- 1Alimentació, Nutrició, Desenvolupament i Salut Mental (ANUT-DSM), Institut d'Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili (IISPV), Reus, Spain
- 2Rovira i Virgili University, Departament of Biochemistry and Biotechnology, Human Nutrition Unit, Reus, Spain
- 3Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Fisiopatología de La Obesidad y La Nutrición (CIBEROBN), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
1 Introduction
Public trust in nutrition science has waned over time in several countries (1, 2). Professional bodies persevere to reverse the trend or earn trust by improving rigor in research conduct and dissemination (3–5). While several biases in research are cataloged and remedial measures suggested, “the investigator bias” arising from investigators themselves, poses a major challenge. Investigator bias rarely receives mention in academic literature. There is currently no universally accepted definition or assessment for investigator bias. In 2003, Ernst and Canter in their short letter on investigator bias and false positive findings defined it as a “bias resulting from a conflict of interest arising from passionate beliefs held by the investigators” (6). We define “investigator bias” as acts of omission or commission that arise from ignorance, hubris or excessive attachment to beliefs, based on existing discourse (7, 8). In our viewpoint, we do not focus on research misconduct such as data falsification or unethical publication practices; as their perils are well-recognized and clear-cut mechanisms exist to prevent, and redress these concerns. Without a clear framework to either identify or assess investigator bias, it is unsurprising that there is a paucity of empirical evidence on the extent of its prevalence, or its impact or more importantly to address it.
The lack of formal evaluation of investigator bias is concerning given that investigator bias encroaches into one or more stages in the research process including study conception, design, conduct, analysis, interpretation and dissemination, challenging research credibility (6). It influences what research questions are posed, how hypotheses are tested and interpreted (6, 7). Such positional biases in reviewers and funders can have consequences for the field, by determining what is rewarded and perpetuated. To tackle bias, it would be imperative to understand what drives it. Opportunities and career advancement in academia and research are increasingly competitive and are linked to the ability to attract funding and publish in indexed journals with high impact. Funders and institutions are predominantly defined by “normal” science characterized by its stability, driven by set precedents, and with a tendency to lean toward the wisdom of accepted figures in the field (9). Therefore, questioning current practices and paradigms may be problematic and unproductive for researchers. Additionally, promotional language in grant applications is known to increase their chance of being funded and the citation impact of resulting publications from these grants (10).
Therefore, it is possible that grant applications or publications apart from reflecting the investigators' beliefs and hypotheses, may also be catering to what funders, journals or reviewers are known to value. It is important to note that it is impossible to personally know all authors to sufficiently discriminate between playing to the gallery and reflecting inherent biases in grants and publications. Thus, we are bound to interpret what is written and reported as a proxy for an author's personal biases. When biased grant applications or publications are successful, the success in itself reinforces current behavior in investigators, creating a feedback loop. Thus, current systems and practices lead to the perpetuation of the practice of investigator bias and its consequences.
Without a global understanding of investigator bias, it is impossible to confidently describe or quantify its presence and impact in nutrition. However, the discussion on dissonance between the cultural basis of food and the Eurocentric values of nutrition science (the formal study of food) over a decade ago (11), makes one ponder if investigator bias can affect nutrition in more profound ways than it does to other areas of science. As if in agreement, in 2018, Ionnadis and Trepanowski published a highly provocative opinion piece. The opening lines of the paper read: “nutrition research is among the most contentious fields of science” and the authors argued that non-financial conflicts of interest in nutrition were beyond “allegiance bias and preference for favorite theories” that exist in any field of study (12). The authors proposed that nutrition scientists faced additional challenges that arose from making personal dietary choices that were shaped by familial, cultural, or religious norms. The controversial perspective evoked passionate response from nutrition scientists. A counterpoint totally dismissed these notions (13). The Editorial board of the European Journal of Nutrition responded with an editorial that highlighted the “need for self-reflection” in scientists funded by the industry and those “seeking to prove their favored hypothesis” (14). Thus there exists some consensus that investigator bias in nutrition can misinform dietary guidelines, create disagreements among experts in the public domain, and further erode trust in nutrition (15) (Figure 1). Thus, it is both urgent and important to tackle this bias from the public health perspective. A potential first step to negate investigator bias and to effect change is to create awareness among all stakeholders. As investigator bias rarely features in current scholarly discussions in nutrition, we aim to create awareness, by providing a few field-specific examples of investigator bias and identify or propose some measures to address it (Table 1). The aim of this perspective piece is not to be an exhaustive reference of examples of investigator bias and recommended remedial measures. Through this piece, we aim to begin a dialogue with stakeholders of nutrition research (which includes funders, ethics committees, academics, scientists, trainees, practitioners, and consumers), to create awareness, and galvanize efforts to appropriately tackle the bias.

