Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 23 September 2022
Sec. Nutrition and Food Science Technology
This article is part of the Research Topic Strengthening Food Labeling Policies in Brazil View all 15 articles

Influence of nutrition claims on different models of front-of-package nutritional labeling in supposedly healthy foods: Impact on the understanding of nutritional information, healthfulness perception, and purchase intention of Brazilian consumers

  • 1Food Science Post-Graduation Program, Department of Food Science, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 2Department of Statistics, Institute of Exact Sciences, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 3Laboratory of Information System Design, Design Post-Graduation Program, Department of Design, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, Brazil

Nutrition claims are positive information about foods, which are widely used as a marketing strategy on labels. On the contrary, front-of-package nutritional labeling (FoPNL) aims to make it easier for consumers to understand the nutritional composition of foods and favor healthy food choices. However, the concomitant presence of nutrition claims and FoPNL may hinder the understanding, judgment, and choices of consumers at the moment of purchase. Therefore, the objective of this study was to evaluate the influence of nutrition claims on the efficacy of FoPNL models in the understanding of nutritional information, healthfulness perception, and purchase intention of Brazilian consumers. It was an experimental cross-sectional study carried out using an online questionnaire, with a total of 720 participants randomly divided into four FoPNL conditions: control, octagon, triangle, and magnifying glass. Each participant looked at 12 food packages, which were produced following the factorial design: (i) food category (cereal bar, whole grain cookies, and snacks); (ii) product type (containing one critical nutrient × containing two critical nutrients); and (iii) nutrition claims (present × absent). The comprehension of nutritional information was evaluated through the identification of excessive nutrients, and the healthfulness perception and purchase intention were evaluated using a seven-point scale. The results indicated that the presence of FoPNL increased the understanding of the information and reduced healthfulness perception and purchase intention. The presence of nutrition claims influenced the three outcomes, decreasing the probability of understanding information about food composition by 32% (OR 0.68, 95% confidence interval 0.58–0.78, p < 0.01) and significantly increasing (p < 0.05) average health scores (1.95–2.02) and purchase intention (2.00–2.05). Nonetheless, the interaction “FoPNL × claims” was not significant, which indicated that claims act independently. All FoPNL models were more effective than the control. For the least healthful type of product (two nutrients in excess), the octagon and triangle models were superior to the magnifying glass, regarding the outcome of healthfulness perception. The results prove the efficacy of FoPNL in consumer understanding and judgment. Despite the positive effects of FoPNL, it did not cancel the positivity bias generated by the claims.

Introduction

In general, nutrient declarations, especially those related to excessive nutrients considered critical (such as added sugars, saturated fat, and sodium), are hard to find and interpret and are therefore rarely used by consumers at the moment of purchase (13). For this reason, the front-of-package nutritional labeling (FoPNL) was proposed to make it easier for consumers to understand the nutritional information of foods (4, 5). Besides improving the comprehension of nutritional information, FoPNL encourages healthier food choices as well as product reformulations by the industry toward less nutritionally unbalanced options (6). Thus, it may aid in the reduction of obesity risk and non-communicable diseases (7).

As recommended by international health bodies (810), FoPNL has been implemented in different countries with differences in design, type of information provided, and the degree/level of guidance provided for consumers to communicate the nutritional content and relative healthfulness of the foods (11, 12). Among the different models of FoPNL, nutritional warning models, which provide direct information about the excessive content of harmful nutrients, using text-based seals (13), have performed better than other FoPNL models (11, 1316). This happens because nutritional warning models better serve the purpose of FoPNL: to allow consumers to correctly, quickly, and easily identify products that contain excessive amounts of critical nutrients, promoting more informed choices (13).

Several Latin American countries already have mandatory frontal nutritional labeling with warning models (17). In Chile (18), Mexico (19), Peru (20), and Uruguay (21), for example, the black octagon is applied to the front panel of packages to inform consumers when a product contains excessive amounts of critical nutrients (13). In Brazil, the triangle model was proposed to the regulatory body as an option for FoPNL, by design and nutrition experts (22). However, the magnifying glass was the chosen model, which will be implemented on a mandatory basis in October 2022 (23, 24). Although the magnifying glass models can promote the identification of “high in” food products as other warning models, its graphic symbol does not refer to the idea of alert or risks like other symbols, such as the octagon and the triangle, but the idea of “magnifying information, making its visualization easier, and suggesting the search for and evaluation of other information” (25).

