ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 10 April 2025

Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health

Volume 12 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnut.2025.1405796

Acute effects of a standardised extract of Hericium erinaceus (Lion’s Mane mushroom) on cognition and mood in healthy younger adults: a double-blind randomised placebo-controlled study

  • 1Beyond Alcohol Ltd., London, United Kingdom
  • 2Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom
  • 3Food and Nutrition Department, Human Sciences and Design Faculty, King Abdulaziz University, Jeddah, Saudi Arabia
  • 4Pharmacognosy and Phytotherapy, UCL School of Pharmacy, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Graduate Institute of Integrated Medicine, College of Chinese Medicine, Chinese Medicine Research Center, China Medical University, Taichung, Taiwan
  • 6Clasado Ltd, Imperium Building, Imperial Way, Worton Grange, Reading, Berkshire, United Kingdom

Introduction: Animal studies have suggested that Lion’s Mane mushroom [Hericium erinaceus (Bull.) Pers.] can enhance cognitive function and mood due to its bioactive metabolites, including erinacines and hericenones. However, despite being an ingredient used both culinarily and therapeutically in the East, and more and more commonly in the West, limited research has focused on the immediate effects of H. erinaceus on the cognitive function and mood of healthy young adults.

Methods: In an acute randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blinded, cross-over intervention study, we investigated the potential benefits of an acute dose of H. erinaceus fruiting body extract (3g of 10:1 extract) on cognitive performance and mood compared to a placebo. Eighteen healthy participants aged 18 to 35 years took part in the study. At baseline and 90 minutes post-consumption of the interventions, cognitive and mood assessments were administered to measure various cognitive abilities such as executive function, working memory, psychomotor skills, attention and information processing speed as well as positive and negative affect.

Results and discussion: The results showed no significant effect of the H. erinaceus fruiting body extract for composite measures of global cognitive function and mood. However, when analysing individual tests, participants exhibited improved performance on the pegboard test at 90 minutes following a single dose of H. erinaceus.

Conclusion: In conclusion, acute consumption of H. erinaceus fruiting body did not demonstrate a significant overall improvement in cognitive performance and mood compared to the placebo and any benefits may be task or domain specific. Further investigations should investigate the effects of chronic supplementation of H. erinaceus fruiting body on cognition and mood in healthy younger adults, as well as establish optimal dosage and the time to peak concentration of H. erinaceus bioactives in the human brain. Additionally, future research should aim to further elucidate potential mechanisms of action to explain potential brain region and cognitive domain specific effects, such as possible regional increases in cerebral blood flow following consumption of H. erinaceus fruiting bodies. It must also be noted that typically only Lion’s mane fruiting bodies are consumed culinarily, where up to 300g of fresh fruiting body are often consumed in the form of mushroom steaks.

Introduction

Hericium erinaceus (Bull.) Pers. (Hericiaceae; Engl,: Lion’s Mane mushroom), is a toothed fungus endemic to the Northern Hemisphere. This unique looking fungus has garnered significant attention in recent years for its potential cognitive and mood-enhancing properties. Traditionally used in East Asian cuisine and medicine, this distinctive fungus has seen a surge in popularity as both a culinary ingredient and dietary supplement in Western countries.

While much research has focused on the effects of H. erinaceus in populations with cognitive impairment or mood disorders, there is growing interest in its potential benefits for healthy individuals. Preclinical studies have demonstrated that bioactive compounds in lion’s mane, such as hericenones and erinacines, can promote nerve growth factor (NGF) synthesis and potentially support neuroplasticity (1). These findings suggest that consuming Lion’s mane may have cognitive benefits even in the absence of pathology.

Addressing cognitive and mood impairments is increasingly important for public health. Neurological diseases contribute significantly to physical and cognitive decline, affecting 15% of the global population (2). The prevalence of mood disorders, such as anxiety and depression, is also rising. In 2019, anxiety disorders affected 4.05% of the global population, a 55% increase from 1990, with rates rising during the COVID-19 pandemic (3, 4).

Due to the increasing prevalence of neurodegenerative and mood disorders, it is more vital than ever to discover treatments that may help to alleviate these ailments. Hericium erinaceus has shown potential in treating a wide range of Central Nervous System (CNS) disorders. H. erinaceus contains a range of different compounds with a variety of biological activities (1) including erinacines and hericenones, which enhance growth factor (NGF) release (5).

Tsai-Teng et al. (6) demonstrated that erinacine A-enriched H. erinaceus supplementation significantly attenuated amyloid β plaque deposition in transgenic mice, indicating potential therapeutic effects for Alzheimer’s disease. Additionally, H. erinaceus extracts have shown antidepressant-like effects in mice, acting through mechanisms such as hippocampal neurogenesis (7) and induction of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) pathway, which is critical for neuronal survival and neurogenesis (8). Another murine study found that H. erinaceus increased levels of serotonin, dopamine, and noradrenaline, neurotransmitters that are often decreased in depression (9).

Human studies on H. erinaceus and depression are limited. One study by Vigna et al. (10) found that 8 weeks of H. erinaceus supplementation improved anxiety and depression measures in overweight or obese participants, also increasing circulating pro-BDNF levels. H. erinaceus has also shown potential in improving cognitive functions in individuals with mild cognitive impairment. Mori et al. (11) found cognitive improvement after 16 weeks of 250 mg daily H. erinaceus supplementation in older adults. Saitsu et al. (12) demonstrated cognitive improvements in participants consuming 2.4 g of H. erinaceus daily for 12 weeks. Docherty et al. (13) found an increase in performance on the Stroop task (a cognitive test measuring response inhibition) following a single 1.8 g dose of H. erinaceus in a young, healthy population indicating acute cognitive-benefits of H. erinaceus. This study also indicated an improvement in reported stress following 28-days chronic supplementation with H. erinaceus.

