Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 30 May 2023
Sec. Nutrition and Metabolism
This article is part of the Research Topic Undetermined Implications of Chronutrition: A Missing Curriculum in Medicine View all 12 articles

Associations of timing of food intake with energy intake, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors in adults with overweight and obesity

Raphaëlle Jacob,,Raphaëlle Jacob1,2,3Angelo Tremblay,,Angelo Tremblay2,3,4Vronique Provencher,Véronique Provencher1,2Shirin Panahi,,Shirin Panahi2,3,5Marie-ve Mathieu,Marie-Ève Mathieu6,7Vicky Drapeau,,
Vicky Drapeau2,3,5*
  • 1School of Nutrition, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 2Centre Nutrition, santé et société (NUTRISS), Institute of Nutrition and Functional Foods (INAF), Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 3Quebec Heart and Lung Institute Research Centre, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 4Department of Kinesiology, Faculty of Medicine, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 5Department of Physical Education, Faculty of Education, Université Laval, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 6School of Kinesiology and Physical Activity Sciences, Faculty of Medicine, Université de Montréal, Quebec, QC, Canada
  • 7Sainte-Justine University Health Centre Research Centre, Université de Montreal, Quebec, QC, Canada

Introduction: Whether a late distribution of food intake impacts obesity through increased energy intake remains uncertain and the behavioural characterization of late eating needs to be further investigated. The first objective of this study was to assess the associations between late eating and body mass index (BMI) and total energy intake (TEI), and whether TEI mediates the association between late eating and BMI. The second objective was to assess the associations between late eating and eating behaviour traits or psychosocial factors and whether eating behaviour traits mediate the association between late eating and TEI.

Methods: Baseline data from 301 individuals (56% women, age = 38.7 ± 8.5 years; BMI = 33.2 ± 3.4 kg/m2), who participated in four weight loss studies were used in this cross-sectional study. Total energy intake was assessed using a three-day food record from which the percentage of TEI after 17:00 and after 20:00 was calculated. Eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors were assessed with questionnaires. Pearson correlations and mediation analyses adjusted for age, sex, underreporting of energy intake, sleep duration and bedtime were performed.

Results: Percent TEI after 17:00 and after 20:00 were associated with TEI (r = 0.13, p = 0.03 for both), and TEI mediated the association between percent TEI after 17:00 and BMI (β = 0.01 ± 0.01, 95% CI: 0.001, 0.02). Percent TEI after 17:00 was associated with disinhibition (r = 0.13, p = 0.03) and percent TEI after 20:00 was associated with susceptibility to hunger (r = 0.13, p = 0.03), stress (r = 0.24, p = 0.002) and anxiety (r = 0.28, p = 0.0004). In women, disinhibition mediated the association between percent TEI after 17:00 and TEI (β = 3.41 ± 1.43, 95% CI: 0.92, 6.47). Susceptibility to hunger mediated the association between percent TEI after 20:00 and TEI (β = 0.96 ± 0.59, 95% CI: 0.02, 2.34) in men and women.

Conclusion: Late eating is associated with TEI and suboptimal eating behaviours which could contribute to explaining the association between timing of food intake and obesity.

1. Introduction

Recent evidence indicates that the timing of food intake is a risk factor for obesity and associated comorbidities such as type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular diseases (13). Cross-sectional, prospective and interventional studies have shown that eating later during the day was associated with obesity or greater adiposity (49), weight gain (10) and reduced weight loss (1116).

The distribution of food intake throughout the day is an important synchronizer of peripheral clocks, located in many organs and tissues (1). Consuming a high proportion of energy later during the day and into the night can produce chronodisruption, a state where peripheral clocks are out of synchrony with the central clock, located in the suprachiasmatic nucleus and aimed to synchronise behaviours with environmental cues (1, 2). A state of chronodisruption can impact many physiological processes (1, 2). However, the mechanisms explaining the increased susceptibility to obesity among late eaters remain to be fully understood. Some studies, but not all (1720), have shown no associations between late eating and total energy intake (TEI) either cross-sectionally (13) or during weight loss (12, 14, 21). Based on these results and those of experimental studies exploring the effect of timing of food intake on metabolism (1, 2224), it has been suggested that late eating impacts body weight mainly through energy expenditure (e.g., lower thermic effect of foods) (24) rather than through energy intake (1). However, late eating resulted in higher daily appetite sensations in two recent cross-over randomised controlled trials in which food intake was fully controlled (25, 26). These results seem inconsistent with the previous hypothesis and suggest that energy intake may be involved in the association between timing of food intake and obesity, but this needs to be assessed in well-designed studies conducted under free-living conditions.

In studies where food intake is not fully controlled, the lack of influence of late eating on energy intake remains uncertain as most studies relied on self-reported dietary assessment tools without consideration of misreporting of energy intake which can result in attenuated or misleading associations (27). Accounting for misreporting of energy intake may be particularly relevant in this context as underreporting is associated with obesity and is more likely to occur with foods of low nutritional value that may be perceived as socially undesirable (28) and that are associated with evening preferences (29, 30). Moreover, late eaters have been characterized as being more prone to eating when stressed, overeating at night and eating while watching television (13). Disinhibition, which refers to an overconsumption of food triggered by different cues (31), habitual and emotional susceptibility to disinhibition, and binge eating severity have also been associated with a higher proportion of TEI consumed as evening snacks (32). Eating behaviour traits such as disinhibition, emotional eating and binge eating have been associated with higher energy intake, weight gain and obesity (3340). Although the behavioural characterization of late eaters in the literature is scarce, these results support the hypothesis that late eating may also impact body weight through energy intake.

To improve obesity treatment and prevention, there is a need to better understand how late eating impacts body weight. Importantly, more studies accounting for systematic bias associated with dietary assessment tools or using objective measurement of dietary intake are needed to shed light on the possible association between late eating and energy intake. Expanding the behavioural characterization of late eaters is also important as it could help develop targeted interventions for these individuals. To our knowledge, only two studies have assessed eating behaviour traits associated with late eating (13, 32). Based on results from these two previous studies showing that late eating is associated with eating in response to negative emotions (13, 32), it is possible that late eaters are characterized by higher levels of psychosocial factors such as stress, anxiety and depressive symptoms, but this remains to be assessed.

