Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 August 2012
Sec. Comparative Psychology

Do horses expect humans to solve their problems?


      C. Lesimple* C. Lesimple1*C. Sankey C. Sankey1M. A. Richard M. A. Richard2M. Hausberger,
   M. Hausberger1,2
  • 1 Laboratoire d’Éthologie Animale et Humaine EthoS – UMR CNRS 6552, Station Biologique, Université de Rennes 1, Paimpont, France
  • 2 Laboratoire d’Éthologie Animale et Humaine EthoS – UMR CNRS 6552, Campus de Beaulieu, Université de Rennes 1, Rennes, France

Domestic animals are highly capable of detecting human cues, while wild relatives tend to perform less well (e.g., responding to pointing gestures). It is suggested that domestication may have led to the development of such cognitive skills. Here, we hypothesized that because domestic animals are so attentive and dependant to humans’ actions for resources, the counter effect may be a decline of self sufficiency, such as individual task solving. Here we show a negative correlation between the performance in a learning task (opening a chest) and the interest shown by horses toward humans, despite high motivation expressed by investigative behaviors directed at the chest. If human-directed attention reflects the development of particular skills in domestic animals, this is to our knowledge the first study highlighting a link between human-directed behaviors and impaired individual solving task skills (ability to solve a task by themselves) in horses.

Introduction

Domestic animals are highly efficient in detecting human cues, even subtle ones (e.g., Gacsi et al., 2004; Maros et al., 2008; Proops et al., 2009), while close wild relatives tend to perform less well in human related tasks, such as responding to pointing gestures or gaze direction (Hare et al., 2002; Miklosi et al., 2003; Gacsi et al., 2005). It is generally suggested that domestication through genetic and/or experiential processes, may have promoted the development of skills related to the detection of human signals (Hare et al., 1998; Virányi et al., 2008).

Non-domestic captive animals also show some ability to respond to human signals such as pointing gestures (e.g., Miklósi and Soproni, 2006 for a review) or to detect human attentional states (dolphins: Xitco et al., 2004; apes: Hattori et al., 2007; Tempelmann et al., 2011). It is therefore quite possible that captive and domestic animals learn through experience, thanks to interactions with humans during ontogeny and learning/conditioning experiences that include reinforcement of responses to human actions (Udell et al., 2008, 2010, but, see Hare et al., 2010 for comments).

Whether the domestication process or experience with humans (or both) are involved, these processes are generally viewed as a progress in the development of specific sophisticated cognitive skills, such as human signals reading. Indeed, both dogs (Adachi et al., 2007) and horses (Proops et al., 2009; Sankey et al., 2011) have been shown to have expectations of humans’ behavior in a given context. Thus, it was shown that dogs that gaze more at their owner have lower success in problem solving tasks (Topál et al., 1997).

In the present study, we hypothesized that because domestic animals are so attentive to humans and so dependent upon human actions for resources, the counter effect of these abilities may be a lowering of other cognitive skills, such as individual task solving. We investigated whether the performance of domestic horses in an instrumental learning task was influenced by their attention toward the human present.

Experimental Procedure

Animals

The experiments were performed between November 2009 and October 2010. The 46 tested horses (24 mares, 22 geldings, 5–23 years old, 8 breeds) were distributed across five French riding schools with similar activities (teaching riders, from beginners to moderate level). Horses were mostly kept singly in 3 m × 3 m straw-bedded individual boxes, fed industrial pellets two or three times a day, and hay once or twice a day. Only seven horses were kept in pasture in group, and fed hay ad libitum. All horses had water ad libitum.

Horses worked 6–20 h per week in riding lessons involving children and teenagers, with at least one closing day.

Chest Test

The chest test performed here, described in Wolff and Hausberger (1996), is an instrumental learning task, leading to easily measurable performances and presenting correlations with learning abilities at work as assessed by riding teachers (LeScolan et al., 1997). It has been useful to measure cognitive abilities according to different factors such as breed, work (Hausberger et al., 2004), or welfare state (Hausberger et al., 2007). The task consisted of opening a wooden chest (60 cm × 50 cm × 40 cm) in order to find food (Figure 1). To ensure the motivation of the horses for the food, the test was performed in the hour preceding the usual meal time, and the food placed in the chest was their usual food.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The chest test: example of behaviors. Sniffing the lid/Lifting the lid/Opening the chest/Eating food.

In accordance with previous studies, the test was composed of three trials of 3 min, each being preceded by a demonstration. During the demonstrations, the horse was led by the experimenter (C.L. female) with a halter and a rope. After the demonstrations, the experimenter took the rope off, and the trials began. During the trials, the experimenter stood motionless, in a neutral position, with her arms hanging beside the body.

– First trial: the experimenter lifted the lid to open the chest, allowing the horse to see the food inside, and moved the food with the hand.