Figure 1. Consequences of investigator bias on public trust in nutrition. The figure shows the impact of personal factors influencing investigator bias, potential expression of investigator-bias at various stages in the research process and its consequence on public trust in nutrition.

Table 1. Examples of investigator bias and potential preventive measures at each stage of the research process.
2 Influence of investigator bias on nutrition research
2.1 Asking questions no one wants or needs answers for
Investigator bias can result in pursuing questions solely of interest to researchers, without optimal considerations of study impact or value addition (16). These pursuits can arise from a deliberate antagonistic positioning to established evidence in public health driven by “science-related populism” (17). This phenomenon increasingly seen in the West (17), has also been noted in nutrition and often adds to confusion and stalls decision-making and policy implementation (18, 19). At other times, biased pursuits include retesting globally well-proven hypotheses. While replication is an important aspect of rigor and validity in science, reproduction of an established concept without clear justification for the same leads to redundancy and resource wastage. For instance, while evidence synthesis is key to policymaking, performing yet another systematic review or meta-analysis without differentiation, just adds to clutter (20). Other trials, even when novel, are unlikely to change practice. For example, a trial comparing the effects of moderate alcohol consumption vs. abstinence, irrespective of its findings, is unlikely to change recommendations given the safety concerns.
2.2 Adopting inappropriate methods
Investigator bias can also affect the framing of a research question as there may be a tendency to “try to prove” a certain hypothesis. This may include conscious or unconscious manipulation of one or more of the Population, Intervention, Comparator, Outcome, and Time (PICOT) variables. For example, caution against the use of surrogate markers as endpoints has often been expressed (21). This concern especially bears weight in nutrition studies where the effects of diet tend to be chronic occurring over time; while feasibility, practicality, and funding make us settle for typically shorter study periods. The problem is compounded by investigator bias, when studies have outcomes (endpoints) that satisfy the researcher's curiosity and belief without sufficient consideration of its relevance or appropriateness. For instance, one's viewpoint on the benefit of high linoleic acid intervention or lack of it in comparison to saturated fat can be reaffirmed through the choice of using total low-density lipoprotein- (LDL)—cholesterol or oxidized LDL as the outcome of interest. While linoleic acid lowers vs. total LDL-Cholesterol, it increases oxidized LDL (22). Therefore, when viewed individually each of these outcomes may provide contradicting results.
Also, when investigators strongly believe or distrust an intervention, the treatment effect in clinical trials may be (un)consciously altered through manipulating the control. This especially happens when no “inert” placebo can be used (as in the majority of nutritional trials). For example, a randomized controlled trial to assess the effect of nuts on LDL-cholesterol could have any of these controls: (a) olive oil matched for added fat, (b) a fruit serving matched for added calories, and (c) a refined carbohydrate snack matching added calories and fat. However, each of these controls varies in their health effects and will yield different effect sizes. A refined carbohydrate snack while seemingly a good model to study the effect of nuts replacing a snack, is more likely to show nuts in a better light. Contrarily, given their potential health benefits, olive oil and fruits as control may decrease the difference in outcomes between study arms. On a similar note, it is often noted that behavioral intervention trials to improve health outcomes through effecting dietary changes often include controls reflecting a dismal standard of care, e.g., providing educational pamphlets vs. reflecting an ideal standard of care (23).
Another form of effect size manipulation occurs when researchers want to test a new behavioral intervention against the standard practice but create a difference in the intensity of delivery between arms. Frequent follow-up or even logging, increases adherence to behavioral interventions. Therefore, a more frequently or intensively followed-up intervention group performs better irrespective of intervention content. This was in part why the findings from a much-awaited personalized nutrition trial were disappointing to readers as the intervention bundling precluded singling out the effect of individualization from intensive intervention (24).
