases of the female menstrual cycle (Puts, 2005; Pipitone and Gallup, 2008; Hodges-Simeon et al., 2010). High vocal masculinity in males are linked to high levels of testosterone, and associated with a higher likelihood for producing healthy offspring (O’Connor et al., 2012). However, in the long run, males with higher levels of vocal masculinity report a larger number of different sexual partners, and are less likely to engage in relationship maintaining and parental behaviors (O’Connor et al., 2011, 2012). It also seems plausible that f0 during relationship initiation stages displays high levels of arousal that is taken by partner as an indicator for higher engagement in a relationship, also hinting at a process similar to polarization.
During maintenance, less research on f0’s role has been conducted so far. F0 has most often been studied in couples’ conflict interactions where it seems to index levels of emotional arousal. F0 during couple conflict has been demonstrated to be significantly positively associated with multiple cardiac, and endocrine indices of arousal, and linked to more negative and less positive communication behavior. F0 is thought to simultaneously display autonomic physiological as well as socially learned reactions in one signal (Weusthoff et al., 2013). Furthermore, it has been shown that spouses influence each other in their levels of arousal. If one partner is highly aroused, it is becomes more difficult for the other one to maintain on a functional level of arousal, thus leading to polarization processes with regard to f0 (Baucom and Atkins, 2013).
As also noted elsewhere (Weusthoff et al., 2013), f0 offers a number of methodological and conceptual benefits with regard to being further used in research on romantic relationships. Its non-invasive nature and lack of need for additional equipment (only a good audio recording device is needed) makes the assessment of f0 an excellent candidate for data collection in situations like conflict interactions that are most often videotaped, or for post-hoc analyses (e.g., spousal discussions, or therapy sessions). Additionally, f0 can be computed and analyzed from the videotapes even after conducting a study in which it was not of primary interest. Furthermore, f0 is more closely associated with individual psychological than physical distress (Johannes et al., 2007), making it more sensitive to detect changes in psychological load and less prone to artifacts that influence physiological measures of arousal like HR or skin conductance (e.g., movements, Sloan and Kring, 2007). Being based on physical properties of speech, f0 can be analyzed objectively and independent of the researcher’s native language and culture (Weusthoff et al., 2013). Perhaps most importantly, f0 is directly involved in and available during the process of communication and can be perceived by a listener (Baucom et al., 2011). There is good evidence that partners in communication directly respond to each other’s f0 without being aware of this fact (Gregory and Webster, 1996). Similar to cardiovascular indices of arousal (e.g., HR), f0 seems to encode aspects of emotional arousal that are part of an autonomic process not entirely controllable by a speaker, similar to conditioned emotional responses (Kliem et al., 2012).
Merging these theories, expression of emotions (emotional expression) toward one’s spouse can be considered as a central part of well-functioning intimate relationships, the role of underlying emotional arousal, however, seems to be somewhat different. Endocrine and psychophysiological indices of heightened levels of emotional arousal have consistently been linked to unwanted relationship outcomes like dysfunctional communication behavior, higher risk for divorce and separation, or lower levels of relationship satisfaction (Gottman and Levenson, 1992; Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001; Gottman and Notarius, 2002). As emotions and their underlying processes like arousal are essential parts of social interactions (Juslin and Laukka, 2003) and can influence them heavily (Scherer, 2005), it makes them important information not only for researchers in the social interaction area but also for people’s everyday lives.
Studies Reviewed
Summary of Findings
Across studies, languages, and gender, f0 has been treated as an index of emotional arousal. Like other indices f0 has been associated with a number of different negative aspects of couple functioning both concurrently and in the long run. Significant associations have been found in conflict interactions of distressed couples, either participating in couples therapy, or in CRE. More specifically, higher levels of a person’s physiological indicators of emotional arousal like HR, BP, or salivary cortisol were associated with higher levels of one’s own f0. Higher levels of a spouse’s f0 have also been linked to higher levels of negative communication behavior, both observed and self-reported, and to lower levels of observed non-verbal positive communication behavior. In highly distressed couples, the likelihood for long-term success of couple therapy (being in the recovered range 2 years after treatment termination) was higher when wives’ displayed lower levels of f0 in a conflict discussion prior to treatment. Elevated levels of f0 in conflict discussions prior to participation in CRE also lead to a higher likelihood for separation and divorce, and a smaller number of communication skills remembered 11 years after the CRE.