Our study extends recent research by Docherty et al. (13) by focusing solely on the immediate effects of a higher, more concentrated dose derived exclusively from the fruiting body of H. erinaceus. This approach addresses the fact that within the EU and UK, mycelial extracts or preparations of lion’s mane mycelia are not allowed to be consumed within food products or nutritional supplements however fruiting body extracts are permitted (14). The distinction between fruiting body and mycelial extracts is significant due to the differing profiles of bioactive compounds. Fruiting bodies are rich in hericenones, which have been shown to enhance cognitive function and mood (1). In contrast, mycelia contain higher concentrations of erinacines, known for their neurotrophic effects.

The effects of Hericium erinaceus on cognition (15, 16) and mood have received considerable attention, with research primarily focusing on its benefits for individuals with nervous system diseases. However, the potential cognitive and mood benefits of H. erinaceus in healthy individuals are also of significant interest. Our study investigates these acute effects in a young, healthy population, which is a less commonly studied demographic. By using an extract derived specifically from the fruiting body of H. erinaceus, our research aims to provide unique insights into its efficacy for cognitive enhancement in healthy adults.

Methodology

Participants

Participants eligible for inclusion in the trial were healthy adults aged between 18 and 35 years and located in the United Kingdom. The exclusion criteria were as follows: pregnant or lactating women, those concurrently participating in any other clinical trial within 6 weeks prior to the commencement of the current trial, individuals who had made significant changes to their diet or lifestyle in the previous month, those with a Body Mass Index (BMI) of 30 or above, individuals with psychiatric or medical conditions, those with clinician diagnosed depression, individuals with existing neurological impairments or diseases, individuals with existing cardiovascular disease or complications, individuals with a previous history of diabetes, individuals taking medication with stimulatory or sedative effects, and those on regular medication for heart disease, hypertension, liver or kidney disease, high cholesterol, autoimmune disease, cancer, psychiatric disorders, or diabetes. Additionally, individuals who were currently smoking or addicted to substances, regularly consuming functional foods (commonly defined foods consumed to be beneficial to health beyond basic nutrition) or dietary supplements with cognitive-enhancing effects similar to H. erinaceus (or unwilling to stop during the study), those with food allergies or intolerances, individuals engaging in frequent vigorous exercise, those with learning difficulties (e.g., dyslexia), asthma, hypoglycemia, bleeding disorders, or those unable to provide consent for themselves were also excluded from participation in the trial.

The software package G*power (17) was used to determine sample size a priori. Based on an effect size f of 0.3 [which was expected according to the existing literature – (see 13)], an alpha of 0.05 and power of 80%, employing a cross-over design and planning to perform the associated within-subject F test for the primary endpoint, the G*Power program determined that a sample size of 17 would be acceptable. Furthermore, as noted by Docherty et al. (13), for the purposes of a pilot study, a sample size of between 12 and 30 per group is recommended (18, 19), therefore, considering potential drop-outs, the aim was to recruit 20 participants in total.

Interventions

The H. erinaceus extract (95% H. erinaceus extract and 5% maltodextrin; Cambridge Commodities Ltd.) was derived from its fruiting body through a process of water and ethanol extraction (8:2), followed by spray drying over a maltodextrin substrate. The country of origin and manufacture of the H. erinaceus extract was China. The decision to use fruiting body extracts was based on the prohibition of using mycelial extracts within the European Union within EU novel foods guidelines, as well as its established use within commercially available, over the counter supplements and food products.

It must be acknowledged that there are some significant differences between the profiles of bioactive compounds between the fruiting bodies and mycelia, which could in turn influence their physiological activity.

Extraction methods can also have a marked impact on the efficacy of an extract. This study employed both aqueous and ethanolic extraction methods, which are widely used for their ability to extract both polar and non-polar compounds. These methods maximise the yield of bioactive compounds without relying on non-potable solvents, thus balancing efficiency and consumer perceptions of safety. The resulting extract is then spray dried, allowing the concentration of bioactives and prevention of degradation by environmental conditions or microbes (20), while ensuring that they are evenly distributed across an easily storable, transportable medium that is convenient to handle and measure prior to further processing or final consumption.

The 250 mL H. erinaceus drink (active drink) was comprised of 3 g of the H. erinaceus extract mixed with 220 mL of water and 30 mL of Robinson’s Lemon Squash as a vehicle. The control drink (control drink) was matched to the active drink in terms of volume, appearance, taste, and macronutrient composition. The composition of the H. erinaceus and control drinks is provided in Tables 1, 2.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Composition of the H. erinaceus extract and control drinks.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. The nutritional content of H. erinaceus extract and control drinks.

Study design

Participants consumed both the H. erinaceus extract drink (active drink) or the control drink (control drink) in accordance with a double-blinded, placebo-controlled, acute, cross-over study design (Figure 1), with the order of consumption counterbalanced across the sample and a seven-day washout between test days. Cognitive function and mood were assessed at baseline (pre-consumption) and at 90 min post-consumption of the test beverages (see procedure). A familiarisation/practise day occurred 7 days prior to the first test day, during which no drink was consumed. Participants were then randomised into two groups to determine whether they would consume the active drink or control drink first, using computer-based non-deterministic randomisation in Microsoft Excel. Group 1 consumed the H. erinaceus extract on test day 1 and control on test day 2 whereas for group 2, the order of consumption of the test beverages was reversed.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Study design.