A series of factors influence the performance and efficacy of FoPNL models, both regarding design characteristics such as color, size, and position in the package (26), and personal characteristics of consumers, such as gender, age, socioeconomic status, and interest in health (12). For instance, studies have shown that individuals with greater interest in health and healthy eating tend to check the nutritional information available on labels more frequently to make their food choices (2730). Nutrient declarations are not the only source of information that compete for the attention of consumers who read labels to choose foods (3133). Nutrition claims, though regulated, are used by the food industry to attract attention, promote health associations/perceptions among consumers, and increase sales (31, 32, 34, 35). According to the taxonomy developed by the International Network for Food and Obesity/Non-communicable Diseases Research, Monitoring, and Action Support (INFORMAS), food claims are classified into three categories: nutritional, health, and other claims that include, for example, claims related to the environment, such as “organic” (36).

Some studies have shown that the presence of claims on foods positively influences consumer perceptions of products (32, 3743). However, in the literature, results concerning the effects of claims on consumer perceptions and behavior are inconsistent (44, 45). A review on the topic, which evaluated different types of claims (nutrition, health, and risk reduction), concluded that specific consumer characteristics, such as nutrition knowledge and health motivation, as well as product characteristics such as perceived healthiness, are the factors responsible for this incongruity of results (45). Furthermore, the influences of these characteristics may be limited to the food categories tested in the studies, which makes it difficult to generalize the results (45).

This positive influence generated by the presence of the claims is defined as a positivity bias or a health halo effect (44). The positivity bias occurs when the presence of information leads to better evaluations of the products by the consumer (46). The halo effect occurs when the consumer uses more specific information, for example, claims, to evaluate the product instead of other available information (46). These positive effects can generate misjudgments about the healthiness of foods (47), especially nutritionally unbalanced foods (45).

For this reason, some studies have evaluated the effects of the concomitant presence of FoPNL with other resources from the label, such as nutrition claims (32, 39, 40, 48, 49). The results of the studies are contradictory; while some have observed that FoPNL can nullify the positivity bias generated by claims (32, 41, 50), others have observed that the positive effect of claims on consumer perceptions occurs even in the presence of FoPNL (39, 40), and claims concerning the nutrients targeted by FoPNL may hinder the effectiveness of the FoPNL itself (49).

In this type of experimental design, there is still a lack of research performed on the Brazilian population (32) and, so far, there have been no studies in the literature that evaluate the influence of nutrition claims on the magnifying glass model, which will be implemented in the country (23). Therefore, the objective of this study was to evaluate the influence of nutrition claims on the efficacy of octagon and triangle FoPNL models, as determined by the Chilean legislation, and of the magnifying glass model, according to Brazilian legislation, in the understanding of nutritional information, healthfulness perception, and purchase intention of Brazilian consumers.

Given the positivity bias generated by nutritional claims on consumer perceptions (15, 32, 39, 41), and the trend toward the better performance of warning labels compared to the magnifying glass model (12, 51, 52), reported in the literature, the following hypotheses were created: (i) The presence of claims will lead to a positivity bias (greater perceptions of healthiness and purchase intent), regardless of FoPNL; (ii) the positivity bias generated by the claims will be weaker in the presence of warning labels (octagon and triangle), intermediate in the presence of the magnifying glass, and higher in the control.

Materials and methods

Participants

The study was performed with 720 participants who were over 18 years of age, representing the Brazilian population regarding gender, geographical region, socioeconomic status, and education characteristics, using quotas defined according to the Brazilian Institute of Geography and Statistics (53). The inclusion criteria of the study were being over 18 years of age and answering the questionnaire with the use of a computer. This last criterion excluded the participation using cellphones and tablets to ensure label visualization was as close as possible to real-sized products. The exclusion criteria were working in the food and/or nutrition field. The participants were recruited from a panel of respondents obtained from a market research company. All participants consented to participate in the research through a free and informed consent form. The study was carried out between December 2021 and February 2022, as approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (Brazil Platform—CAAE 2395020.1.0000.5149).