– Second trial: the experimenter lifted the lid, and then allowed the horse to put its nose inside the chest in order to smell the food.

– Third trial: the experimenter lifted the lid and allowed the horse to introduce its head in the chest in order to eat one mouthful of food.

The total time required to open the chest was recorded (maximum 540 s). The experimenter recorded, using a digital voice recorder (Thomson DK 300).

The following behaviors were recorded:

Exploration behaviors toward the chest, the trough, and/or the experimenter (see Waring, 1983):

– Sniffing: nose near the object of interest, drawing air in through the nostrils.

– Licking: touch an object with the tongue.

– Biting: take the object between the teeth.

– Nibbling: touch an object with lips, moving the lips laterally.

– Pushing: touch the object with the nose or head and pushes on it.

Frustration/excitement behaviors:

– Headshaking: quick vertical movement of the head.

– Startling: quick involuntary movement associated with surprise or alarm.

– Vacuum chewing: chewing with nothing in the mouth (Bergeron et al., 2006).

– Pawing: a foreleg is lifted off the ground, extended in the object direction, and brought back (Waring, 1983).

– Snorting: forceful exhalation through the nostrils (Waring, 1983).

– Whinnying: loud prolonged call associated with alert (Waring, 1983).

Gazes directed to the chest, the trough, and the experimenter were also recorded (i.e., when horse turned its head toward the object of interest and fixed its eyes on it.).

All horses were naïve to this task. Only one experimenter was involved (C. Lesimple), preventing risks of inter observer differences.

Data and Statistical Analysis

As data were not normally distributed, we used non-parametric statistical tests (Siegel and Castellan, 1988), with a significance threshold at 0.05.

The homogeneity of the data between the five schools was assessed using the Kruskall–Wallis test (KW). As no differences emerged between the five schools, neither in the time nor in the number of trials required to open the chest (KW, p > 0.1 in each case), data from the different schools were pooled.

Fisher’s exact test and Spearman’s correlation tests were used to explore the links between horse performance and behavior during the test. Friedman ANOVAs were used to assess the change during the test (i.e., between the three trials).

Results

During the hour preceding the meal time, horses were required to open a chest containing their usual food. The test was divided into three trials lasting 3 min each, so that horses had a maximum of 9 min to succeed in opening the chest.

Half of the horses succeeded in opening the chest within three trials (23/46), and most of them showed some interest in the experimenter, from mere gazes (N = 38, 82.6%) to exploration (sniffing, nibbling, N = 30, 65.2%).

Interest in the experimenter was associated with failure to perform the task, as more than half of the horses that showed exploration behaviors toward the experimenter failed to open the chest (N = 18, 60%), while only a third of those that did not (N = 5, 31.2%) failed (Fisher’s exact test, p = 0.04).

In fact, there was even a clear correlation between horses’ interest toward the experimenter (exploration + gazes) and the time (Spearman’s correlation test: N = 46, rs = 0.72, p = 0.0001; Figure 2) and number of trials (Spearman’s correlation test: N = 46, rs = 0.71, p = 0.0001) required to open the chest. Interestingly, this was not related to a lack of interest in the chest.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Correlation between the interest of the horse toward the experimenter and the time required to open the chest (Spearman correlation test, ***p < 0.001). Data are represented in number of behaviors and time (s) required to open the chest.

Indeed, horses that showed most interest in the experimenter were also those that showed most direct (but inappropriate) investigative behaviors toward the chest (sniffing sides: Spearman correlation test: N = 46, rs = 0.45, p = 0.001, and lid rs = 0.35, p = 0.02; licking the chest rs = 0.6, p = 0.003; biting the chest rs = 0.31, p = 0.03), while being at the same time those that performed the less (in the context) “appropriate” behaviors toward the chest (creeping their nose under the lid, rs = −0.37, p = 0.001).

Therefore, the “human-directed” horses appeared to investigate more the chest without properly trying to open it, expressing at the same time behaviors that indicated frustration such as head shaking (rs = 0.35, p = 0.02), snorting (rs = 0.34, p = 0.02), startling (rs = 0.42, p = 0.004), and vacuum chewing (rs = 0.29, p = 0.05).

As the amount of human-directed, trough-directed (Table 1) and frustration behaviors did not change across trials (no differences between trials 1, 2, and 3, p > 0.05), it seems that this tendency to depend upon humans and be less efficient in opening the chest could be an intrinsic characteristic of these animals more than a consequence of the waiting for the food.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Change of the number of human-directed and trough-directed behaviors across trials for horses that failed to open the chest (N = 23).

