Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Nutr., 21 September 2022
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic The Present and Future of Chrono-nutrition Studies View all 15 articles

Studying food entrainment: Models, methods, and musings

  • Department of Biological Sciences, California State Polytechnic University Pomona, Pomona, CA, United States

The ability to tell time relative to predictable feeding opportunities has a long history of research, going back more than 100 years with behavioral observations of honeybees and rats. Animals that have access to food at a particular time of day exhibit “food anticipatory activity” (FAA), which is a preprandial increase in activity and arousal thought to be driven by food entrained circadian oscillator(s). However, the mechanisms behind adaptation of behavior to timed feeding continue to elude our grasp. Methods used to study circadian entrainment by food vary depending on the model system and the laboratory conducting the experiments. Most studies have relied on rodent model systems due to neuroanatomical tools and genetic tractability, but even among studies of laboratory mice, methods vary considerably. A lack of consistency within the field in experimental design, reporting, and definition of food entrainment, or even FAA, makes it difficult to compare results across studies or even within the same mutant mouse strain, hindering interpretation of replication studies. Here we examine the conditions used to study food as a time cue and make recommendations for study design and reporting.

Introduction

Given the over-abundance of food in modern society, it is easy to lose sight of the importance of feeding to animals in nature and in sculpting our own evolutionary history. For most animals, food is extremely scarce and being wise to opportunities to eat is essential for survival. As such, the circadian system has evolved to receive a number of different stimuli—i.e., light, temperature, and food—to keep biological processes coordinated and allow for adaptation to ever changing conditions (1, 2). Food is an often neglected zeitgeber, being outshined by light, which is more intensively studied and much better understood (3). The light entrainment pathway works via excitement of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells that project to the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), which is the major regulator of circadian rhythms (4). The SCN serves as a light entrained oscillator (LEO) and its near 24-h neuronal activity is so robust that it persists in explanted tissue for more than a year (5). Application of the same logic used to tease apart the neural circuitry and molecular clockwork of the LEO has not yielded great success when applied to studies of the food entrained oscillator(s) (FEO) (6). In this review, we describe attempts at delineating the circadian biology of food entrainment using a multitude of model systems, including extensive use of rodents, and given the wide range of methodology we make suggestions for improvements and considerations in methods and interpretation of data.

Model systems used to study food entrainment

FAA research has a rich history with early published descriptions of the behavior dating back 100 years (7). In this time an array of organisms of varying complexities have been studied. Honey bees (A. mellifera) have been shown to entrain to restricted feeding schedules and exhibit phase differences in expression of cryptochrome2 (cry2) and period (per) with time-restricted foraging (8, 9). By contrast, studies in D. melanogaster show that restricted food availability is not able to entrain activity cycles (10, 11). Food anticipation has also been well-documented in fish. This behavior was first explored in 1964, when a pre-meal peak in locomotion was observed in both largemouth bass (M. salmonides) and bluegill (L. macrochirus) (12). Other studies have followed suit, utilizing goldfish (C. auratus) (13, 14) and zebrafish (D. rerio) (15, 16), among other species, to attempt to tease out an FEO separate from the LEO. Studies have demonstrated that fish align their activity periods with mealtimes; however, the presence of a FEO in fish that is separate and independent from the LEO is still an open question. Some unconventional mammalian systems have also been utilized; a recent study employed a pig model to show that ghrelin antagonist administration lowered FAA (17). While the bulk of research in the field has been conducted on rats and mice, alternative rodent models like Syrian hamsters (M. auratus), gerbils (M. unguiculatus), and the diurnal rodent A. ansorgei have been helpful in demonstrating the widespread nature of FAA and testing variables such as age and photoperiod (18, 19). For example, Syrian hamsters may express lower FAA under restricted feeding conditions due to a stronger sensitivity to the masking effect of daytime light conditions (2023). Syrian hamsters have also been utilized for SCN ablation experiments to confirm that the SCN is not required for FAA in a rodent model aside from rats or mice (24).

Rabbits (O. cuniculus) naturally nurse their young once daily in the early morning hours, providing a natural system for studying FAA (25, 26). The eyes and ears of rabbit pups remain sealed until postnatal day (PD) 10; in addition, while retinal projections to the SCN are present at birth, its response to photic input is not fully developed until PD12 (27). Furthermore, circadian cycling of clock genes Per1, Bmal1, and Cry1 in the SCN lacks a clear rhythm until PD45 (27). Even with the lack of rhythmic clock gene expression, young pups develop increased preprandial locomotor activity, altered expression of hormones such as ghrelin and corticosterone, and induction of c-Fos expression in several hypothalamic nuclei, including oxytocin neurons in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) (2831). These factors have made the rabbit pup an important model in which to explore the FEO with minimal interaction from the LEO.

Rats (R. norvegicus) have been a vital system for studying food entrainment and have been utilized for lesion experiments that provided many key negative results, ruling out structures of interest. Rats with lesions of the SCN lose their light:dark (LD) entrained rhythms when fed ad libitum, but maintain robust FAA when given restricted food access (3234). These findings were repeated in mice (35), supporting the presence of a FEO separate from the LEO and that the SCN is not necessary for the development of food entrainment. Further ablation experiments have been conducted in rats in an attempt to find a region of control for FAA, showing that ablation of the olfactory bulb, dorsomedial hypothalamus, and thalamic paraventricular nucleus, as well as vagotomy, fail to prevent FAA (3641). Historically these studies have lacked consistency, both in methodology and results, and as such the existence of a single structure regulating food entrainment remains a theoretical possibility (42).

Due to their increased size and complexity, many experiments have been completed in rats that would likely not be possible with mice. In a recent study, rats were taught to press a lever for food and then subjected to restricted food access, using lever pressing as a measure of FAA; the lever pressing showed a preprandial increase, even in animals with SCN lesions (43). Rats have also been shown to be more attuned to entrainment by palatable meals, developing FAA to the presence of a palatable treat provided in addition to standard chow ad libitum (44, 45). Another advantage of rats is their increased size in comparison to mice, making them more suitable for meal withdrawal experiments, which helped to rule out “hourglass” timers for food entrainment (46). Withdrawal of food is a key element to proof of entrainment; however doing so in mice, which are much smaller and extremely metabolically active, causes rapid weight loss and protocols with this withdrawal period are often not approved by local research ethics boards.