Procedure

Upon recruitment and after obtaining informed consent, participants were invited to attend a baseline familiarisation/practise day during which demographic information was collected, eligibility for participation in the trial was confirmed, and a practise version of the cognitive battery was administered. The purpose of familiarising the participants with the cognitive tests prior to the first test day was to reduce the disproportionate number of errors during the initial testing session, the ultimate aim of which was to minimise any practise effects. Researchers were present throughout the practise session to answer any questions participants might have about the tasks and ensure they understood the instructions.

For the 24 h prior to each visit, participants followed a restricted diet which included the prohibition of alcohol consumption. Participants also fasted for the 12 h prior to the visit. These measures were taken in order to prevent any potential acute effects of foods/beverages that could influence cognitive performance, such as caffeine (21). Extreme exercise (for example weightlifting and high intensity interval training) were also prohibited for the 24 h prior to each visit, as exercise has also been shown to alter cognitive function (22) and extreme exercise can potentially introduce confounding factors, such as fatigue which could affect test performance. All test visits took place at the clinical investigation unit (CIU) of the University of Surrey in March 2023 at 9 am. During test days, participants were given 15 min to consume a standardised breakfast of a croissant and water upon arrival. The participants then carried out a series of baseline measurements of cognition and mood which takes approximately 1 h to complete the tasks, depending on the individuals’ speed.

Following the baseline test session, the participants were asked to consume either the active drink or control drink within 15 min, depending on which intervention they had been randomly assigned to consume at that test visit. Half an hour before consumption, a confederate prepared the drinks, which were consumed with a black straw and a vessel that obscured the beverage from the subject, ensuring double-blinding. The confederate observed the subject throughout to ensure the drink was consumed in its entirety. After a delay of 90 min the same cognitive and mood assessments were repeated.

On completion of the initial test day, participants observed a washout period of one-week. Following this, they attended the clinic for a second test day during which the same procedure as for the first was repeated, but participants were given the drink they did not consume on their first test visit. Participants were given parallel versions of the cognitive tasks at the different testing timepoints to minimise learning effects.

Outcomes

The primary outcome was global cognitive function as determined via a composite measure reflecting combined performance across all of the cognitive tasks administered. The cognitive test battery included the following tasks: Trail Making Test (TMT) parts A and B (27, 48), Digit Span Test (DST) (29, 45), Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST) (28, 46), Grooved Pegboard Test (GPT) (47), Deary-Liewald Task (23), and the Flanker Task (25, 44). The TMT is commonly used to measure executive function, the DST to measure working memory, the DSST is a measure of information processing, the GPT psychomotor skills, whilst the Deary-Liewald task measures reaction time and processing speed, and finally the Flanker task is a measure of inhibitory control which is often classed as an aspect of executive function (24). The references provided for the cognitive tests in this methods section are intended to provide further details of each and explain how they are administered in practice, to aid replicability. Dependent variables were either accuracy or reaction time as appropriate for the task.

Secondary outcomes were overall mood (composite measure) as measured by the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (26), performance on each individual cognitive task and positive and negative affect separately. The PANAS is a psychometric questionnaire comprised of 20 mood-related adjectives (e.g., ‘hostile’, ‘alert’, ‘afraid’, ‘excited’) for which the participant has to indicate on a scale of 1–5 with 1 being ‘very slightly/not at all’ and 5 being ‘extremely’. Typically this questionnaire is used to determine how often they have felt that way over the past week, however it can also be used to determine acute changes to mood by asking volunteers to answer the questions based on how they currently feel.

The study’s cognitive and mood test battery included both paper-based tasks (Trail Making Test, Digit Symbol Substitution Test, and Digit Span Test) as well as computerised tasks hosted on Gorilla.sc (Deary-Liewald Task, Flanker Task, and PANAS questionnaire), benefiting from its ability to be robust to environmental variance (30). The grooved pegboard test, separate from the paper and computerised tasks, measured motor dexterity by scoring the number of pegs placed into a board within 30 s using both preferred and non-preferred hands. These tests were completed at baseline and 90 min post-intervention, with parallel versions of the cognitive tasks administered at both timepoints.

Ethical approval

This study was granted ethical approval by the University of Surrey Research Integrity & Governance Office (RIGO), FHMS 22-23 117 EGA. This study was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki, relevant University policies, ethical and professional standards, and all mandatory training was provided by the University of Surrey.

Statistical analysis

Raw scores were standardised by converting to Z scores. The grand means (X) and standard deviations (SD) across all conditions and timepoints for each of the cognitive tests were determined. Z scores were then calculated by subtracting the grand mean of the relevant cognitive test from each raw score then dividing by the SD of that test [i.e., (μ – X)/SD where μ is the raw score]. For Deary Liewald Task, Flanker test and PANAS negative outcomes, Z scores were inverted in order that for all variables, a higher score indicated better cognition or mood (the inversion was achieved by multiplying the relevant Z scores by −1). Calculating Z scores overcomes the issue of differences in the nature of dependent variables (e.g., units of measurement) across the various cognitive tests when combining data into composite measures.

A global composite measure of overall cognitive performance (primary endpoint) was calculated for every subject for each condition and timepoint. This was achieved by summing the Z scores across the different cognitive tests and dividing by the number of cognitive tests contributing to the composite measure. A composite measure of mood was calculated using the same method. It is important to calculate composite measures of cognition for comparability with other studies (31, 32).