Experimental design

This was an experimental, cross-sectional study, carried out through an online questionnaire, to evaluate the influence of nutrition claims on different FoPNL models. It was based on the studies by Deliza et al. (12) and Bandeira et al. (51), who evaluated the performance of different FoPNL models according to observations from Brazilian consumers, among which was a preliminary model of the magnifying glass model. The study was also based on the research by Nobrega et al. (32), who evaluated the effect of the presence of nutrition claims and warnings on the healthfulness perception of Brazilian consumers. In this study, the FoPNL models tested were black octagon, black magnifying glass, and black triangle (Figure 1). Data on three outcomes were collected: the understanding of nutritional information (correct or incorrect), healthfulness perception, and purchase intention (both measured using a seven-point scale).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Models of front-of-package nutritional labeling tested in the study. OCT, octagon; TRI, triangle; MAG, magnifying glass.

The participants were randomized in one of the four experimental conditions: control (without FoPNL), octagon, magnifying glass, and triangle (Figure 2). For each condition, the combinations between the following three factors tested in the food labels were assessed: (i) product category (cereal bar, whole grain cookies, and snacks); (ii) product type (containing one critical nutrient × containing two critical nutrients); and (iii) nutrition claims (present × absent). The combination of these three factors (3 × 2 × 2) resulted in 12 front panels of food labels, by experimental condition, and the presentation of the images varied among the participants, following a Latin square experimental design (54). Considering operational issues regarding data collection that would make it impossible to obtain preliminary data to calculate the sample size, the calculations were done assuming a continuous response to be analyzed by an ANOVA applied to the data of an experiment designed with blocks (participants) that evaluated 12 treatments (repeated measures). The number of participants in each one of the four experiments was limited to 180, resulting in a total of 720 participants (Figure 2). Since each Latin square required a minimum of 12 participants, it was replicated 180/12 = 15 times. Considering a probability of 5% of Type I Error and a test power of 90%, the number of 180 participants would be able to detect a size effect equal to or higher than 0.345 (a moderate one). All participants looked at the same food labels, with changes only in the presence/model of FoPNL.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Allocation of participants in the intervention conditions of the study.

Stimuli

The three food categories (cereal bars, whole grain cookies, and snacks) with a “healthy snack” visual appeal (Figure 3) were selected due to the potential of FoPNL to modify consumer healthfulness perception and purchase intention, especially for foods that are mistakenly regarded as healthy (55), such as cereal bars, breakfast cereals, among others, which traditionally use the healthfulness “appeal” as a marketing strategy (33, 5658). For each category, two types of products with different nutritional compositions were selected, one with only one excessive critical nutrient and another with two excessive critical nutrients (Figure 4). The presence and absence of positive nutrition claims on food labels were controlled. Two nutrition claims were included in products that were supposed to carry such information: nutrient content claim and another nutrient claim related to health, according to the INFORMAS taxonomy (36) (Figure 4). The combination of the three mentioned factors, (i) product category, (ii) product type, and (iii) nutrition claims, resulted in 12 (3*2*2) package front panels with different food labels, for each experimental condition. Table 1 shows the nutritional composition of the products used as a reference to elaborate the labels, with the respective information on FoPNL inclusion and nutrition claims.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Food categories considered in the study with an example of the octagon symbol.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Combination of factors considered for label development: food category (cereal bars, snacks, and crackers/cookies); type of product (containing 1 or 2 nutrients in excess); nutrition claim (present vs. absent).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Nutritional composition of the products used as a reference for the study and their respective FoPNL and nutrition claim information.

The front panels of packages with labels were developed by an information designer, using Photoshop and Illustrator software (Adobe©), to combine graphic elements (colors, typography, photography, and illustrations) from different products that exist in the Brazilian market, fictitious brands, and at least one excessive critical nutrient (added sugars, saturated fat, and sodium). Although the Chilean legislation (18) encompasses a greater number of critical nutrients in FoPNL, only the three nutrients mentioned were considered, once that is how the Brazilian legislation (23) defines it. The added sugar, saturated fat, and sodium limits considered for the inclusion/declaration in the FoPNL were also defined according to the parameters of the Brazilian legislation (23), considering the nutritional composition of the used products as a reference. Once the total and added sugars were not available in the nutritional information of the products, an estimate of the added sugars was performed based on the method proposed by the Pan American Health Organization Nutrient Profile Model, considering the quantity of total sugars declared (59). Even though this method estimates the quantity of free sugars in the product, all the free sugar corresponding to the added sugars in the products are used as a reference (cereal bars and sweet whole grain cookies). The FoPNL models were applied to the top-right corner of the labels, as suggested by the Chilean legislation that regulates FoPNL (18), and the percentage of the main panel area of the packages they occupied followed the model guidelines. The octagon and triangle models followed the proportions established in the Chilean legislation and application manual (18), and the labels with magnifying glasses followed the proportions in the Brazilian legislation (23).