Even within the laboratory mouse (M. musculus), there is substantial variation in genetic makeup that has been shown to affect any number of phenotypes (47). Most studies of FAA in mice are done on the C57BL/6 background, with many utilizing the J substrain from Jackson labs; however, the BTBR strain, which is a popular model of autism (48), was recently shown to have superior FAA compared to C57BL/6J (49). Moreover, phase shifting experiments also demonstrated that the BTBR strain was better able to adjust to altered light or meal delivery times when compared to C57BL/6J mice (49). BALB/C mice are believed to have weakly-coupled circadian oscillators controlling their light-based rhythms (50). When these mice were used to study FAA, they quickly developed high levels of FAA when compared to the C57BL/6J strain (51). The inbred 129S1 strain has been tested under 60% calorie restriction (CR) conditions and showed that they redistributed almost all high activity behaviors to the pre-meal window, displaying “exceptional” FAA (52). Historically, it has been shown that C57BL/6(J) mice have relatively weak FAA when compared to rats (35), which begs the question- why has this been the primary mouse strain utilized for studying FAA when other strains are available that are more attuned to scheduled feeding? We suggest that researchers continue to explore alternative strain backgrounds and consider using different strain backgrounds that demonstrate stronger FAA. Furthermore, prior research has found both major and minor variations in the genomes of commonly used strains of mice, such as C57BL/6J (B6/J) and C57BL/6N (B6/N). For example, a common mutation in nicotinamide nucleotide transhydrogenase (Nnt) gene, which is found solely in the B6/J strain, appears to negatively impact mice in metabolic studies (53). Mice with the Nnt mutation have an increased chance of diet-induced obesity due to glucose intolerance, as Nnt is necessary for the maintenance of ATP synthesis (53, 54). On the other hand, B6/N mice carry a frameshift mutation in retinal degeneration 8 (rd8) and in the cell polarity complex component (Crb1), impairing vision (55). The effects of these particular mutations have not been studied for their effect on FAA.

Pendergast and Yamazaki (6) provided an authoritative review of food entrainment studies of mouse mutants, summarizing every knockout (KO) mouse tested (6). As a thought experiment, we describe the idealized mouse mutant that we have hoped to discover through genetic approaches to studying food entrainment. Firstly, this mouse would have normal light entrained rhythms and its total activity levels would be unaltered. Secondly, both its feeding, body weight, and body composition would be indistinguishable from controls. Third, its digestive and nutrient absorption system would be fully intact. Finally, it would have a complete deficit in predicting scheduled mealtime at a behavioral level–both when fed in the light cycle, dark cycle, and under constant conditions (DD or LL). Peripheral oscillations, such as liver gene expression, corticosterone secretion, and body temperature would all show normal entrainment. Whatever gene deletion is present in this mouse would be recapitulated in a conditional KO where this gene was removed from only a subset of neurons, indicating a brain region(s) that serves as the FEO. Alas, we are lacking such a mouse, but important strides have been made in this direction, including a mouse mutant lacking Nr1d1 in neurons that cannot predict scheduled mealtime when fed during the day (56).

Food entrainment methodology in rodents

Since we study FAA in rodents, we are focusing on this system here but the methodological issues described herein can be applied similarly to other organisms. For studies of food entrainment, experimental conditions vary considerably. For example, temporal restriction (RF) of feeding is the most common method, but the duration of food availability has a large range. Similarly, some studies offer a larger temporal window of food availability that tapers down over time while others do not. Calorie restriction (CR), feeding anywhere between 60 and 80% of normal food intake, is another commonly employed method for inducing FAA. CR has the advantage of fewer handling steps (one feeding vs. delivering and removing food) but the disadvantage is that mice can potentially “ration” their food and may not consume it all during a tight window (we have not observed this to be the case after the first few days of CR). Despite these concerns, a side-by-side comparison of body temperature and FAA in mice on 60% CR vs. 3-h RF showed similar entrainment of activity and body temperature to feeding (57). Multiple feedings can also be employed: for example, Luby and colleagues demonstrated that mice can anticipate several meals per day but not with high precision while Petersen et al. demonstrated that rats are much better at anticipation of multiple mealtimes (43, 58).

Regarding RF, the temporal window of food access and when that occurs during the LD cycle varies a lot from study to study. At one extreme would be studies like that of del Rio-Martin et al. (59) that fed mice during the entire 12 h dark cycle, leading to minimal FAA in controls and the erroneous conclusion that a mutant mouse (Pitx3ak) failed to show metabolic or behavior entrainment to scheduled feeding (59). When we studied the same mutant under a CR feeding regimen for many days (>40 of 60% CR), we observed robust FAA in the mutant and controls, highlighting the importance of feeding protocols and duration of experiments (60). Other studies like those of Li et al. (61) and Kaur et al. (62) give a 4-h access to food, but the overall duration of their experiments are quite short, lasting only 9–10 days of RF (61, 62). Such short duration studies can be problematic. For example, when we studied the same orexin KO mouse used by Kaur et al. (62), it had a slight delay in acquisition of FAA by about 1 week compared to littermate control mice (57). However, by 2 weeks of timed 60% CR, there were no differences between groups; moreover, the orexin KO showed resistance to weight loss on CR, making it difficult to interpret this delay in establishing FAA. Another important point discussed by Pendergast and Yamazaki (6) is the use of fasting days both pre- and post RF. For example, fasting a mouse for 24 h prior to beginning RF reduces both fat and lean mass and leads to much more rapid appearance of FAA, even within a few days (63). Some laboratories even combine temporal and calorie restriction (64). Finally, it will be important to examine the effect of repeated testing on the same animals for parameters of food entrainment. For example, LeSauter and colleagues concluded from their studies of D2 dopamine receptor overexpression transgenic mice that this manipulation lowered “motivation” for food, but in their studies they repeatedly tested the same mice in a short- (4 h), medium (6 h), and long-duration (8 h) RF schedule, finding that only under the longest duration RF that the D2 transgenic mice had less FAA (65). Between rounds of RF, ad libitum access to food was given for variable amounts of time, ranging from 7 to 10 days but also including a fasting day in one of these respites (65). Expression of FAA should not necessarily be interpreted as lower motivation and it is important to apply instrumental assays to test this more explicitly.