The first objective of this study was to assess the associations between a late distribution of food intake (i.e., late eating), body mass index (BMI) and TEI and to determine whether TEI mediates the association between late eating and BMI, while considering underreporting of energy intake. The second objective was to examine the associations between late eating, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors (i.e., stress, anxiety and depressive symptoms), and to investigate whether eating behaviour traits related to overeating (e.g., disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger) mediate the association between late eating and TEI. We hypothesised that late eating is associated with TEI and BMI and that TEI mediates the association between late eating and BMI. We also hypothesised that late eating is associated with overeating-related eating behaviour traits and that these traits mediate the association between late eating and TEI.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants

This cross-sectional study included baseline data from 301 individuals with overweight or obesity from the Weight Loss Intervention Studies (WeLIS) Cohort, which includes four previous weight loss studies with similar designs conducted at Université Laval (4144). These studies aimed to assess the effect of various supplements (i.e., probiotic, calcium and vitamin D, or a multivitamin and mineral) compared to placebo as part of a 12–15-week energy-restricted intervention (4244), or the effect of a non-restrictive satiating diet compared to standard nutritional guidelines (i.e., Canada’s Food Guide 2007) for 16 weeks (41), on weight loss. Inclusion criteria were to be aged 20 to 55 years, living with overweight or obesity, having a body weight variation of less than 4 kg for at least 2 months before the study, being inactive to low active (i.e., maximum of three periods of 30 min of low to vigorous intensity physical activity per week), being in apparent good health (i.e., absence of any disease or condition that may pose a risk to the participant or alter the results of the study) based on a standard medical examination performed by a physician during the screening visit of each study, having no comorbidities such as type 2 diabetes or cardiovascular diseases, not taking medications or supplements that could impact study outcomes, consumption of less than 10 alcoholic beverages per week and a maximum of 2 drinks per day, consumption of less than 5 cups of coffee per day, not being pregnant or lactating and absence of menopause for women. Individuals working at night during the completion of the three-day food record (n = 1) and those who completed less than 2 days of food record (n = 3) were excluded from the present study (Supplementary Figure S1). Each study was approved by the Research Ethics Board of Université Laval and written informed consent was obtained from each participant before the study.

2.2. Anthropometric measurements

Anthropometric measurements were performed according to standardised procedures recommended at the Airlie Conference (45). Body weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg using a digital scale and height was measured to the nearest 0.1 cm using a standard statiometer. Body mass index was calculated as kg/m2.

2.3. Dietary assessment and distribution of food intake

Dietary intakes were assessed with a three-day food record completed on two weekdays and one weekend day at baseline (46). Participants received instructions on how to complete the food record and measure quantity of food consumed. The research dietitian reviewed the completed food record with the participant to ensure that all information was clear and complete. Food records were analyzed using the Nutrific software (47) linked to the Canadian Nutrient File version 1997 or 2005, depending on the studies (48, 49).

The distribution of food intake was assessed by calculating the percentage of TEI from six intervals throughout the day. Period 1 corresponded to the first moment of food consumption recorded until 8:59, period 2 was from 9:00 to 11:29, period 3 was from 11:30 to 14:29, period 4 was from 14:30 to 16:59, period 5 was from 17:00 to 19:59 and period 6 was from 20:00 until the time of the last food consumption recorded. The percentage of TEI from each period was calculated as the sum of the three-day energy intake from each period divided by the sum of the three-day TEI, multiplied by 100. These periods were based on hours delimiting periods in previous American and Canadian studies (6, 17, 18, 32) and adapted to the usual Canadian meal pattern (i.e., frequency and time of intakes) (5052). They were also designed to capture late eating and main peaks of intake (i.e., meals) and snacks in different periods. Further details about the rationale behind the definition of time periods established in this study to assess the distribution of food intake have been previously published (53). To obtain the percentage of TEI from morning, afternoon and evening, the periods were combined as follows: morning (periods 1 and 2), afternoon (periods 3 and 4) and evening (periods 5 and 6). In the present study, we used the percentage of TEI consumed after 17:00 (i.e., periods 5 and 6 combined) and after 20:00 (period 6). A higher percentage of TEI consumed during the evening reflects a later distribution of food intake.

2.4. Eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors

Eating behaviour traits were assessed with the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire (TFEQ) (31, 54) and the Binge Eating Scale (BES) (55). The TFEQ is a 51-item questionnaire that measures three main dimensions of eating behaviour traits, namely cognitive restraint, disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger, with higher scores reflecting higher levels of these eating behaviours. Cognitive restraint reflects the tendency to restrain food intake to control or lose body weight. It is assessed with 21 items and can be separated into specific types, namely rigid and flexible restraint, each assessed with 7 items (56). Disinhibition is based on 16 items and reflects an overconsumption of food triggered by different cues representing its three subscales, namely habitual (5 items), emotional (3 items) and situational (5 items) susceptibility to disinhibition (57). Susceptibility to hunger (14 items) represents a susceptibility to experience feelings of hunger triggered by internal (internal locus of hunger, 6 items) or external (external locus of hunger, 6 items) cues (57). Thirty-six items are in a true or false format coded as 0 or 1 and 15 items are based on a 4 or 6-point scale coded as 0 or 1. The Binge Eating Scale assesses binge eating severity with 16 items describing the behavioural manifestations of binge eating and the feelings and cognitions surrounding binge eating episodes (55). Items are on a scale of 0 to 2 or 0 to 3, providing a total score of 0 to 46, with higher scores denoting higher binge eating severity.

Psychosocial factors, namely stress, anxiety and depressive symptoms were assessed with questionnaires. The Perceived Stress Scale measures the degree to which situations in one’s life are considered stressful during the last month (58). This questionnaire comprises 10 items assessed on a 5-point scale coded as 0 to 4, providing a total score ranging from 0 to 40 (59). The anxiety trait was assessed with 20 items of the State and Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) (60). These items are measured on a 4-point scale, ranging from 1 to 4, providing a total score for the trait section ranging between 20 and 80. Depressive symptoms were assessed with the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (61). The questionnaire comprises 21 items assessed on a 4 to 6-point scale, scored from 0 to 3, which provide a total score for the questionnaire ranging between 0 and 63. Higher scores on these questionnaires reflect higher levels of stress, anxiety or depressive symptoms (55, 58, 59, 61).

2.5. Assessment of covariates

Information on sex and age were collected at screening by the research staff. Sleep duration and habitual bedtime over the last month were self-reported with two questions based on the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (62). Misreporting of energy intake was assessed by the method of Huang et al. (27) according to which under- and over reporting of energy intake are identified based on confidence limits around a ratio of self-reported energy intake (rEI) to total energy expenditure (TEE) calculated from a formula accounting for measurement error in rEI and TEE. Since an objective measure of resting metabolic rate (RMR) was available for 99% (n = 298) of participants, TEE was based on a factorial method using RMR and physical activity level (PAL) (63) rather than on equations such as those developed by the National Academy of Medicine as done in the original method of Huang et al. (27, 64). Resting metabolic rate was measured in a fasted state using indirect calorimetry in each study, as detailed elsewhere (65). For participants with missing data on RMR (n = 3), the latter was estimated using the Mifflin St-Jeor equation (66), which was found to be the most reliable equation to predict RMR in adults with normal weight and obesity, with the narrowest error range (67). Since participants had to be inactive to low active to be included in each study, RMR was multiplied by a PAL coefficient of 1.4 to determine TEE. A PAL coefficient of 1.4 represents the cut-off value between inactive and low active (64). Assuming a standard PAL for each participant in the assessment of misreporting of energy intake has been previously done (68). The ±1 standard deviation (SD) for confidence limits was established by using a within-individual coefficient of variation (CV) of 23.0% for rEI, n = 3 days for dietary assessment, a CV of 16.8% for TEE and a CV of 4.0% or 8.5% to account for within-individual day-to-day variation and error associated with objective measurement of TEE for measured or estimated RMR, respectively (69). To account for skewness of TEI, the ±1 SD confidence intervals were exponentiated using a multiplicative factor of 1 (70). For participants with an objective measure of RMR, the resulting confidence interval was 0.80 to 1.24 whereas the confidence interval was 0.79 to 1.26 for participants with estimated RMR. Individuals with values corresponding to or within the confidence interval were considered plausible reporters. Underreporting of energy intake was defined as rEI/TEE <0.80 and < 0.79 for participants with measured and estimated RMR, respectively. Overreporting of energy intake corresponded to rEI/TEE >1.24 and > 1.26 for participants with measured and estimated RMR, respectively. Based on these values, two indicator variables were created to represent underreporting (yes, 1; no, 0) and overreporting (yes, 1; no, 0). Because misreporting is based on a deviation from an exact correspondence between rEI and TEE (27), overreporting may also represent overconsumption in some individuals. Consequently, the main analyses were only adjusted for underreporting, but overreporting was further considered in supplemental analyses. Adjustment, rather than exclusion of under- and overreporters was used as it has been suggested as a more appropriate way to address misreporting since it avoids selection bias and reduction in statistical power (71). Ethnicity was also assessed at screening, but was not used as a covariate since there was low diversity in this sample.

