Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 03 June 2013
Sec. Perception Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Aftereffects in face processing View all 14 articles

Face adaptation effects: reviewing the impact of adapting information, time, and transfer

  • 1Department of Psychology, Humboldt-University, Berlin, Germany
  • 2Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany
  • 3Department of General Psychology and Methodology, University of Bamberg, Bamberg, Germany

The ability to adapt is essential to live and survive in an ever-changing environment such as the human ecosystem. Here we review the literature on adaptation effects of face stimuli to give an overview of existing findings in this area, highlight gaps in its research literature, initiate new directions in face adaptation research, and help to design future adaptation studies. Furthermore, this review should lead to better understanding of the processing characteristics as well as the mental representations of face-relevant information. The review systematizes studies at a behavioral level in respect of a framework which includes three dimensions representing the major characteristics of studies in this field of research. These dimensions comprise (1) the specificity of adapting face information, e.g., identity, gender, or age aspects of the material to be adapted to (2) aspects of timing (e.g., the sustainability of adaptation effects) and (3) transfer relations between face images presented during adaptation and adaptation tests (e.g., images of the same or different identities). The review concludes with options for how to combine findings across different dimensions to demonstrate the relevance of our framework for future studies.

The term adaptation refers to the ability to adjust to novel information and experiences. This ability to adapt is essential for living and surviving in an ever-changing environment such as the human ecosystem (Carbon and Ditye, 2012). Visual adaptation in particular is an effect of the processes by which the visual system encodes and represents information, and includes a process by which the visual system (passively or actively) alters its function in response to the lack of fit between mental representations and perceived objects (e.g., Clifford et al., 2000; Clifford and Rhodes, 2005). Such responses result in adaptation effects. In experimental situations, such adaptation effects are typically assessed in an adaptation test phase after an adaptation phase. Intensive investigations of these adaptation effects provides an excellent opportunity for an exploration and deeper understanding of the processing architecture as well as the representation of particular stimuli (Li et al., 2008a; Webster, 2011).

The present paper includes a systematic review about the literature on adaptation effects of face stimuli1: which areas does this review on face adaptation cover and which areas does it negotiate? This review focuses on empirical studies that investigate face adaptation effects on a behavioral level; this is typically realized by an overt categorization of face stimuli in a test phase. In fact, we focus on adaptation under optimal conditions in a fully developed and (more or less) optimally functioning cognitive system; i.e., we review findings of studies typically investigating adaptation effects in younger adults possessing face recognition skills that are particularly impressive, for instance the fact that normal persons can discriminate thousands of faces (Jeffery and Rhodes, 2011) when they reach so-called “face expertise” (Schwaninger et al., 2003). This focus on complex objects of the face category is realized in an exclusive and extensive way; that is, we do not relate findings in the area of face adaptation effects to other visual coding mechanisms such as color coding as realized in previous work (Webster, 2011; Webster and MacLeod, 2011). By reviewing face issues exclusively, we assume to provide the main aim of this review most efficiently: we aim to clearly highlight and systematize existing findings as well as gaps in the research literature in the area of face adaptation effects. This systematization should stimulate new directions in face adaptation research and help to design future adaptation studies. In contrast to what we provide, we do not include results about the adaptation of neural processes to face stimuli: for instance, studies on modulations of the N170 as a result of prior adaptation (e.g., Kovács et al., 2006; Kloth et al., 2010) as questions regarding this area of research refer to further dimensions and use different theoretical frameworks, mostly based on specific brain processes and structures. In addition, we omit research from developmental and evolutionary perspectives on face adaptation effects as they were already the major aim of recent alternative review papers (Leopold and Rhodes, 2010; Jeffery and Rhodes, 2011).

We start this review by presenting a framework that enables a systematic organization of findings in the field of face adaptation effects. This framework includes dimensions representing the major characteristics of studies (i.e., experimental manipulations) or operational parameters in this field. As discussed in detail in a later section, the dimensions enable a categorization of the (1) various adapting face information (2) timing characteristics of adaptation effects (e.g., the delay between adaptation and adaptation test phases), and (3) transfer relations between face images presented during these phases (e.g., images of the same or different identities). In the main section of the present review, we discuss the research literature specifically toward each of the framework’s dimensions. Finally, we present options for how to combine findings across different dimensions to demonstrate the relevance of our framework for future studies.

Investigations on adaptation effects in faces are very relevant for the progress of cognitive research, since these effects offer a window into the processes and dynamics of highly complex object processing. First of all, faces are arguably the most important social stimuli since they are the primary means by which we perceive identity information, emotional information, etc. (see below). This expertise and its investigation in adaptation effects provide an essential tool or window for dissecting different levels of neural code and the visual pathway in face processing. This latter fact is also the motivation for a close look at timing and additionally the transfer characteristics of adaptation effects in faces. As research on adaptation effects in faces is moreover a broad and elaborated field today, represented by a great number of adaptation studies employing different procedures and aiming at different research questions, this research field also offers an excellent opportunity for taking a more general perspective on the found effects in the form of a review.

Framework to Conceptualize Research on Face Adaptation Effects

As illustrated in Figure 1 and in Table 1, we integrate findings in the field of face adaptation research into a conceptual framework that includes three dimensions. The first dimension of this framework represents different types of potential facial information which are susceptible to adaptation; we call this dimension adapting information. Instances of adapting information are identity information (e.g., Leopold et al., 2001; Jiang et al., 2006), configural information (e.g., Carbon et al., 2007b; Little et al., 2008), gaze information (e.g., Jenkins et al., 2006), emotional information (e.g., Webster et al., 2004; Ng et al., 2008; Adams et al., 2010), age information (e.g., Schweinberger et al., 2010), gender information (e.g., Kovács et al., 2007; Bestelmeyer et al., 2008), ethnicity information (e.g., Jaquet et al., 2008; Ng et al., 2008), attractiveness information (e.g., Anzures et al., 2009), or viewpoint information (e.g., Chen et al., 2010). The present list of adapting information is completed by face information investigated in the context of face distortion aftereffects (FDAEs; e.g., Webster and MacLin, 1999). This method of manipulating faces may affect types of facial information that are listed above (e.g., configural information, age, identity, gender; please see also later sections). However, FDAEs are realized by unique manipulation algorithms (i.e., distortions by expanding or contracting the frontal-view original face image relative to a midpoint on the nose). Further, these algorithms relax the controlling of which specific information types are affected. For example, manipulating faces in the context of FDAEs affect facial features (e.g., eyes, mouth, etc.) while manipulations of configural information (i.e., spatial distances between these features) rather leave these features unaffected. Generally, these different types of instances of adapting information realize different levels of ecological validity. While differences in age, viewpoint, gaze, or emotion are plausible and realistic in an ecological context, manipulations of identity or configural information have less validity since such changes do not typically occur in the ecosystem.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Framework to review face adaptation effects including dimensions for different types of adapting information, transfer effects, and timing between adaptation and adaptation test phases.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Overview of types of adapting face information and related references.