Does restriction amount or the amount of weight loss matter? In a study using C57BL/6J male mice, we compared 60 vs. 80% CR fed in the middle of the light cycle (66). Both the 60% and 80% CR groups demonstrated FAA within 7 days of scheduled feeding, losing about 10% body weight during that first week and weight loss continued to decline for both groups until day 14. However, the 80% group's body weight rebounded slightly as the study progressed and their level of FAA declined after day 14, whereas weight loss in the 60% group continued and the magnitude of their FAA was almost twice that of mice on 80% CR at later time points when both groups had clearly developed FAA. Thus, the amount of restriction and accompanying weight loss appeared to dictate the amount and maintenance of FAA. However, and quite unexpectedly, examining individual mice within either the 60% or the 80% group showed that there was no correlation between the amount of weight loss experienced by each individual mouse and the magnitude of FAA that it expressed. Thus, weight loss or at least having mice in a negative energy balance appears to be an important aspect of obtaining maximal FAA (i.e., 60% has greater FAA than 80% after day 14), but at an individual level the weight loss is not predictive of the amplitude of FAA. In the future, a study that explicitly tests the effect of starting body weight on food entrainment would be of interest. For example, a cohort of same-aged mice could be either over-fed, under-fed (without a timing component), or normally fed prior to entering a timed feeding study. This would allow for comparison of overweight, normal weight, and under-weight status on induction and maintenance of FAA. It is important to note that some laboratories distinguish between hypo-and normocaloric food entrainment and obtain some measure of food entrainment by scheduled feeding that does not result in a net negative energy balance (67).

Diet plays a large role in metabolism and has effects on feeding behavior. The ability to achieve FAA on specific diets corresponds with a macronutrient restrictive food anticipatory study in which subsequent restriction of protein, carbohydrate, and fat nutrients in rats did not substantially affect locomotor behavior prior to reward stimulus (68). Binge-eating behaviors can lead to obesity and often stem from not only the addictive behavior of eating at a specific time during the day but even just from the sight or smell of palatable food (69). In the past, Mistlberger et al. attempted to observe the potential effects of food in relation to activity in mice by testing rats on two different types of diets. The main difference between these two diets was the lack of a simple sugar in unformulated or standard mouse chow, whereas formulated chow had sucrose. Rats in this study were deprived of either protein, carbohydrates, or high fat diet for about 2 weeks, then introduced to the missing macronutrient and tested for locomotor behavior using tilt cages (68). In other words, a palatable meal offered during caloric restriction is sufficient as an entrainment cue for food anticipation. These findings suggest an independence exists between a single macronutrient and FEO(s), thus diet in itself is not a sufficient cue to entrain any FEO(s).

High-fat palatable meals and “treats” have been shown to be a strong promoter of FAA. Palatable meals are loosely defined as being nutrient-rich with early studies utilizing a “nutrient-rich palatable mash” (45), but have included a variety of formulas such as high-fat rodent diets and treats like chocolate and cheese containing varying levels of sugar and fat content (52, 70). Overall, rats and mice have been shown to develop FAA and binge-type eating with the addition of high-fat meals, with correlating c-Fos activation in reward centers of the brain (44, 52, 71). A recent study also showed that, in rats, a high-fat diet stimulated higher c-Fos expression, stronger FAA, and more binge-type eating than a high-sugar diet (72). That being said, restricted access to a high-sucrose meal is known to activate c-Fos expression in the prefrontal cortex, lateral septum, nucleus accumbens and anterior lateral hypothalamus during the pre-meal window (73). It would be of interest to test whether the palatability, sweetness, and formulation of the diet would influence food entrainment, in particular the amount of FAA.

Factors other than food

Aside from diet and feeding times, there are a number of other variables that have been shown to impact FAA but are not always reported or considered. Overall, sex differences in FAA in rodents are modest, but should be studied and reported. Mice on RF schedules indicate clear differences between the sexes in regard to food intake, body weight, and food anticipation (61). This experiment, which separated male and female mice, depicts males to be generally more active and to have exhibited greater wheel-running activity during RF (61). Another study focused on sex differences in D1 receptor (D1R) KO mice also showed that female D1R KO mice had more severely attenuated FAA than the D1R males (74). In addition, ghrelin levels, the hormone responsible for triggering an appetite, were analyzed to be higher in female than male mice (61), which suggests gonadal hormonal differences can account for sex-related differences in regards to FAA. Supporting this, gonadectomized mice were observed to have similar food anticipation levels over a span of 10 days of RF (61). However, a series of follow-up experiments opposes this finding that differences in FAA are related to gonadal sex hormone differences. Instead, Aguayo et al. showed that singular manipulations to gonadal hormones, sex chromosomes, and developmental patterning in WT mice are not enough to explain the sex differences (75). This study used gonadectomized mice, sex chromosome copy mutants, and masculinized female mice that were treated with 17-β estradiol during their neonatal period and still reported no differences between males and females while on a calorie-restricted feeding paradigm (75). Therefore, while there are reports that indicate male mice to be generally more active than females, gonadal hormone differences alone cannot account for these findings. We showed that sex difference in FAA was actually age dependent. Female mice that were 9–11 months old no longer had a decreased level of FAA compared to male mice (75).

In addition to the loss of sex differences in mice aged 9–11 months (75), age has also been shown to reduce amplitude and onset of FAA. A study comparing FAA in young and aged rats showed that rats aged 24–25 months, being of extreme old age, had a longer onset time and a lower amplitude of FAA when compared to rats aged 3–21 months (76). Another study examined FAA in dysrhythmic rats aged 13–18 months compared to young control rats and young SCN-lesioned rats; here, they found that the aged rats were able to develop FAA and successfully entrain to a restricted feeding schedule, although aged rats with intact LD cycling were not run in concurrence to compare onset and amplitude of activity (77). As it has been shown that aged mice display a decreased capacity for re-entrainment to LD phase shifts (78) it would stand to reason that the same would be true for entrainment to feeding schedules.

Manipulations in photoperiod have shown that the duration of the light cycle has a minimal effect on FAA and that the behavior is able to persist in both constant conditions (both LL and DD) (79, 80). Mice have also been tested in long- and short-day photoperiods, but neither consistently affected FAA (81). Interestingly, skeleton photoperiods enhanced FAA though an unknown mechanism (82).

Numerous studies of the gut microbiome have shown that time-restricted feeding or addition of specific microbiome derivatives (83) are capable of altering expression patterns of core clock genes both in vitro and in vivo [reviewed in Dass and de Ross (84)]. It has been shown that mice given standard chow ad libitum exhibit cyclical expression of gut microflora and that ad libitum access to high fat diet reduces these cyclical fluctuations; however, animals given time-restricted access to the same high fat diet retain the cyclical expression of the microflora (85). Another study demonstrated that disruption of LD rhythms by regular phase shifts resulted in changes in intestinal microflora only when mice were fed a high-fat diet, while mice fed normal chow ad libitum did not experience a similar change (86). In this study, it was discussed that a secondary stressor on top of phase shifting, such as alcohol consumption (87), high-fat & high-sugar diet, or general stress is often required to induce these microbiome changes and they may not occur in an “unchallenging” environment (88). While neither paper examined the gut microbiome of mice on a restricted feeding schedule of normal chow, given the stress likely induced by single housing and food restriction regimens, it would follow that such conditions may also induce changes in the gut microbiome of animals undergoing a “typical” FAA experiment, some of which may be cyclical as seen by Zarrinpar et al. Given the known interaction of the microbiome and expression of clock genes in peripheral tissues [reviewed in Dass and de Ross (84)], more investigation of how the microbiome affects behavior and FAA may be warranted. Initial studies could measure FAA in germ-free mice to establish the necessity of gut microbiota for food entrainment.