2.6. Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Sex differences in baseline characteristics, distribution of food intake, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors were assessed with Student’s T tests and Chi-square tests. Differences in these variables between studies were assessed with general linear models and Chi-square tests.

The associations between the percentage of TEI consumed in the evening (after 17:00 and after 20:00) and BMI, TEI, eating behaviour traits or psychosocial factors were assessed with Pearson correlations. The main analyses were adjusted for age, sex (men, 0; women, 1), underreporting of energy intake, sleep duration and bedtime. Due to sex differences in eating behaviour traits (34, 35), linear regressions were used to investigate sex interactions in these associations. In case of significant sex interaction, Pearson’s correlations were performed separately among men and women. The strength of associations for Pearson’s correlations was interpreted based on Funder and Ozer guidelines, where coefficients of 0.10, 0.20, and 0.30 represent small, moderate and large effect sizes, respectively (72).

Mediation analyses were conducted to assess if TEI mediates the potential associations between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 and BMI. Mediation analyses were also performed to determine if the association between late eating and TEI is mediated by some eating behaviour traits that confer a susceptibility to overeating and showed a significant and positive association with late eating in correlation analyses. These analyses were performed separately among men and women when a sex interaction was previously observed in regression analyses. Mediation analyses were performed with model 4 of the Process macro for SAS, version 3.4.1 (73). Process is an ordinary least square regression path analysis modelling tool that assesses the mediating (or indirect) effect through which an independent variable influences a dependent variable using percentile bootstrap confidence intervals (73). The current study used 5,000 bootstrap samples. In the mediation model, the association between the independent variable and the mediator is represented by path a, and the association between the mediator and the dependent variable, adjusted for the independent variable, is represented by path b. The total effect (c) represents the association between the independent (late eating) and dependent variables (BMI or TEI) and the direct effect (c’) represents this same association but adjusted for the mediator (TEI or eating behaviour traits). Mediation analyses were adjusted for the same covariates as correlations. According to Hayes, a mediation effect could occur despite no evidence that the association between the independent and dependent variables is different from zero since the indirect effect is not determined nor constrained by the size of the total effect (73).

In secondary analyses, correlations and mediation models were adjusted for (1) age, sex and underreporting (i.e., model 2), (2) age, sex, misreporting (i.e., both under-and overreporting), sleep duration and bedtime (i.e., model 3) and (3) age, sex, underreporting, sleep duration, bedtime and studies (i.e., model 4). Adjustment for the different studies was performed by creating three indicator variables for Major et al. (42, 43) and Arguin et al. (41) studies. The Sanchez et al. (44) study was used as reference since this study had all of the questionnaires available and was performed in both men and women. For the associations between late eating and binge eating severity, perceived stress and anxiety, only one indicator variable for Sanchez et al. (44) was used as these questionnaires were only available in Arguin et al. (41) and Sanchez et al. (44).

3. Results

3.1. Participant characteristics

This study included 168 women and 133 men, with a mean age of 38.7 ± 8.5 years (range 19.7 to 55.2 years) and a mean BMI of 33.2 ± 3.4 kg/m2 (range 26.2 to 45.4 kg/m2) (Table 1). Participants consumed 43.7 ± 8.7% of TEI during the evening (after 17:00). Of this value, 9.2 ± 10.0% of TEI was consumed after 20:00. Except for TEI, there were no differences in these baseline characteristics nor distribution of food intake between men and women. However, as expected, there were several sex differences in eating behaviour traits, with women presenting higher levels of cognitive restraint (8.4 ± 4.0 vs. 5.8 ± 3.0, p < 0.0001) and emotional susceptibility to disinhibition (1.9 ± 1.2 vs. 1.4 ± 1.3, p = 0.002), and lower levels of situational susceptibility to disinhibition (3.3 ± 1.4 vs. 3.7 ± 1.2, p = 0.01) and susceptibility to hunger (5.7 ± 3.3 vs. 7.1 ± 3.6, p = 0.001). Women also reported higher levels of perceived stress (15.3 ± 6.0 vs. 12.5 ± 5.7, p = 0.002). Differences in participant characteristics, distribution of food intake, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors between studies included in this cohort are presented in Supplementary Table S1. Note that differences between studies are mainly attributed to sex.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics.

3.2. Associations between the percentage of TEI in the evening, TEI and BMI

There was a non-significant trend for the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 and BMI (r = 0.11, p = 0.07), but no association between the percentage of TEI after 20:00 and BMI (r = 0.05, p = 0.44). However, the percentage of TEI consumed after 17:00 or after 20:00 were positively associated with TEI (r = 0.13, p = 0.03 for both). No sex interaction was observed for these correlations (data not shown). Similar results were observed for correlations that did not consider sleep duration and bedtime or that were further adjusted for overreporting of energy intake or the different studies (Supplementary Table S2).

Mediation analyses showed a significant mediating effect of TEI on the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 and BMI (β = 0.008 ± 0.005, 95% CI: 0.0006, 0.02) (Figure 1A). Although of similar magnitude, the mediating effect for the model related to the percentage of TEI after 20:00 did not reach significance (β = 0.008 ± 0.006, 95% CI: −0.0004, 0.02) (Figure 1B). Similar results were observed after adjustment for the different sets of covariates (Supplementary Table S3). The model adjusted for age, sex and underreporting of energy intake showed a significant mediating effect of TEI in the association between the percentage of TEI after 20:00 and BMI, but the effect was similar to the original model.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mediation of TEI in the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 (A) or after 20:00 (B) and BMI. Mediation analyses were conducted using the Process Macro v. 3.4.1 for SAS that uses percentile bootstrap confidence intervals to assess the mediating or indirect effect. 95% CI for indirect effect are estimated through 5, 000 bootstrap samples. Models were adjusted for age (continuous), sex (men, 0; women, 1), underreporting of energy intake (yes, 1; no, 0), sleep duration (continuous) and bedtime (continuous). a, Association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or 20:00 and the TEI; b, Association between TEI and BMI adjusted for the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00; total effect (c), association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or 20:00 and BMI without adjustment for the mediator (TEI); direct effect (c’), association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or 20:00 and BMI adjusted for the mediator (TEI); indirect effect (ab), mediation effect; Boot, Bootstrap; CI, confidence interval; TEI, total energy intake; BMI, body mass index. n = 278.

