Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 15 September 2022
Sec. Social Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Rising Stars in Social Cognition in Psychiatry View all 7 articles

Social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy: A meta-analysis

\nYang SunYang Sun1Jing ZhaoJing Zhao2PanWen ZhaoPanWen Zhao2Hui ZhangHui Zhang2JianGuo ZhongJianGuo Zhong3PingLei Pan,PingLei Pan4,5GenDi WangGenDi Wang3ZhongQuan Yi
ZhongQuan Yi2*LiLi Xie
LiLi Xie3*
  • 1Department of Clinical Laboratory, The Fourth Affiliated Hospital of Nantong University, The First People's Hospital of Yancheng, Yancheng, Jiangsu, China
  • 2Department of Central Laboratory, The Sixth Affiliated Hospital of Nantong University, Yancheng Third People's Hospital, Yancheng, Jiangsu, China
  • 3Department of Neurology, The Sixth Affiliated Hospital of Nantong University, Yancheng Third People's Hospital, Yancheng, Jiangsu, China
  • 4Department of Neurology, The Sixth Affiliated Hospital of Nantong University, Yancheng Third People's Hospital, Yancheng, Jiangsu, China
  • 5Department of Central Laboratory, The Sixth Affiliated Hospital of Nantong University, Yancheng Third People's Hospital, Yancheng, Jiangsu, China

Many studies have investigated impairments in two key domains of social cognition (theory of mind [ToM] and facial emotion recognition [FER]) in children and adolescents with epilepsy. However, inconsistent conclusions were found. Our objective was to characterize social cognition performance of children and adolescents with epilepsy. A literature search was conducted using Web of Science, PubMed, and Embase databases. The article retrieval, screening, quality assessment (Newcastle-Ottawa-Scale), and data extraction were performed independently by two investigators. A random-effects model was used to examine estimates. The meta-analysis included 19 studies, with a combined sample of 623 children and adolescents with epilepsy (mean [SD] age, 12.13 [2.62] years; 46.1% female) and 677 healthy controls [HCs]) (mean [SD] age, 11.48 [2.71] years; 50.7% female). The results revealed that relative to HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited deficits in ToM (g = −1.08, 95% CI [−1.38, −0.78], p < 0.001, the number of studies [k] = 13), FER (g = −0.98, 95% CI [−1.33, −0.64], p < 0.001, k = 12), and ToM subcomponents (cognitive ToM: g = −1.04, 95% CI [−1.35, −0.72], p < 0.001, k = 12] and affective ToM: g = −0.73, 95% CI [−1.12, −0.34], p < 0.001, k = 8). In addition, there were no statistically significant differences in social cognition deficits between children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy. Meta-regressions confirmed the robustness of the results. These quantitative results further deepen our understanding of the two core domains of social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy and may assist in the development of cognitive interventions for this patient population.

Systematic review registration: https://inplasy.com/inplasy-2022-3-0011/, identifier INPLASY202230011.

Highlights

- Children and adolescents with epilepsy affect social cognition—theory of mind (ToM) and facial emotion recognition (FER).

- We quantify the magnitude of deficits in ToM and FER in children and adolescents with epilepsy.

- Childhood focal and generalized epilepsy show similar deficits in social cognition.

- This may inform the development of structured cognitive interventions for this patient population.

Introduction

Epilepsy, characterized by chronic, unprovoked and recurrent seizures (1), is a common neurological condition and usually has its onset in early development (2, 3). An estimated 50 million people worldwide suffer from epileptic seizures (4), with more than half of these cases beginning in childhood and adolescence (5). Recent studies suggests that children and adolescents with epilepsy frequently have psychosocial dysfunction, which commonly lead to pervasive social problems, such as severe economic burdens and lower quality of life (6, 7). Although psychosocial function is influenced by many factors, there is growing evidence that social cognitive skills may be the key contributor (810). Social cognitive skills include the abilities to perceive, encode, process, and interpret social information (11, 12). Social cognition contains different domains, mainly involving social knowledge, social perception, theory of mind (ToM), attribution style, empathy, and emotion recognition (11, 13).

Among them, ToM and facial emotion recognition (FER) are two core structures that have been frequently studied. ToM is the ability to attribute mental states of other people [intentions, beliefs, and emotions]) (14). It is a complex construct with multiple components that is generally divided into two categories: cognitive and affective ToM. FER is the ability to identify a specific emotional state through the interpretation of another person's facial features (1517).

To date, many studies have investigated impairments in ToM or FER in children and adolescents with epilepsy (1821). However, most studies had small sample sizes and inconsistent conclusions. For example, some studies found that children and adolescents with epilepsy have significant ToM deficits compared to healthy controls (HCs) (20, 2224), while others found no significant between-group differences (25, 26). Besides, for the recognition of anger, Pastorino et al. (20) found that children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited large impairment compared to HCs, and Morningstar et al. (27) showed moderate impairment, whereas Wu et al. (21) and Braams et al. (28) found no significant differences between groups. Besides, the assessment methods used varied across studies (6, 1922, 25, 29). A meta-analysis can improve statistical power and help refine the conclusions drawn from the inconsistent findings of previous studies.

To the best of our knowledge, six meta-analyses have examined the differences in social cognition between patients with epilepsy and HCs (3035). However, one included only adult patients with epilepsy (34), the others included patients of different age groups (3035). Furthermore, to date, no meta-analysis has examined social cognition performance in children with epilepsy or adolescents with epilepsy. Consequently, the primary aim of this study was to provide the first meta-analysis to examine differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and HCs in terms of ToM and FER performance. Besides, subgroup analyses were performed to assess the impairment in different aspects of ToM (including cognitive ToM and affective ToM) and individual ToM tasks (such as strange stories test [SST]). In addition, taking into account appropriate control measures of ToM or FER tasks were also important factors (36), subgroup analyses were performed to assess the impairment in control measures of ToM or FER tasks (such as control measures of SST and control measures of FER). Moreover, considering that epileptic seizures were categorized by seizure onset into partial or generalized (37), subgroup analyses were conducted to examine social cognition deficits in children and adolescents with focal vs. generalized epilepsy. Additionally, given that temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) and frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) are more commonly studied in focal epilepsy (38, 39), subgroup analyses were also conducted to examine social cognition deficits in children and adolescents with TLE vs. FLE. Furthermore, meta-regression analyses were established to investigate whether the severity of ToM or FER impairment was moderated by potential demographic and epilepsy-related factors (such as sex, education level, age at testing, age at epilepsy onset, duration of epilepsy, full-scale Intelligence Quotient [IQ], verbal IQ, monthly seizure frequency, and number of anti-epileptic drugs [AEDs]). With this meta-analysis, we hope to promote a more comprehensive and nuanced understanding of how these two core domains of social cognition (ToM and FER) are affected in children and adolescents with epilepsy.