Additionally, investigators may apply solutions generated in a specific population to others without contextualization. Total diet replacements facilitating diabetes remission in the United Kingdom, for instance, may be impractical for India or Spain for different reasons. Again, building prediction models, which is expected to be increasingly used in nutrition (25), should desist from using available input variables in a dataset, without considering the feasibility of using these variables in the intended context. Such models are highly likely to fail to meet its intended purpose.
2.3 Manipulating analytical techniques
Observational studies are often criticized for reporting exaggerated associations between dietary risks and health outcomes (8), and frequently not corroborated results coming from randomized controlled trials (26, 27). The observed differences in effect estimates were found to be driven by heterogeneities in population, definitions of intervention, exposure, comparator, or outcomes (28, 29). Additionally, cohort studies were noted to be more prone to residual confounding, and the variables included in the statistical models had significant consequences for the direction and magnitude of associations estimated (29). Thus, it is likely that bias in nutrition research can arise from inappropriate modeling or biased covariate choices that inadequately control for confounding, or result in over-adjustment or failure to recognize unrecoverable selection bias. In this context, unspecified exploratory analyses are particularly vulnerable to investigator bias-related p-hacking, due to the lack of prior evidence for biological plausibility (30).
2.4 Inappropriate interpretation
While manipulation or subversion of negative results favoring industry is well established similar bias that occurs in the absence of industry collaborations are ignored (16, 31, 32). Obsession with pre-conceived ideas leads to exaggerated reporting of weak, statistically significant results without clear clinical relevance (33), creating unnecessary distraction. White hat bias, another investigator-linked bias displayed on strongly felt topics, arises from perceived action for righteous ends and leads to the distortion of research findings (34). White-hat bias has been previously identified in nutrition topics such as obesity, non-nutritive sweeteners, and breastfeeding (34, 35). More recently, white-hat bias has come to display as strong polarized views on ultra-processed food, some of which could also be driven by the populist media attention. Furthermore, over interpretation of results from observational studies assuming a cause-effect relationship is also frequently reported in literature (28, 29) and may arise from investigator bias.
2.5 Inappropriate evaluation and feedback
While there is some acceptance that positional biases in investigators, both implicit and explicit, can affect research evidence creation and reporting in several ways, its role in shaping the evidence-base for nutrition through evaluation and peer-review activities is even less understood or discussed. As a relatively recent science that emerged from medical sciences, nutrition continues to be bound by tendencies for reductionism and pharmaceutical modeling logics (11). Lack of sufficient cross-cultural engagement and poor understanding of the complex, holistic, cultural and social underpinnings of nutrition in evaluators, and reviewers is likely to lead to irrelevant feedback and inappropriate evaluation. As investigators also service the profession through peer-review and editorial activities, it is important to acknowledge that personal biases arising from disciplinary, cultural and/or linguistic basis have the potential to skew the existing literature. In the absence of diversity among reviewers and editors, publication bias could arise from the compulsion to conform to mainstream ideas and practices.
3 Conclusion: final thoughts and the way forward
Since we are but human, we are all touched by ignorance, hubris and beliefs, and therefore are not immune to investigator bias. Established expertise by default does not invite or encourage criticism. Therefore, the current focus of the research ecosystem on financial interests to address conflict of interest, with no clear mandate on managing non-financial interests (36), is inadequate to deal with the bias that can arise from investigators themselves and support good science. Moreover, reporting non-financial conflicts predominantly occurs after study completion, and during publication. To prevent the colossal waste of resources, attempts to identify investigator bias are needed earlier and throughout the research process.
Despite acknowledgment of investigator bias in nutrition, the existing nihilism in certain sections is defeatist and has prevented progress in addressing the bias. As a way forward, we propose the following suggestions. First, in the absence of punitive measures for the consequences of errors in nutrition research and practice, the first line of defense against investigator bias is self and peer regulation. Thus, the earlier we choose to acknowledge and sensitize ourselves to this bias that arises from within, the better prepared we are to prevent ourselves and our peers from falling prey to it. As a starting point for these discussions and to create awareness and encourage introspection and reflection within the nutrition research community, we have collated a few existing safeguards, suggested the repurposing of a few others or propose a few other aligned with steps in the research process in Table 1. Reflective practices, we believe, can result in a more thorough evaluation of the bias, and further deliberation on whether formal processes to recommend best practices to deal with the bias (such as developing assessment tools or checklists) are necessary.