Discussion

Observations of horses faced to a food related instrumental task (opening a chest) showed clear correlations between their interest in the human and success or failure to the task. Interest toward human was associated with increased (“inappropriate”) investigation behaviors toward the chest, frustration behaviors, and failure to open the chest. As it was also correlated with interest toward the usual feeding trough (where humans usually put the food), one interpretation could be that these horses expected humans to pour the food in the feeder as they usually do. They made less “appropriate” real trials to open (creeping their nose under the lid) which may mean that either they did not “understand the task” (i.e., lack of “causal learning”) and relied upon humans to solve the problem and/or they had their attention focused on the humans’ face (monitoring its attentional state, e.g., Sankey et al., 2011) and therefore not on the way the chest could be opened (lifting the lid; i.e., “oversensitivity to human distractor”). In the same way, dogs that were more dependent to their owners (more glancing and following) showed decreased problem solving abilities in a food related task (Topál et al., 1997), suggesting that a strong attachment to humans could lead to an impairment of these abilities.

Horses do not learn to perform a spatial or instrumental task from a human demonstrator (Wolff and Hausberger, 1996) and therefore they had to find the solution to open it by lifting the lid. Half of them succeeded, which is in accordance with some earlier findings in domestic horses (LeScolan et al., 1997; Lesimple et al., 2011). Both genetic and environmental factors (such as type of work) have been shown to influence performances in this task (Hausberger et al., 2004), which means that both the domestication process and experience may be involved in determining the success rate observed. The data here do not allow to determine which kind of experience could be involved. Horses fed with automatic distribution have been shown to exhibit less frustration than those fed manually (Fureix et al., 2011). One could think also that the quality of the human-horse relationship may influence the horses’ interest toward the experimenter (Hausberger et al., 2008; Fureix et al., 2009; Sankey et al., 2010). Only further experiments will bring possible answers. Both may also be involved in horses’ expectations of “human help”. Further studies comparing wild/feral to domestic horses and different feeding conditions in domestic horses should now be performed.

Domestication however cannot fully explain the observed individual differences which may also reflect different levels of dependency on human actions (Topál et al., 1997; Smith and Litchfield, 2010; Jakovcevic et al., 2012). Relation to humans varies individually in horses (e.g., Hausberger et al., 2008), and we suggest that the level of dependency may also vary. What selection and domestication may well have brought is a finer “knowledge” of human behavior but also maybe a decrease of self sufficiency when faced to non-usual challenges.

Author Contribution Statement

C. Lesimple performed the experiments, analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. C. Sankey contributed to data analyses and corrected the English. M. A. Richard designed the experiment and contributed to data analyses. M. Hausberger designed the experiments, contributed to data analyses and to manuscript writing. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the owner and staff of the riding schools for allowing us to work with their horses and for their help and understanding; M. Madec for her help during observations; Séverine Henry for helpful discussions. The Caisse Centrale de la Mutualité Sociale Agricole supported this study.

References

Adachi, I., Kuwahata, H., and Fujita, K. (2007). Dogs recall their owner’s face upon hearing their owner’s voice. Anim. Cogn. 10, 17–21.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bergeron, R., Badnell-Waters, S., Lambton, S., and Mason, G. (2006). “Captive ungulates: foraging, diet and gastrointestinal function,” in Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare, 2nd Edn, eds G. Mason and J. Rushen (Oxford: CABI Press), 19–57.

Fureix, C., Gorecka-Bruzda, A., Gautier, E., and Hausberger, M. (2011). Co-occurrence of yawning and stereotypic behaviour in horses Equus caballus. ISRN Zool. 98, 583–592.

Fureix, C., Jego, P., Sankey, C., and Hausberger, M. (2009). How horses (Equus caballus) see the world: humans as significant “objects.” Anim. Cogn. 12, 643–654.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gacsi, M., Gyori, B., Miklósi, A., Viranyi, Z., and Kubinyi, E. (2005). Species-specific differences and similarities in the behavior of hand-raised dogs and wolf pups in social situations with humans. Dev. Psychobiol. 47, 111–122.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gacsi, M., Miklósi, A., Varga, O., Topál, J., and Csányi, V. (2004). Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention. Anim. Cogn. 7, 144–153.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hare, B., Brown, M., Williamson, C., and Tomasello, M. (2002). The domestication of social cognition in dogs. Science 298, 1634–1636.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hare, B., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Communication of food location between human and dog (Canis familiaris). Evol. Comm. 2, 137–159.

CrossRef Full Text

Hare, B., Rosati, A., Kaminski, J., Bräuer, J., Call, J., and Tomasello, M. (2010). The domestication hypothesis for dogs’ skills with human communication: a response to Udell et al. (2008) and Wynne et al. (2008). Anim. Behav. 79, e1–e6.