3.3. Associations between the percentage of TEI in the evening, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors

The percentage of TEI after 17:00 was positively associated with disinhibition (r = 0.13, p = 0.03) (Table 2). The percentage of TEI after 20:00 was positively associated with habitual susceptibility to disinhibition (r = 0.13, p = 0.04), susceptibility to hunger (r = 0.13, p = 0.03), perceived stress (r = 0.24, p = 0.002) and anxiety trait (r = 0.28, p = 0.0004). A sex by distribution of food intake interaction indicated different patterns of association in men and women for some eating behaviour traits and depressive symptoms. The percentage of TEI after 17:00 was positively associated with disinhibition (r = 0.27, p = 0.0009) and its subscale habitual susceptibility to disinhibition (r = 0.21, p = 0.01) in women (Table 3). In men, the percentage of TEI after 17:00 was negatively associated with susceptibility to hunger (r = −0.20, p = 0.04) and its subscale external locus of hunger (r = −0.19, p = 0.04). Depressive symptoms were not associated with the percentage of TEI after 20:00 neither in men nor in women. Again, these results remained similar after adjustment for the different sets of covariates (Supplementary Tables S2, S4).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Associations between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factorsa.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 and eating behaviour traits or depressive symptoms, respectively, in men and womena.

In women, mediation analyses showed that disinhibition and its subscale habitual susceptibility to disinhibition mediated the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 and TEI (β = 3.41 ± 1.43, 95% CI: 0.92, 6.47 and β = 3.53 ± 1.80, 95% CI: 0.66, 7.68, respectively) (Figures 2A,B). In men and women combined, susceptibility to hunger mediated the association between the percentage of TEI after 20:00 and TEI (β = 0.96 ± 0.59, 95% CI: 0.02, 2.34) (Figure 2D) but no mediating effect of habitual susceptibility to disinhibition in the association between the percentage of TEI after 20:00 and TEI was observed (β = 0.83 ± 0.63, 95% CI: −0.11, 2.27) (Figure 2C). These results remained similar after adjustment for the different sets of covariates (Supplementary Table S5).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediation of disinhibition, habitual susceptibility to disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger in the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or 20:00 and TEI in women (% TEI after 17:00) or in men and women (% TEI after 20:00). Mediation analyses were conducted using the Process Macro v. 3.4.1 for SAS that uses percentile bootstrap confidence intervals to assess the mediating or indirect effect. 95% CI for indirect effect are estimated through 5,000 bootstrap samples. Models were adjusted for age (continuous), underreporting of energy intake (yes, 1; no, 0), sleep duration (continuous) and bedtime (continuous). (C,D) are further adjusted for sex (men, 0; women, 1). a, association between percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 and eating behaviour trait; b, association between eating behaviour trait and TEI adjusted for the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00; total effect (c), association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 and TEI without adjustment for the mediator (eating behaviour trait); direct effect (c’), association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 and TEI adjusted for the mediator (eating behaviour trait); indirect effect (ab), mediation effect; Boot, Bootstrap; CI, confidence interval; TEI, total energy intake. (A) n = 152, (B) n = 147, (C) n = 259, (D) n = 267.

4. Discussion

The timing of food intake has been identified as a risk factor for obesity. However, whether a late distribution of food intake impacts body weight through increased energy intake remains controversial and the behavioural characterization of late eaters needs to be further investigated. This study first examined the associations between a late distribution of food intake and TEI, BMI and eating and psychosocial traits, while considering underreporting of energy intake. This study also aimed to assess whether TEI and eating behaviour traits mediate the association between late eating and BMI or TEI, respectively. The results showed that a higher percentage of TEI during the evening was positively associated with TEI and that TEI mediated the association between the percentage of TEI after 17:00 and BMI. The results also showed that late eating was associated with higher levels of disinhibition, susceptibility to hunger, stress and anxiety, and that disinhibition in women and susceptibility to hunger in men and women combined mediated the association between late eating and TEI. Although cross-sectional, this study suggests that late eating could influence obesity through increased energy intake and that suboptimal eating behaviour traits could contribute to explaining the susceptibility to overeating among late eaters.

The association between the percentage of TEI in the evening and TEI was small, but consistent with previous observational studies (1720). Moreover, although late eating (i.e., percentage of TEI after 17:00) showed a non-significant trend towards higher BMI, mediation analyses showed that late eating could be associated with BMI through higher energy intake. As indicated previously, an indirect effect can be different from zero even if the total effect is not (73). This is explained by the fact that the size of the indirect effect is not constrained nor determined by the size of the total effect (73). As shown in path a of Figures 1A,B each percent increase in TEI after 17:00 or after 20:00 resulted in an increase of approximately 7 kcal/day. When considering women only, the total effect (path c) of Figures 2A,B indicates that each percent increase in TEI after 17:00 results in an increase in energy intake of approximately 12 kcal/day. Through energy intake, the mediation models showed that each percent increase in TEI after 17:00 resulted in 0.008 unit of BMI. For an individual 1.75 m tall, an increase of 5% in TEI after 17:00 would result in an extra 0.12 kg of body weight. Given that the average increase in body weight among Canadian adults is 0.5 to 1 kg per 2-year period (74), the effect of the timing of food intake through energy intake may be a meaningful factor to consider in the aetiology of obesity.

Several factors may explain why a late distribution of food intake may lead to higher energy intake. Ghrelin, hunger, appetite for specific foods (i.e., sweets, salty and starchy foods, fruits and meat), desire to eat, prospective food consumption and fullness showed endogenous circadian rhythms with higher levels in the evening and lower levels in the morning and the opposite for fullness (75, 76). This potentially facilitates a positive energy balance towards the end of the day. These results were corroborated in an experimental study comparing appetite and food reward in response to a test meal during morning or late afternoon (77). Appetite, liking and wanting for high-fat foods were higher and post-meal fullness was lower in the late meal condition (77). The distribution of food intake may also influence appetite, as late eating resulted in higher daily levels of ghrelin and hunger and lower levels of fullness during weight loss (12). Results of this latter study were recently corroborated in two cross-over randomised controlled trials in which all foods were provided to participants during weight loss or weight stable conditions (25, 26). These studies reported higher daily levels of hunger, prospective food consumption and desire to eat in the late eating condition (25, 26). Homeostatic and hedonic mechanisms may thus be implicated in the effect of late eating on TEI.