Considering the cross-over design of the study, with measurements at baseline and post-consumption of the drinks, such that all subjects completed both drink conditions and measurements at each of the timepoints, the global composite measures of cognition and mood were analysed using a two-factor (Intervention; Time) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Intervention (active drink; control drink) and Time (Baseline; Post-drink) as within-subject factors.

Z score data for the individual cognitive and mood measures were analysed using a two-factor (Intervention; Time) repeated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA), which enables the effect of independent variables and their interaction on several dependent variables to be analysed in one analysis, allowing for adequate control over type 1 error.

For all statistical analyses, Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons were examined for Treatment related effects regardless of the significance of the overall multi-or univariate test statistic [(see 33)] and a significance level of p < .05 was applied.

IBM SPSS statistics version 29.0.0.0 was used to conduct all statistical analyses.

Results

A total of twenty adults were initially recruited, however two participants failed to attend the familiarisation/practise day, therefore eighteen participants of which 10 were male and 8 were female (see Table 3 for demographic information of the sample) were randomised and all completed the study (see Figure 2). Only main effects and interactions related to Intervention are reported as these are the most pertinent to establishing the acute effects of H. erinaceus on cognition.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Demographic information of participants.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Consort diagram showing group assignment.

There was no significant main effect of Intervention (active drink vs. control drink), or an Intervention x Time interaction for the composite measure of global cognitive function (all p > 0.05) and the composite measure of mood (all p > 0.05), see Table 4 for means and standard deviations. The higher the value, the better the cognitive performance or mood.

Table 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Z scores for the global cognitive and mood composite measures for each condition and timepoint.

However, the MANOVA of the individual cognitive and mood measures revealed a main effect of Intervention (V = 0.81, F (10.8) = 3.30, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.81) as well as an Intervention x Time interaction (V = 0.79, F (10.8) = 2.94, p = 0.07, ηp2 = 0.79) that approached significance.

In relation to the trend for a main effect of Intervention, examination of the Univariate tests suggest this could be due to effects for the Pegboard – Dominant (F (1.17) = 10.41, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.38) and Flanker (F (1.17) = 4.50, p = 0.049, ηp2 = 0.21) tasks, with pairwise comparisons showing better performance in the active drink compared with the control drink condition for the Pegboard - Dominant task (mean difference = −0.40, SE = 0.12, p = 0.01; active drink M = 0.20, SE = 0.23; control drink M = −0.20, SE = 0.19) but a trend for worse performance in the active drink condition compared to control drink for the Flanker task (mean difference = 0.38, SE = 0.18, p = 0.05; active drink M = −0.19, SE = 0.13; control drink M = 0.19, SE = 0.21).

However, perhaps of most interest and relevance is the trend toward an Intervention x Time interaction, with Univariate tests revealing this could be due to effects for the Pegboard – Dominant (F (1.17) = 15.75, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.48), Pegboard – Non-dominant (F (1.17) = 19.31, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.53), and Flanker (F (1.17) = 4.87, p = 0.04, ηp2 = 0.22) cognitive tasks and possibly the Trails B (F (1.17) = 4.42, p = 0.05, ηp2 = 0.21) task and PANAS-positive mood measure (F (1.17) = 3.59, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.17), although notably the effects for the latter two measures were only trends.

Pairwise comparisons showed significantly better performance in the active drink condition compared to control drink at the post-drink timepoint for the Pegboard – Dominant task (mean difference = −0.83, SE = 0.14, p < 0.001; active drink M = 0.49, SE =0.21; control drink M = −0.34, SE = 0.24) with no significant difference at baseline (p > 0.05). Furthermore, there was significant improvement in performance following the active drink compared to baseline (mean difference = −0.58, SE = 0.12, p < 0.001; Baseline M = −0.09, SE = 0.25; Post-drink M = 0.49, SE = 0.21), which was not observed for the control drink (Figure 3).

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Performance on the pegboard (dominant) test for both interventions (H. erinaceus and control) at each timepoint.

For the Pegboard – Non-dominant task, although performance was significantly better in the control drink compared with the active drink condition at baseline (mean difference = 0.72, SE = 0.28, p = 0.02; active drink M = −0.35, SE = 0.20; control drink M = 0.37, SE = 0.31), performance significantly increased following the active drink compared to baseline for this task (mean difference = −0.52, SE = 0.12, p < 0.001; Baseline M = −0.35, SE = 0.20; Post-drink M = 0.17, SE = 0.23), whereas there was a significant decline in performance following the control drink (mean difference = 0.55, SE = 0.23, p = 0.03; Baseline M = 0.37, SE = 0.31; Post-drink M = −0.18, SE = 0.16).

Furthermore, for the Trails A task, performance significantly decreased following the control drink compared to baseline (mean difference = 0.35, SE = 0.12, p = 0.01; Baseline M = 0.11, SE = 0.22; Post-drink M = −0.24, SE = 0.16).

Conversely, for the Flanker task, performance was significantly worse for the active drink condition compared with control drink (mean difference = 0.84, SE = 0.20, p < 0.001; active drink M = −0.37, SE = 0.16; control drink M = 0.47, SE = 0.09) at the post-drink timepoint (no significant difference at baseline, p > 0.05).

Also, for the Trails B task, a significant decline in performance was evident following the active drink compared to baseline (mean difference = 0.42, SE = 0.13, p = 0.01; Baseline M = 0.13, SE = 0.20; Post-drink M = −0.28, SE = 0.21).