Discussion
This review aimed at examining research on vocally encoded emotional arousal, namely f0, in close personal relationship communication (interaction between spouses). Significant associations were found with psychophysiological and endocrine indices of emotional arousal, observed and self-reported communication behavior, and relationship functioning (stability and satisfaction). Furthermore, f0 has been found to be related to skills remembrance in CRE. Significant results were found for concurrent as well as longitudinal links between f0 and variables of interest, and for a time frame as long as 11 years. Significant gender differences emerged for f0 range’s association with some variables of interest but not for f0 mean’s.
Findings Considered from an Evolutionary Perspective
Across studies, f0 was analyzed as an index of emotional arousal thought to display information about the internal state of the speaker to an interaction partner via the human voice. Stable associations between f0 and different forms of communication behavior (positive, negative, observed, self-reported, verbal, and non-verbal) emerged across studies. Social signaling theories assume evolutionary reasons for this: In order to heighten chances of survival in ancient hunter-gatherer societies, emotions were adaptive developments helping to avoid danger and to cooperate with others (Buss, 2005). Vocally encoded emotional arousal enables individuals to communicate emotions non-verbally from one person to another, independent of words and language. Vocal communication of emotion is more likely to appear in goal-relevant behavior (Juslin and Laukka, 2003). F0 scores indexing vocally encoded emotional arousal should therefore be higher during goal-relevant behavior. What is considered as goal-relevant behavior depends on situational aspects: Couple and family conflict is conceptually thought of as a chronic stressor (Doss et al., 2004) where often a wide range of negative communication behavior (like DW) is displayed in order to achieve change in one’s spouse (Christensen et al., 2006) and high levels of emotional arousal are displayed via multiple channels (Kiecolt-Glaser and Newton, 2001). Positive associations between f0 and these variables as found in the reviewed studies investigating conflict could be indexing goal-achieving strategies by spouses.
With regard to the theoretical foundation of emotional arousal, coordinated responses in multiple physiological systems stem from the same source, namely the cognitive appraisal in a given situation and the following changes in autonomous and somatic nervous system activity (Scherer, 2009). These changes were assessed via multiple channel of arousal in Weusthoff et al.’s 2013 study, with the associations between f0 and psychophysiological indices of arousal being in expected directions. With the periaqueductal grey (PAG), a brain-stem based areal (and thus an evolutionary old one) in humans is involved in both vocal and cardiovascular responses to different kinds of stress. The PAG is thought to integrate emotional aspect of stress responses into the autonomic nervous system responses in different channels (Linnman et al., 2012). These findings suggest that f0 as an index of emotional arousal, and thus the intensity of the emotional reaction (Weusthoff et al., 2013), are influenced by both basic biological processes as well as by socially learned communication behaviors stemming from a similar evolutionary basis (Juslin and Laukka, 2003).
Different brain regions are found to be involved in processing male and female voices (Sokhi et al., 2005), and discriminatory performance for human voices was better for opposite-sex stimuli than for same sex-stimuli (faster identification; Junger et al., 2013). F0 seems to enable a stronger attendance toward stimuli higher in social significance like potential mates (Feinberg, 2008), which could be considered as an important aspect of relationship functioning not only in initiation but also in maintaining phases. The studies covered in this review seem to foster this interpretation: during later phases of relationship and especially in “rough” times, spouses seem to especially use f0 as a non-verbal signal in relationship-relevant information like expression of distress.