The possible effect of late eating on energy intake is also supported by positive, small to moderate associations between late eating and eating behaviour traits associated with overeating, namely disinhibition and susceptibility to hunger. Moreover, these eating behaviours mediated the associations between late eating and TEI in women or in the whole group, respectively. These results are in line with previous studies showing that late eating was associated with overeating at night and emotional eating (13) and that evening snacking was positively associated with disinhibition (32). More broadly, these results are also in line with studies on chronotype showing that evening type individuals, who also present a delayed food pattern (78, 79), are more likely to present higher levels of disinhibition, susceptibility to hunger and binge eating and lower levels of cognitive restraint (80, 81). While cross-sectional, these results collectively suggest that late eating could lead to overconsumption through suboptimal eating behaviour traits.

Disinhibition behaviours and overconsumption triggered by susceptibility to hunger may be more likely to occur during the evening as a result of homeostatic and hedonic mechanisms potentially facilitating overeating during the evening presented above. Overeating associated with these eating behaviours may also be more likely to occur during the evening as a result of lower self-regulation capacity and increased tiredness (82), eating because others are eating (83, 84), eating in front of television (83), expecting eating to be more rewarding (83), alcohol consumption (83, 85), and more opportunities to eat alone (83) or when at home compared to at work or at school. It may also be a consequence of reduced energy intake during the day. Tani et al. showed that a lower proportion of TEI in the morning and in the afternoon were associated with higher energy intake during the evening, and a higher proportion of TEI during the evening was associated with higher TEI (19). A reduced energy intake earlier in the day may also be a consequence of overeating during the evening. Indeed, late eaters showed reduced morning appetite (13) and a Mendelian randomisation study showed that evening chronotype was causally associated with breakfast skipping, the latter also being causally associated with obesity (86).

Sex differences were observed in the associations between late eating and eating behaviour traits. Women with disinhibition seem more susceptible to overeating after 17:00, while both men and women presenting higher levels of susceptibility to hunger, showed a higher percentage of TEI after 20:00. This may be related to gender differences in social norms regarding food intake and body weight, as eating light meals are perceived as more feminine and heavy meals as more masculine (87). Eating smaller meals during the day may promote overeating in women with disinhibition during the evening, when at home, as opposed to men who may not perceive such social pressure to restrain eating during the day. Sex differences in chronotype may also be involved, as men usually present a later chronotype (78).

The positive associations between the proportion of TEI consumed after 20:00 and psychosocial factors such as stress and anxiety are in line with a previous study showing that late eaters were more likely to eat when stressed (13). More broadly, this is also in line with studies indicating that individuals with evening chronotype were more likely to experience negative psychological symptoms including anxiety (88), emotional overeating and stress-related eating (79, 89, 90). Because affective states and cognitive functioning worsen later in the day, resulting in a decrease in self-regulation capacity (82), it may be hypothesised that individuals with higher levels of stress and anxiety are more susceptible to overeating during the evening or at night. This hypothesis is supported by a study showing a positive association between psychological stress and self-reported overeating at dinner in workers (91). More broadly, this is also in line with the literature showing a positive association between emotional overeating and evening snacking (32) and that evening and night eating are used to regulate negative emotions among individuals with night eating syndrome (92). Participants of this latter study also reported that evening and night eating resulted in calmness (92). Lastly, a positive association between stress and hunger was found to be predominant during late afternoon and evening (93), which may also explain the increased susceptibility to overeating during the evening in individuals with stress and anxiety.

This study has several strengths and limitations. One of the main strengths is the use of a three-day food record with the consideration of underreporting of energy intake, and overreporting in supplemental analyses, to mitigate the effect of systematic bias in self-reported dietary assessment (27). As day-to-day variation was observed in the timing of food intake, combining several days of food intake is more likely to reflect a more habitual daily distribution of food intake than a single day (78, 94). The distribution of food intake was based on meal and snack times reported on the food record, which is more precise than using meals and snacks without considering hours. However, this classification is limited by not being defined relative to endogenous circadian timing or sleep–wake cycle (3). Yet, the consideration of mean sleep duration and bedtime over the last month as covariates suggests that the associations observed are not simply reflecting a shifted sleep–wake cycle providing more opportunities to eat in the evening in individuals with later bedtime and shorter sleep duration. It should be noted that these sleep parameters could have been more accurately assessed using objective measurements (e.g., accelerometer) or if sleep duration had been derived from self-reported data on usual bedtime, wake time, and length of time to fall asleep (95). One main limitation is the cross-sectional design that precludes causation to be inferred. Moreover, the relatively low sample size resulted in lower capacity to detect statistically significant results for some analyses. In addition, since the WeLIS cohort includes individuals living with overweight and obesity who are interested in losing weight, the results of this study may not be generalizable to other populations. Replication of these results within larger longitudinal cohorts and with objective measurements of food intake are needed.

In conclusion, this study suggests that late eating is positively associated with TEI, which may be related to higher BMI, in the long term. This association is supported by positive associations between late eating and eating behaviour traits, which contributed to explaining the susceptibility to overeating among late eaters. These results suggest that the timing of food intake and eating behaviour traits are important determinants to consider in obesity treatment and prevention.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because it will be made available upon request pending approval from the authors as well as the funding agency. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to dmlja3kuZHJhcGVhdUBmc2UudWxhdmFsLmNh.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Board of Université Laval. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in their respective study.

Author contributions

VD and RJ designed research. RJ collected data related to the timing of food intake, analyzed data, and wrote the first draught of the manuscript. VD had primary responsibility for the final content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Data collection that allowed the present study was partly funded by a grant of the Ministère de l’Enseignement supérieur, de la Recherche, de la Science et de la Technologie du Québec. RJ is the recipient of PhD scholarships from the Fonds de recherche du Québec–Santé (FRQS) and Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR, MFE-171302). AT is the holder of the Canada Research Chair in Environment and Energy Balance. MEM holds a Canada Research Chair (Tier 1) in Physical Activity and Juvenile Obesity (950-232353). The funding agencies were not involved in designing and conducting the study, analyzing and interpreting the data or preparing and reviewing the manuscript before submission.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Christian Couture for database management.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2023.1155971/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

TEI, total energy intake; Percent TEI, percentage of total energy intake; TFEQ, Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire; rEI, self-reported energy intake; TEE, total energy expenditure; RMR, resting metabolic rate; PAL, physical activity level.