There were no other significant main effects, interactions or pairwise comparisons.

Z score data (means and standard deviations), for each cognitive and mood measure are shown in Table 5. The higher the value, the better the cognitive performance or mood.

Table 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Z scores for each of the cognitive and mood measures for each condition and timepoint.

Discussion

This is one of the few studies to investigate the acute effects of H. erinaceus extracts on cognition and mood in healthy younger adults. Whilst there was no overall effect on cognition as determined by the global composite measure, when cognitive tasks were analysed individually, acute consumption of H. erinaceus resulted in improved psychomotor skill. However, there was no evidence for a beneficial effect of the extract on mood in this study.

Interestingly, our investigation unveils the potential advantages of a single H. erinaceus dose on psychomotor skill/manual dexterity in a young and healthy cohort, as evidenced by improved performance on the pegboard test (dominant and non-dominant forms). The pegboard task is a widely used measure of psychomotor performance and manual dexterity. In this task, participants are required to insert pegs into holes on a board as quickly and accurately as possible. It assesses hand-eye coordination, fine motor skills, and the speed of information processing. The primary outcome measure for this task is the total number of pegs correctly placed within a specified time limit. Including the pegboard task in our study aimed to capture any potential acute effects of H. erinaceus on these specific cognitive and motor abilities.

The nuanced nature of acute cognitive effects seen with H. erinaceus extract in the current trial are in agreement with those of a recent trial in a similar population by Docherty et al. (13) who also observed an acute benefit to cognition only for a specific test measuring a particular cognitive ability. However, whilst in this acute trial we did not observe any beneficial effects on mood, Docherty et al. (13) reported a trend toward reduced subjective stress following chronic supplementation, suggesting that whilst specific cognitive effects can occur acutely in healthy younger adults, mood effects may only be evident following chronic supplementation.

Conversely, acute consumption of the H. erinaceus extract appeared to impair performance on the Flanker and Trail making B tests, both of which assess aspects of executive function (34, 35). Taken together, these conflicting results suggest that the effects of H. erinaceus extract on cognitive function in a young population 90 min after a single (3 g) dose are inconclusive based on this study. The lack of conclusive evidence on the beneficial cognitive effects of H. erinaceus extract is in alignment with the findings of a study conducted by Grozier et al. (36), who also measured cognitive changes in a young, healthy adult population after 4 weeks of H. erinaceus extract consumption and found no significant changes in cognition. However, considering this was a trial with a relatively small sample size, all cognitive results should be interpreted with caution and require replication in future studies to validate the findings. Furthermore, the mechanisms that underlie potential acute cognitive benefits of H. erinaceus are largely unknown and establishing these could be an aim of future research.

Previous studies demonstrating positive effects of H. erinaceus on depression and anxiety, such as those by Okamura et al. (37) and Vigna et al. (10), took place over 4 and 8 weeks, respectively. In the 2019 study by Vigna et al., participants were supplemented with a 500-milligram capsule, using an 80:20 mycelium to fruiting body ratio with each individual ingesting 84 grams of H. erinaceus over an eight-week period. These findings may be attributed to its role in impacting monoaminergic pathways, promoting neurogenesis and neurotrophic factors expression (including nerve growth factor), and/or exhibiting anti-inflammatory effects by modulating expression levels of IL-6, TNF-α, and NF-κB (38). Our acute study showed no statistically significant improvements in positive and negative mood as measured by the PANAS after H. erinaceus consumption when compared to the control. Our findings align with those of a study in a population of healthy adults where a single dose of H. erinaceus did not result in a reduction in stress. However, when supplemented for 28-days, a trend toward reduced subjective stress was observed (13). Most previous studies were conducted on populations that were depressed, whereas our study only included individuals without any clinician diagnosed mood disorders. It may be the case that H. erinaceus only confers positive mood changes in those experiencing mood disorders. Another possibility is that there is less measurable variance post-intervention in those with stable mood patterns such as the population we tested in this study.

A notable aspect of this study is that neither the H. erinaceus extract beverage nor the control contained additional active ingredients. Thus, any cognitive improvements are likely due to H. erinaceus metabolites. The study used a dosage of 3 grams of extract, equivalent to 30 grams of fresh lion’s mane fruiting body, which is higher than previous trials that typically used lower dosages. Furthermore, this study focused solely on the fruiting body, while culinary consumption of lion’s mane can be tenfold this amount. The 5% maltodextrin content, incidental from the spray drying process, is unlikely to significantly affect cognitive tests. It is possible the positive changes in mood and cognition that occurred in previous studies take longer than 90 min or are only evident following chronic supplementation. Due to the difficulty in observing measurable change in healthy individuals compared to those with cognitive decline, should benefits in such a population be evident, a longer period of supplementation may be necessary in order to observe them.

Future research might benefit from encapsulating the extract, as olfactory stimuli can influence mood and cognition (39). Positive changes in mood and cognition might take longer than 90 min to manifest or require chronic supplementation. Observing measurable changes in healthy individuals can be challenging; hence, a longer supplementation period may be necessary to detect benefits.