Findings with Regard to Gender Differences
Across age and interaction settings, gender differences were found in a number of studies. Biologically speaking, f0 is based on vocal cord length and tension. Given the physiological differences in body size and thus, throat length between men and women, gender differences in f0 emerge after puberty with females on average having significantly higher f0 scores than males (Titze, 1989). Two studies in this review (Baucom et al., 2012b; Weusthoff et al., 2013) explicitly investigated gender differences and as expected found higher f0 range scores for female than for male speakers. Except for Baucom et al. (2011) and Weusthoff et al. (2013), all studies investigating adult speakers included in this review found variables of interest to have significant associations with f0 range for female speakers only, or found different associations for men and women. Given that female partners are more likely to seek help with regard to problems and/or conflict in close personal relationships (O’Brien, 1988) it seems also plausible that gender differences could stem from different goals and resulting goal-oriented behavior in males and females (especially as no associations being significant for men only emerged). Finding the best partner to produce healthy offspring seems to be an important goal during selection of a potential mate and sex partner during courtship. Significant associations between perceived, and self-rated attractiveness and health, and f0 for both sexes during sexual selection (though due to endocrine reasons in different directions, with higher attractiveness and perceived health being associated to f0 scores in women and lower f0 scores in men; Puts, 2010) also hint at f0 being an important aspect in goal-relevant behavior.
Limitations
The work reviewed in this manuscript has taken into account only three indicators of vocally encoded emotional arousal out of a wide variety of potential ones (Juslin and Scherer, 2005, pp. 103-104): f0 range, f0 mean, and f0-time-to-peak as a time-varying aspect of f0. This has happened for a number of reasons. As earlier work on vocally encoded emotional arousal has focused on f0 mean in a number of different research settings (i.e., f0 in speech of psychiatric patients, Tolkmitt et al., 1982; or emotion portrayal by professional actors, Banse and Scherer, 1996), it was chosen as the parameter of interest in the first study conducted on f0 in close personal relationships (Baucom et al., 2011) in order to enable comparisons to these published empirical findings. However, among the different f0 indices that can be calculated f0 range seems to bear a number of advantages in research on close personal relationships (see Juslin and Scherer, 2005 for a review), and was therefore chosen for the studies conducted later. f0 range is considered to be the cleanest vocal indicator of emotional arousal, and to depict the biggest amount of information on emotional arousal (Juslin and Scherer, 2005; Busso et al., 2009). Furthermore, it facilitates the interpretation of gender differences as it adjusts for potential individual differences by way of its calculation (subtracting the individual’s minimum f0 score from the individual’s f0 maximum score; Weusthoff et al., 2013). Nonetheless, it is possible that other indicators of vocally emotional arousal (e.g., jitter) might contain additional information on the nature of emotional arousal in close personal relationship communication.
Though biological reasons for differences in male and female voices are well-documented and have been discussed in this review, it remains unclear why expected sex differences for f0 indices and their associations with different variables of interest did not emerge consistently across studies. Particularly, none of the studies found significant gender differences in the associations between f0 mean (compared to studies using f0 range) and variables of interest. Future research should regard gender as a covariate in f0 research in close personal relationship but more detailed investigations on the details of gender differences in f0 indices are needed.
Investigating emotional arousal in human speech using f0 includes the drawback of participants being able to willingly influence and control f0 to a certain degree. Empirical evidence has shown that conscious manipulations can result in both elevated and lowered pitch levels that are identifiable by a communication partner (Kuenzel, 2000). However, given the arousing, stressful, and cognitively demanding situation of couple conflict, it is quite unlikely that participants in the studies reviewed made use of this hypothetical possibility.
Implications for Clinical Work and Future Research
As f0 seems to be able to display an individual’s internal levels of emotional arousal, other dyadic settings in which emotional arousal is considered to be important information could also benefit from research on vocally encoded arousal. During social support interactions between spouses, showing the interaction partner positive and helpful behaviors in order to help him or her to deal with stressful situations is the main goal. Social support is considered to consist of different positive and helpful behaviors being observable in communication between interaction partners leading to changes in individual cardiovascular, and endocrine functioning which influences individual health outcomes. Consistent with this model, lower levels of social support behavior have been empirically linked to higher levels of physiological indices of arousal (most often BP), and to poorer health outcomes in affected individuals (Uchino et al., 1996). Given the associations between BP, and HR with f0 in couple conflict, vocally encoded emotional arousal also seems to be important during social support interactions between spouses. However, in comparison to conflict discussions, more positive and less emotionally arousing behaviors seem be goal-relevant in social support and should therefore be displayed more often between interaction partners (Verhoftstadt et al., 2005). Links between f0 and variables of interest in social support behavior could therefore be of different direction and magnitude than the ones found in conflict discussions.