The second dimension of the present framework, time, orders adaptation effects according to different types of temporal information. The first information type, delay, is related to the robustness and sustainability of adaptation effects; basically, the time interval between an adaptation and an adaptation test phase. Delays range from milliseconds (e.g., Leopold et al., 2001; Rhodes et al., 2003) to minutes (e.g., Carbon and Leder, 2006; Kloth and Schweinberger, 2008), but also include days and even weeks under typical laboratory (e.g., Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011) as well as ecologically more valid test contexts (Carbon and Ditye, 2012). The delay characteristics of adaptation effects are essential for providing useful information about the decay of adaptation effects and thus the “recalibration” and “readaptation” abilities of the visual system (Carbon and Ditye, 2011). Furthermore, they allow inferences about the robustness and consistency of perceptual information in general. In parallel to the “time” information delay, we focus on adaptation duration, the time span during which the adapting stimulus is presented (e.g., Strobach et al., 2011). Adaptation duration information provides insights into how this time span can modulate the adaptation effect size or the adaptability of faces. Moreover, this type of time information was compared with simple adaptation effects (e.g., with tilt information; Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007) to explore the dynamics of adaptation effects at different levels of cortical visual hierarchy. Finally, we focus on “time” information of the test duration type, establishing the time span during which the test stimulus is presented (e.g., Rhodes et al., 2007). Similar to the adaptation duration, test duration can give insights into to the dynamics of adaptation in face stimuli in contrast to simple adaptation effects.

The third dimension in the present framework is associated with the transfer of adaptation effects. This transfer dimension reflects the range and limits of adaptation transfer effects providing important inferences about the nature of processing being linked with specific adapted stimuli or being of more general quality. In this way, the investigation of adaptation is a tool (rather than a topic) for localizing the plasticity and pointing out common coding principles of various levels of visual processing (from retinotopic to high and possibly face-specific levels of visual processing; Webster, 2011; Webster and MacLeod, 2011). There exists two systems of structuring transfer effects: transferring between different (manipulated) image versions of the identical identity during adaptation and test phases (e.g., variations in size or orientation) enables exclusive low-level perceptual effects of adaptation (e.g., on a retinal level; Zhao and Chubb, 2001) to be excluded. Additionally, as proposed by Carbon et al. (2007b), adaptation transfer can be systematically tested with face images used in the adaptation and test phases showing the same images of the same identity vs. different images of the same identity vs. different images of different identities2.

The approach can be extended by investigating transfer effects, inter alia, across family members, gender, and/or ethnicity.

As will be seen later, not all studies in the field of face adaptation research allow a localization of its research in all dimensions of the applied framework. For example, many studies apply sets of face images of different identities during adaption and the identical set of images during a following test phase (e.g., Buckingham et al., 2006; Chen et al., 2010). Such a procedure prevents conclusions about the transferability of adaptation effects, since potential effects in an adaptation test phase may originate from the presentation of the identical image and/or the presentation of other identities’ images during the adaptation phase.

Investigating the Adaptation Effects of Different Types of Face Information – The Adapting Information Dimension

Basically, the result patterns in studies on adaptation effects showed an adaptation bias and were thus consistent in the following way: values of adaptation test ratings tend toward the (typically extreme) values of adapting information presented during the adaptation phase; in other words, average or neutral faces are perceptually biased away from the adapting face. After introducing findings in the context of FDAEs, we show findings of facial information loosely ordered with increasing abstractedness.

Face Distortion Aftereffects

Webster and MacLin (1999) investigated FDAEs within face images of a single identity by presenting adaptation images that were distorted by expanding or contracting the frontal-view original face image relative to a midpoint on the nose. After viewing distorted faces during adaptation (e.g., contracted face images), original faces appear distorted in the direction opposite to the distraction (e.g., expanded face images). In contrast to this effect after adaptations to distorted faces, no such adaptation effect followed the presentation of original face images (i.e., distorted faces still appeared distorted). In this way, Webster and MacLin provided among the first evidence for adaptation effects in complex, natural objects, suggesting that adaptation may play an important normalizing role in face perception and adaptation effects may strongly influence form perception (see also Zhao and Chubb, 2001; Morikawa, 2005; Yamashita et al., 2005; Jeffery et al., 2006, 2007; Jaquet et al., 2007, 2008; Robbins et al., 2007; Jaquet and Rhodes, 2008; Burkhardt et al., 2010; Hills et al., 2010); such a “complex” adaptation phenomenon was recently transferred to animals, trees, or every-day objects (e.g., light bulb; Daelli et al., 2010).

However, the FDAE enables no specification of which types of facial information are precisely involved in face adaptation. For example, the usage of FDAEs simultaneously affects feature information such as mouth, eyes, or eyebrows (e.g., Tanaka and Sengco, 1997; Cabeza and Kato, 2000), configural information such as nose-mouth distance (Young et al., 1987; Rhodes, 1988; Leder and Carbon, 2006), as well as holistic information referring to face processing “as a whole” (e.g., Tanaka and Farah, 1993; Leder and Carbon, 2005). As a consequence, it is essential to systematically vary distinct face information between adaptation and test phases in order to generate precise conclusions about the mechanisms of face adaptation effects, a fact that is not necessarily granted in the context of FDAEs.

Configural Information

Carbon and colleagues (Carbon and Leder, 2005; Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011; Strobach et al., 2011) aimed instead at investigating adaptation effects of distorted configural information on subsequent adaptation tests. Participants were either presented face images during adaptation of familiar identities with decreased eyes-mouth distance or face images with increased eyes-mouth distance relative to the original. In a subsequent test phase participants were asked to select the veridical version (1) out of a series of versions of gradually altered eyes-mouth distances, or (2) from one original and one slightly altered version (e.g., slightly decreased or increased eyes-mouth distance). The results showed a bias in participants’ selections in the direction of the respective manipulations, e.g., after viewing face images with extremely decreased eyes-mouth distance there was an increased likelihood of selecting a version with slightly decreased distances (for similar results with exclusive shifting the eyes in the vertical axis, see Walton and Hills, 2012). Thus, these studies demonstrated adaptation effects of configural information. Similar results are demonstrated by Little et al. (2005, 2008) following the inspection of manipulated eyes-spacing: inspecting faces with extreme narrow or wide eye distances resulted in increased normality rating for subsequently presented face images with moderately manipulated distances. These latter findings demonstrate the generalization of adaptation effects after exposure to manipulated configural information.