Other factors to consider are the activity measurement method and cage environment. For example, we observed that higher ambient temperature is suppressive of FAA (66) while others have obtained robust inductions of FAA and other readouts of entrainment at thermoneutral temperatures in mice (29°C) (89). There are a variety of methods utilized for quantification of food entrainment, including but not limited to wheel running, lever pressing, telemetry sensors, photobeams, motion sensors, and video-based homecage behavior analysis (40, 43, 52, 60, 90, 91). Direct comparison between telemetry sensors and motion sensors have shown that quantification through these methods are equivalent (92). Alternatively, the inclusion of a running wheel in a cage has been shown to increase FAA; even mice with previous wheel-running experience showed increased FAA when given a locked running wheel, although mice without prior wheel-running failed to show a similar increase (90). It was also shown that circadian organization, observed by Per2:Luc expression, is affected by wheel running. Phase-shifting of the lung, spleen, and liver was observed and shown to be wheel-running specific, as an enriched environment alone did not produce the same effect (93). A major setback of a number of behavioral analysis systems is the requirement of single housing the mice in a barren cage. One example of this is in Gallardo et al. (66), where video-based homecage behavior is analyzed using the HomeCageScan software (behavioral definitions described in 88). This method allows for a more detailed breakdown of behaviors than simple activity sensors or photobeam breaks, which has shown to be useful in certain mouse mutant lines. For example, in leptin KO mice we observed an increase in preprandial hanging and jumping behavior but walking was not increased (57). The sum of high activity behaviors was not significantly increased in leptin KO mice on CR, because this measurement is dominated by walking behavior. We concluded that the leptin KO mice had an intact FEO because of their timing of jumping and hanging, but other groups have reported that leptin negatively regulates FAA, making their measurements using locomotor activity via photobeam breaks (94). In this study leptin KO mice had enhanced FAA, consistent with studies in rats that harbor a homozygous null mutation in the leptin receptor gene (95). Administration of leptin blunted FAA in leptin KO mice but leptin administration in wild-type mice had no effect on FAA (94). That being said, there are downsides of video recording behavior that should be mentioned: most video-based methods require an empty cage with minimal bedding, depriving the animals of enrichment (96). In Pendergast et al. (93), three separate measures of activity were taken by way of infrared motion sensors, wheel running, and infrared video analysis of eating behavior. This use of multiple techniques provides several measures of behavior while also allowing for an enriched environment for the animals (93). Given that FAA is likely an adaptation of animals to forage during periods of food availability, providing more forms of enrichment to allow for those behaviors may be ideal. Furthermore, studying food entrainment in socially housed animals has not been conducted to our knowledge, as most measures of activity require individual housing.

Recommendations and considerations for food entrainment studies

Based on the considerations and methodological issues described above, we advocate for the collection, presentation, and analysis of food entrainment data as follows:

Food intake values should be reported as raw and/or normalized to body weight. These feeding measurements should be reported prior to restriction and during, if appropriate (i.e. during RF it is possible to measure food intake whereas during CR the value is fixed).

Although on its own, body weight loss is only loosely predictive of FAA, it is still important to report both as raw data (i.e. grams) and as a percentage of starting weight. Ideally, measurements of lean and fat mass would be presented as well, especially in cases of mouse mutants that have adipose or metabolic phenotypes (57). For example, it was reported that orexin KO or orexin neuron ablated mice had less FAA (62, 97), but when we tested timed CR in orexin KO mice we observed that they were resistant to weight loss during CR and eventually developed comparable FAA to controls (57). Since starting body weight and/or fat stores can be affected by genetic manipulations and will likely affect the development of FAA, these data need to be considered and reported.

With respect to metabolism, measurements of indirect calorimetry (i.e. respiratory exchange ratios) are desirable especially when mutations cause metabolic phenotypes. We are only aware of a couple of examples of studies of food entrainment that measure energy expenditure (60, 98) and recognize the difficulty in conducting them given the cost of obtaining the equipment. More studies of respiratory exchange ratios and energy usage under different feeding protocols and in different mouse mutants may reveal previously unappreciated principle components of food entrainment.

Raw and normalized behavioral activity data should be reported. This will allow for i) visualizing total activity levels, which are often affected by gene deletions, and ii) for comparison to nocturnal activity peaks as a frame of reference. Thirdly, normalization to total activity corrects for differences within lines of mice and also allows for comparisons between studies (i.e. a 30% redistribution of activity to precede scheduled mealtime). In our study of sex differences in FAA using C57BL/6J mice, we observed no difference in premeal high activity behavior when comparing males vs. females, but when we normalized these data, the females clearly showed less FAA and higher nighttime activity (75).

• As recommended by Pendergast and Yamazaki (6), if permitted by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee, include at least one fasting day or meal omission after FAA has been established in order to discern if the mutation/manipulation in question is affecting the expression of FAA or the FEO itself.

Conduct initial studies of FAA in 12:12 LD conditions with feeding in the middle of the light cycle. This can be followed by testing FAA for nighttime feeding as some mutant mice have specific deficits in FAA only for light cycle feeding [for example, see Podyma et al. (63)]. Finally, examining FAA in constant darkness or constant light and/or meal shifting experiments are an important third and fourth lines of inquiry that allow for studying FAA with less contribution from the LEO.

Conduct longer duration experiments or do a fasting day(s) prior to timed feeding for shorter duration experiments. Studies of FAA range from less than a week (63) on the low end, to at least 2 months on the high end (60, 64). As starvation increases activity acutely (52), it is important to distinguish whether mice have a phenotype relating to hunger induced activity vs. food entrained activity, thus adding a week of RF/CR to a study can assist with interpretation of results in combination with body weight data (i.e. when weights have plateaued).

Report strain information and track genetic purity of inbred rodent models to monitor genetic background and possible interacting alleles, especially in the context of KO/Cre transgenic mice made from ES cells on different genetic backgrounds where linkage disequilibrium makes it nearly impossible to obtain alleles on a pure background via backcrosses (47).

Report lab diet formulations. As discussed above, foods with higher fat content tend to be suppressive of FAA while perplexingly, the palatable treats used to induce FAA in mice need to have high fat content (52, 70). It is also an outstanding question as to whether formulated diets will lead to more consistent or stronger food entrainment.