Second, systematic and formalized collective efforts to define and catalog investigator bias and identify where the bias arises from are important, warranting dedicated task forces established by professional societies for the purpose. These efforts should preferably translate, as described earlier, to developing assessment tools to identify the bias, and therefore help quantify its presence and further enable studying trends over time. The task force could also deliberate and recommend appropriate ways to report positional biases (such as those arising from dietary preferences) in authors, reviewers, and editors. While mandating authors to declare conflicts of interest has received major attention in recent decades, it is also important to acknowledge and manage reviewer and editorial bias as their actions are definitive and determine how science is shaped.
Thirdly, a conscious approach to building tolerance to alternative views and improving diversity within research teams, Institutional Review Boards (IRB), editorial boards and reviewer panels should be encouraged at personal and institutional levels. Several of the issues discussed above, would have been prevented if red teaming/adversarial collaborative approaches were practiced (37, 38).
Finally, and importantly, being prescriptive or improving declaration of conflicts of interest has never been the panacea to eradicating biases. Therefore, emphasizing reflection on its occurrence and emphasizing this in training of younger generation of scientists may be the best bet yet.
Author contributions
SS: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft. JS-S: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. SS is a Miguel Servet Investigator at the Institut d'Investigació Sanitària Pere Virgili (IISPV). Her salary is co-funded by Carlos III Health Institute, Spain and the European Union (CP24/0006). JS-S is partially supported by ICREA. Funding support for the article processing fee was received from Carlos III Health Institute, Spain.
Conflict of interest
SS has received consulting fees from Abbott Laboratories Sdn. Bhd. SS is a member of the Federation of European Nutrition Working Groups on Improving Standards in the Science of Nutrition. She also leads the ACTIVATOR project a benchmarking exercise of nutrition research in Asia. JS-S has received grants and support for attending meetings/travel from Nut and Dried Fruit Foundation. He is an honorary Member of the International Advisory Board of the Project “Effect of cashew nut supplementation on glycemic status and lipid profile in type 2 diabetes subjects,” and was Member of the Scientific Committee of Danone Institute International. He has received personal fees for serving as a Member of the Institute Danone Advisory Board.
The reviewer TA declared a past co-authorship with the author JS-S to the handling editor.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Author disclaimer
The authors alone are responsible for the views expressed in this article and they do not necessarily represent the views, decisions, or policies of the institutions with which they are affiliated.
References
1. Penders B, Wolters A, Feskens EF, Brouns F, Huber M, Maeckelberghe ELM, et al. Capable and credible? Chall Nutr Sci Eur J Nutr. (2017) 56:2009. doi: 10.1007/s00394-017-1507-y
2. Trust at a tipping point: new survey reveals the need for clarity in science communication to improve public health. Int. Food Inf. Counc. (2024). Available online at: https://ific.org/media-information/press-releases/trust-at-a-tipping-point/ [accessed November 4, 2024].
3. Calder PC, Feskens EJM, Kraneveld AD, Plat J, van 't Veer P, de Vries J. Towards “improved standards in the science of nutrition” through the establishment of federation of european nutrition societies working groups. Ann Nutr Metab. (2020) 76:2–5. doi: 10.1159/000506325
4. Trust in Nutrition Science. Am. Soc. Nutr. (2016). Available online at: https://nutrition.org/trust/ [accessed July 5, 2024].
5. Garza C, Stover PJ, Ohlhorst SD, Field MS, Steinbrook R, Rowe S, et al. Best practices in nutrition science to earn and keep the public's trust. Am J Clin Nutr. (2019) 109:225–43. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy337
6. Ernst E, Canter PH. Investigator bias and false positive findings in medical research. Trends Pharmacol Sci. (2003) 24:219–21. doi: 10.1016/S0165-6147(03)00075-0
7. Dahly D. The Clueless, the hopeless, and the hubris. My 2 Cents. (2023). Available online at: https://statsepi.substack.com/p/the-clueless-the-hopeless-and-the [accessed July 5, 2024].