Gaze and Viewpoint Information

Adaptation to a consistent leftward or rightward gaze produces ratings that demonstrate an elimination of observers’ perception of the gaze in the adapted direction (Jenkins et al., 2006; Schweinberger et al., 2007). That is, a gaze to the adapted side was subsequently seen as pointing straight. Leftward and rightward viewpoint adaptation resulted in similar adaptation effects (Fang et al., 2007; Chen et al., 2010); thus, a face turned to the adapted side was subsequently seen as pointing straight. Again, these effects can be interpreted as a recalibration mechanism: probably the best heuristic to use if one constantly lacks a straight viewpoint is to retune the processing of gaze direction or viewpoint.

Emotional and Attractiveness Information

Another example for testing adaptation effects is represented by investigations on the effects of different facial expressions. For instance, after perceiving a happy face, a previously ambiguous happy-angry face appeared distinctly angry, and thus the boundary between happy and angry faces was shifted toward the happy expression (Webster et al., 2004; Fox and Barton, 2007; Juricevic and Webster, 2012). Such shifted emotion categorization was even evident with adapting stimuli having been suppressed from awareness (Benton and Burgess, 2008; Adams et al., 2010) illustrating the fast and automatic processing of such expressions. Attractiveness adaptation effects demonstrated that viewing consistently distorted faces shifts attractiveness preferences toward the adapting stimuli; for instance, contracted face images (Webster and MacLin, 1999) appeared more attractive after adapting to contracted faces than after adapting to expanded faces (MacLin and Webster, 2001; Rhodes et al., 2003; Carbon et al., 2007a; Anzures et al., 2009). Similarly, faces with left-right asymmetries appeared more attractive when asymmetrical faces were presented during adaptation (Rhodes et al., 2009b).

Gender Information

Adaptation to either masculine or feminine faces increases preferences for novel faces that are (gender-wise) similar to those that were recently seen (Buckingham et al., 2006), as well as increasing the femininity and masculinity ratings of test faces, respectively (see also Webster et al., 2004; Ng et al., 2006, 2008; Kovács et al., 2007). An alternative measurement of gender-adaptation effects demonstrated that adapting to a male/female face selectively enhances discrimination for male/female faces (Yang et al., 2011).

Age and Ethnicity Information

When participants viewed young or old adult faces (i.e., adults of different ages), their “young/old boundary” was biased toward the age of the adapting face (O’Neil and Webster, 2011). Consistently, test faces appeared older or younger when the adapting faces were young or old, respectively (Schweinberger et al., 2010). Therefore, there is evidence for an adaptation bias for facial age as well (see also Lai et al., 2012). An analog bias also exists for face ethnicity, exemplarily shown for Caucasian vs. Asian faces: adaptation to an average Asian or Caucasian face reduced identification thresholds for faces from the adapted relative to the unadapted ethnicity (Webster et al., 2004; Rhodes et al., 2010).

Identity Information

Leopold et al. (2001) provided evidence for increased sensitivity for particular face identities after adaptation, as investigated in the context of face identity aftereffects (FIAEs). Based on the ideas of a “face space” (e.g., a multidimensional representation of faces as their distance to a prototypical “center” face, Valentine, 2001), the authors were able to explain the effect in the theoretical framework of a computationally derived mental representation (Figure 2). After perceiving an “anti-face” (located opposite an original face of an identity, on a trajectory crossing this original face and a face-space average), adaptation specifically shifted perception along a trajectory passing through the adapting anti-face and average face away from the original face, selectively facilitating recognition of a test face lying on this trajectory. Such adaptation effects on the identity level were replicated in a number of studies and variations (Hurlbert, 2001; Anderson and Wilson, 2005; Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes and Jeffery, 2006; Rhodes et al., 2009a, 2010, 2011; Palermo et al., 2011) and are often explicitly referred to as changes of the face space.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Computationally derived face space in which the stimuli were generated to investigate face identity adaptation effects (FIAE). The original faces (green ellipses) are connected to the average face (blue ellipse) by an “identity trajectory.” Numbers refer to the “identity strength” possessed by the given face (taken from Leopold et al., 2001).

In sum, this section demonstrates adaptation effects of numerous facial attributes. To our knowledge there are no attributes that don’t show any such effects in the research literature, which indicates that adult face coding systems are more flexible than was previously thought (Bruce, 1994). At this point it is essential to stress again, that the class of faces is the only object class that allows investigating and reviewing this large number of information types.

Investigating Temporal Characteristics of Adaptation Effects – The Time Dimension

Delay

Face adaptation effects are typically tested with a delay interval of only a few seconds or even less (Webster and MacLin, 1999; Leopold et al., 2001; Rhodes et al., 2003; Benton and Burgess, 2008; Bestelmeyer et al., 2008; Carbon and Ditye, 2011). Given these constraints, the studies mainly show that (A) adapting face information such as face identity occurs on a perceptual level, and (B) there is no recalibration of the visual system after this delay. In sum, they allow conclusions about neither adaptation effects on the representational level nor on the robustness of visual system recalibration. One of the first systematic investigations of adaptation effects and their delay characteristics focused on gaze information (Kloth and Schweinberger, 2008); Table 2 gives an overview of studies testing adaptation effects with short and long delays. This study demonstrated a decrease in the gaze adaptation effect over time but this effect was still measureable up to 385 s after the end of the adaptation phase. This is evidence for the idea that adaptation effects are not completely perceptually-based. Also it suggests that there is no complete “return to normal” (“recalibration,” see Carbon and Ditye, 2011) of the visual system, associated to gaze processing, within this time range of more than 6 min. Consequently, adaptation effects are “stickier” than many of the traditional low-level adaptation effects (e.g., Köhler and Wallach, 1944) and can be initially interpreted as evidence of representation-based effects.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of examples of face adaptation studies and their delays between adaptation and test phases.

Carbon and colleagues systematically extended research on effects of adaptation to manipulated configural information of famous faces. These studies demonstrated adaptation effects after 5 min (Carbon and Leder, 2005; Carbon et al., 2007b; Strobach et al., 2011), 80 min (Carbon and Leder, 2006), 24 h (Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011; Strobach et al., 2011), and even 1 week (Carbon and Ditye, 2011). Therefore, adaptation seems to be very robust and refers to effects on the functional level of representations. According to Carbon et al.’s research, it takes at least 1 week for the visual system to return to its original state before adaptation (i.e., to recalibrate to its pre-adaptation state), at least in terms of adaptation effects for configural facial information.