Methods

Study registration

This meta-analysis was conducted in line with the Preferred Reporting Items of Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (40). The protocol of this meta-analysis was registered at the International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols (ID: INPLASY 202230011).

Literature search strategy and data sources

Two investigators independently completed article retrieval, screening, quality assessment, and data extraction. Any disagreements were first discussed between the two investigators, and further disagreements were arbitrated by a third investigator.

A systematic literature search was conducted using Web of Science, PubMed, and Embase databases (up to July 21th, 2022). The following search terms were used: epilepsy or epileps* or convulsion* or seizure disorder combined with: social cognition or theory of mind or ToM or mentalizing or mentalising or Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test or Faux pas task or False Belief or the Awareness of Social Inference Test or Virtual Assessment of Mentalising Ability or the Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition or picture sequencing task or Cartoon Test or Hinting Test or Strange Stories Test or sarcas* or lie* or joke* or facial expression* or facial emotion recognition. A backward citation search was also performed.

Study selection

First, duplicate items were removed. Subsequent primary screening of titles and abstracts were screened to remove ineligibility (i.e. literature reviews, abstracts, animal studies, no mention of epilepsy, or non-social cognition measures; see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flowchart of study screening and selection process.

Finally, a full-text screen was conducted.

Studies were included if they fit 5 criteria. First, they had to be published in peer-reviewed journals in English. Second, they included individuals with epilepsy < 18 years of age. Third, they compared children and adolescents with epilepsy to HCs group. Fourth, they assessed ToM or FER performance. Fifth, the statistics were published and could be used to calculate precise group comparison effect sizes. The authors were contacted if data were insufficient to calculate effect sizes. Studies were excluded if authors did not respond after 2 weeks. Studies were included if they provided data that could be used to calculate effect sizes for group comparisons.

Studies were excluded for 4 reasons. First, studies were excluded if they lacked a HCs group. Second, studies were excluded if they lacked comparisons between children and adolescents with epilepsy and HCs. Third, studies were excluded if the sample overlapped with another study with larger sample sizes. Fourth, studies were excluded if the sample size was <10 (41).

Study quality assessment

Study quality was assessed using a nine-star protocol based on the Newcastle-Ottawa-Scale (NOS) (42). For case-control studies, the NOS contains eight items grouped into three dimensions including selection, comparability, and exposure. Selection 1: Is the case definition adequate?; Selection 2: Representativeness of the cases; Selection 3: Selection of Controls; Selection 4: Definition of Controls; Comparability: Comparability of cases and controls on the basis of the design or analysis. Here, we selected “age at testing” as the most important adjusting factor and selected “sex” as other controlled factor; Exposure 1: Ascertainment of exposure; Exposure 2: Same method of ascertainment for cases and controls; Exposure 3: Non-Response rate. For each item a series of response options is provided. A maximum of one star each item is awarded for high-quality research, except for item related to comparability, where assignment of two stars is permitted. If star rating ≥ 7, the studies were considered high-quality (42).

Data extraction

The data included:

1) The basic title information mainly included name of first author, year of publication, and title.

2) Characteristics of the sample. The key variables here were number of participants in the epilepsy and control groups, epilepsy type, education level, age at testing, monthly seizure frequency, sex (female and male), number of AEDs, IQ score, and illness duration.

3) For ToM tasks, tasks were divided into cognitive and affective subcomponents.

4) Data used to calculate precise effect sizes for ToM or FER measures (e.g., mean and standard deviations, [SD] for the epilepsy group and HCs group).

Social cognition measures

Table 1 summarizes the different individual ToM tasks used, most common being the SST, faux pas task (FPT), and reading the mind in the eyes test (RMET); other tasks included false-belief test (FBT), ToM subscale from the second edition of the developmental neuropsychological assessment battery (NEPSY-II), Yoni task, ToM storybooks, ToM: Intentional lying, ToM: Sarcasm, ToM Inventory (TOMI).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics and patient demographics of studies included in the meta-analysis.

Different FER tasks were used across studies, such as the FER subscale from NEPSY-II, facial expression of emotion stimuli and tests, emotion detection task, the test de reconnaissance des emotions faciales pour enfants, the pictures of facial affect, and the eye basic emotion discrimination task. Although definitions differed across tasks, all tasks required participants to identify and discriminate between the emotional states of others based on sets of photographs/drawings of basic facial emotions.

Cognitive ToM and affective ToM

Cognitive ToM involves the cognitive understanding of the other person's thoughts, intentions, and beliefs (4749). It can be evaluated through several tasks such as the SST, ToM storybooks, FBT, and TOMI, as well as the cognitive subcomponents of the Yoni task, FPT, and ToM subscale from NEPSY-II.

Affective ToM is described as the capacity to infer another person's emotional states (4749). It can be evaluated through several tasks such as the RMET, ToM: Intentional lying, and ToM: Sarcasm, as well as the affective subcomponents of the Yoni task, FPT, and ToM subscale from NEPSY-II.

Statistical analysis

For analyses, the Stata 15.0 software package was used, with Hedges g the index of effect size (50, 51), and effects of 0.2 interpreted as small, 0.5 indicates a medium effect, and values equal to or larger than 0.8 indicates a large effect (52). Meta-analyses were completed using a random-effects model. Negative effect sizes values indicated poorer performance for children and adolescents with epilepsy compared to HCs.

For studies that did not provide total scores on overall ToM performance, but reported multiple individual ToM tasks, pooled effect sizes were aggregated by calculating the mean effect size and standard error (53). Aggregate effect sizes for cognitive ToM, affective ToM, or overall FER were calculated in a similar manner.

We used I2 statistics to assess study heterogeneity. As suggested by Higgins, values of 0–25% indicate no heterogeneity, I2 values between 25 and 50% indicate small magnitudes of heterogeneity, and I2 values in the range 50 and 75% are interpreted as moderate heterogeneity, while >75% are explained for large heterogeneity (54, 55). To assess risk of publication bias, the Egger's test was evaluated (56). In the case of a significant Egger's test, the trim-and-fill method was applied to provide adjusted effect sizes (57).

Subgroup analysis were conducted for different aspects of ToM (including cognitive ToM and affective ToM), individual ToM tasks (including SST, FPT, RMET, FBT, and ToM subscale from NEPSY-II), control measures of ToM tasks (including control measures of SST, control measures of FPT, and control measures of FBT), and individual emotions recognition (including happy, anger, fear, sad, disgust, neutral, and surprise). Besides, subgroup analyses were also conducted to examine social cognition deficits in children and adolescents with focal vs. generalized epilepsy, and in children and adolescents with TLE vs. FLE (more information is provided in Supplementary Table 1).