8. Brown AW, Ioannidis JPA, Cope MB, Bier DM, Allison DB. Unscientific beliefs about scientific topics in nutrition. Adv Nutr. (2014) 5:563–5. doi: 10.3945/an.114.006577
9. Layman CA, Rypel AL. Beyond kuhnian paradigms: normal science and theory dependence in ecology. Ecol Evol. (2023) 13:e10255. doi: 10.1002/ece3.10255
10. Peng H, Qiu HS, Fosse HB, Uzzi B. Promotional language and the adoption of innovative ideas in science. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2024) 121:e2320066121. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2320066121
11. Hassel CA. Reconsidering nutrition science: critical reflection with a cultural lens. Nutr J. (2014) 13:42. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-13-42
12. Ioannidis JPA, Trepanowski JF. Disclosures in nutrition research: why it is different. JAMA. (2018) 319:547–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.18571
13. Jones PJH, Carlberg JG. Are disclosures in nutrition research really any different? Br J Nutr. (2018) 120:481–3. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518001915
14. Soares MJ, Müller MJ, Boeing H, Maffeis C, Misra A, Muscogiuri G, et al. Conflict of interest in nutrition research: an editorial perspective. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2019) 73:1213–5. doi: 10.1038/s41430-019-0488-8
15. Williams CM. Mechanistic evidence underpinning dietary policy: bringing the jigsaw pieces together? Proc Nutr Soc. (2023) 82:219–26. doi: 10.1017/S0029665122002750
16. Brown AW, Bohan Brown MM, Allison DB. Belief beyond the evidence: using the proposed effect of breakfast on obesity to show 2 practices that distort scientific evidence1234. Am J Clin Nutr. (2013) 98:1298–308. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.064410
17. Meyer JM. Power and truth in science-related populism: rethinking the role of knowledge and expertise in climate politics. Polit Stud. (2024) 72:845–61. doi: 10.1177/00323217231160370
18. Mohanty P, Rai R, Sudhagar DP. New populism and food. In: Chacko Chennattuserry J, Deshpande M, Hong P, , editors. Encyclopedia of New Populism and Responses in the 21st Century. Singapore: Springer Nature (2023). p. 1–7.
19. Michielsen YJE, van der Horst HM. Backlash against meat curtailment policies in online discourse: populism as a missing link. Appetite. (2022) 171:105931. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2022.105931
20. Ioannidis JP. The mass production of redundant, misleading, and conflicted systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Milbank Q. (2016) 94:485. doi: 10.1111/1468-0009.12210
21. Yetley EA, DeMets DL, William R, Harlan J. Surrogate disease markers as substitutes for chronic disease outcomes in studies of diet and chronic disease relations. Am J Clin Nutr. (2017) 106:1175. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.164046
22. DiNicolantonio JJ, O'Keefe JH. Omega-6 vegetable oils as a driver of coronary heart disease: the oxidized linoleic acid hypothesis. Open Heart. (2018) 5:e000898. doi: 10.1136/openhrt-2018-000898
23. Byrd-Bredbenner C, Wu F, Spaccarotella K, Quick V, Martin-Biggers J, Zhang Y. Systematic review of control groups in nutrition education intervention research. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2017) 14:91. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0546-3
24. Rycroft I. The promises of personalised nutrition: what is the science behind ZOE? Oxf Sci. (2024). Available online at: https://oxsci.org/the-promises-of-personalised-nutrition/ [accessed July 5, 2024].
25. Predictive Modeling in Nutrition - American Society for Nutrition (2017). Available online at: https://nutrition.org/predictive-modeling-nutrition/ [accessed October 17, 2024].