Communication Accomodation Theory (CAT; Giles and Coupland, 1991) describes changes in partners’ communication styles during a conversation leading to more, or less similitarity between the partners (convergence and divergence). Convergence can occur on different levels (e.g., pitch pattern, speech rate, or emotional expression), and has empirically been shown to occur in various dyads. Convergence seems to happen somewhat naturally but is associated with different outcomes depending on the interaction setting and other external factors in intimate relationships. Convergence seems to be a beneficial process during interactions asking for social support between partners. For example, high levels of emotional convergence (meaning similarity in emotional expressions between spouses) while sharing emotional events of one’s day are related to higher levels of concurrent and longitudinal relationship satisfaction, and higher relationship stability (Anderson et al., 2003). During couple conflict, however, convergence in emotional expression and behavior like the DW pattern have negative impacts on relationship functioning and outcomes (Lee et al., 2012; Baucom and Atkins, 2013). A closer look at potential reasons for these differential effects of convergence in couples’ communication could be a fruitful avenue for future research. Are the associations due to levels of relationship satisfaction (happy vs. unhappy couples), do they depend on the context of the interaction (social support vs. conflict), or do both factors and/or a third one contribute?
Vocal characteristics between talkshow host and guests have also been demonstrated to converge across the course of an interview. However, across different dyads, status and/or power seem to be an important factor influencing convergence: The conversation partner lower in status and power (having a greater “need for social approval”; Giles and Coupland, 1991, p. 73) exhibits more change in vocal and emotional expressions, moving in the direction of the more powerful partner (Gregory and Webster, 1996).
Power processes are also influential in couples communication (Baucom et al., 2011), and are interdependently linked to each other. For example, f0 in distressed spouses’ problem-solving discussions is known to converge in terms of magnitude: if one spouse is highly aroused, the other partner is also more likely to be highly aroused. Furthermore, this covariation seems to lead to problems in partner’s ability to regulate their own state of arousal to a comfortable level (Baucom and Atkins, 2013).
Given these findings, it seems likely that spouses’ f0 scores could also be associated with each other across speakers. Gottman and Notarius (2002, p.185) explicitly state that there is a “need for continued focus on sequences or patterns of interaction” in order to identify beneficial and harmful aspects of spousal communication. Sequential analyses of f0 could shed light on presence, magnitude, and direction of interdependent and cyclical aspects of emotional arousal in couple communication.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG; DFG Fe 263/5-1, Ha 1400/4-1, Ha 1400/16-1, and Ha 1400/16-2) awarded to Kurt Hahlweg and from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (F32 HD060410) and the University of Utah awarded to Brian R. Baucom.
References
Amato, P. R. (2010). Research on divorce: continuing trends and new developments. J. Marriage Fam. 72, 650–666. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00723.x
Anderson, C., Keltner, D., and John, O. P. (2003). Emotional convergence between people over time. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84, 1054–1068. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1054
Aron, A., and Aron, E. N. (1997). “Self-expansion motivation and including other in the self,” in Handbook of Personal Relationships: Theory, Research, and Interventions, ed. S. Duck (London: Wiley), 251–270.
Banse, R., and Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal emotion expression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 614–636. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.614
Baucom, B. R., and Atkins, D. C. (2013). “Polarization in marriage,” in Family Theories: A content-based approach, eds M. Fine and F. Fincham (New York: Routledge), 145–166.
Baucom, B. R., Atkins, D. C., Eldridge, K., McFarland, P., Sevier, M., and Christensen, A. (2011). The language of demand/withdraw: verbal and vocal expression in dyadic interactions. J. Fam. Psychol. 25, 570–580. doi: 10.1037/a0024064
Baucom, B. R., Atkins, D. C., Simpson, L., and Christensen, A. (2009). Prediction of response to treatment in a randomized clinical trial of couple therapy: a 2-year follow-up. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 77, 160–173. doi: 10.1037/a0014405
Baucom, B. R., Saxbe, D. E., Ramos, M. C., Spies, L. A., Iturralde, E., Duman, S., et al. (2012a). Correlates and characteristics of adolescents’ encoded emotional arousal during family conflict. Emotion 12, 1281–1291. doi: 10.1037/a0028872
Baucom, B. R., Weusthoff, S., Atkins, D. C., and Hahlweg, K. (2012b). Greater emotional arousal predicts poorer long-term memory of communication skills in couples. Behav. Res. Ther. 50, 442–447. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.03.010
Boersma, P., and Weenink, D. (2013). Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program], Version 5.3.42. Available at: from http://www.praat.org (retrieved March 22, 2013).