In addition, of the included studies, 12 of the 19 studies explicitly limited their samples to individuals with an IQ score >70 (5, 6, 1722, 2427, 43). Although the other 4 studies did not explicitly limit IQ scores in the exclusion/inclusion criteria, the mean IQ scores of the samples were significantly >70 (23, 28, 29, 45). The other two included children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS) (25, 44), who usually exhibited normal intelligence (5860). The remaining 1 study limited samples to individuals with an IQ score >60, and of the 56 patients included, 6 were in the range of 60 to 69 and the others ≥70 (46). Considering that the IQ of the included sample may impact the results (60), subgroup analysis were conducted to examine social cognition deficits in samples with an IQ score >70.

Meta-regression analyses were conducted to investigate whether the severity of overall ToM or overall FER impairment was affected by potential demographic and epilepsy-related factors (sex, education level, age at testing, age at epilepsy onset, duration of epilepsy, full-scale IQ, verbal IQ, monthly seizure frequency, and number of AEDs). The significance level was set at p < 0.05. For each individual factors in the meta-regression model, a minimum of 3 data points was required (61).

Results

Study characteristics

The study selection process is summarized in Figure 1. In total, 3643 potentially eligible articles were retrieved. After the removal of duplicates, 2904 articles remained, which were then subjected to title and abstract screening. Of these, 31 initially met the inclusion criteria. After full-text screening, 12 were excluded for the following reasons: the study did not provide sufficient data to calculate the effect sizes of ToM or FER (the number of studies [k] = 6) (6267); the study population included adults (k = 3) (6870); the samples overlapped with those of other studies (k = 2) (71, 72); and the sample size was under 10 (k = 1) (73). Finally, a total of 19 studies with 623 children and adolescents with epilepsy (mean age = 12.13 years, SD = 2.62, 46.1 % female) and 677 HCs (mean age = 11.48 years, SD = 2.71, 50.7 % female) were included in this meta-analysis (Table 1) (5, 6, 1729, 4346). Of these studies included, 12 studies (384 children and adolescents with epilepsy and 473 HCs) examined overall ToM and 9 datasets examining FER (324 children and adolescents with epilepsy and 459 HCs).

The mean score of the study quality assessment was 7.21 (SD = 0.92), and 15 of the 19 case-control studies were awarded ≥ 7 stars and considered of high quality (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Quality evaluation of included studies.

ToM in children and adolescents with epilepsy

Table 3 shows the relevant results of this meta-analysis. Compared to HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy had impairment in overall ToM, with this deficit being significant in the magnitude (g = −1.08, 95% CI [−1.38, −0.78], z = −7.10, p < 0.001, k = 13, see Figure 2). When considering the different subcomponents of ToM, the findings showed that children and adolescents with epilepsy was associated with large impairment in cognitive ToM (g = −1.04, 95% CI [−1.35, −0.72], z = −6.38, p < 0.001, k = 12, see Figure 2) and medium impairment in affective ToM (g = −0.73, 95% CI [−1.12, −0.34], z = −3.65, p < 0.001, k = 8, see Figure 2). For individual ToM tasks (Supplementary Figure 1), children and adolescents with epilepsy performed worse than the HCs on SST, FPT, and FBT measures. However, no group differences were observed for performance as indicated by RMET or ToM subscale from NEPSY-II measures. Besides, for control measures of ToM tasks (Supplementary Figure 2), relative to the HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy performed worse on control measures of SST and FPT, but no significant differences were observed for control measures of FBT. For studies that included only samples with an IQ score >70, relative to HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited large impairments in overall ToM and cognitive ToM, and medium impairment in affective ToM.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean effects for ToM and FER subcomponents comparing children and adolescents with epilepsy against healthy controls and tests for publication bias.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall ToM, cognitive ToM, and affective ToM differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and healthy controls.

There was no heterogeneity across studies for FPT, FBT, RMET, control measures of SST, or control measures of FBT (I2 = 0). Small heterogeneity was found for control measures of FPT (I2 = 43%), moderate heterogeneity was found for overall ToM (I2 = 70%), cognitive ToM (I2 = 69%), and affective ToM (I2 = 72%), and significant heterogeneity was found among studies on SST (I2 = 82%) and NEPSY-II (I2 = 92%). There was no significant publication bias for overall ToM, cognitive ToM, affective ToM, any individual ToM tasks, or any control measures of ToM tasks.

Meta-regression analysis for overall ToM

The following variables did not account for the significant variance in overall ToM (sex, t = −0.18, p = 0.857, k =12; age at testing, t = 0.14, p = 0.892, k =13; age at epilepsy onset, t =1.4, p = 0.190, k =13; duration of epilepsy, t = −1.41, p =0.195, k =10; full-scale IQ, t = 0.66, p =0.554, k =5; education level, t = −1.76, p =0.328, k =3; monthly seizure frequency, t = −1.34, p = 0.274, k =5; number of AEDs, t = −0.21, p = 0.886, k =3). As fewer than 3 studies contributed to the data for this subcomponent, no meta-regression analysis was performed for the effect of verbal IQ on the severity of overall ToM.

FER in children and adolescents with epilepsy

Table 3 shows the significant results of this meta-analysis. For overall FER, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited a large impairment compared to HCs (g = −0.98, 95% CI [−1.33, −0.64], z = −5.62, p < 0.001, k = 12, see Figure 3). For the analyses of individual emotions (Supplementary Figure 3), epilepsy in children and adolescents was associated with medium impairments in sad, disgust, and neutral recognition, and small impairments in anger and fear recognition. However, no group differences were evident for happy or surprise. As the included studies did not report control measures of FER tasks, we did not perform an analysis of the measures of FER tasks here.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for FER differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and healthy controls.

There was no heterogeneity across studies for neutral, small heterogeneity for happy, sad, and disgust (I2 = 26%, I2 = 27%, and I2 = 44%, respectively), moderate heterogeneity for anger and fear (I2 = 67% and I2 = 71%, respectively), and significant heterogeneity for studies on overall FER (I2 = 83%) and surprise (I2 = 83%). There was no significant publication bias for overall FER or any individual emotions.

Meta-regression analysis for FER

The variables did not account for the significant variance in the overall FER (sex, t = −0.64, p = 0.543, k = 10; age at testing, t = 0.31, p = 0.764, k = 9; age at epilepsy onset, t = −0.22, p = 0.832, k = 10; duration of epilepsy, t = −0.37, p = 0.721, k = 9; full-scale IQ, t = 1.18, p = 0.322, k = 5; verbal IQ, t = −1.94, p = 0.192, k = 4; number of AEDs, t = 0.27, p = 0.83, k = 3). As fewer than 3 studies contributed to the data for this subcomponent, no meta-regression analysis was performed for the effect of education level or monthly seizure frequency on the severity of overall FER.