Experimental procedure

The research questionnaire consisted of four steps. In the first one, participants provided some personal information such as weight (kg), height (cm), whether they were responsible for the household grocery shopping, and the frequency of consumption of the evaluated products. Sociodemographic information such as gender, age, geographical region, socioeconomic status, and education was not requested, once the research company already had such data.

In the second step, the participants were introduced to the 12 food labels, one by one, following the Latin square design, and answered three questions:

(i) does this product contain any nutrient in a higher quantity than recommended for a healthy diet? With the following answer options: “No,” “Yes, sugar,” “Yes, saturated fat,” and “Yes, sodium.” The participant could choose more than one answer;

(ii) show how healthy you consider this product by choosing one of the options on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1—“not healthy” to 7—“very healthy”;

(iii) show your intention to purchase this product by choosing one of the options on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1—“I would definitely not buy it” to 7—“I would definitely buy it.”

The third step of the questionnaire was applied only to the groups that looked at FoPNL labels (octagon, magnifying glass, and triangle). The participants observed the respective FoPNL model and answered eight questions that assessed consumer opinions and perceptions regarding visibility, ease of understanding, and credibility of the information, among other aspects related to front-of-package labeling. Such questions were adapted from the study by Khandpur et al. (60) and answered according to a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1—“I strongly disagree” to 7—“I strongly agree.”

In the fourth and last step of the questionnaire, the participants were asked to answer the General Health Interest questionnaire, a subscale of the Health and Taste Attitude Scales (61), translated to Portuguese (62) and validated. This questionnaire consists of eight questions designed to assess consumer interest in healthy eating, which were answered according to a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1—“I strongly disagree” to 7—“I strongly agree” (62).

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) software, versions 20.0 and 28.0, at a significance level of 5%. Differences in the frequency of participant sociodemographic characteristics were assessed using the chi-square test.

To evaluate the understanding of nutritional information, a binary variable was created for each label/product to identify the correct answers, using the answers to the question “Does this product contain any nutrient in a higher quantity than recommended for a healthy diet?” (“No;” “Yes, sugar;” “Yes, saturated fat;” “Yes, sodium”). Since the participant could choose more than one option, an answer to this question was considered correct only if the participant mentioned those nutrients that were excessive. Answers that considered the nutrients in excess, as well as others whose values were not above the ones recommended by the nutritional profile adopted (23), were not considered correct. The percentage of correct answers was calculated for each FoPNL condition. A generalized linear mixed model (GLM), through binary logistic regression, with random effects on the individuals was used to evaluate the influence of FoPNL conditions (control, octagon, magnifying glass, and triangle), nutrition claim condition (presence and absence), food category (cereal bar, cookie, and snack), and type of product (containing one or two excessive nutrients) and their two-way interactions in the probability of a correct understanding of the nutritional information. The model was adjusted for questions about interest in health in which significant differences between experimental groups were detected.

Healthfulness perception and purchase intention scores were evaluated using a mixed model analysis of variance (ANOVA). FoPNL, nutrition claims, food category, type of product, and their interactions were considered fixed effects, and participants were considered random effects. When interactions were significant, ANOVA was performed for each type of product separately. Sidak’s test was used for a post hoc comparison of the average values. The models included, as adjustment variables, those questions about interest in health in which significant differences between experimental groups were detected.

The average and standard deviation of agreement regarding the Likert scale were calculated separately for each question of the opinion and perception questionnaire and the interest in health one. ANOVA was used to assess the existence of significant differences between the conditions of FoPNL, and Tukey’s test was used for post hoc comparisons.

Results

Participants

The samples consisted of 51.7% women and 48.3% men, and the median age was 30 years, ranging from 18 to 73 years (Table 2). Significant differences were found between groups solely regarding the consumption frequency of cereal bars and snacks (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sociodemographic characteristics of the study participants.

Understanding of nutritional information

The understanding of nutritional information was significantly affected by FoPNL, food category, type of product, nutrition claims, and by “FoPNL × food category” and “Food category × type of product” interactions (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. F values and p-values of fixed effects and interactions of the GLM performed to the understanding of nutritional information and the ANOVA models used for healthfulness perception and purchase intention.