References

1. Garaulet, M, and Gomez-Abellan, P. Timing of food intake and obesity: a novel association. Physiol Behav. (2014) 134:44–50. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Chellappa, SL, Vujovic, N, Williams, JS, and Scheer, F. Impact of circadian disruption on cardiovascular function and disease. Trends Endocrinol Metab. (2019) 30:767–79. doi: 10.1016/j.tem.2019.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lopez-Minguez, J, Gomez-Abellan, P, and Garaulet, M, Timing of Breakfast, Lunch, and Dinner. Effects on obesity and metabolic risk. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2624. doi: 10.3390/nu11112624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bo, S, Musso, G, Beccuti, G, Fadda, M, Fedele, D, Gambino, R, et al. Consuming more of daily caloric intake at dinner predisposes to obesity. A 6-year population-based prospective cohort study. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e108467. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0108467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Maukonen, M, Kanerva, N, Partonen, T, and Mannisto, S. Chronotype and energy intake timing in relation to changes in anthropometrics: a 7-year follow-up study in adults. Chronobiol Int. (2019) 36:27–41. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1515772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Wang, JB, Patterson, RE, Ang, A, Emond, JA, Shetty, N, and Arab, L. Timing of energy intake during the day is associated with the risk of obesity in adults. J Hum Nutr Diet. (2014) 27:255–62. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12141

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Xiao, Q, Garaulet, M, and Scheer, F. Meal timing and obesity: interactions with macronutrient intake and chronotype. Int J Obes. (2019) 43:1701–11. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0284-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. McHill, AW, Czeisler, CA, Phillips, AJK, Keating, L, Barger, LK, Garaulet, M, et al. Caloric and macronutrient intake differ with circadian phase and between lean and overweight young adults. Nutrients. (2019) 11:587. doi: 10.3390/nu11030587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. McHill, AW, Phillips, AJ, Czeisler, CA, Keating, L, Yee, K, Barger, LK, et al. Later circadian timing of food intake is associated with increased body fat. Am J Clin Nutr. (2017) 106:1213–9. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.117.161588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Hermenegildo, Y, Lopez-Garcia, E, Garcia-Esquinas, E, Perez-Tasigchana, RF, Rodriguez-Artalejo, F, and Guallar-Castillon, P. Distribution of energy intake throughout the day and weight gain: a population-based cohort study in Spain. Br J Nutr. (2016) 115:2003–10. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516000891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Raynor, HA, Li, F, and Cardoso, C. Daily pattern of energy distribution and weight loss. Physiol Behav. (2018) 192:167–72. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.02.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Jakubowicz, D, Barnea, M, Wainstein, J, and Froy, O. High caloric intake at breakfast vs. dinner differentially influences weight loss of overweight and obese women. Obesity (Silver Spring). (2013) 21:2504–12. doi: 10.1002/oby.20460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dashti, HS, Gomez-Abellan, P, Qian, J, Esteban, A, Morales, E, Scheer, F, et al. Late eating is associated with cardiometabolic risk traits, obesogenic behaviors, and impaired weight loss. Am J Clin Nutr. (2021) 113:154–61. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqaa264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Garaulet, M, Gomez-Abellan, P, Alburquerque-Bejar, JJ, Lee, YC, Ordovas, JM, and Scheer, FA. Timing of food intake predicts weight loss effectiveness. Int J Obes. (2013) 37:604–11. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Lombardo, M, Bellia, A, Padua, E, Annino, G, Guglielmi, V, D’Adamo, M, et al. Morning meal more efficient for fat loss in a 3-month lifestyle intervention. J Am Coll Nutr. (2014) 33:198–205. doi: 10.1080/07315724.2013.863169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Madjd, A, Taylor, MA, Delavari, A, Malekzadeh, R, Macdonald, IA, and Farshchi, HR. Effects of consuming later evening meal v. earlier evening meal on weight loss during a weight loss diet: a randomised clinical trial. Br J Nutr. (2021) 126:632–40. doi: 10.1017/S0007114520004456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. de Castro, JM. The time of day of food intake influences overall intake in humans. J Nutr. (2004) 134:104–11. doi: 10.1093/jn/134.1.104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Baron, KG, Reid, KJ, Kern, AS, and Zee, PC. Role of sleep timing in caloric intake and BMI. Obesity (Silver Spring). (2011) 19:1374–81. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tani, Y, Asakura, K, Sasaki, S, Hirota, N, Notsu, A, Todoriki, H, et al. Higher proportion of total and fat energy intake during the morning may reduce absolute intake of energy within the day. An observational study in free-living Japanese adults. Appetite. (2015) 92:66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.04.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Dattilo, M, Crispim, CA, Zimberg, IZ, Tufik, S, and de Mello, MT. Meal distribution across the day and its relationship with body composition. Biol Rhythm Res. (2011) 42:119–29. doi: 10.1080/09291011003729270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ruiz-Lozano, T, Vidal, J, de Hollanda, A, Scheer, F, Garaulet, M, and Izquierdo-Pulido, M. Timing of food intake is associated with weight loss evolution in severe obese patients after bariatric surgery. Clin Nutr. (2016) 35:1308–14. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2016.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Allison, KC, Hopkins, CM, Ruggieri, M, Spaeth, AM, Ahima, RS, Zhang, Z, et al. Prolonged, controlled daytime versus delayed eating impacts weight and metabolism. Curr Biol. (2021) 31:908. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.01.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ruddick-Collins, LC, Johnston, JD, Morgan, PJ, and Johnstone, AM. The big breakfast study: Chrono-nutrition influence on energy expenditure and bodyweight. Nutr Bull. (2018) 43:174–83. doi: 10.1111/nbu.12323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Shaw, E, Leung, GKW, Jong, J, Coates, AM, Davis, R, Blair, M, et al. The impact of time of day on energy expenditure: implications for long-term energy balance. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2383. doi: 10.3390/nu11102383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ruddick-Collins, LC, Morgan, PJ, Fyfe, CL, Filipe, JAN, Horgan, GW, Westerterp, KR, et al. Timing of daily calorie loading affects appetite and hunger responses without changes in energy metabolism in healthy subjects with obesity. Cell Metab. (2022) 34:1472–85.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2022.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Vujović, N, Piron, MJ, Qian, J, Chellappa, SL, Nedeltcheva, A, Barr, D, et al. Late isocaloric eating increases hunger, decreases energy expenditure, and modifies metabolic pathways in adults with overweight and obesity. Cell Metab. (2022) 34:1486–98.e7. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2022.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Huang, TT, Roberts, SB, Howarth, NC, and McCrory, MA. Effect of screening out implausible energy intake reports on relationships between diet and BMI. Obes Res. (2005) 13:1205–17. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Livingstone, MB, and Black, AE. Markers of the validity of reported energy intake. J Nutr. (2003) 133:895S–920S. doi: 10.1093/jn/133.3.895S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kanerva, N, Kronholm, E, Partonen, T, Ovaskainen, ML, Kaartinen, NE, Konttinen, H, et al. Tendency toward eveningness is associated with unhealthy dietary habits. Chronobiol Int. (2012) 29:920–7. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2012.699128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Maukonen, M, Kanerva, N, Partonen, T, Kronholm, E, Konttinen, H, Wennman, H, et al. The associations between chronotype, a healthy diet and obesity. Chronobiol Int. (2016) 33:972–81. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1183022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Stunkard, AJ, and Messick, S. The three-factor eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger. J Psychosom Res. (1985) 29:71–83. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(85)90010-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Leblanc, V, Provencher, V, Begin, C, Gagnon-Girouard, MP, Corneau, L, Tremblay, A, et al. Associations between eating patterns, dietary intakes and eating behaviors in premenopausal overweight women. Eat Behav. (2012) 13:162–5. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Dakin, C, Beaulieu, K, Hopkins, M, Gibbons, C, Finlayson, G, and Stubbs, RJ. Do eating behavior traits predict energy intake and body mass index? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes Rev. (2023) 24:e13515. doi: 10.1111/obr.13515