ToM and FER in children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy

Table 4 depicts the key results obtained from this meta-analysis. Subgroup analyses revealed that the performance of children and adolescents with focal epilepsy (Figure 4) and generalized epilepsy (Figure 5) with respect to overall ToM (g = −1.27, 95% CI [−1.85, −0.70], z = −4.34, p < 0.001, k = 6 and g = −1.07, 95% CI [−1.49, −0.65], z = −5.01, p < 0.001, k = 4, respectively), cognitive ToM (g = −1.24, 95% CI [−1.86, −0.61], z = −3.89, p < 0.001, k = 5 and g = −1.03, 95% CI [−1.27, −0.78], z = −8.21, p < 0.001, k = 4, respectively),and overall FER (g = −1.03, 95% CI [−1.54, −0.51], z = −3.92, p < 0.001, k = 8 and g = −1.42, 95% CI [−2.07, −0.77], z = −4.27, p < 0.001, k = 1, respectively) was inferior to that of the HCs. In affective ToM, children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy did not differ from HCs (g = −0.81, 95% CI [−1.80, 0.18], z = −1.61, p = 0.107, k = 3 and g = −0.95, 95% CI [−2.45, 0.55], z = −1.24, p = 0.214, k = 2, respectively).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Mean effects for ToM and FER subcomponents comparing focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy against healthy controls and tests for publication bias.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for o overall ToM, cognitive ToM, affective ToM, and overall FER differences between focal epilepsy and healthy controls.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for overall ToM, cognitive ToM, affective ToM, and overall FER differences between generalized epilepsy and healthy controls.

The effect sizes of the focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy groups were comparable for overall ToM (Q = 0.32, df = 1, p = 0.570), cognitive ToM (Q = 0.38, df = 1, p = 0.538), affective ToM (Q = 0.02, df = 1, p = 0.880), and overall FER (Q = 0.86, df = 1, p = 0.353).

ToM and FER in children and adolescents with TLE and FLE

Supplementary Table 1 depicts the key results obtained from this meta-analysis. Compared to HCs, children and adolescents with TLE had large impairment in overall ToM (g = −1.56, 95% CI [−2.50, −0.63], z = −3.27, p = 0.001, k = 1), cognitive ToM (g = −1.58, 95% CI [−2.94, −0.23], z = −2.30, p = 0.022, k = 1), and affective ToM (g = −1.70, 95% CI [−2.38, −1.02], z = −4.91, p < 0.001, k = 1). For overall FER, relative to HCs, children and adolescents with TLE showed large impairment (g = −1.06, 95% CI [−1.37, −0.74], z = −6.58, p < 0.001, k = 3), while children and adolescents with FLE showed moderate impairment (g = −0.64, 95% CI [−1.08, −0.21], z = −2.89, p = 0.004, k = 2).

The effect sizes of the TLE and FLE groups were comparable overall FER (Q = 2.398, df = 1, p = 0.121).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analysis to investigate the patterns of ToM and FER function in children and adolescents with epilepsy. It included 19 studies, with a combined total sample size of 623 children and adolescents with epilepsy and 677 HCs. The results showed that, relative to HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited large impairments in overall ToM, cognitive ToM, overall FER (g = −1.08, g = −1.04, and g = −0.98, respectively), and medium impairment in affective ToM (g = −0.73). The subgroup analyses found no statistically significant differences in the degree of ToM or FER impairments between children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy. Besides, the degree of FER impairment was not statistically different between children and adolescents with TLE and FLE. Furthermore, in individual ToM tasks, compared to the HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited impairments in SST, FPT, and FBT, but no significant differences were observed for RMET or ToM subscale from NEPSY-II. Besides, in control measures of ToM tasks, relative to the HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited impairments in control measures of SST and FPT, but no significant differences were observed for control measures of FBT. Additionally, for studies that included only samples with an IQ score >70, relative to HCs, children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited large impairments in overall ToM and cognitive ToM, and medium impairment in affective ToM. Regarding individual emotions, children and adolescents with epilepsy were associated with the medium impairments in sad, disgust, and neutral recognition, and small impairments in anger and fear recognition. However, no group differences were evident for happy or surprise recognition. The meta-regression analyses indicated that the variables (sex, education level, age at testing, age at epilepsy onset, duration of epilepsy, full-scale IQ, verbal IQ, monthly seizure frequency, and number of AEDs) did not affect the magnitude of the effect sizes observed.

Our meta-analysis showed significant overall ToM dysfunction in children and adolescents with epilepsy (g = −1.08, k = 13). The results support the findings of Eicher and Jokeit (33) (g = −0.87, k = 26), who conducted a meta-analysis to report differences in the overall ToM between patients with epilepsy (including different epilepsy phenotypes and different age groups) and the HCs. However, it was different from the findings of Stewart et al. (31) and Wang et al. (34) (g = −0.68, k = 45 and g = −0.73, k = 12, respectively), which indicate a moderate-sized impairment. In terms of the subcomponents of the ToM, children and adolescents with epilepsy had a large impairment in cognitive ToM and a medium impairment in affective ToM. This finding confirms the previous consensus that the cognitive and affective parts of ToM are partly separate skills, relying on common and distinct neural networks (7477). Specifically, they involve a common network of brain regions, including the superior temporal sulcus, bilateral temporal poles, medial prefrontal cortex, and temporo-parietal junction (78, 79). In addition to activation of a common network, cognitive ToM and affective ToM are associated with greater activation in the dorsolateral (80) and ventromedial (74, 81), prefrontal cortices, respectively. Coincidentally, these areas, especially the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, appear to be more susceptible to impairment in children and adolescents with epilepsy (8285).

Similar to ToM, our meta-analysis showed that children and adolescents with epilepsy had significant FER dysfunction (g = −0.98, k = 12). The result was consistent with the findings of Edwards et al. (32), who conducted a meta-analysis of 24 studies and reported a difference in the overall FER (g = −0.99) between patients with epilepsy and the HCs. In reference to the individual emotions, children and adolescents with epilepsy had small effect sizes for the recognition of fear and anger, and medium impairment in sad, disgust, and neutral, while no differences were observed for the recognition of happy or surprise. These results support the conclusions of previous qualitative studies indicating that children and adolescents with epilepsy have marked deficits in recognizing negative emotional states, but have little difficulty recognizing positive emotional states (18, 21, 71, 86, 87). Among negative emotional states, recognition defect of fear emotion is the most common in epilepsy, which may be related to structural and functional abnormalities in the insular cortex, dorsal striatum, and amygdala (20, 21, 8891). It has been reported that found that deficits in recognizing fear may be related to psychosocial adjustment difficulties in children with epilepsy (18). Similar relationships have been observed in previous psychopathological studies for children and adolescents: impaired fear recognition correlated with externalizing and internalizing problems (9296). In addition, the impairment of recognition of anger, sad, disgust, and neutral in children and adolescents with epilepsy may be associated with the abnormalities in the structure and function of the orbital frontal cortex, anterior insula, and somatosensory cortices (97100). The relatively intact recognition of happy is not unique to children and adolescents with epilepsy, but is common in a variety of neurodegenerative diseases (13, 101103). This may be since happy, the first emotion humans recognize, is easier to be identified than other emotions (71, 104, 105). Furthermore, the relatively intact recognition of surprise may be because surprise, as one positive emotion, is a common and basic facial expression (100). This differential recognition of individual emotions is consistent with previous findings that different types of neural dysfunction may present different emotional recognition impairments (13, 101, 106).