Borkowska, B., and Pawlowski, B. (2011). Female voice frequency in the context of dominance and attractiveness perception. Anim. Behav. 82, 55–59. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011. 03.024
Brown, W. S. Jr., Morris, R. J., Hollien, H., and Howell, E. (1991). Speaking fundamental frequency characteristics as a function of age and professional singing. J. Voice 5,310–315. doi: 10.1016/S0892-1997(05) 80061-X
Burman, B., and Margolin, G. (1992). Analysis of the association between marital relationships and health problems: an interactional perspective. Psychol. Bull. 112, 39–63. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.39
Busso, C., Lee, S., and Narayanan, S. (2009). Analysis of emotionally salient aspects of fundamental frequency for emotion detection. IEEE T Audio Speech 17, 582–596. doi: 10.1109/TASL.2008.2009578
Caughlin, J. P., and Huston, T. L. (2002). A contextual analysis of the association between demand/withdraw and marital satisfaction. Pers. Relationsh. 9, 95–119. doi: 10.1111/1475-6811.00007
Christensen, A. (2010). “A unified protocol for couple therapy,” in Enhancing Couples. The Shape of Couple Therapy to Come, eds K. Hahlweg, M. Grawe-Gerber, and D. H. Baucom (Cambridge: Hogrefe), 33–46.
Christensen, A., Eldridge, K., Catta-Preta, A. B., Lim, V. R., and Santagata, R. (2006). Cross-cultural consistency of the demand-withdraw interaction pattern in couples. J. Marriage Fam. 68, 1029–1044. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00311.x
DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., Boothroyd, L. G., Perrett, D. I., Penton-Voak, I. S., et al. (2006). Correlated preferences for facial masculinity and ideal or actual partner’s masculinity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 1355–1360. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3445
Ditzen, B., Hahlweg, K., Fehm-Wolfsdorf, G., Groth, T., and Baucom, D. H. (2011). Assisting couples to develop healthy relationships: effects of couples relationship education on cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology 36, 597–607. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.019
Doss, B., Simpson, L. S., and Christensen, A. (2004).Why do couples seek marital therapy? Prof. Psychol. Res. 35,608–614. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.35.6.608
Feinberg, D. R. (2008). Are human faces and voices ornaments signaling common underlying cues to mate value? Evol. Anthropol. 17, 112–118. doi: 10.1002/evan.20166
Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., O’Connor, J. J. M., and Tigue, C. C. (2012). Women’s self-perceived health and attractiveness predict their male vocal masculinity preferences in different directions across short- and long-term relationship contexts. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 66, 413–418. doi: 10.1007/s00265-011-1287-y
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law-Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Horm. Behav. 49, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.07.004
Gottman, J. M. (1993). A theory of marital dissolution and stability. J. Fam. Psychol. 7, 57–75. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.7.1.57
Gottman, J. M., and Levenson, R. (1992). Marital processes predictive of later dissolution: behavior, physiology, and health. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 221–233. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.221
Gottman, J. M., and Notarius, C. I. (2002). Martial research in the 20th century and a research agenda for the 21st century. Fam. Proc. 41, 159–197. doi: 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2002.41203.x
Greenberg, L. S., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2006). Emotion in psychotherapy: a practive-friendly research review. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 611–630. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20252
Gregory, S. W., and Webster, S. (1996). A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perception. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 70, 1231–1240. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1231
Hahlweg, K., Baucom, D. H., Grawe-Gerber, M., and Snyder, D. K. (2010). “Strengthening couples and families: Dissemination of interventions for the treatment and prevention of couple distress,” in Enhancing Couples, eds K. Hahlweg, M. Grawe-Gerber, and D. H. Baucom (Cambridge: Hogrefe), 3-30.