CrossRef Full Text

Hattori, Y., Kuroshima, H., and Fujita, K. (2007). I know you are not looking at me: capuchin monkey’s (Cebus apella) sensitivity to human attentional states. Anim. Cogn. 10, 141–148.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hausberger, M., Bruderer, C., LeScolan, N., and Pierre, J.-S. (2004). Interplay between environmental and genetic factors in temperament/personality traits in horses (Equus caballus). J. Comp. Psychol. 118, 434–446.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hausberger, M., Gautier, E., Müller, C., and Jego, P. (2007). Lower learning abilities in stereotypic horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 107, 299–306.

CrossRef Full Text

Hausberger, M., Roche, H., Henry, S., and Visser, K. (2008). A review of the human–horse relationship. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 109, 1–24.

CrossRef Full Text

Jakovcevic, A., Mustaca, A., and Bentosela, M. (2012). Do more sociable dogs gaze longer to the human face than less sociable ones? Behav. Processes 90, 217–222.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

LeScolan, N., Hausberger, M., and Wolff, A. (1997). Stability over situations in temperamental traits of horses as revealed by experimental and scoring approaches. Behav. Processes 41, 257–266.

CrossRef Full Text

Lesimple, C., Fureix, C., LeScolan, N., Richard-Yris, M.-A., and Hausberger, M. (2011). Housing conditions and breed are associated with emotionality and cognitive abilities in riding school horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 129, 92–99.

CrossRef Full Text

Maros, K., Gácsi, M., and Miklósi, A. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses Equus caballus. Anim. Cogn. 11, 457–466.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miklosi, A., Kubinyi, E., Topal, J., Gacsi, M., Viranyi, Z., and Csanyi, V. (2003). A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do. Curr. Biol. 13, 763–766.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Miklósi, A., and Soproni, K. (2006). A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim. Cogn. 9, 81–93.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Proops, L., McComb, K., and Reby, D. (2009). Cross-modal recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 947–951.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sankey, C., Henry, S., André, N., Richard-Yris, M.-A., and Hausberger, M. (2011). Do horses have a concept of person? PLoS ONE 6, e18331. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018331

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sankey, C., Richard, M.-A., Leroy, H., Henry, S., and Hausberger, M. (2010). Positive interactions lead to lasting positive memories in horses, Equus caballus. Anim. Behav. 79, 869–875.

CrossRef Full Text

Siegel, S., and Castellan, N. (1988). Non Parametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill, 399.

Smith, B., and Litchfield, C. (2010). How well do dingoes, Canis dingo, perform on the detour task? Anim. Behav. 80, 155–162.

CrossRef Full Text

Tempelmann, S., Kaminski, J., and Liebal, K. (2011). Focus on the essential: all great apes know when others are being attentive. Anim. Cogn. 14, 433–439.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Topál, J., Miklósi, A., and Csányi, V. (1997). Dog-human relationship affects problem solving behavior in the dog. Anthrozoos 10, 214–224.

CrossRef Full Text

Udell, M., Dorey, N., and Wynne, C. (2008). Wolves outperform dogs in following human social cues. Anim. Behav. 76, 1767–1773.

CrossRef Full Text

Udell, M., Dorey, N., and Wynne, C. (2010). What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions. Biol. Rev. 85, 327–345.

CrossRef Full Text

Virányi, Z., Gácsi, M., Kubinyi, E., Topál, J., Belényi, B., Ujfalussy, D., and Miklósi, A. (2008). Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris). Anim. Cogn. 11, 373–387.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Waring, G. H. (1983). Horse Behavior: The Behavioral Traits and Adaptations of Domestic and Wild Horses, Including Ponies. New Jersey: Noyes Publications, 292.

Wolff, A., and Hausberger, M. (1996). Learning and memorization of two different tasks in horses: the effects of age, sex and sire. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 46, 137–143.

CrossRef Full Text

Xitco, M., Gory, J., and Kuczaj, S. (2004). Dolphin pointing is linked to the attention behaviour of a receiver. Anim. Cogn. 7, 231–238.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: horses, cognitive skills, human-animal relationship, attention

Citation: Lesimple C, Sankey C, Richard MA and Hausberger M (2012) Do horses expect humans to solve their problems? Front. Psychology 3:306. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00306

Received: 04 May 2012; Accepted: 04 August 2012;
Published online: 24 August 2012.

Edited by:

Jeffrey R. Stevens, University of Nebraska-Lincoln, USA

Reviewed by:

Jozsef Topal, Institute of Psychology Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Hungary
Valérie Dufour, CNRS, France

Copyright: © 2012 Lesimple, Sankey, Richard and Hausberger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.

*Correspondence: C. Lesimple, Laboratoire EthoS – UMR 6552, Station Biologique, Université de Rennes 1, 35380 Paimpont, France. e-mail: clemence.lesimple@univ-rennes1.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.