In the subgroup meta-analyses, no statistically significant differences were found in the degree of ToM or FER impairment between children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy or the degree of FER impairment between children and adolescents with TLE and FLE. These results of this study support previous reports that children and adolescents with focal and generalized epilepsy may experience roughly equivalent social cognitive impairments (107). This is consistent with the understanding of epilepsy as a network disease revised by the International League Against Epilepsy (37, 71, 108). Specifically, seizures arise from a shared neural network involving overlapping cortical and subcortical structures, regardless of whether seizures originate from an identified pathological site (37, 108, 109). In addition, our study showed no difference in affective ToM between children and adolescents with focal epilepsy or generalized epilepsy and HCs. However, it should be noted the fact that different focal seizures were grouped together in this analysis. Considering that focal epilepsy contains multiple epilepsy phenotypes, and that the pathophysiology of different epilepsy phenotypes is different (110), we should interpret the conclusions of focal epilepsy vs. generalized epilepsy with caution. In addition, although we performed an analysis of TLE vs. FLE commonly seen in focal epilepsy, however, due to the limited number of studies included (k = 3 and k = 2 in this study), one must conclude cautiously.

From a therapeutic perspective, interventions targeting social cognition may be an effective approach to address social difficulties in children and adolescents with epilepsy (107). Currently, only one research protocol has been reported outlining a novel cognitive-behavioral intervention using ToM training specifically for children with epilepsy (111). In addition, in children and adolescents with other neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorders, and typically developing children with social handicaps, social function and social cognition were significantly improved through social cognitive interventions (112117). These findings suggest that social cognitive therapy holds promise for children and adolescents with epilepsy. These quantitative results further deepen our understanding of the two core domains of social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy and may assist in the development of cognitive interventions for this patient population.

Limitations

Our findings have to take into account certain limitations. First, although 19 studies were included in this meta-analysis, none investigated ToM performance in children and adolescents with FLE, and only 1 study specifically investigated ToM performance in children and adolescents with TLE. In addition, there are few studies available for some individual ToM tasks or individual emotions, such as NEPSY-II, FBT, and recognition of surprise (k = 2, respectively). Therefore, more research is needed in the future to solidify conclusions. Second, the current meta-analysis is a cross-sectional study. Longitudinal studies are necessary to further evaluate the dynamic changes of social cognition function in children and adolescents with epilepsy. Third, although we investigated the potential effects of some factors (sex, age at testing, age at epilepsy onset, duration of epilepsy, full-scale IQ, verbal IQ, education level, monthly seizure frequency, and number of AEDs) on social cognition function, we should interpret the results with caution due to the lack of literature, such as the association of education level or number of AEDs on ToM performance (k = 3, respectively). In addition, due to the insufficient data included, other factors (such as functional impairment, depression, and neuropsychiatric symptoms) were not examined. Therefore, more research is needed in these aspects in the future. Fourth, social cognition contains different domains, of which the most researched are ToM, FER, and empathy. Our study only examined ToM and FER patterns in children and adolescents with epilepsy. It is necessary to assess empathy performance in children and adolescents with epilepsy in the future.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this is the first comprehensive meta-analysis examining the patterns of ToM and FER in children and adolescents with epilepsy. This quantitative result showed that children and adolescents with epilepsy exhibited deficits in two key domains of social cognition (ToM and FER) and ToM subcomponents (cognitive and affective ToM). In addition, children and adolescents with focal epilepsy and generalized epilepsy had no statistically significant differences in social cognition deficits. Future studies investigating the neural correlates of social cognition deficits in children and adolescents with epilepsy and longitudinal studies are needed, which may further reveal the nature and course of social cognition impairment in children and adolescents with epilepsy.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Author contributions

ZQY and LLX designed this study. YS, JZ, PWZ, HZ, and JGZ analyzed and interpreted relevant data. YS and ZQY drafted the manuscript. ZQY and LLX revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by Jiangsu Commission of Health (LGY20180390).

Acknowledgments

We thank all the authors of the studies included.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.983565/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for individual ToM tasks differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and healthy controls.

Supplementary Figure 2. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for control measures of ToM tasks differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and healthy controls.

Supplementary Figure 3. Forest plots showing effect size estimates for individual emotions differences between children and adolescents with epilepsy and healthy controls.

Supplementary Table 1. Mean effects for ToM and FER subcomponents comparing TLE and FLE against healthy controls and tests for publication bias.

References

1. Adamczyk B, Wegrzyn K, Wilczyński T, Maciarz J, Morawiec N, Adamczyk-Sowa M. The most common lesions detected by neuroimaging as causes of epilepsy. Medicina (Kaunas). (2021) 57:3. doi: 10.3390/medicina57030294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Aaberg KM, Gunnes N, Bakken IJ, Lund Søraas C, Berntsen A, Magnus P, et al. Incidence and prevalence of childhood epilepsy: a nationwide cohort study. Pediatrics. (2017) 139:5. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-3908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Winsor AA, Richards C, Bissell S, Seri S, Liew A, Bagshaw AP. Sleep disruption in children and adolescents with epilepsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev. (2021) 57:101416. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Neligan A, Hauser WA, Sander JW. The epidemiology of the epilepsies. Handb Clin Neurol. (2012) 107:113–33. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-52898-8.00006-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Zhang T, Chen L, Wang Y, Zhang M, Wang L, Xu X, et al. Impaired theory of mind in Chinese children and adolescents with idiopathic generalized epilepsy: association with behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction. Epilepsy Behav. (2018) 79:205–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2017.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Gonzalez L, Gill D, Webster R, Lawson J, et al. Theory of mind and social competence in children and adolescents with temporal lobe epilepsy. Neuropsychology. (2019) 33:986–95. doi: 10.1037/neu0000543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Besag FMC, Vasey MJ. Neurocognitive effects of antiseizure medications in children and adolescents with epilepsy. Paediatr Drugs. (2021) 23:253–86. doi: 10.1007/s40272-021-00448-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Beauchamp MH, Anderson V. SOCIAL: an integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychol Bull. (2010) 136:39–64. doi: 10.1037/a0017768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rantanen K, Eriksson K, Nieminen P. Social competence in children with epilepsy–a review. Epilepsy Behav. (2012) 24:295–303. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2012.04.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Lin X, Zhang X, Liu Q, Zhao P, Zhang H, Wang H, et al. Theory of mind in adults with traumatic brain injury: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 121:106–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:1211–20. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bora E, Walterfang M, Velakoulis D. Theory of mind in Parkinson's disease: a meta-analysis. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 292:515–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Lin X, Zhang X, Liu Q, Zhao P, Zhong J, Pan P, et al. Social cognition in multiple sclerosis and its subtypes: a meta-analysis. Mult Scler Relat Disord. (2021) 52:102973. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2021.102973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Frith C, Frith U. Theory of mind. Curr Biol. (2005) 15:R644–646. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions From Facial Clues. London: Prentice-Hall (1975).