Hahlweg, K., and Richter, D. (2010). Prevention of marital distress and instability: results of an 11-year follow-up study. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 377–383. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.010
Hassan, I., McCabe, R., and Priebe, S. (2007). Professional-patient communication in the treatment of mental disorders: a review. Commun. Med. 4, 141–152. doi: 10.1515/CAM.2007.018
Hawkins, A. J., Blanchard, V. L., Baldwin, S. A., and Fawcett, E. B. (2008). Does marriage and relationship education work? A meta-analytic study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 76, 723–734. doi: 10.1037/a0012584
Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Gaulin, S. J. C., and Puts, D. A. (2010). Different vocal parameters predict perceptions of dominance and attractiveness. Hum. Nat. 21, 406–427. doi: 10.1007/s12110-010-9101-5
Hollien, H., Hollien, P. A., and de Jong, G. (1997). Effects of three parameters on speaking fundamental frequency. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102, 2984–2992. doi: 10.1121/1.420353
Hollien, H., Green, R., and Massey, K. (1994). Longitudinal research on adolescent voice change in male. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95, 2646–2654. doi: 10.1121/1.411275
Hollien, H., and Shipp, T. (1972). Speaking fundamental frequency and chronological age in males. J. Speech Hear Res. 15, 155–159.
Hulsman, R. L., Smets, E. M. A., Karemaker, J. M., and de Haes, H. J. C. J. M. (2011). The psychophysiology of medical communication. Linking two worlds of research. Patient Educ. Couns. 84, 420–427. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2011.05.008
Irwin, J. R. (2003). Parent and nonparent perception of the multimodal infant cry. Infancy 4, 503–516. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0404-06
Jacobson, N. S., and Christensen, A. (1998). Acceptance and Change in Couple Therapy: A Therapist’s Guide to Transforming Relationships. New York: Norton.
Johannes, B., Wittels, P., Enne, R., Eisinger, G., Castro, C. A., Thomas, J. L., et al. (2007). Non-linear function model of voice pitch dependency on physical and mental load. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 101, 267–276. doi: 10.1007/s00421-007-0496-6
Junger, J., Pauly, K., Bröhr, S., Birkholz, P., Neuschaefer-Rube, C., Kohler, C., et al. (2013). Sex matters: Neural correlates of voice gender perception. Neuroimage 79, 275–287. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.105
Juslin, P. N., and Laukka, P. (2003). Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: different channels, same code? Psychol. Bull. 129, 770–814. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.770
Juslin, P. N., and Scherer, K. (2005). “Vocal expression of affect,” in The new Handbook of Methods in Nonverbal Behavioral Research, eds J. Harrigan, R. Rosenthal, and K. R. Scherer (New York: Oxford University Press), 65–136.
Kaiser, A., Hahlweg, K., Fehm-Wolfsdorf, G., and Groth, T. (1998). The efficacy of a compact psychoeducational group training program for married couples. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 66, 753–760. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.5.753
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., and Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and health: his and hers. Psychol. Bull. 127, 472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472
Kliem, S., Weusthoff, S., Baucom, B. R., and Hahlweg, K. (2012). “Predicting long-term risk for divorce using non-parametric conditional survival trees,” in Poster presented at the 46th Annual Convention of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, National Harbor, USA.
Kuenzel, H. J. (2000). Effects of voice disguise on speaking fundamental frequency. For. Linguist 7, 1350–1771.
Larsen, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Poehlmann, K. M., Ito, T. A., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). “The psychophysiology of emotion,” in The Handbook of Emotions, eds R. Lewis, M. Haviland-Jones, and L. F. Barrett (New York: Guilford), 180–195.
Larson, K., and Halfon, N. (2013). Parental divorce and adult longevity. Int. J. Public Health 58, 89–97. doi: 10.1007/s00038-012-0373-x
Lee, C., Baucom, B. R., Katsamanis, A., Christensen, A., Georgiou, P., and Narayanan, S. (2012). Modeling vocal interdependence during marital interaction: a vector-based approach. Talk presented at the 46th Annual Convention of the Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies, National Harbor, USA.