Report sexes and ages of animals. Females are understudied in circadian biology and metabolism (99). The age of animals affects their size and adiposity, and as discussed above can have an impact on FAA (75).

Conduct follow-up and replication studies, both within and between laboratories. There are many interesting papers on FAA that lack followup; for example, a study indicating that the cerebellum harbors a FEO (100), interaction between SCN and the dorsomedial hypothalamus using dual lesions in rats (101), the complete lack of FAA in Nr1d1 neuronal KO mice (56), and the dramatically decreased FAA in melanocortin 3 receptor KO (98), which was not replicated in a separate study by Ribeiro et al. (94). In our own experience, the deficit in FAA displayed by D1R KO mice diminished over time (66, 102). Follow-up studies using modern neurogenetic techniques and/or more refined deletion techniques, using viral vectors in combination with single or dual recombinase systems, could yield greater insights into the FEO using these initial observations as inroads should they prove reproducible. Furthermore, unclear or conflicting results from broad lesion experiments could be further refined with more detailed analyses of these brain regions. For example, c-Fos staining in the supraoptic nucleus and main body of the PVN in the rabbit and rat brain showed activation after feeding, while the posterior PVN showed activation prior to the scheduled feeding time (103, 104). These smaller subregions are more difficult to target and may be treated as a single, larger “region” under less precise techniques.

Come to a consensus on a more rigorous definition of “food anticipatory activity”, and be more clear about whether FAA is reduced vs. eliminated. Could the field agree on a mathematical definition or a heuristic rule for defining FAA? For example, conduct a one-way ANOVA comparing pre- and post-restriction with a significant effect of time? Or a simple rule such as the amplitude of the FAA peak must be at least half that nighttime activity peak to qualify as FAA? (52, 105). Other ideas to consider would be defining the start of FAA to be when activity surpasses the mean, or when an “acceleration” of a certain magnitude occurs (change in activity from previous bin). Given that most researchers collect food entrainment data in different ways, perhaps fitting a logistic curve to data could be useful since there is an upper limit to the amount of FAA that can be expressed. This objective will require a board consensus perhaps assisted by a metaanalysis of FAA or other food entrainment data. Finally, we need to appreciate the difference between a loss vs. a reduction in FAA or other metrics of entrainment. When FAA is reduced, it does not necessarily follow that the timing mechanism, or the FEO, is not working. As we collect more examples of mutations and manipulations that lower FAA, it will be important to measure other aspects of entrainment and potential masking effects (i.e., studies under DD, for example). In the future, it will be important to move beyond activity alone (as measured by running wheels, photobeam breaks, or computer vision) and try to assay motivation to feed using instrumental behavior assays (nose-poking, lever pressing, etc) (43). If we find that all mice with reduced FAA also have reduced instrumental behavior then we can simply use activity, but we cannot make this correlation until we have more examples of animals lacking FAA.

Author contributions

AS conceived of the manuscript. JT and AS wrote it together. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences of the National Institutes of Health (SC3GM125570 to AS). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Dr. Ralph Mistlberger for helpful comments and suggestions on the preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Author disclaimer

The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

References

1. Mistlberger RE. Food as circadian time cue for appetitive behavior. F1000Res. (2020) 9:61. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.20829.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Tang Q, Assali DR, Güler AD, Steele AD. Dopamine systems and biological rhythms: let's get a move on. Front Integr Neurosci. (2022) 16:957193. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.957193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stephan FK. The “Other” circadian system: food as a Zeitgeber. J Biol Rhythms. (2002) 17:284–92. doi: 10.1177/074873002129002591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Welsh DK, Takahashi JS, Kay SA. Suprachiasmatic nucleus: cell autonomy and network properties. Annu Rev Physiol. (2010) 72:551–77. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021909-135919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Yoo S-H, Yamazaki S, Lowrey PL, Shimomura K, Ko CH, Buhr ED, et al. PERIOD2::LUCIFERASE real-time reporting of circadian dynamics reveals persistent circadian oscillations in mouse peripheral tissues. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2004) 101:5339–46. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308709101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pendergast JS, Yamazaki S. The mysterious food-entrainable oscillator: insights from mutant and engineered mouse models. J Biol Rhythms. (2018) 33:458–74. doi: 10.1177/0748730418789043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Richter CP. A behavioristic study of the activity of the rat. Comp Psychol Monogr. (1922) 1:1–54.