As this was an investigation with 18 participants, larger randomised controlled trials with a wide range of doses are warranted in order to demonstrate the potential beneficial effects of a single dose of H. erinaceus on mood and cognition. In addition, when designing acute trials, researchers could also consider increasing the delay between consumption of the test interventions and the post-intervention cognitive test session to more than 8 h. Previous studies have indicated that erinacine S, thought to be one of the bioactive compounds in H. erinaceus, can be detected in rats 2 h after administration, reaching peak concentration 8 h after oral administration in rats (40). However, this was not considered to be practical for the present human intervention study and due to the limited research of H. erinaceus in humans, the time it takes for erinacine S to reach peak concentration in human plasma is currently unknown, therefore post-intervention testing after a delay of 90 min was considered appropriate for this study. Furthermore, it is important to note that different organisms have different metabolism rates, therefore a study to determine bioavailability in humans would be warranted. It may also be prudent to test at more than one post-intervention timepoint to increase the likelihood of detecting any cognitive and mood effects and to determine changes over time.

Lion’s Mane mushroom (LMM) extracts may yield varying benefits due to differences in active ingredients. Not all LMM extracts contain equal proportions of purported bioactives. Variability arises from factors such as the source of raw material, extraction methods, and extract constituents. For example, extracts derived from the fruiting body of Hericium erinaceus, as regulated by the EU Novel Foods Catalogue (14), differ from those utilising both fruiting body and mycelium. These differences are underscored by research showing distinct bioactive profiles between the fruiting body and mycelium (1, 41). Moreover, the use of solvents such as ethanol, methanol, or water significantly impacts bioactive yield and consumer acceptance. While water extraction aligns with preferences for natural and safe products, ethanol extraction is more favoured in extraction of terpenoids including hericenones, as highlighted by Chang et al. (42).

Aqueous extractions have been used traditionally in foods, extensively, such as in soups and mushroom teas made from the fruiting body, remain traditional and widely accepted, whereas using organic solvents or supercritical CO2 extraction methods (43) —which may enhance yield and stability— require more consumer awareness. Historically tinctures using mushrooms as a traditional medical product were extracted by ethanol, but there is no evidence it has been used for food. Additionally, techniques like spray drying improve extract stability and ease of use (20), addressing consumer and regulatory expectations.

While this study utilised a H. erinaceus extract derived from the fruiting body, future research could delve deeper into analysing factors affecting efficacy. This highlights the importance of considering Hericium erinaceus extract variability in future studies. Future research should also consider including clinical and biochemical parameters, as well as quantifying bioactives within the administrative extract to help elucidate the potential mechanisms of action behind any beneficial effects of H. erinaceus on cognition and mood.

In conclusion, this study investigating the effect of H. erinaceus on cognition and mood in healthy younger adults was inconclusive. Whilst improved scores on certain cognitive measures suggest potential benefits to motor dexterity, this finding was contradicted by negative results in other tests. Furthermore, no beneficial effects on mood were detected, possibly due to the research being conducted on healthy participants, the dosage of H. erinaceus administered and the acute design of the study with only a single dose consumed. To date there are still a limited number of studies looking at the effects of H. erinaceus in humans. Future studies looking at the acute effects after a single dose of H. erinaceus should consider appropriate dosing for the population of interest, as well as the time between consumption and testing, or potentially testing at several time points following consumption. Further studies in healthy populations could clarify the mixed results of this study and expand our knowledge of the therapeutic potential of H. erinaceus for the wider population. It would also be interesting to investigate acute supplementation with H. erinaceus in compromised populations where there may be a greater opportunity to observe cognitive and mood benefits. Finally, understanding of the potential mechanisms of action related to effects of H. erinaceus on cognition and mood is currently limited and should be a focus of future research.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by University of Surrey Research Integrity & Governance Office (RIGO), EGA ref.: FHMS 22-23 117 EGA. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

GS: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JSa: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. SB: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JSp: Investigation, Writing – original draft. KD: Software, Writing – review & editing. IS: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. DL: Conceptualization, Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing. MH: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. GD: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. SS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. Beyond Alcohol Ltd., (trading as Three Spirit Drinks) contributed towards the publication fee of this article.

Conflict of interest

GS and DL were employed by the company Beyond Alcohol Ltd., (trading as Three Spirit Drinks). GS and DL are employed by Beyond Alcohol Ltd., who use plants containing Hericium erinaceus (Lion’s Mane mushroom) in their products.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Friedman, M. Chemistry, nutrition, and health-promoting properties of Hericium erinaceus (Lion’s mane) mushroom fruiting bodies and mycelia and their bioactive compounds. J Agric Food Chem. (2015) 63:7108–23. doi: 10.1021/acs.jafc.5b02914

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Van Schependom, J, and D’haeseleer, M. Advances in neurodegenerative diseases. J Clin Med. (2023) 12:1709. doi: 10.3390/jcm12051709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Javaid, SF, Hashim, IJ, Hashim, MJ, Stip, E, Samad, MA, and Ahbabi, AA. Epidemiology of anxiety disorders: global burden and sociodemographic associations. Middle East Curr Psychiatry. (2023) 30:1–11. doi: 10.1186/s43045-023-00315-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nochaiwong, S, Ruengorn, C, Thavorn, K, Hutton, B, Awiphan, R, Phosuya, C, et al. Global prevalence of mental health issues among the general population during the coronavirus disease-2019 pandemic: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:10173. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-89700-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ma, B-J, Shen, JW, Yu, HY, Ruan, Y, Wu, TT, and Zhao, X. Hericenones and erinacines: stimulators of nerve growth factor (NGF) biosynthesis in Hericium erinaceus. Mycology. (2010) 1:92–8. doi: 10.1080/21501201003735556