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jacob, R, Drapeau, V, Tremblay, A, Provencher, V, Bouchard, C, and Perusse, L. The role of eating behavior traits in mediating genetic susceptibility to obesity. Am J Clin Nutr. (2018) 108:445–52. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Bryant, EJ, Rehman, J, Pepper, LB, and Walters, ER. Obesity and eating disturbance: the role of TFEQ restraint and disinhibition. Curr Obes Rep. (2019) 8:363–72. doi: 10.1007/s13679-019-00365-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Brunner, EJ, Maruyama, K, Shipley, M, Cable, N, Iso, H, Hiyoshi, A, et al. Appetite disinhibition rather than hunger explains genetic effects on adult BMI trajectory. Int J Obes. (2021) 45:758–65. doi: 10.1038/s41366-020-00735-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Konttinen, H, van Strien, T, Mannisto, S, Jousilahti, P, and Haukkala, A. Depression emotional eating and long-term weight changes: a population-based prospective study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2019) 16:28. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0791-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Provencher, V, Drapeau, V, Tremblay, A, Despres, JP, and Lemieux, S. Eating behaviors and indexes of body composition in men and women from the Quebec family study. Obes Res. (2003) 11:783–92. doi: 10.1038/oby.2003.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Khoury, M, Chamsine, S, Merheb, C, Arfoul, E, Rached, M, Younes, F, et al. Binge eating among young adults: association with sociodemographic factors, nutritional intake, dietary n-6:n-3 ratio and impulsivity. Br J Nutr. (2021) 126:1431–40. doi: 10.1017/S0007114521000118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Nightingale, BA, and Cassin, SE. Disordered eating among individuals with excess weight: a review of recent research. Curr Obes Rep. (2019) 8:112–27. doi: 10.1007/s13679-019-00333-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Arguin, H, Tremblay, A, Blundell, JE, Despres, JP, Richard, D, Lamarche, B, et al. Impact of a non-restrictive satiating diet on anthropometrics, satiety responsiveness and eating behaviour traits in obese men displaying a high or a low satiety phenotype. Br J Nutr. (2017) 118:750–60. doi: 10.1017/S0007114517002549

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Major, GC, Alarie, F, Dore, J, Phouttama, S, and Tremblay, A. Supplementation with calcium + vitamin D enhances the beneficial effect of weight loss on plasma lipid and lipoprotein concentrations. Am J Clin Nutr. (2007) 85:54–9. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.1.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Major, GC, Doucet, E, Jacqmain, M, St-Onge, M, Bouchard, C, and Tremblay, A. Multivitamin and dietary supplements, body weight and appetite: results from a cross-sectional and a randomised double-blind placebo-controlled study. Br J Nutr. (2008) 99:1157–67. doi: 10.1017/S0007114507853335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sanchez, M, Darimont, C, Drapeau, V, Emady-Azar, S, Lepage, M, Rezzonico, E, et al. Effect of Lactobacillus rhamnosus CGMCC1.3724 supplementation on weight loss and maintenance in obese men and women. Br J Nutr. (2014) 111:1507–19. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513003875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. The Airlie (VA) Consensus Conference. Standardization of anthropometric measurements. Champaign. IL: Human Kinetics. doi (1988).

Google Scholar

46. Tremblay, A, Sévigny, J, Leblanc, C, and Bouchard, C. The reproducibility of a three-day dietary record. Nutr Res. (1983) 3:819–30. doi: 10.1016/s0271-5317(83)80035-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nutrific. Département des sciences des aliments et de nutrition, Faculté des sciences de l’agriculture et de l’alimentation, Université Laval. (2008).

Google Scholar

48. Health Canada Canadian nutrient file. Ottawa, Canada (1997).

Google Scholar

49. Health Canada Canadian nutrient file. Ottawa, Canada (2005).

Google Scholar

50. Vatanparast, H, Islam, N, Masoodi, H, Shafiee, M, Patil, RP, Smith, J, et al. Time, location and frequency of snack consumption in different age groups of Canadians. Nutr J. (2020) 19:85. doi: 10.1186/s12937-020-00600-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Vatanparast, H, Islam, N, Patil, RP, Shafiee, M, Smith, J, and Whiting, S. Snack consumption patterns among Canadians. Nutrients. (2019) 11:1152. doi: 10.3390/nu11051152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Nishi, SK, Jessri, M, and L’Abbe, M. Assessing the dietary habits of Canadians by eating location and occasion: findings from the Canadian community health survey, cycle 2.2. Nutrients. (2018) 10:682. doi: 10.3390/nu10060682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jacob, R, Tremblay, A, Panahi, S, Provencher, V, and Drapeau, V. Is the timing of food intake a potential indicator of low weight loss responders? A secondary analysis of three weight loss studies. Clin Obes. (2020) 10:e12360. doi: 10.1111/cob.12360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Llunch, A. Identification des conduites alimentaires par approches nutritionnelles et psychomériques: Implications thérapeutiques et prévention dans l’obésité humaine [identification of food intake behaviors by nutritional and psychometric means: Implications for prevention and treatment of human obesity]. France: Université Henri Poincaré (1995).

Google Scholar

55. Gormally, J, Black, S, Daston, S, and Rardin, D. The assessment of binge eating severity among obese persons. Addict Behav. (1982) 7:47–55. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90024-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Westenhoefer, J, Stunkard, AJ, and Pudel, V. Validation of the flexible and rigid control dimensions of dietary restraint. Int J Eat Disord. (1999) 26:53–64. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199907)26:1<53::aid-eat7>3.0.co;2-n

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Bond, MJ, McDowell, AJ, and Wilkinson, JY. The measurement of dietary restraint, disinhibition and hunger: an examination of the factor structure of the three factor eating questionnaire (TFEQ). Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2001) 25:900–6. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Cohen, S, Kamarck, T, and Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. (1983) 24:385–96. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Cohen, S, and Williamson, G. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States In: S Spacapan and S Oskamp, editors. The social psychology of health. Newbury Park, CA: Sage (1988)

Google Scholar

60. Spielberger, CD, Gorsuch, RL, Lushene, PR, Vagg, PR, and Jacobs, AG. Manual of the state-trait anxiety inventory (form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, inc (1983).

Google Scholar

61. Beck, AT, Steer, RA, and Carbin, MG. Psychometric properties of the Beck depression inventory: twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (1988) 8:77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Buysse, DJ, Reynolds, CF 3rd, Monk, TH, Berman, SR, and Kupfer, DJ. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. (1989) 28:193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Banna, JC, McCrory, MA, Fialkowski, MK, and Boushey, C. Examining plausibility of self-reported energy intake data: considerations for method selection. Front Nutr. (2017) 4:45. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2017.00045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Institute of Medicine. Panel on macronutrients. Dietary reference intakes for energy, carbohydrate, fiber, fat, fatty acids, cholesterol, protein, and amino acids. Washington, DC: National Academies Press (2005).