To sum up, regarding the delay dimension, a series of recent experiments revealed relatively long-lasting adaptation effects. This evidence is related to face attributes of gaze information (Kloth and Schweinberger, 2008), and configural information (e.g., Carbon et al., 2007b). It illustrates aspects of face processing that are related to an increased participation of representations and do not only rely on simple iconic traces or simple visual aftereffects (Carbon et al., 2007b). To learn about mental representations, these findings of long-lasting adaptation effects demonstrate that the investigated types of facial information (i.e., gaze information, configural information) are not exclusively processed and coded at a perceptual level. One may speculate that such processing and coding involves long-term memory functions. However, there are no investigations of “delay” effects on facial information aspects of age, attractiveness, emotion, ethnicity, gender, FIAEs, or viewpoint information. Such investigations are essential to assess the functional level (representation level and/or perceptual level), the robustness/sustainability and the time needed for recalibrations of adaptation effects of these types of information. We will come back to this immense gap in the adaptation literature in a later section.

Adaptation Duration

The increase in the time interval for presenting visual adaptation material typically results in an increase of simple perceptual adaptation effects demonstrating the adaptability of tilt, motion, or shape information (Rhodes et al., 2007). In fact, this relation is characterized by a logarithmic function between adaptation duration and effect size. A comparable relation was found in faces. Rhodes, Leopold, Jeffery and colleagues (Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007) tested the FDAEs as well as FIAEs after varying presentation times of adapting face stimuli; in fact, test stimuli appeared immediately after adaptation material was presented between 1,000 and 16,000 ms. Independent of size relations between adaptation and test faces, FDAEs and FIAEs increased with adaptation time. The relationship between adaptation duration and effect is thus comparable in simple perceptual information as well as complex face objects, illustrating common coding principles at different levels of cortical visual hierarchy.

There were however no, or very short, time delays between adaptation and test phases in these prior studies (Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007) and thus it is likely that adaptation effects were investigated at the perceptual level only. In contrast, Carbon et al. (2007b) introduced delays of 5 min to 24 h between these phases, allowing investigation of the effects of adaptation delay on the adaptation of memory face representations (in this case, the adapting information was configural information). On the basis of this argument, Strobach et al. (2011) performed multiple regression analyses on the individual adaptation duration and their adaptation effects after both 5 min and 24 h. Positive relations between both measures (i.e., longer presentation times of adaptation faces resulted in increased adaptation effects) demonstrated an impact of the adaptation time on the effect size, extending findings on the perceptual level to findings demonstrating mechanisms instead on a memory level.

There is a lack of studies that explicitly investigate the effects of adaptation time for facial attributes other than FDAEs, FIAEs, and configural information – e.g., for age, gender, and attractiveness. Such investigations would provide elaborated knowledge about the coding principles of face-specific and simple visual information.

Test duration

Similarly to changes in the magnitude of adaptation effects following variability in adaptation duration, the time span of presenting a test stimulus modulates the magnitude of these effects (Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007). In fact, test faces were presented for 100, 200, 400, 800, or 1,600 ms and the adaptation effects, as measured in paradigms of FDAEs and FIAEs, reduced with increasing test time. Since similar effects are evident with simple aftereffects, face and simple perceptual information (i.e., tilt, orientation) illustrate common coding principles at different levels of cortical visual hierarchy. However, what is clearly lacking in this domain is research on the question of whether the test duration has an effect when adaptation effects are tested on a memory rather than on a perceptual level. That is to say, it is an open issue in the literature whether the negative relation of test duration and adaptation effect (e.g., increasing test duration and decreasing adaptation effects) is evident after a delay of minutes, hours, or days. Furthermore, this negative relationship was established for FDAEs and FIAEs. However, it is lacking for alternative facial information and such tests should be attempted in future studies. They are essential to establish a more elaborate knowledge of the coding principles of face-specific types of information.

Investigating the Transferability of Adaptation Effects – The Transfer Dimension

After reviewing adaptation effects of different face information and time characteristics (e.g., the sustainability of adaptation effects), it is essential to review the relationship between the adapting and test face images; i.e., to test transfers of adaptation effects to new face images (or new image versions) not presented during adaptation. Here, we review findings that investigated transfer effects between the same image of one identity (identical images or images differing in viewpoint, orientation, or size between adapting and test images) and different images of the same identity. Additionally, we also discuss adaptation transfer effects between images of different identities. As illustrated in Table 3, we test these transfer effects at the pictorial level (identical face image during adaptation and test), identity level (different face images of the same identity during adaptation and test), and novel level (different face images of different identities during adaptation and test, Carbon and Ditye, 2011). In the following section, we primarily discuss types of adapting face information that demonstrate transfer effects. We followed this strategy because, for the remaining types of face information (e.g., gaze, emotion), there was (A) no investigation of transfer effects and/or (B) no conclusive evidence for such effects.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Different transfer levels of adaptation effects as realized in studies of Carbon and colleagues (Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011, 2012; Strobach et al., 2011).

When focusing on FIAEs, Hole (2011) demonstrated adaptation effects with identical face images during adaptation and testing which also transfer onto new image versions changed in viewpoints, orientation, and vertically stretched versions from the adapting face images when using familiar; this confirmed Jiang et al.’s (2006) finding of viewpoint invariance of FIAEs who also added evidence of their transfers across shape and surface reflectance information. Anderson and Wilson (2005) supported Hole’s finding of size independent transfer with unfamiliar, synthetic faces while their study provided no support of a viewpoint-invariant FIAE for this face type. Guo et al. (2009) revealed limits to the transferability of the FIAEs by showing that this effect exclusively works from upright to inverted orientation, but not vice versa with unfamiliar faces. With familiar faces, however, Hole showed that the FIAE produced by inverted adaptation faces and upright test faces was similar to that produced by upright adapting faces. Furthermore, this type of adaptation effect seems to be gender-specific since there is an effect from adaptation to test faces when these faces are related via a gender-specific prototype, whereas there was no such effect with an androgynous face (i.e., combined male and female prototype; Rhodes et al., 2011). Leopold et al. (2001, 2005) showed that relations between a facial prototype and the individual faces in face space could be manipulated by face adaptations of different identities. In other words, this manipulation includes face images during adaptation that are not located on a trajectory crossing an original face and a face-space average (similar to Figure 1); thus, there is evidence for FIAEs on the novel level. This conclusion is consistent with the fact that FIAEs are rather high-level perceptual effects: composite faces (different views of a composite face comprised of the top half of a famous face and the bottom half of a non-famous face) either did or did not produce a FIAE depending on whether or not the famous face is explicitly recognized before the post-adaptation test phase (Laurence and Hole, 2012).