Google Scholar

8. Frisch B, Aschoff J. Circadian rhythms in honeybees: entrainment by feeding cycles. Physiol Entomol. (1987) 12:41–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-3032.1987.tb00722.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Jain R, Brockmann A. Time-restricted foraging under natural light/dark condition shifts the molecular clock in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Chronobiol Int. (2018) 35:1723–34. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2018.1509867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Oishi K, Shiota M, Sakamoto K, Kasamatsu M, Ishida N. Feeding is not a more potent Zeitgeber than the light-dark cycle in Drosophila: NeuroReport. (2004) 15:739–43. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200403220-00034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Singh VJ, Potdar S, Sheeba V. Effects of food availability cycles on phase and period of activity-rest rhythm in Drosophila melanogaster. J Biol Rhythms. (2022) 074873042211112. doi: 10.1177/07487304221111287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Davis RE. Daily “predawn” peak of locomotion in fish. Anim Behav. (1964) 12:272–83. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(64)90014-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Aranda A, Madrid JA, Sánchez-Vázquez FJ. Influence of light on feeding anticipatory activity in goldfish. J Biol Rhythms. (2001) 16:50–7. doi: 10.1177/074873040101600106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sánchez-Vázquez FJ, Aranda A, Madrid JA. Differential effects of meal size and food energy density on feeding entrainment in goldfish. J Biol Rhythms. (2001) 16:58–65. doi: 10.1177/074873040101600107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. López-Olmeda JF, Tartaglione EV, de la Iglesia HO, Sánchez-Vázquez FJ. Feeding entrainment of food-anticipatory activity and per1 expression in the brain and liver of zebrafish under different lighting and feeding conditions. Chronobiology Int. (2010) 27:1380–400. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2010.501926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Sanchez JA, Sanchez-Vazquez FJ. Feeding entrainment of daily rhythms of locomotor activity and clock gene expression in zebrafish brain. Chronobiol Int. (2009) 26:1120–35. doi: 10.3109/07420520903232092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Zhang H, Yan X, Lin A, Xia P, Jia M, Su Y. Effect of feeding regimen on circadian activity rhythms of food anticipatory by ghrelin hormone in a pig model. Nutritional Neurosci. (2022) 1–19. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2022.2047436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Karakaş A. The effects of photoperiod and age on food anticipatory activity in Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus). Biological Rhythm Res. (2011) 42:53–65. doi: 10.1080/09291011003729239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mendoza J, Gourmelen S, Dumont S, Sage-Ciocca D, Pévet P, Challet E. Setting the main circadian clock of a diurnal mammal by hypocaloric feeding: Food entrainment of a diurnal clock. J Physiol. (2012) 590:3155–68. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.230300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Abe H, Rusak B. Anticipatory activity and entrainment of circadian rhythms in Syrian hamsters exposed to restricted palatable diets. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (1992) 263:R116–24. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1992.263.1.R116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dantas-Ferreira RF, Dumont S, Gourmelen S, Cipolla-Neto J, Simonneaux V, Pévet P, et al. Food-anticipatory activity in syrian hamsters: behavioral and molecular responses in the hypothalamus according to photoperiodic conditions. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0126519. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Mistlberger RE. Effects of scheduled food and water access on circadian rhythms of hamsters in constant light, dark, and light:dark. Physiol Behav. (1993) 53:509–16. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90145-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Mistlberger RE. Scheduled daily exercise or feeding alters the phase of photic entrainment in Syrian hamsters. Physiol Behav. (1991) 50:1257–60. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90592-C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mistlberger RE. Nonphotic entrainment of circadian activity rhythms in suprachiasmatic nuclei-ablated hamsters. Behav Neurosci. (1992) 106:192–202. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Caba M, González-Mariscal G. The rabbit pup, a natural model of nursing-anticipatory activity: Natural model of anticipatory behavior. Eur J Neurosci. (2009) 30:1697–706. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06964.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Caba M, Mendoza J. Food-anticipatory behavior in neonatal rabbits and rodents: an update on the role of clock genes. Front Endocrinol. (2018) 9:266. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2018.00266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Juárez C, Morgado E, Meza E, Waliszewski SM, Aguilar-Roblero R, Caba M. Development of retinal projections and response to photic input in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of New Zealand White Rabbits. Brain Res. (2013) 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Caba M, Lehman MN, Caba-Flores MD. Food entrainment, arousal, and motivation in the neonatal rabbit pup. Front Neurosci. (2021) 15:636764. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.636764

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Juárez C, Morgado E, Waliszewski SM, Martínez AJ, Meza E, Caba M. Synchronization of PER1 protein in parabrachial nucleus in a natural model of food anticipatory activity: Food synchronizes parabrachial nucleus. Eur J Neurosci. (2012) 35:1458–65. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08051.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Morgado E, Meza E, Gordon MK, Pau FKY, Juárez C, Caba M. Persistence of hormonal and metabolic rhythms during fasting in 7- to 9-day-old rabbits entrained by nursing during the night. Horm Behav. (2010) 58:465–72. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Nolasco N, Juárez C, Morgado E, Meza E, Caba M. A circadian clock in the olfactory bulb anticipates feeding during food anticipatory activity. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e47779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Krieger DT, Hauser H, Krey LC. Suprachiasmatic nuclear lesions do not abolish food-shifted circadian adrenal and temperature rhythmicity. Science. (1977) 197:398–9. doi: 10.1126/science.877566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Stephan FK. Entrainment of activity to multiple feeding times in rats with suprachiasmatic lesions. Physiol Behav. (1989) 46:489–97. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90026-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Stephan FK, Swann JM. Entrainment of circadian rhythms by feeding schedules in rats with suprachiasmatic lesions. Behav Neural Biol. (1979) 25:545–54. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(79)90332-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Marchant EG, Mistlberger RE. Anticipation and entrainment to feeding time in intact and SCN-ablated C57BLr6j. Mice. (1997) 10. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(97)00571-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Comperatore CA, Stephan FK. Effects of vagotomy on entrainment of activity rhythms to food access. Physiol Behav. (1990) 47:671–8. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(90)90076-G

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Davidson AJ, Aragona BJ, Werner RM, Schroeder E, Smith JC, Stephan FK. Food-anticipatory activity persists after olfactory bulb ablation in the rat. Physiol Behav. (2001) 72:231–5. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00417-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Landry GJ, Simon MM, Webb IC, Mistlberger RE. Persistence of a behavioral food-anticipatory circadian rhythm following dorsomedial hypothalamic ablation in rats. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2006) 290:R1527–34. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00874.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Moriya T, Aida R, Kudo T, Akiyama M, Doi M, Hayasaka N, et al. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is not necessary for food-anticipatory circadian rhythms of behavior, temperature or clock gene expression in mice. European Journal of Neuroscience. (2009) 29:1447–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06697.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Landry G, lenn J., Yamakawa GR, Webb IC, Mear RJ, Mistlberger RE The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is not necessary for the expression of circadian food-anticipatory activity in rats. J Biol Rhythms. (2007) 22:467–78. doi: 10.1177/0748730407307804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Landry GJ, Yamakawa GRS, Mistlberger RE. Robust food anticipatory circadian rhythms in rats with complete ablation of the thalamic paraventricular nucleus. Brain Res. (2007) 108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Davidson AJ. Lesion studies targeting food-anticipatory activity. Eur J Neurosci. (2009) 30:1658–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06961.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Petersen CC, Cao F, Stinchcombe AR, Mistlberger RE. Multiple entrained oscillator model of food anticipatory circadian rhythms. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:9306. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-13242-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Mendoza J, Angeles-Castellanos M, Escobar C. Entrainment by a palatable meal induces food-anticipatory activity and c-Fos expression in reward-related areas of the brain. Neuroscience. (2005) 133:293–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.01.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mistlberger R, Rusak B. Palatable daily meals entrain anticipatory activity rhythms in free-feeding rats: Dependence on meal size and nutrient content. Physiol Behav. (1987) 41:219–26. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90356-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Mistlberger RE. Circadian food-anticipatory activity: Formal models and physiological mechanisms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1994) 18:171–95. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)90023-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Gerlai R. Gene-targeting studies of mammalian behavior: is it the mutation or the background genotype? Trends Neurosci. (1996) 19:177–81. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)20020-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. McFarlane HG, Kusek GK, Yang M, Phoenix JL, Bolivar VJ, Crawley JN. Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes Brain Behav. (2008) 7:152–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00330.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Vijaya Shankara J, Mistlberger RE, Antle MC. Anticipation of scheduled feeding in BTBR mice reveals independence and interactions between the light- and food-entrainable circadian clocks. Front Integr Neurosci. (2022) 16:896200. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2022.896200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Rosenwasser AM. Circadian activity rhythms in BALB/c mice: a weakly-coupled circadian system? J Interdisiplinary Cycle Res. (1990) 21:91–6. doi: 10.1080/09291019009360030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Holmes MM, Mistlberger RE. Food anticipatory activity and photic entrainment in food-restricted BALB/c mice. Physiol Behav. (2000) 68:655–66. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00231-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Gallardo CM, Gunapala KM, King OD, Steele AD. Daily scheduled high fat meals moderately entrain behavioral anticipatory activity, body temperature, and hypothalamic c-Fos activation. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e41161. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0041161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Nicholson A, Reifsnyder PC, Malcolm RD, Lucas CA, MacGregor GR, Zhang W, et al. Diet-induced obesity in two C57BL/6 substrains with intact or mutant nicotinamide nucleotide transhydrogenase (Nnt) gene. Obesity. (2010) 18:1902–5. doi: 10.1038/oby.2009.477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ronchi JA, Figueira TR, Ravagnani FG, Oliveira HCF, Vercesi AE, Castilho RF. A spontaneous mutation in the nicotinamide nucleotide transhydrogenase gene of C57BL/6J mice results in mitochondrial redox abnormalities. Free Radic Biol Med. (2013) 63:446–56. doi: 10.1016/j.freeradbiomed.2013.05.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mattapallil MJ, Wawrousek EF, Chan C-C, Zhao H, Roychoudhury J, Ferguson TA, et al. The Rd8 mutation of the Crb1 gene is present in vendor lines of C57BL/6N mice and embryonic stem cells, and confounds ocular induced mutant phenotypes invest. Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (2012) 53:2921. doi: 10.1167/iovs.12-9662