Google Scholar

16. Callahan BL, Ueda K, Sakata D, Plamondon A, Murai T. Liberal bias mediates emotion recognition deficits in frontal traumatic brain injury. Brain Cogn. (2011) 77:412–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Gonzalez L, Gill D, Webster R, Lawson J, et al. Facial emotion perception and social competence in children (8 to 16 years old) with genetic generalized epilepsy and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2019) 100:106301. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.04.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Golouboff N, Fiori N, Delalande O, Fohlen M, Dellatolas G, Jambaqué I. Impaired facial expression recognition in children with temporal lobe epilepsy: impact of early seizure onset on fear recognition. Neuropsychologia. (2008) 46:1415–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Raud T, Kaldoja ML, Kolk A. Relationship between social competence and neurocognitive performance in children with epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior. (2015) 52:93–101. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pastorino GMG, Operto FF, Padovano C, Vivenzio V, Scuoppo C, Pastorino N, et al. Social cognition in neurodevelopmental disorders and epilepsy. Front Neurol. (2021) 12:68523. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.658823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wu L, Yang X, Zhang K, Wang X, Yang B. Impairment of eye emotion discrimination in benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes: a neuropsychological study. Brain Behav. (2021) 11:e02154. doi: 10.1002/brb3.2154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Gill D, Webster R, Lawson J, Mandalis A, et al. Theory of Mind and social competence in children and adolescents with genetic generalised epilepsy (GGE): relationships to epilepsy severity and anti-epileptic drugs. Seizure. (2018) 60:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2018.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Braams OB, Meekes J, van Nieuwenhuizen O, Schappin R, van Rijen PC, Blijd-Hoogewys EMA, et al. Epilepsy surgery in children: no further threat to theory of mind. Epileptic Disord. (2019) 21:166–76. doi: 10.1684/epd.2019.1053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Lima EM, Rzezak P, Montenegro MA, Guerreiro MM, Valente KDR. Social cognition in childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Seizure. (2020) 78:102–8. doi: 10.1016/j.seizure.2020.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Genizi J, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Shahar E, Yaniv S, Aharon-Perez J. Impaired social behavior in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. J Child Neurol. (2012) 27:156–61. doi: 10.1177/0883073811414420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zilli T, Zanini S, Conte S, Borgatti R, Urgesi C. Neuropsychological assessment of children with epilepsy and average intelligence using NEPSY II. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2015) 37:1036–51. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2015.1076380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Morningstar M, Hung A, Grannis C, French RC, Mattson WI, Ostendorf AP, et al. Blunted neural response to emotional faces in the fusiform and superior temporal gyrus may be marker of emotion recognition deficits in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2020) 112:107432. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Braams O, Meekes J, van Nieuwenhuizen O, Schappin R, van Rijen PC, Veenstra W, et al. Two years after epilepsy surgery in children: recognition of emotions expressed by faces. Epilepsy Behav. (2015) 51:140–5. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Esteso Orduña B, Fournier del Castillo MDLC, Cámara Barrio S, García Fernández M, Andrés Esteban EM, Álvarez-Linera Prado J, et al. Cognitive and behavioral profiles of pediatric surgical candidates with frontal and temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2021) 21:117. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2021.107808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Bora E, Meletti S. Social cognition in temporal lobe epilepsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Epilepsy Behav. (2016) 60:50–7. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Lah S. Theory of mind in patients with epilepsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. (2016) 26:3–24. doi: 10.1007/s11065-015-9313-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Edwards M, Stewart E, Palermo R, Lah S. Facial emotion perception in patients with epilepsy: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:212–25. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Eicher M, Jokeit H. Toward social neuropsychology of epilepsy: a meta-analysis on social cognition in epilepsy phenotypes and a critical narrative review on assessment methods. Acta Epileptologica. (2022) 4:1–17. doi: 10.1186/s42494-022-00093-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Wang H, Zhao P, Zhao J, Zhong J, Pan P, Wang G, et al. Theory of mind and empathy in adults with epilepsy: a meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. (2022) 13:877957. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.877957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ziaei M, Arnold C, Thompson K, Reutens DC. Social cognition in temporal and frontal lobe epilepsy: systematic review, meta-analysis, and clinical recommendations. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2022) 22:1–25. doi: 10.1017/S1355617722000066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Henry JD, von Hippel W, Molenberghs P, Lee T, Sachdev PS. Clinical assessment of social cognitive function in neurological disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. (2016) 12:28–39. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2015.229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Berg AT, Berkovic SF, Brodie MJ, Buchhalter J, Cross JH, van Emde Boas W, et al. Revised terminology and concepts for organization of seizures and epilepsies: report of the ILAE Commission on Classification and Terminology, 2005-2009. Epilepsia. (2010) 51:676–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02522.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Arrotta K, Reyes A, Kaestner E, McDonald CR, Hermann BP, Barr WB, et al. Cognitive phenotypes in frontal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2022) 63:1671–81. doi: 10.1111/epi.17260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Johnson GW, Cai LY, Narasimhan S, González HFJ, Wills KE, Morgan VL, et al. Temporal lobe epilepsy lateralisation and surgical outcome prediction using diffusion imaging. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2022) 93:599–608. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2021-328185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Bmj. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Leppanen J, Sedgewick F, Treasure J, Tchanturia K. Differences in the Theory of Mind profiles of patients with anorexia nervosa and individuals on the autism spectrum: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2018) 90:146–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Stang A. Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol. (2010) 25:603–5. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bailey K, Im-Bolter N. Theory of mind and language in childhood epilepsy. Mind Brain Educ. (2020) 14:146–54. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ciumas C, Laurent A, Saignavongs M, Ilski F, de Bellescize J, Panagiotakaki E, et al. Behavioral and fMRI responses to fearful faces are altered in benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes (BCECTS). Epilepsia. (2017) 58:1716–27. doi: 10.1111/epi.13858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Lew AR, Lewis C, Lunn J, Tomlin P, Basu H, Roach J, et al. Social cognition in children with epilepsy in mainstream education. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2015) 57:53–9. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Lunn J, Lewis C, Sherlock C. Impaired performance on advanced Theory of Mind tasks in children with epilepsy is related to poor communication and increased attention problems. Epilepsy Behav. (2015) 43:109–16. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kalbe E, Grabenhorst F, Brand M, Kessler J, Hilker R, Markowitsch HJ. Elevated emotional reactivity in affective but not cognitive components of theory of mind: a psychophysiological study. Bmj. (2007) 1:27−38. doi: 10.1348/174866407X180792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ayribas B, Ayhan G, Topcuoglu V, Kose S, Sayar K. Theory of Mind Abilities and Insight Dimension in Patients with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Schizophrenia. Psychiatr Clinic Psychopharmacology. (2020) 30:434. doi: 10.5455/PCP.20201221104133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Hillmann K, Neukel C, Krauch M, Spohn A, Schnell K, Herpertz SC, et al. Cognitive and affective theory of mind in female patients with borderline personality disorder. J Pers Disord. (2021) 35:672–90. doi: 10.1521/pedi.2021.35.5.672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Hedges LV. Distribution theory for glass's estimator of effect size and related estimators. J Educat Statistic. (1981) 6:107–28. doi: 10.3102/10769986006002107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Masi A, Quintana DS, Glozier N, Lloyd AR, Hickie IB, Guastella AJ. Cytokine aberrations in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatr. (2015) 20:440–6. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cohen J, Cohen J, Cohen JW, Cohen J, Cohen J, Cohen J, et al. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science. Technometrics. (1988) 31:499–500. doi: 10.1063/1.866910