Conversely, adaptation to facial expressions (i.e., emotional information) was partly independent from the represented identities. That is, adaptation effects with a focus on facial expressions were transferred to different faces and thus include at least portions of novel-level processing (Fox and Barton, 2007). Interestingly, the above discussed FIAE is not affected by expressional information. That is, FIAEs were not modulated by congruency of facial expression during adaptation and test phases (i.e., same expression vs. different expression; Fox et al., 2008). Thus, expressional adaptation and FIAEs tend toward asymmetry with impact of identity information on expression adaptation, but there is no reverse effect.

For facial gender, Yang et al. (2011) demonstrated that gender discrimination enhancement induced by face adaptation can transfer across a substantial change in three-dimensional facial orientation. Additionally, gender-adaptation effects are position invariant effects (Kovács et al., 2007). These effects also seem to be age-independent, since Barrett and O’Toole (2009) demonstrated an effect of gender adaptation within sets of children’s and adults’ faces and also between these sets of faces. These age-independent effects also demonstrate that gender-adaptation effects may work on a novel level since adaptation and test faces were derived from different identities. However, this novel level is limited to the orientation of faces; that is, adaptation effects work independently with upright and inverted face presentations (Watson and Clifford, 2006). Alternatively, the limitation of emotion adaptation effects is set at race boundaries: adapting to one type of emotion in, for instance, a Caucasian face, affects the later processing of an alternative Caucasian face but not that of an alternative ethnicity (i.e., black faces; Otten and Banaji, 2012).

The adaptation effect realized in the form of viewpoint adaptation (i.e., adaptation to left or right-turned faces) occurs at the novel level as demonstrated by transfer effects across different identities, different gender, and different vertical orientations (Fang et al., 2007). In the case of face normality ratings and their adaptation effects, there exists evidence for at least orientation-transferable adaptation effects, i.e., between upright and inverted orientations of face images (Rhodes et al., 2003).

Transfer effects of adapting configural information are not only in action at the pictorial and identity levels, but also at the novel level (i.e., different face images of different identities during adaptation and test; Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011); even though the effect was slightly reduced compared with pictorial and identity conditions. This was demonstrated by the transfer effects of adapting configural information in combinations of identical adaptation and test facial images, of different adaptation and test facial images from the same identity and transfers to new identities, i.e., transfers to test face images of identities not presented during prior adaptation (Walton and Hills, 2012, for comparable results with exclusive eyes shifts in the vertical axis). Little et al. (2008) assumed that such transfer effects are gender-specific. FDAEs are not transferable to images mirrored after the adaptation phase (Morikawa, 2005), but there is evidence of the transfer of such effects to different ethnicities (Jaquet et al., 2007), between different viewpoints (Jeffery et al., 2006, 2007), different orientations of upright and inverted faces (e.g., adapting face is oriented 45° from vertically upright and the test face 45° in the opposite direction; Watson and Clifford, 2003) between different identities and orientations (Webster and MacLin, 1999) as well as different facial image sizes (Zhao and Chubb, 2001; Yamashita et al., 2005). Consequently, FDAEs occur up to the novel level. Consistently, adaptation effects of facial age on a novel level are demonstrated by transfers between different genders (O’Neil and Webster, 2011) and identities (Schweinberger et al., 2010).

In sum, there is clear evidence for adaptation effects across different identities for a first set of face information (e.g., gender, age, configural information), i.e., transfer effects on a novel level. Adaptation of this set of information seems to affect the high-order visual system and/or memory representations. In contrast to these transferable adaptation effects, there is no clear evidence of transfer effects for other face information, such as attractiveness. Furthermore, there is evidence that some face information is only transferable between different identities when the specific subgroups are not changed simultaneously (e.g., FDAE transfers are gender-specific). To learn about processing characteristics and mental representations of faces, this section indicates that face coding is hierarchically structured with an orchestration of common underlying structures. This common structure was demonstrated at the novel level and maybe theoretically represented in a prototype in face space (Valentine, 2001). However, the processing of some facial aspects is characterized by and related to specific modules (e.g., gender-specific modules of FDAEs) potentially working in parallel to a general face-space prototype.

Future Investigations of Face Adaptation Effects

A summarizing overview of existing and lacking research in the field of face adaptation effects is illustrated in Table 4. Future studies may apply the present framework’s dimensions or operational parameters (adapting information, time, and transfer) for a systematic continuation of investigating face adaptation effects. For instance, for a number of adapting information types (e.g., emotion, age, attractiveness, gender) there exists no test for the robustness of adaptation effects; that is, adaptation effects of these types of face information are demonstrated after short delays between adaptation and adaptation test phases. However, there are no studies that test these adaptation effects after long delays and their decay over time. To present one specific example, in accordance with time intervals applied by Carbon and colleagues (Carbon et al., 2007b; Carbon and Ditye, 2011), the adaptation effect of facial age should be tested after time intervals of 5 min, 24 h, and 1 week in order to cover a broad range of time periods and to test the robustness of the age adaptation effect. Likewise, testing the impact of adaptation and test time should be extended to forms of facial information beyond phenomena investigated in the context of FDAEs (Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2007). As illustrated, a first extension of investigations on adaptation time occurs in the context of adaptation of configural information (Strobach et al., 2011), but other contexts are definitely needed to generate a broader picture of timing aspects in face adaptation. In this way, our framework is able to characterize gaps in the adaptation research literature while combining findings along the dimension adaptation information and delay.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Overview of existing and lacking research in the field of face adaptation effects: what types of face information does this research include? What types are neglected?.

Similarly, such an expansion of tests should also be performed on the transfer dimension since this type of test is essential for investigating the functional level of adaptation effects and increasing the ecological validity of these studies. While transfer tests on the same face image of the same identity (e.g., varying orientation or context during adaptation and test; Carbon and Ditye, 2012) enable investigations on picture or “iconic” processing (Carbon, 2008), and thus not necessarily on face processing per se, transfer tests on different face images of the same identity as well as different identities’ face images allow for conclusions about the characteristics of face representations. So far, these two types of transfer tests [i.e., (1) different face images of the same identity, (2) different face images of different identities] were separately conducted in most studies. Future studies may combine these two transfer types.

An additional way to continue face adaptation research, in terms of gaining knowledge on the basis of adaptation effects, is to combine aspects (i.e., our framework’s dimensions) of time and transfer. For instance, future studies should systemically investigate the effects of manipulating face information across different delays between adaptation and test phases, combined with tests for the different transfer levels between adaptation and test faces. Additionally, the effects of gender adaptation can be investigated after relatively short and long delays between faces of the same or different age groups, ethnicities, or gaze points. A related investigation focused on transfer effects between different emotional expressions and gender in the context of FDAEs (Tillman and Webster, 2012). This systematic investigation of the interplay of delay and transfer may provide conclusions about the range of adaptation effects and their origin from similar or different methods of neural coding.