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Delezie J, Dumont S, Sandu C, Reibel S, Pevet P, Challet E. Rev-erbα in the brain is essential for circadian food entrainment. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:29386. doi: 10.1038/srep29386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Gunapala KM, Gallardo CM, Hsu CT, Steele AD. Single gene deletions of orexin, leptin, neuropeptide Y, and ghrelin do not appreciably alter food anticipatory activity in mice. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e18377. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Luby MD, Hsu CT, Shuster SA, Gallardo CM, Mistlberger RE, King OD, et al. Food anticipatory activity behavior of mice across a wide range of circadian and non-circadian intervals. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e37992. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. del Río-Martín A, Pérez-Taboada I, Fernández-Pérez A, Moratalla R, de la Villa P, Vallejo M. Hypomorphic expression of Pitx3 disrupts circadian clocks and prevents metabolic entrainment of energy expenditure. Cell Reports. (2019) 29:3678–92.e4. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2019.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Scarpa LL, Wanken B, Smidt M, Mistlberger RE, Steele AD. Mice hypomorphic for Pitx3 show robust entrainment of circadian behavioral and metabolic rhythms to scheduled feeding. Cell Rep. (2022) 38:109865. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Li Z, Wang Y, Sun KK, Wang K, Sun ZS, Zhao M, et al. Sex-related difference in food-anticipatory activity of mice. Horm Behav. (2015) 70:38–46. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kaur S, Thankachan S, Begum S, Blanco-Centurion C, Sakurai T, Yanagisawa M, et al. Entrainment of temperature and activity rhythms to restricted feeding in orexin knock out mice. Brain Res. (2008) 47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Podyma B, Johnson D-A, Sipe L, Remcho TP, Battin K, Liu Y, et al. The p75 neurotrophin receptor in AgRP neurons is necessary for homeostatic feeding and food anticipation. Elife. (2020) 9:e52623. doi: 10.7554/eLife.52623.sa2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Acosta-Rodríguez VA, de Groot MHM, Rijo-Ferreira F, Green CB, Takahashi JS. Mice under caloric restriction self-impose a temporal restriction of food intake as revealed by an automated feeder system. Cell Metabol. (2017) 26:267–77.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2017.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. LeSauter J, Balsam PD, Simpson EH, Silver R. Overexpression of striatal D2 receptors reduces motivation thereby decreasing food anticipatory activity. Eur J Neurosci. (2020) 51:71–81. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Gallardo CM, Hsu CT, Gunapala KM, Parfyonov M, Chang CH, Mistlberger RE, et al. Behavioral and neural correlates of acute and scheduled hunger in C57BL/6 mice. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e95990. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095990

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Challet E, Solberg LC, Turek FW. Entrainment in calorie-restricted mice: conflicting zeitgebers and free-running conditions. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (1998) 274:R1751–61. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.274.6.R1751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Mistlberger RE, Houpt TA, Moore-Ede MC. Food-anticipatory rhythms under 24-hour schedules of limited access to single macronutrients. J Biol Rhythms. (1990) 5:35–46. doi: 10.1177/074873049000500104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hambly C, Speakman JR. Mice that gorged during dietary restriction increased foraging related behaviors and differed in their macronutrient preference when released from restriction. PeerJ. (2015) 3:e1091. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hsu CT, Patton DF, Mistlberger RE, Steele AD. Palatable meal anticipation in mice. PLoS ONE. (2010) 5:e12903. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Bake T, Murphy M, Morgan DGA, Mercer JG. Large, binge-type meals of high fat diet change feeding behaviour and entrain food anticipatory activity in mice?. Appetite. (2014) 77:62–73. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Espitia-Bautista E, Escobar C. Fat rather than sugar diet leads to binge-type eating, anticipation, effort behavior and activation of the corticolimbic system. Nutr Neurosci. (2021) 24:508–19. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2019.1651104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Mitra A, Lenglos C, Martin J, Mbende N, Gagné A, Timofeeva E. Sucrose modifies c-fos mRNA expression in the brain of rats maintained on feeding schedules. Neurosci. (2011) 192:459–474. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.06.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Michalik M, Steele AD, Mistlberger RE. A sex difference in circadian food-anticipatory rhythms in mice: Interaction with dopamine D1 receptor knockout. Behav Neurosci. (2015) 129:351–60. doi: 10.1037/bne0000058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Aguayo A, Martin CS, Huddy TF, Ogawa-Okada M, Adkins JL, Steele AD. Sex differences in circadian food anticipatory activity are not altered by individual manipulations of sex hormones or sex chromosome copy number in mice. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0191373. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Mistlberger RE, Houpt TA, Moore-Ede MC. Effects of aging on food-entrained circadian rhythms in the rat. Neurobiol Aging. (1990) 11:619–24. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(90)90027-W