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Tsai-Teng, T, Chin-Chu, C, Li-Ya, L, Wan-Ping, C, Chung-Kuang, L, Chien-Chang, S, et al. Erinacine a-enriched Hericium erinaceus mycelium ameliorates alzheimer’s disease-related pathologies in APPswe/PS1DE9 transgenic mice. J Biomed Sci. (2016) 23:49. doi: 10.1186/s12929-016-0266-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Ryu, S, Kim, H, Kim, J, Kim, S, and Cho, K. Hericium erinaceus extract reduces anxiety and depressive behaviors by promoting hippocampal neurogenesis in the adult mouse brain. J Med Food. (2018) 21:174–80. doi: 10.1089/jmf.2017.4006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Kowiański, P, Lietzau, G, Czuba, E, Waśkow, M, Steliga, A, and Moryś, J. BDNF: a key factor with multipotent impact on brain signaling and synaptic plasticity. Cell Mol Neurobiol. (2017) 38:579–93. doi: 10.1007/s10571-017-0510-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Chiu, C, Chyau, C, Chen, C, Lee, L, Chen, W, Liu, J, et al. Erinacine A-enriched Hericium erinaceus mycelium produces antidepressant-like effects through modulating BDNF/PI3K/Akt/GSK-3β signaling in mice. Int J Mol Sci. (2018) 19:341. doi: 10.3390/ijms19020341

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Vigna, L, Morelli, F, Agnelli, GM, Napolitano, F, Ratto, D, Occhinegro, A, et al. Hericium erinaceus improves mood and sleep disorders in patients affected by overweight or obesity: could circulating pro-BDNF and BDNF be potential biomarkers? Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. (2019) 2019:1–12. doi: 10.1155/2019/7861297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Mori, K, Inatomi, S, Ouchi, K, Azumi, Y, and Tuchida, T. Improving effects of the mushroom Yamabushitake (Hericium Erinaceus) on mild cognitive impairment: a double-blind placebo-controlled clinical trial. Phytother Res. (2008) 23:367–72. doi: 10.1002/ptr.2634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Saitsu, Y, Nishide, A, Kikushima, K, Shimizu, K, and Ohnuki, K. Improvement of cognitive functions by oral intake of Hericium erinaceus. Biomed Res. (2019) 40:125–31. doi: 10.2220/biomedres.40.125

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Docherty, S, Doughty, FL, and Smith, EF. The acute and chronic effects of Lion’s mane mushroom supplementation on cognitive function, stress and mood in young adults: a double-blind, parallel groups. Pilot Study Nut. (2023) 15:4842. doi: 10.3390/nu15224842

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. European Commission. Novel food catalogue. (2023). Available online at:https://ec.europa.eu/food/food-feed-portal/screen/novel-food-catalogue/search

Google Scholar

15. Cha, S, Bell, L, Shukitt-Hale, B, and Williams, CM. A review of the effects of mushrooms on mood and neurocognitive health across the lifespan. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2024) 158:105548. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2024.105548

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. La Monica, MB, Raub, B, Ziegenfuss, EJ, Hartshorn, S, Grdic, J, Gustat, A, et al. Acute effects of naturally occurring Guayusa tea and Nordic Lion’s mane extracts on cognitive performance. Nutrients. (2023) 15:5018. doi: 10.3390/nu15245018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, A-G, and Buchner, A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Browne, RH. On the use of a pilot sample for sample size determination. Stat Med. (1995) 14:1933–40. doi: 10.1002/sim.4780141709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Julious, SA. Sample size of 12 per group rule of thumb for a pilot study. Pharm Stat J Appl Stat Pharm Ind. (2005) 4:287–91. doi: 10.1002/pst.185

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Gallo, L, and Bucalá, V. A review on influence of spray drying process parameters on the production of medicinal plant powders. Curr. Drug Discov. Technol. (2019) 16:340–54. doi: 10.2174/1570163815666180801152918

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Nehlig, A. Is caffeine a cognitive enhancer? J Alzheimers Dis. (2010) 20 Suppl 1:S85. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-091315

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Polverino, M, Polverino, A, Montuori, S, Foti, F, Ferraioli, G, Sorrentino, P, et al. Effects of physical exercise on cognitive functioning and wellbeing: Biological and psychological benefits. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:509. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00509

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Deary, IJ, Liewald, D, and Nissan, J. A free, easy-to-use, computer-based simple and four-choice reaction time programme: the Deary-Liewald reaction time task. Behav Res Methods. (2011) 43:258–68. doi: 10.3758/s13428-010-0024-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. de Jager, CA, Dye, L, de Bruin, EA, Butler, L, Fletcher, J, Lamport, DJ, et al. Criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev. (2014) 72:162–79. doi: 10.1111/nure.12094

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Eriksen, BA, and Eriksen, CW. Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a non-search task. Percept Psychophys. (1974) 16:143–9. doi: 10.3758/bf03203267

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Watson, D, Clark, LA, and Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1988) 54:1063–70. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Reitan, RM, and Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery: Theory and clinical interpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press (1993).

Google Scholar

28. Jaeger, J. Digit symbol substitution test. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2018) 38:513–9. doi: 10.1097/jcp.0000000000000941

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lamar, M, Rhodes, E, Wambach, DM, Swenson, R, and Libon, DJ. Digit span as a neuropsychological instrument of error processing. In: L Ashendorf, R Swenson, and DJ Libon, editors. The Boston Process Approach to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Practitioner’s Guide. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2013). 65–76.