26. Janiaud P, Agarwal A, Tzoulaki I, Theodoratou E, Tsilidis KK, Evangelou E, et al. Validity of observational evidence on putative risk and protective factors: appraisal of 3744 meta-analyses on 57 topics. BMC Med. (2021) 19:157. doi: 10.1186/s12916-021-02020-6
27. Maki KC, Slavin JL, Rains TM, Kris-Etherton PM. Limitations of observational evidence: implications for evidence-based dietary recommendations. Adv Nutr. (2014) 5:7–15. doi: 10.3945/an.113.004929
28. Stadelmaier J, Beyerbach J, Roux I, Harms L, Eble J, Nikolakopoulou A, et al. Evaluating agreement between evidence from randomised controlled trials and cohort studies in nutrition: a meta-research replication study. Eur J Epidemiol. (2024) 39:363–78. doi: 10.1007/s10654-023-01058-5
29. Schwingshackl L, Balduzzi S, Beyerbach J, Bröckelmann N, Werner SS, Zähringer J, et al. Evaluating agreement between bodies of evidence from randomised controlled trials and cohort studies in nutrition research: meta-epidemiological study. BMJ. (2021) 374:n1864. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n1864
30. Baldwin JR, Pingault J-B, Schoeler T, Sallis HM, Munafò MR. Protecting against researcher bias in secondary data analysis: challenges and potential solutions. Eur J Epidemiol. (2022) 37:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s10654-021-00839-0
31. Steele S, Ruskin G, Sarcevic L, McKee M, Stuckler D. Are industry-funded charities promoting “advocacy-led studies” or “evidence-based science”?: a case study of the International Life Sciences Institute. Glob Health. (2019) 15:36. doi: 10.1186/s12992-019-0478-6
32. Larrick BM, Dwyer JT, Erdman JW, D'Aloisio RF, Jones W. An updated framework for industry funding of food and nutrition research: managing financial conflicts and scientific integrity. J Nutr. (2022) 152:1812–8. doi: 10.1093/jn/nxac106
33. Pi-Sunyer X. Glycemic index in early type 2 diabetes. Am J Clin Nutr. (2008) 87:3–4. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/87.1.3
34. Cope MB, Allison DB. White hat bias: examples of its presence in obesity research and a call for renewed commitment to faithfulness in research reporting. Int J Obes. (2010) 34:84–8. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.239
35. Litman EA, Gortmaker SL, Ebbeling CB, Ludwig DS. Source of bias in sugar-sweetened beverage research: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. (2018) 21:2345–50. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018000575
36. Mela DJ. Conflicts of interest in nutrition: categorical thinking and the stigma of commercial collaboration. Curr Dev Nutr. (2024) 8:104413. doi: 10.1016/j.cdnut.2024.104413
37. Lakens D. Pandemic researchers — recruit your own best critics. Nature. (2020) 581:121–121. doi: 10.1038/d41586-020-01392-8
38. The Use of Adversarial Collaboration in Nutrition & Obesity Research - American Society for Nutrition (2023). Available online at: https://nutrition.org/nutrition-2022-live-online/adversarial-collaboration/ [accessed December 9, 2024].
39. Puljak L, Lund H. Definition, harms, and prevention of redundant systematic reviews. Syst Rev. (2023) 12:63. doi: 10.1186/s13643-023-02191-8
40. Science C for O. Registered reports (2014). Available online at: https://www.cos.io/initiatives/registered-reports [accessed November 4, 2024].
41. Liu H, Huffman MD, Trieu K. The role of contextualisation in enhancing non-communicable disease programmes and policy implementation to achieve health for all. Health Res Policy Syst. (2020) 18:38. doi: 10.1186/s12961-020-00553-5
Keywords: bias, meta-research, research methodology, ethics, nutrition
Citation: Shyam S and Salas-Salvadó J (2025) The impact of investigator bias in nutrition research. Front. Nutr. 12:1513343. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2025.1513343
Received: 18 October 2024; Accepted: 01 April 2025;
Published: 17 April 2025.
Edited by:
Colby Vorland, Indiana University, United StatesReviewed by:
Tauseef Ahmad Khan, University of Toronto, CanadaSabrina Ayoub-Charette, University of Toronto, Canada, in collaboration with reviewer TA
Dylan MacKay, University of Manitoba, Canada
Copyright © 2025 Shyam and Salas-Salvadó. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Sangeetha Shyam, c2FuZ2VldGhhLnNoeWFtQHVydi5jYXQ=; Jordi Salas-Salvadó, am9yZGkuc2FsYXNAdXJ2LmNhdA==