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Walcott EC, Tate BA. Entrainment of aged, dysrhythmic rats to a restricted feeding schedule. Physiol Behav. (1996) 60:1205–8. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00215-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Valentinuzzi VS, Scarbrough K, Takahashi JS, Turek FW. Effects of aging on the circadian rhythm of wheel-running activity in C57BL/6 mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (1997) 273:R1957–64. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1997.273.6.R1957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Lax P, Zamora S, Madrid JA. Food entrainment to 4-h T cycles in rats kept under constant lighting conditions. Physiol Behav. (1999) 67:307–14. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00067-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Mistlberger RE, Houpt TA, Moore-Ede MC. Characteristics of food-entrained orcadian rhythms in rats during long-term exposure to constant light. Chronobiol Int. (1990) 7:383–91. doi: 10.3109/07420529009059149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Power SC, Mistlberger RE. Food anticipatory circadian rhythms in mice entrained to long or short day photoperiods. Physiol Behav. (2020) 222:112939. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Patton DF, Parfyonov M, Gourmelen S, Opiol H, Pavlovski I, Marchant EG, et al. Photic and pineal modulation of food anticipatory circadian activity rhythms in rodents. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e81588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Tahara Y, Yamazaki M, Sukigara H, Motohashi H, Sasaki H, Miyakawa H, et al. Gut microbiota-derived short chain fatty acids induce circadian clock entrainment in mouse peripheral tissue. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:1395. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-19836-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Daas MC, de Roos NM. Intermittent fasting contributes to aligned circadian rhythms through interactions with the gut microbiome. Benef Microbes. (2021) 12:147–61. doi: 10.3920/BM2020.0149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Zarrinpar A, Chaix A, Yooseph S, Panda S. Diet feeding pattern affect the diurnal dynamics of the gut microbiome. Cell Metab. (2014) 20:1006–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2014.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Voigt RM, Forsyth CB, Green SJ, Mutlu E, Engen P, Vitaterna MH, et al. Circadian disorganization alters intestinal microbiota. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e97500. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0097500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Summa KC, Voigt RM, Forsyth CB, Shaikh M, Cavanaugh K, Tang Y, et al. Disruption of the circadian clock in mice increases intestinal permeability and promotes alcohol-induced hepatic pathology and inflammation. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e67102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Preuss F, Tang Y, Laposky AD, Arble D, Keshavarzian A, Turek FW. Adverse effects of chronic circadian desynchronization in animals in a “challenging” environment. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2008) 295:R2034–40. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00118.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Szentirmai É, Kapás L, Sun Y, Smith RG, Krueger JM. Restricted feeding-induced sleep, activity, and body temperature changes in normal and preproghrelin-deficient mice. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. (2010) 298:R467–77. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00557.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Flôres DEFL, Bettilyon CN, Jia L, Yamazaki S. The running wheel enhances food anticipatory activity: an exploratory study. Front Behav Neurosci. (2016) 10. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Gooley JJ, Schomer A, Saper CB. The dorsomedial hypothalamic nucleus is critical for the expression of food-entrainable circadian rhythms. Nat Neurosci. (2006) 9:398–407. doi: 10.1038/nn1651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Mistlberger RE, Kent BA, Landry GJ. Phenotyping food entrainment: motion sensors and telemetry are equivalent. J Biol Rhythms. (2009) 24:95–8. doi: 10.1177/0748730408329573

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Pendergast JS, Branecky KL, Huang R, Niswender KD, Yamazaki S. Wheel-running activity modulates circadian organization and the daily rhythm of eating behavior. Front Psychol. (2014) 5. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Ribeiro AC, Ceccarini G, Dupré C, Friedman JM, Pfaff DW, Mark AL. Contrasting effects of leptin on food anticipatory and total locomotor activity. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e23364. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Mistlberger RE, Marchant EG. Enhanced food-anticipatory circadian rhythms in the genetically obese zucker rat. Physiol Behav. (1999) 66:329–35. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00311-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Steele AD, Jackson WS, King OD, Lindquist S. The power of automated high-resolution behavior analysis revealed by its application to mouse models of Huntington's and prion diseases. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:1983–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610779104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Akiyama M, Yuasa T, Hayasaka N, Horikawa K, Sakurai T, Shibata S. Reduced food anticipatory activity in genetically orexin (hypocretin) neuron-ablated mice: Food anticipatory activity of orexin/ataxin-3 mice. Eur J Neurosci. (2004) 20:3054–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03749.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Sutton GM, Perez-Tilve D, Nogueiras R, Fang J, Kim JK, Cone RD, et al. The melanocortin-3 receptor is required for entrainment to meal intake. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:12946–55. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3615-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Kumar TR Reusch JEB Kohrt WM Regensteiner JG on on behalf of the Conference Participants. Sex differences across the lifespan: a focus on cardiometabolism. J Women's Health. (2020) 29:899–909. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2020.8408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Mendoza J, Pevet P, Felder-Schmittbuhl M-P, Bailly Y, Challet E. The cerebellum harbors a circadian oscillator involved in food anticipation. J Neurosci. (2010) 30:1894–904. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5855-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Acosta-Galvan G, Yi C-X, van der Vliet J, Jhamandas JH, Panula P, Angeles-Castellanos M, et al. Interaction between hypothalamic dorsomedial nucleus and the suprachiasmatic nucleus determines intensity of food anticipatory behavior. Proc Nat Acad Sci. (2011) 108:5813–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015551108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Assali DR, Sidikpramana M, Villa AP, Falkenstein J, Steele AD. Type 1 dopamine receptor (D1R)-independent circadian food anticipatory activity in mice. PLoS ONE. (2021) 16:e0242897. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Caba M, Huerta C, Meza E, Hernández M, Rovirosa-Hernández MJ. Oxytocinergic cells of the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus are involved in food entrainment. Front Neurosci. (2020) 14:49. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2020.00049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Caba M, Meza E, Escobar C, Jiménez A, Caba-Flores MD, Moreno-Cortés ML, et al. Oxytocinergic cells of the posterior hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus participate in the food entrained clock. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:19957. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-99266-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Hsu CT, Dollár P, Chang D, Steele AD. Daily timed sexual interaction induces moderate anticipatory activity in mice. PLoS ONE. (2010) 5:e15429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: circadian, food anticipatory activity (FAA), feeding, restricted feeding, time restricted eating, time-restricted feeding

Citation: Trzeciak JR and Steele AD (2022) Studying food entrainment: Models, methods, and musings. Front. Nutr. 9:998331. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.998331

Received: 19 July 2022; Accepted: 24 August 2022;
Published: 21 September 2022.

Edited by:

Yu Tahara, Hiroshima University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Mario Caba, Universidad Veracruzana, Mexico
Manuel Miranda-Anaya, National Autonomous University of Mexico, Mexico

Copyright © 2022 Trzeciak and Steele. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andrew D. Steele, YWRzdGVlbGUmI3gwMDA0MDtjcHAuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.