Additionally, future studies should relate investigations on the functional level of face adaptation effects to concepts applied to other types of processes or skills. One option might be in relation to improved skills acquired during cognitive training (e.g., working memory training) and their range of transferability. Existing studies on this issue (Li et al., 2008b; Karbach and Kray, 2009; Strobach et al., 2012a,b) categorize the range of skill transfer into near transfer (transfer between situations with common basic characteristics) and far transfer (transfer between situations with structural differences). Conversely, near transfer tests could investigate the adaptation effects of facial images on other facial images, while far transfer tests investigate adaptation effects on facial images after prior adaptation to specific properties of cars (Carbon, 2010) or adaptations to mental sets due to the presentation of gender-typical objects (e.g., lipstick vs. motor bike, Javadi and Wee, 2012). For instance, one could test whether there is a transfer of adaptation effects of configural information (i.e., spatial relations between features) from face stimuli to car stimuli (i.e., far transfer). In particular, front views of cars with a similar setting of parts as can be found in faces are favorable for performing such transfer tests (see Windhager et al., 2010). In fact, this type of future transfer study would cross boundaries between different types of visual objects and may show similarities and differences in these general object characteristics.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by a research grant (FNK) of the University of Bamberg and by a project funded by the Doktor Robert Pfleger-Stiftung.

Footnotes

  1. ^We refer to face adaptation effects as aftereffects (Köhler and Wallach, 1944) at higher perceptual and cognitive levels (Carbon and Ditye, 2011).
  2. ^In the present work, we define that different facial images can originate from the same identity. For example, the same identity can produce face images differing in emotionality. In contrast, the same facial image of the same identity can produce different versions that vary in size or orientation.

References

Adams, W. J., Gray, K. L. H., Garner, M., and Graf, E. W. (2010). High-level face adaptation without awareness. Psychol. Sci. 21, 205–210. doi:10.1177/0956797609359508

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Anderson, N. D., and Wilson, H. R. (2005). The nature of synthetic face adaptation. Vision Res. 45, 1815–1828. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.01.012

CrossRef Full Text

Anzures, G., Mondloch, C. J., and Lackner, C. (2009). Face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects in 8-year-olds and adults. Child Dev. 80, 178–191. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01253.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Barrett, S. E., and O’Toole, A. J. (2009). Face adaptation to gender: does adaptation transfer across age categories? Vis. Cogn. 17, 700–715. doi:10.1080/13506280802332197

CrossRef Full Text

Benton, C. P., and Burgess, E. C. (2008). The direction of measured face aftereffects. J. Vis. 8, 1–6. doi:10.1167/8.15.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bestelmeyer, P. E. G., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Perrett, D. I., Schneider, A., et al. (2008). Sex-contingent face aftereffects depend on perceptual category rather than structural encoding. Cognition 107, 353–365. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bruce, V. (1994). Stability from variation: the case of face recognition. The M.D. Vernon memorial lecture. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 47A, 5–28. doi:10.1080/14640749408401141

CrossRef Full Text

Buckingham, G., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., Welling, L. L. M., Conway, C. A., Tiddeman, B. P., et al. (2006). Visual adaptation to masculine and feminine faces influences generalized preferences and perceptions of trustworthiness. Evol. Hum. Behav. 27, 381–389. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.03.001

CrossRef Full Text

Burkhardt, A., Blaha, L. M., Jurs, B. S., Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Wyatte, D., et al. (2010). Adaptation modulates the electrophysiological substrates of perceived facial distortion: support for opponent coding. Neuropsychologia 48, 3743–3756. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cabeza, R., and Kato, T. (2000). Features are also important: contributions of featural and configural processing to face recognition. Psychol. Sci. 11, 429–433. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00283

CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C. (2008). Last but not least: famous faces as icons. The illusion of being an expert in the recognition of famous faces. Perception 37, 801–806. doi:10.1068/p5789

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C. (2010). The cycle of preference: long-term dynamics of aesthetic appreciation. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 134, 233–244. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C., and Ditye, T. (2011). Sustained effects of adaptation on the perception of familiar faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 37, 615–625. doi:10.1037/a0019949

CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C., and Ditye, T. (2012). Face adaptation effects show strong and long-lasting transfer from lab to more ecological contexts. Front. Psychol. 3:3. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C., and Leder, H. (2005). Face adaptation: changing stable representations of familiar faces within minutes? Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 1, 1–7. doi:10.2478/v10053-008-0038-8

CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C., and Leder, H. (2006). The Mona Lisa effect: is ‘our’ Lisa fame or fake? Perception 35, 411–414. doi:10.1068/p5452

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Carbon, C.-C., Leder, H., and Ditye, T. (2007a). When style matters. Art-specific adaptation effects. Perception 36, 17.

Carbon, C.-C., Strobach, T., Langton, S. R. H., Harsányi, G., Leder, H., and Kovács, G. (2007b). Adaptation effects of highly familiar faces: immediate and long lasting. Mem. Cognit. 35, 1966–1976.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Chen, J., Yang, H., Wang, A., and Fang, F. (2010). Perceptual consequences of face viewpoint adaptation: face viewpoint aftereffect, changes of differential sensitivity to face view, and their relationship. J. Vis. 10, 1–11. doi:10.1167/10.3.12

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Clifford, C. W. G., and Rhodes, G. (2005). Fitting the Mind to the World: Adaptation and Aftereffects in High Level Vision. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Clifford, C. W. G., Wenderoth, P., and Spehar, B. (2000). A functional angle on some aftereffects in cortical vision. Proc. Biol. Sci. 267, 1705–1710. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1198

CrossRef Full Text

Daelli, V., van Rijsbergen, N. J., and Treves, A. (2010). How recent experience affects the perception of ambiguous objects. Brain Res. 1322, 81–91. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.01.060

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fang, F., Ijichi, K., and He, S. (2007). Transfer of the face viewpoint aftereffect from adaptation to different and inverted faces. J. Vis. 7, 1–9. doi:10.1167/7.13.6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fox, C. J., and Barton, J. J. S. (2007). What is adapted in face adaptation? The neural representations of expression in the human visual system. Brain Res. 1127, 80–89. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.09.104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fox, C. J., Oruc, I., and Barton, J. J. S. (2008). It doesn’t matter how you feel. The facial identity aftereffect is invariant to changes in facial expression. J. Vis. 8, 1–13. doi:10.1167/8.3.11