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Scammacca N, Roberts G, Stuebing KK. Meta-analysis with complex research designs: dealing with dependence from multiple measures and multiple group comparisons. Rev Educ Res. (2014) 84:328–64. doi: 10.3102/0034654313500826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. (2002) 21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. Bmj. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Duval S, Tweedie R. Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics. (2000) 56:455–63. doi: 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Hughes JR. Benign epilepsy of childhood with centrotemporal spikes (BECTS): to treat or not to treat, that is the question. Epilepsy Behav. (2010) 19:197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tristano I, Nicita F, Garone G, Ursitti F, Nardone C, Rocchi V, et al. Could Rolandic spikes be a prognostic factor of the neurocognitive outcome of children with BECTS? Epilepsy Behav. (2018) 86:157–62. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Li Z, Zhang J, Wang F, Yang Y, Hu J, Li Q, et al. Surface-based morphometry study of the brain in benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Ann Transl Med. (2020) 8:1150. doi: 10.21037/atm-20-5845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins J, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis || when does it make sense to perform a meta-analysis? Bmj. (2009) 20:357−64. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Meletti S, Benuzzi F, Nichelli P, Tassinari CA. Damage to the right hippocampal-amygdala formation during early infancy and recognition of fearful faces: neuropsychological and fMRI evidence in subjects with temporal lobe epilepsy. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2003) 1000:385–8. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Laurent A, Arzimanoglou A, Panagiotakaki E, Sfaello I, Kahane P, Ryvlin P, et al. Visual and auditory socio-cognitive perception in unilateral temporal lobe epilepsy in children and adolescents: a prospective controlled study. Epileptic Disorders. (2014) 16:456–70. doi: 10.1684/epd.2014.0716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kuchukhidze G, Höfler J, Kronbichler M, Schmid E, Kirschner M, Rainer L, et al. Emotion recognition and social cognition in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy. Zeitschrift fur Epileptologie. (2019) 32:177–82. doi: 10.1007/s10309-019-0261-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Lunn J, Lewis C, Gannon E. Parent-child mentalizing in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2019) 96:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.03.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Aburahma SK, Hammouri H, Hazaimeh E, Jbarah O, Nassar A, Almasri A, et al. Social impairment in children with epilepsy assessed by the social responsiveness scale. Clin Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 21:13591045211033176. doi: 10.1177/13591045211033176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Morningstar M, French RC, Mattson WI, Englot DJ, Nelson EE. Social brain networks: Resting-state and task-based connectivity in youth with and without epilepsy. Neuropsychologia. (2021) 21:157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.107882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Pinabiaux C, Bulteau C, Fohlen M, Dorfmüller G, Chiron C, Hertz-Pannier L, et al. Impaired emotional memory recognition after early temporal lobe epilepsy surgery: the fearful face exception? Cortex. (2013) 49:1386–93. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Jiang Y, Hu Y, Wang Y, Zhou N, Zhu L, Wang K. Empathy and emotion recognition in patients with idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2014) 37:139–44. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2014.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hu Y, Jiang Y, Hu P, Ma H, Wang K. Impaired social cognition in patients with interictal epileptiform discharges in the frontal lobe. Epilepsy Behav. (2016) 57:46–54. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Operto FF, Pastorino GMG, Mazza R, Di Bonaventura C, Marotta R, Pastorino N, et al. Social cognition and executive functions in children and adolescents with focal epilepsy. Euro J Paediatric Neurol. (2020) 28:167–75. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2020.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Operto FF, Pastorino GMG, Padovano C, Scuoppo C, Vivenzio V, Coppola G. Social cognition in neuropsychiatric disorders in pediatric age. Brain-Broad Res Artific Intell Neurosci. (2020) 11:81–8. doi: 10.18662/brain/11.3Sup1/124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Breuillard D, Leunen D, Chemaly N, Auclair L, Pinard JM, Kaminska A, et al. Autism spectrum disorder phenotype and intellectual disability in females with epilepsy and PCDH-19 mutations. Epilepsy Behav. (2016) 60:75–80. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2016.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J. Dissociable prefrontal networks for cognitive and affective theory of mind: a lesion study. Neuropsychologia. (2007) 45:3054–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Abu-Akel A, Shamay-Tsoory S. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of theory of mind. Neuropsychologia. (2011) 49:2971–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sebastian CL, Fontaine NM, Bird G, Blakemore SJ, Brito SA, McCrory EJ, et al. Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective Theory of Mind in adolescents and adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2012) 7:53–63. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Molenberghs P, Johnson H, Henry JD, Mattingley JB. Understanding the minds of others: a neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 65:276–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'. Trends Cogn Sci. (2003) 7:77–83. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Amodio DM, Frith CD. Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2006) 7:268–77. doi: 10.1038/nrn1884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Kalbe E, Schlegel M, Sack AT, Nowak DA, Dafotakis M, Bangard C, et al. Dissociating cognitive from affective theory of mind: a TMS study. Cortex. (2010) 46:769–80. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Tomer R, Berger BD, Goldsher D, Aharon-Peretz J. Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage. Cogn Behav Neurol. (2005) 18:55–67. doi: 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152228.90129.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Fahoum F, Zelmann R, Tyvaert L, Dubeau F, Gotman J. Epileptic discharges affect the default mode network–FMRI and intracerebral EEG evidence. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e68038. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Tan Q, Sun H, Wang W, Wu X, Hao N, Su X, et al. Quantitative MR spectroscopy reveals metabolic changes in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Eur Radiol. (2018) 28:4496–503. doi: 10.1007/s00330-018-5443-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Kalamangalam GP, Chelaru MI. Functional connectivity in dorsolateral frontal cortex: intracranial electroencephalogram study. Brain Connect. (2021) 11:850–64. doi: 10.1089/brain.2020.0816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Wang W, Wu X, Su X, Sun H, Tan Q, Zhang S, et al. Metabolic alterations of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in sleep-related hypermotor epilepsy: A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. J Neurosci Res. (2021) 99:2657–68. doi: 10.1002/jnr.24866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Monti G, Meletti S. Emotion recognition in temporal lobe epilepsy: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 55:280–93. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Bujarski KA, Flashman L, Li Z, Tosteson TD, Jobst BC, Thadani VM, et al. Investigating social cognition in epilepsy using a naturalistic task. Epilepsia. (2016) 57:1515–20. doi: 10.1111/epi.13477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Adolphs R. Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2002) 12:169–77. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00301-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Samejima K, Ueda Y, Doya K, Kimura M. Representation of action-specific reward values in the striatum. Science. (2005) 310:1337–40. doi: 10.1126/science.1115270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Paulus MP, Stein MB. An insular view of anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:383–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Ives-Deliperi VL, Jokeit H. Impaired social cognition in epilepsy: a review of what we have learnt from neuroimaging studies. Front Neurol. (2019) 10:940. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Simonian SJ, Beidel DC, Turner SM, Berkes JL, Long JH. Recognition of facial affect by children and adolescents diagnosed with social phobia. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2001) 32:137–45. doi: 10.1023/A:1012298707253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Stevens D, Charman T, Blair RJ. Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies. J Genet Psychol. (2001) 162:201–11. doi: 10.1080/00221320109597961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. McClure EB, Pope K, Hoberman AJ, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Facial expression recognition in adolescents with mood and anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry. (2003) 160:1172–4. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Easter J, McClure EB, Monk CS, Dhanani M, Hodgdon H, Leibenluft E, et al. Emotion recognition deficits in pediatric anxiety disorders: implications for amygdala research. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2005) 15:563–70. doi: 10.1089/cap.2005.15.563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Sfärlea A, Greimel E, Platt B, Dieler AC, Schulte-Körne G. Recognition of emotional facial expressions in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and adolescents with major depression. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 262:586–94. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Adolphs R, Damasio H, Tranel D, Cooper G, Damasio AR. A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. J Neurosci. (2000) 20:2683–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-07-02683.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Adolphs R, Tranel D, Damasio H. Emotion recognition from faces and prosody following temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychology. (2001) 15:396–404. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.15.3.396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Meletti S, Benuzzi F, Cantalupo G, Rubboli G, Tassinari CA, Nichelli P. Facial emotion recognition impairment in chronic temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. (2009) 50:1547–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2008.01978.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Bonora A, Benuzzi F, Monti G, Mirandola L, Pugnaghi M, Nichelli P, et al. Recognition of emotions from faces and voices in medial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. (2011) 20:648–54. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.01.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Gray HM, Tickle-Degnen L. A meta-analysis of performance on emotion recognition tasks in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychology. (2010) 24:176–91. doi: 10.1037/a0018104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Bora E, Ozerdem A. Social cognition in first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2017) 27:293–300. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Bora E, Yener GG. Meta-analysis of social cognition in mild cognitive impairment. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2017) 30:206–13. doi: 10.1177/0891988717710337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Rosenberg H, McDonald S, Dethier M, Kessels RP, Westbrook RF. Facial emotion recognition deficits following moderate-severe Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI): re-examining the valence effect and the role of emotion intensity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2014) 20:994–1003. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Garrido MV, Prada M. KDEF-PT: valence, emotional intensity, familiarity and attractiveness ratings of angry, neutral, and happy faces. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:2181. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Beatty WW, McDonald S, Longley WA, Joscelyne A, et al. Evidence for deficits in facial affect recognition and theory of mind in multiple sclerosis. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2009) 15:277–85. doi: 10.1017/S1355617709090195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Stewart E, Lah S, Smith ML. Patterns of impaired social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy: the borders between different epilepsy phenotypes. Epilepsy Behav. (2019) 100:106146. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Englot DJ, Konrad PE, Morgan VL. Regional and global connectivity disturbances in focal epilepsy, related neurocognitive sequelae, and potential mechanistic underpinnings. Epilepsia. (2016) 57:1546–57. doi: 10.1111/epi.13510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Berg AT, Millichap JJ. The 2010 revised classification of seizures and epilepsy. Continuum (Minneap Minn). (2013) 19:571–97. doi: 10.1212/01.CON.0000431377.44312.9e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Stevens CE, Stafstrom CE. Pharmacotherapy for focal seizures in children and adolescents. Drugs. (2018) 78:1321–37. doi: 10.1007/s40265-018-0959-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Stewart E, Catroppa C, Lah S. A novel cognitive behavioural intervention with Theory of Mind (ToM) training for children with epilepsy: protocol for a case series feasibility study. Pilot Feasibility Stud. (2019) 5:12. doi: 10.1186/s40814-019-0393-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Hadwin JA, Kovshoff H. A review of theory of mind interventions for children and adolescents with autism spectrum conditions. Rfo. (2013) 137:605–8. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199692972.003.0023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Hofmann SG, Doan SN, Sprung M, Wilson A, Ebesutani C, Andrews LA, et al. Training children's theory-of-mind: a meta-analysis of controlled studies. Cognition. (2016) 150:200–12. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Corbett BA, Ioannou S, Key AP, Coke C, Muscatello R, Vandekar S, et al. Treatment effects in social cognition and behavior following a theater-based intervention for youth with autism. Dev Neuropsychol. (2019) 44:481–94. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2019.1676244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Tseng A, Biagianti B, Francis SM, Conelea CA, Jacob S. Social cognitive interventions for adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. (2020) 274:199–204. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Cheung PPP, Brown T, Yu ML, Siu AMH. The effectiveness of a school-based social cognitive intervention on the social participation of chinese children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 51:1894–908. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04683-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Darling SJ, Goods M, Ryan NP, Chisholm AK, Haebich K, Payne JM. Behavioral intervention for social challenges in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. (2021) 175:e213982. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.3982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children and adolescents, epilepsy, theory of mind, facial emotion recognition, meta-analysis, cognitive, affective

Citation: Sun Y, Zhao J, Zhao PW, Zhang H, Zhong JG, Pan PL, Wang GD, Yi ZQ and Xie LL (2022) Social cognition in children and adolescents with epilepsy: A meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 13:983565. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.983565

Received: 01 July 2022; Accepted: 29 August 2022;
Published: 15 September 2022.

Edited by:

Meng-Chuan Lai, University of Toronto, Canada

Reviewed by:

Tamara Tavares, Hospital for Sick Children, Canada
Lindsay Oliver, University of Toronto, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Sun, Zhao, Zhao, Zhang, Zhong, Pan, Wang, Yi and Xie. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: LiLi Xie, Lililucky333@163.com; ZhongQuan Yi, yizhongquan@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.