Google Scholar

65. Tremblay, A, Lepage, C, Panahi, S, Couture, C, and Drapeau, V. Adaptations to a diet-based weight-reducing programme in obese women resistant to weight loss. Clin Obes. (2015) 5:145–53. doi: 10.1111/cob.12094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Mifflin, MD, St Jeor, ST, Hill, LA, Scott, BJ, Daugherty, SA, and Koh, YO. A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. Am J Clin Nutr. (1990) 51:241–7. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/51.2.241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Frankenfield, D, Roth-Yousey, L, and Compher, C. Comparison of predictive equations for resting metabolic rate in healthy nonobese and obese adults: a systematic review. J Am Diet Assoc. (2005) 105:775–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2005.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Garriguet, D. Accounting for misreporting when comparing energy intake across time in Canada. Health Rep. (2018) 29:3–12.

Google Scholar

69. Black, AE. Critical evaluation of energy intake using the Goldberg cut-off for energy intake:basal metabolic rate. A practical guide to its calculation, use and limitations. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. (2000) 24:1119–30. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0801376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Garriguet, D. Impact of identifying plausible respondents on the under-reporting of energy intake in the Canadian community health survey. Health Rep. (2008) 19:47–55.

Google Scholar

71. Jessri, M, Lou, WY, and L’Abbe, MR. Evaluation of different methods to handle misreporting in obesity research: evidence from the Canadian national nutrition survey. Br J Nutr. (2016) 115:147–59. doi: 10.1017/S0007114515004237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Funder DC Ozer, DJ. Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense. Adv Meth Pract in Psychol Sci. (2019) 2:156–68. doi: 10.1177/2515245919847202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Hayes, AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. 2nd ed. New York City: Guildford Press (2017). 692 p.

Google Scholar

74. Orpana, HM, Tremblay, MS, and Fines, P. Trends in weight change among Canadian adults. Health Rep. (2007) 18:9–16.

Google Scholar

75. Scheer, FA, Morris, CJ, and Shea, SA. The internal circadian clock increases hunger and appetite in the evening independent of food intake and other behaviors. Obesity (Silver Spring). (2013) 21:421–3. doi: 10.1002/oby.20351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Qian, J, Morris, CJ, Caputo, R, Garaulet, M, and Scheer, F. Ghrelin is impacted by the endogenous circadian system and by circadian misalignment in humans. Int J Obes. (2019) 43:1644–9. doi: 10.1038/s41366-018-0208-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Beaulieu, K, Oustric, P, Alkahtani, S, Alhussain, M, Pedersen, H, Quist, JS, et al. Impact of meal timing and Chronotype on food reward and appetite control in young adults. Nutrients. (2020) 12:1506. doi: 10.3390/nu12051506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Dashti, HS, Scheer, F, Saxena, R, and Garaulet, M. Timing of food intake: identifying contributing factors to design effective interventions. Adv Nutr. (2019) 10:606–20. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmy131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Mazri, FH, Manaf, ZA, Shahar, S, and Mat Ludin, AF. The association between Chronotype and dietary pattern among adults: a scoping review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 17:68. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17010068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Schubert, E, and Randler, C. Association between chronotype and the constructs of the three-factor-eating-questionnaire. Appetite. (2008) 51:501–5. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Walker, RJ, Christopher, AN, Wieth, MB, and Buchanan, J. Personality, time-of-day preference, and eating behavior: the mediational role of morning-eveningness. Pers Individ Dif. (2015) 77:13–7. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Millar, BM. Clocking self-regulation: why time of day matters for health psychology. Health Psychol Rev. (2017) 11:345–57. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1316673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Goldschmidt, AB, Crosby, RD, Cao, L, Pearson, CM, Utzinger, LM, Pacanowski, CR, et al. Contextual factors associated with eating in the absence of hunger among adults with obesity. Eat Behav. (2017) 26:33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Herman, CP. The social facilitation of eating. Rev Appetite. (2015) 86:61–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.09.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Bryant, EJ, King, NA, and Blundell, JE. Disinhibition: its effects on appetite and weight regulation. Obes Rev. (2008) 9:409–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00426.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Dashti, HS, Merino, J, Lane, JM, Song, Y, Smith, CE, Tanaka, T, et al. Genome-wide association study of breakfast skipping links clock regulation with food timing. Am J Clin Nutr. (2019) 110:473–84. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqz076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Vartanian, LR, Herman, CP, and Polivy, J. Consumption stereotypes and impression management: how you are what you eat. Appetite. (2007) 48:265–77. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Gulec, M, Selvi, Y, Boysan, M, Aydin, A, Oral, E, and Aydin, EF. Chronotype effects on general well-being and psychopathology levels in healthy young adults. Biol Rhythm Res. (2013) 44:457–68. doi: 10.1080/09291016.2012.704795

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Konttinen, H, Kronholm, E, Partonen, T, Kanerva, N, Mannisto, S, and Haukkala, A. Morningness-eveningness, depressive symptoms, and emotional eating: a population-based study. Chronobiol Int. (2014) 31:554–63. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.877922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Vera, B, Dashti, HS, Gomez-Abellan, P, Hernandez-Martinez, AM, Esteban, A, Scheer, F, et al. Modifiable lifestyle behaviors, but not a genetic risk score, associate with metabolic syndrome in evening chronotypes. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:945. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-18268-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Suzuki, A, Sakurazawa, H, Fujita, T, and Akamatsu, R. Overeating at dinner time among Japanese workers: is overeating related to stress response and late dinner times? Appetite. (2016) 101:8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Shillito, JA, Lea, J, Tierney, S, Cleator, J, Tai, S, and Wilding, JPH. Why I eat at night: a qualitative exploration of the development, maintenance and consequences of night eating syndrome. Appetite. (2018) 125:270–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Huh, J, Shiyko, M, Keller, S, Dunton, G, and Schembre, SM. The time-varying association between perceived stress and hunger within and between days. Appetite. (2015) 89:145–51. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. McHill, AW, Hilditch, CJ, Fischer, D, Czeisler, CA, Garaulet, M, Scheer, F, et al. Stability of the timing of food intake at daily and monthly timescales in young adults. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:20849. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77851-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. St-Onge, MP, Campbell, A, Salazar, I, Pizinger, T, Liao, M, and Aggarwal, B. Information on bedtimes and wake times improves the relation between self-reported and objective assessments of sleep in adults. J Clin Sleep Med. (2019) 15:1031–6. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.7888

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: timing of food intake, obesity, eating behaviours, late eating, psychosocial factors, energy intake

Citation: Jacob R, Tremblay A, Provencher V, Panahi S, Mathieu M-È and Drapeau V (2023) Associations of timing of food intake with energy intake, eating behaviour traits and psychosocial factors in adults with overweight and obesity. Front. Nutr. 10:1155971. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2023.1155971

Received: 01 February 2023; Accepted: 28 April 2023;
Published: 30 May 2023.

Edited by:

Reza Rastmanesh, The Nutrition Society, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Naomi Kakoschke, Commonwealth Scientific and Industrial Research Organisation (CSIRO), Australia
Nicola Joy Buckland, The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Jacob, Tremblay, Provencher, Panahi, Mathieu and Drapeau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Vicky Drapeau, dmlja3kuZHJhcGVhdUBmc2UudWxhdmFsLmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.