Google Scholar

30. Anwyl-Irvine, AL, Massonnié, J, Flitton, A, Kirkham, N, and Evershed, JK. Gorilla in our midst: an online behavioral experiment builder. Behav Res Methods. (2020) 52:388–407. doi: 10.3758/s13428-019-01237-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Alharbi, MH, Lamport, DJ, Dodd, GF, Saunders, C, Harkness, L, Butler, LT, et al. Flavonoid rich orange juice is associated with acute improvements in cognitive function in healthy middle-aged males. Eur J Neurosci. (2015) 55:2021–9. doi: 10.1007/s00394-015-1016-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kean, RJ, Lamport, DJ, Dodd, GF, Freeman, JE, Williams, CM, Ellis, JA, et al. Chronic consumption of flavanone-rich orange juice is associated with cognitive benefits: an 8-wk, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in healthy older adults. Am J Clin Nutr. (2015) 101:506–14. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.114.088518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Howell, DC. Statistical methods for psychology. Boston, Massachusetts: Wadsworth Cengage Learning (2013).

Google Scholar

34. Servant, M, and Logan, GD. ‘Dynamics of attentional focusing in the Eriksen flanker task’. Attention Percep Psycho. (2019) 81:2710–21. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01796-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Tombaugh, TN. Trail making test a and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2004) 19:203–14. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Grozier, C, Alves, V, Killen, LG, Simpson, JD, O'Neal, EK, and Waldman, HS. Four weeks of Hericium erinaceus supplementation does not impact markers of metabolic flexibility or cognition. Int J Exerc Sci. (2022) 15:1366–80. doi: 10.70252/XZKO8571

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Okamura, H, Anno, N, Tsuda, A, Inokuchi, T, Uchimura, N, and Inanaga, K. The effects of Hericium Erinaceus (Amyloban® 3399) on sleep quality and subjective well-being among female undergraduate students: a pilot study. Personalized Med Univ. (2015) 4:76–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pmu.2015.03.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chong, PS, Fung, ML, Wong, KH, and Lim, LW. Therapeutic potential of Hericium erinaceus for depressive disorder. Int J Mol Sci. (2020) 21:163. doi: 10.3390/ijms21010163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Moss, L, Rouse, M, Wesnes, KA, and Moss, M. Differential effects of the aromas of Salvia species on memory and mood. Hum Psychopharmacol. (2010) 25:388–96. doi: 10.1002/hup.1129

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Li, H, Li, IC, Lin, TW, Chen, WP, Lee, LY, Chen, CC, et al. Absolute bioavailability, tissue distribution, and excretion of Erinacine S in Hericium Erinaceus mycelia. Molecules. (2019) 24:1624. doi: 10.3390/molecules24081624

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lazur, J, Kała, K, Krakowska, A, Sułkowska-Ziaja, K, Szewczyk, A, Piotrowska, J, et al. Analysis of bioactive substances and essential elements of mycelia and fruiting bodies of Hericium spp. J Food Compos Anal. (2024) 127:105981. doi: 10.1016/j.jfca.2024.105981

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Chang, CH, Chen, Y, Yew, XX, Chen, HX, Kim, JX, Chang, CC, et al. Improvement of erinacine a productivity in Hericium erinaceus mycelia and its neuroprotective bioactivity against the glutamate-insulted apoptosis. LWT. (2016) 65:1100–8. doi: 10.1016/j.lwt.2015.08.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Joradon, P., Rungsardthong, V., Ruktanonchai, U., Suttisintong, K., Iempridee, T., Thumthanaruk, B., et al. (2022). A comparative study of conventional and supercritical carbon dioxide extraction methods for the recovery of bioactive compounds from Lion’s mane mushroom (Hericium erinaceus). In E3S web of conferences 355, 02015.

Google Scholar

44. Baars, BJ, and Gage,. ‘Conciousness and Attention’, Elsevier, (2010) 8, pp. 238–303.

Google Scholar

45. Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on prehension. Mind, os-12, 75–79. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-12.45.75

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Woodworth, R. S., and Wells, F. L. (1911). Association tests. Being a part of the Report of the Committee ofthe American Psychological Association on the Standardization of Procedure in Experimental Tests. Psychological Monographs, 13 (Whole no. 57).

Google Scholar

47. Matthews, C. G., and Kløve, H. (1964). Neuropsychological test battery manual. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Neuropsychology Laboratory.

Google Scholar

48. Army Individual Test Battery (1944). Manual of Directions and Scoring. War Department, Adjutant General’s Office, Washington DC.

Google Scholar

Keywords: mushroom, Lion’s Mane mushroom, Hericium erinaceus, cognition, mood

Citation: Surendran G, Saye J, Binti Mohd Jalil S, Spreadborough J, Duong K, Shatwan IM, Lilley D, Heinrich M, Dodd GF and Surendran S (2025) Acute effects of a standardised extract of Hericium erinaceus (Lion’s Mane mushroom) on cognition and mood in healthy younger adults: a double-blind randomised placebo-controlled study. Front. Nutr. 12:1405796. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2025.1405796

Received: 23 March 2024; Accepted: 28 February 2025;
Published: 10 April 2025.

Edited by:

Andrew Scholey, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Shawn M. Talbott, Amare Global, United States
Emma Wightman, Northumbria University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Surendran, Saye, Binti Mohd Jalil, Spreadborough, Duong, Shatwan, Lilley, Heinrich, Dodd and Surendran. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shelini Surendran, cy5zdXJlbmRyYW5Ac3VycmV5LmFjLnVr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more