Linnman, C., Moulton, E. A., Barmettler, G., Becerra, L., and Borsool, D. (2012). Neuroimaging of the periaqueductal gray: state of the field. Neuroimage 60, 505–522. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.095
O’Brien, M. (1988). Men and fathers in therapy. J. Fam. Ther. 10, 109–123. doi: 10.1046/j..1988.00306.x
O’Connor, J. J. M., Fraccaro, P. J., and Feinberg, D. R. (2012). The influence of male voice pitch on women’s perceptions of relationship investment. J. Evol. Psychol. 10, 1–13. doi: 10.1556/JEP.10.2012.1.1
O’Connor, J. J. M., Re, D. E., and Feinberg, D. R. (2011). Voice pitch influences perception of sexual infidelity. Evol. Psychol. 11, 64–78.
Owren, M. J., and Bachorowski, J. (2007). “Measuring emotion-related arousal,” in Handbook of Emotion Elicitation and Assessment, eds J. A. Coan, and J. J. B. Allen (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 239–266.
Pipitone, R. N., and Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2008). Women’s voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evol. Hum. Behav. 29, 268–274. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.02.001
Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evol. Hum. Behav. 26, 388–397. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.03.001
Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 157–175. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.02.005
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., and Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 283–296. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.003
Puts, D. A., Jones, B. C., and DeBruine, L. M. (2012). Sexual selection on human faces and voices. J. Sex Res. 49, 227–243. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2012.658924
Robles, T. F., and Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (2003). The physiology of marriage: pathways to health. Physiol. Behav. 79, 409–416. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00160-4
Scherer, K. R. (2003). Vocal communication of emotion: a review of research paradigm. Speech Commun. 40, 227–256. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00084-5
Scherer, K. R. (2005). What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc. Sc Inform. 44, 695–725. doi: 10.1177/0539018405058216
Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: evidence for the component process model. Cogn. Emot. 23, 1307–1351. doi: 10.1080/02699930902928969
Sloan, D. M., and Kring, A. M. (2007). Measuring changes in emotion during psychotherapy: Conceptual and methodological issues. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 14, 307–322. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.2007.00092.x
Sokhi, D. S., Hunter, M. D., Wilkinson, I. D., and Woodruff, P. W. R. (2005). Male and female voices activate distinct regions in the male brain. Neuroimage 27, 572–578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.04.023
Streeter, L. A., Macdonald, N. H., Apple, W., Krauss, R. M., and Galotti, K. M. (1982). Acoustic and perceptual indicators of emotional stress. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 73, 1354–1360. doi: 10.1121/1.389239
Titze, I. (1989). Physiological and acoustic differences between male and female voices. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 85, 1699–1707. doi: 10.1121/1.397959
Tolkmitt, F., Helfrich, H., Standke, R., and Scherer, K. R. (1982). Vocal indicators of psychiatric treatment effects in depressives and schizophrenics. J. Commun. Disord. 15, 209–222. doi: 10.1016/0021-9924(82)90034-X
Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., and Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: a review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implpications for health. Psychol. Bull. 119, 488–531. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.488
Verhoftstadt, L. L., Buyssee, A., Ickes, W., De Clercq, A., and Peene, O. J. (2005). Conflict and support interactions in marriage: an analysis of couples’ interactive behavior and on-line cognition. Pers. Relationsh. 12, 23–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00100.x
Weusthoff, S., Baucom, B. R., and Hahlweg, K. (2013). Understanding fundamental frequency during couple conflict: an analysis of physiological, behavioral, and sex-linked information encoded in emotional arousal. J. Fam. Psychol. 27, 212–220. doi: 10.1037/a0031887
Keywords: fundamental frequency, attraction, emotional arousal, romantic relationships, couples, communication, conflict
Citation: Weusthoff S, Baucom BR and Hahlweg K (2013) The Siren song of vocal fundamental frequency for romantic relationships. Front. Psychol. 4:439. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00439
Received: 28 March 2013; Accepted: 25 June 2013;
Published online: 15 July 2013.
Edited by:
Anjali Bhatara, Université Paris Descartes, FranceReviewed by:
Alexandra Suppes, Columbia University, USACheryl Carmichael, City University of New York, USA
Copyright: © 2013 Weusthoff, Baucom and Hahlweg. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
*Correspondence: Sarah Weusthoff, Clinical Psychology, Psychotherapy, and Assessment, Department of Psychology, Technische Universität Braunschweig, Humboldtstrasse 33, 38106 Braunschweig, Germany e-mail: s.weusthoff@tu-bs.de