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Guo, X. M., Oruç, I., and Barton, J. J. S. (2009). Cross-orientation transfer of adaptation for facial identity is asymmetric: a study using contrast-based recognition thresholds. Vision Res. 49, 2254–2260. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.012

CrossRef Full Text

Hills, P. J., Holland, A. M., and Lewis, M. B. (2010). Aftereffects for face attributes with different natural variability: children are more adaptable than adolescents. Cogn. Dev. 25, 278–289. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.01.002

CrossRef Full Text

Hole, G. (2011). Identity-specific face adaptation effects: evidence for abstractive face representations. Cognition 119, 216–228. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hurlbert, A. (2001). Trading faces. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 3–5. doi:10.1038/82877

CrossRef Full Text

Jaquet, E., and Rhodes, G. (2008). Face aftereffects indicate dissociable, but not distinct, coding of male and female faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 34, 101–112. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.34.1.101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jaquet, E., Rhodes, G., and Hayward, W. G. (2007). Opposite aftereffects for Chinese and Caucasian faces are selective for social category information and not just physical face differences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 1457–1467. doi:10.1080/17470210701467870

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jaquet, E., Rhodes, G., and Hayward, W. G. (2008). Race-contingent aftereffects suggest distinct perceptual norms for different race faces. Vis. Cogn. 16, 734–753. doi:10.1080/13506280701350647

CrossRef Full Text

Javadi, A. H., and Wee, N. (2012). Cross-category adaptation: objects produce gender adaptation in the perception of faces. PLoS ONE 9:e46079. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046079

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeffery, L., and Rhodes, G. (2011). Insights into the development of face recognition mechanisms revealed by face aftereffects. Br. J. Psychol. 102, 799–815. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02066.x

CrossRef Full Text

Jeffery, L., Rhodes, G., and Busey, T. (2006). View-specific coding of face shape. Psychol. Sci. 17, 501–505. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01735.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeffery, L., Rhodes, G., and Busey, T. (2007). Broadly tuned, view-specific coding of face shape: opposing figural aftereffects can be induced in different views. Vision Res. 47, 3070–3077. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jenkins, R., Beaver, J. D., and Calder, A. J. (2006). I thought you were looking at me: direction-specific aftereffects in gaze perception. Psychol. Sci. 17, 506–513. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01736.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jiang, F., Blanz, V., and O’Toole, A. J. (2006). Probing the visual representation of faces with adaptation: a view from the other side of the mean. Psychol. Sci. 17, 493–500. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01734.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Juricevic, I., and Webster, M. (2012). Selectivity of face aftereffects for expressions and anti-expressions. Front. Psychol. 3:4. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Karbach, J., and Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 12, 978–990. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00846.x

CrossRef Full Text

Kloth, N., and Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects. J. Vis. 8, 1–11. doi:10.1167/8.11.4

CrossRef Full Text

Kloth, N., Schweinberger, S. R., and Kovács, G. (2010). Neural correlates of generic versus gender-specific face adaptation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2345–2356. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21329

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Köhler, W., and Wallach, H. (1944). Figural aftereffects: an investigation of visual processes. Proc. Am. Philos. Soc. 88, 269–357.

Kovács, G., Zimmer, M., Bankó, É., Harza, I., Antal, A., and Vidnyánszky, Z. (2006). Electrophysiological correlates of visual adaptation to faces and body parts in humans. Cereb. Cortex 16, 742–753. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj020

CrossRef Full Text

Kovács, G., Zimmer, M., Harza, I., and Vidnyánszky, Z. (2007). Adaptation duration affects the spatial selectivity of facial aftereffects. Vision Res. 47, 3141–3149. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lai, M., Oruc, I., and Barton, J. J. S. (2012). Facial age after-effects show partial identity invariance and transfer from hands to faces. Cortex 48, 477–486. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2010.11.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Laurence, S., and Hole, G. (2012). Identity specific adaptation with composite faces. Vis. Cogn. 20, 109–120. doi:10.1080/13506285.2012.655805

CrossRef Full Text

Leder, H., and Carbon, C.-C. (2005). When context hinders! Learn-test compatibility in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 58A, 235–250.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Leder, H., and Carbon, C.-C. (2006). Face-specific configural processing of relational information. Br. J. Psychol. 97, 19–29. doi:10.1348/000712605X54794

CrossRef Full Text

Leopold, D. A., O’Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., and Blanz, V. (2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealed by high-level aftereffects. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 89–94. doi:10.1038/82947

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leopold, D. A., and Rhodes, G. (2010). A comparative view of face perception. J. Comp. Psychol. 124, 233–251. doi:10.1037/a0019460

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Leopold, D. A., Rhodes, G., Müller, K. M., and Jeffery, L. (2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 897–904. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3022

CrossRef Full Text

Li, A., Tzen, B., Yadgarova, A., and Zaidi, Q. (2008a). Neural basis of 3-D shape aftereffects. Vision Res. 48, 244–252. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Li, S.-C., Schmiedek, F., Huxhold, O., Röcke, C., Smith, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2008b). Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer, and maintenance. Psychol. Aging 23, 731–742. doi:10.1037/a0014343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Little, A., DeBruine, L. M., and Jones, B. C. (2005). Sex-contingent face after-effects suggest distinct neural populations code male and female faces. Proc. Biol. Sci. 272, 2283–2287. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3220

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Little, A., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., and Waitt, C. (2008). Category contingent aftereffects for faces of different races, ages and species. Cognition 106, 1537–1547. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacLin, O. H., and Webster, M. A. (2001). Influence of adaptation on the perception of distortions in natural images. J. Electron. Imaging 10, 100–109. doi:10.1117/1.1330573

CrossRef Full Text

McKone, E., Aitkin, A., and Edwards, M. (2005). Categorical and coordinate relations in faces, or Fechner’s law and face space instead? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 1181–1198. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.6.1181

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Morikawa, K. (2005). Adaptation to asymmetrically distorted faces and its lack of effect on mirror images. Vision Res. 45, 3180–3188. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Ng, M., Boynton, G. M., and Fine, I. (2008). Face adaptation does not improve performance on search or discrimination tasks. J. Vis. 8, 1–20. doi:10.1167/8.1.1

CrossRef Full Text

Ng, M., Ciaramitaro, V. M., Anstis, S., Boynton, G. M., Fine, I., and Squire, L. R. (2006). Selectivity for the configural cues that identify the gender, ethnicity, and identity of faces in human cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 19552–19557. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605358104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

O’Neil, S. F., and Webster, M. A. (2011). Adaptation and the perception of facial age. Vis. Cogn. 19, 534–550. doi:10.1080/13506285.2011.561262

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Otten, M., and Banaji, M. R. (2012). Social categories shape the neural representation of emotion: evidence from a visual face adaptation task. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:9. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Palermo, R., Rivolta, D., Wilson, C. E., and Jeffery, L. (2011). Adaptive face space coding in congenital prosopagnosia: typical figural aftereffects but abnormal identity aftereffects. Neuropsychologia 49, 3801–3812. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.039

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G. (1988). Looking at faces: first-order and second-order features as determinants of facial appearance. Perception 17, 43–63. doi:10.1068/p170043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Evangelista, E., and Jeffery, L. (2009a). Orientation-sensitivity of face identity aftereffects. Vision Res. 49, 2379–2385. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.07.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Louw, K., and Evangelista, E. (2009b). Perceptual adaptation to facial asymmetries. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 16, 503–508. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.3.503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Jaquet, E., Jeffery, L., Evangelista, E., Keane, J., and Calder, A. J. (2011). Sex-specific norms code face identity. J. Vis. 11, 1–11. doi:10.1167/11.1.1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., and Jeffery, L. (2006). Adaptive norm-based coding of facial identity. Vision Res. 46, 2977–2987. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Clifford, C. W. G., and Leopold, D. A. (2007). The timecourse of higher-level face aftereffects. Vision Res. 47, 2291–2296. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2007.05.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Clifford, C. W. G., and Nakayama, K. (2003). Fitting the mind to the world: face adaptation and attractiveness aftereffects. Psychol. Sci. 14, 558–566. doi:10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1465.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Rhodes, G., Watson, T. L., Jeffery, L., and Clifford, C. W. G. (2010). Perceptual adaptation helps us identify faces. Vision Res. 50, 963–968. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Robbins, R., McKone, E., and Edwards, M. (2007). Aftereffects for face attributes with different natural variability: adapter position effects and neural models. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 33, 570–592. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.3.570

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Schwaninger, A., Carbon, C. C., and Leder, H. (2003). “Expert face processing: specialization and constraints,” in The Development of Face Processing, eds G. Schwarzer, and H. Leder (Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber), 81–97.

Schweinberger, S. R., Kloth, N., and Jenkins, R. (2007). Are you looking at me? Neural correlates of gaze adaptation. Neuroreport 18, 693–696.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Schweinberger, S. R., Zäske, R., Walther, C., Golle, J., Kovács, G., and Wiese, H. (2010). Young without plastic surgery: perceptual adaptation to the age of female and male faces. Vision Res. 50, 2570–2576. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strobach, T., Ditye, T., and Carbon, C.-C. (2011). Long-term adaptation effects of highly familiar faces are modulated by adaptation duration. Perception 40, 1000–1004. doi:10.1068/p6986

CrossRef Full Text

Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., and Schubert, T. (2012a). Video game practice optimizes executive control skills in dual-task and task switching situations. Acta Psychol. (Amst.) 140, 13–24. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.02.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Strobach, T., Frensch, P. A., Soutschek, A., and Schubert, T. (2012b). Investigation on the improvement and transfer of dual-task coordination skills. Psychol. Res. 76, 794–811. doi:10.1007/s00426-011-0381-0

CrossRef Full Text

Tanaka, J. W., and Farah, M. J. (1993). Parts and wholes in face recognition. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A 46A, 225–245.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tanaka, J. W., and Sengco, J. A. (1997). Features and their configuration in face recognition. Mem. Cognit. 25, 583–592.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Tillman, M., and Webster, M. (2012). Selectivity of face distortion aftereffects for differences in expression or gender. Front. Psychol. 3:14. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00014

CrossRef Full Text

Valentine, T. (2001). “Face-space models of face recognition,” in Computational, Geometric, and Process Perspectives on Facial Cognition: Contexts and Challenges, eds. M. J. Wenger, and J. T. Townsend (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 83–113.

Walton, B. R. P., and Hills, P. J. (2012). Face distortion aftereffects in personally familiar and unfamiliar faces. Front. Psychol. 3:258. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00258

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Watson, T. L., and Clifford, C. W. G. (2003). Pulling faces: an investigation of the face-distortion aftereffect. Perception 32, 1109–1116. doi:10.1068/p5082

CrossRef Full Text

Watson, T. L., and Clifford, C. W. G. (2006). Orientation dependence of the orientation-contingent face aftereffect. Vision Res. 46, 3422–3429. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.03.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. A. (2011). Adaptation and visual coding. J. Vis. 11, 1–23. doi:10.1167/11.5.3

CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., and Duhamel, P. (2004). Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature 428, 557–561. doi:10.1038/nature02420

CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. A., and MacLeod, D. I. A. (2011). Visual adaptation and the perception of faces. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1702–1725. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0360

CrossRef Full Text

Webster, M. A., and MacLin, O. H. (1999). Figural aftereffects in the perception of faces. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 647–653.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Windhager, S., Hutzler, F., Carbon, C. C., Oberzaucher, E., Schaefer, K., Thorstensen, T., et al. (2010). Laying eyes on headlights: eye tracking reveals facial features in cars. Coll. Antropol. 34, 1075–1080.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Yamashita, J. A., Hardy, J. L., De Valois, K. K., and Webster, M. A. (2005). Stimulus selectivity of figural aftereffects for faces. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 31, 420–437. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.31.3.420

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, H., Shen, J., Chen, J., and Fang, F. (2011). Face adaptation improves gender discrimination. Vision Res. 51, 105–110. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Young, A. W., Hellawell, D., and Hay, D. C. (1987). Configurational information in face perception. Perception 16, 747–759. doi:10.1068/p160747

CrossRef Full Text

Zhao, L., and Chubb, C. (2001). The size-tuning of the face-distortion after-effect. Vision Res. 41, 2979–2994. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00202-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: face adaptation, figural adaptation effects, memory representation, learning, plasticity, perception, transfer, delay

Citation: Strobach T and Carbon C-C (2013) Face adaptation effects: reviewing the impact of adapting information, time, and transfer. Front. Psychol. 4:318. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00318

Received: 01 March 2013; Accepted: 16 May 2013;
Published online: 03 June 2013.

Edited by:

Peter J. Hills, Anglia Ruskin University, UK

Reviewed by:

Alice O’Toole, University of Texas at Dallas, USA
Graham Hole, University of Sussex, UK

Copyright: © 2013 Strobach and Carbon. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: Tilo Strobach, Department of Psychology, Humboldt-University Berlin, Rudower Chaussee 18, 12489 Berlin, Germany e-mail: tilo.strobach@hu-berlin.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.