The presence of nutritional claims reduced by 32% the chance of answering correctly in relation to the content of critical nutrients (Table 4). The inclusion of FoPNL increased the chance of correct answers in the three food categories (Table 4). For the cereal bar category, for instance, the chance of answering correctly was 63 times greater for the octagon group and 54 and 44 times greater for the triangle and magnifying glass, respectively, when compared to the control (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Odds ratios (IC95%) of the significant effects and interactions of the model in the understanding of nutritional information.

Comparing the products with one and two critical nutrients in excess, it was possible to observe that the greater amount of critical nutrients (i.e., two nutrients) reduced the understanding of nutritional information by 49, 21, and 49% for the cereal bars, cookies, and snacks, respectively (Table 4).

Healthfulness perception

Healthfulness perception of products was significantly affected by all the main effects included in the model (FoPNL, food category, type of product, and nutrition claims) and by the interaction “FoPNL × type of product.” The significance of this interaction suggested that the impact of FoPNL on healthfulness perception depends on the number of critical nutrients in the product (1 or 2 nutrients in excess) (Table 3).

Table 5 shows the average health scores for the evaluated products, considering the effects included in the model, and their interactions. It is possible to observe that cereal bars were perceived as least healthy than cookies and snacks and the presence of nutrition claims statistically increased health perceptions (Table 5).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Average healthfulness perception scores by FoPNL (control, octagon, triangle, and magnifying glass), food category (cereal bar, cookie, and snack), type of product (containing 1 and 2 excessive nutrients), nutrition claims (absence and presence), and their interactions.

FoPNL reduced the healthfulness perception in the two types of the evaluated products (1 or 2 nutrients in excess). However, it is important to note that for products with only one nutrient in excess, the FoPNL models did not differ from each other, all being more effective than the control. In products with two critical nutrients, the octagon and triangle significantly reduced the perception of healthfulness in relation to the magnifying glass and all models were superior to the control. The least healthy products (two excessive nutrients) received lower health scores than the products with one critical nutrient in excess, across all experimental arms.

Purchase intention

Purchase intention was significantly affected by all the main effects of the model and by the interactions “FoPNL*food category,” “FoPNL*type of product,” “Food category*type of product,” “Food category*nutrition claim,” and “FoPNL*food category*type of product” (Table 3).

In the presence of claims, purchase intention was higher in the cookies and snacks categories, but for cereal bars, the inclusion of this information did not change consumers’ intentions (Table 6). The effect of FoPNL varied according to food category and type of product, reducing consumers’ purchase intention. In the snacks category, no statistical difference was observed between the evaluated FoPNL models in either of the two types of products. This also happened in the most healthy version of the cookie. However, considering the least healthy version of the cookie, the purchase intentions of consumers who saw the octagon and the triangle were lower than the ones who saw the magnifying glass (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Average purchase intention scores by FoPNL (control, octagon, triangle, and magnifying glass), food category (cereal bar, cookie, and snack), type of product (containing 1 and 2 excessive nutrients), nutrition claims (absence and presence), and their interactions.

In the cereal bar category, while in the most healthy version of the product, the octagon symbol reduced the purchase intention in relation to the magnifying glass and control, and in the least healthy version, only the triangle symbol had this effect. It is worth noting that in all experimental arms, participants had different purchase intentions among the most and least healthy versions of the evaluated products, except for the cereal bars without FoPNL (Table 6).

Consumer perceptions of front-of-package nutritional labeling models

The opinions and perceptions of participants regarding the evaluated FoPNL models are shown in Table 7. No significant difference between models was observed in any of the questions presented and, for all of them, the median answer was 7.0. Participant interest in health only varied between the experimental groups for three of the eight questions in the questionnaire (Table 8).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Participant perception of the FoPNL models evaluated.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Participant interest in health, evaluated through the General Health Interest Questionnaire, by experimental condition.

Discussion

Front-of-package nutritional labeling will be implemented in Brazil in October 2022 and will become mandatory for the vast majority of food and beverage producers in October 2023. However, so far, there has been little research on how Brazilian consumers understand and perceive different FoPNL models, especially the magnifying glass model, as it will be implemented in the country, as it will be implemented in the country. In this context, the present work compared the magnifying glass model with two FoPNL models in the warning format, in terms of understanding nutritional information, healthfulness perception, and purchase intention of Brazilian consumers.

Effect on the understanding of nutritional information

The results found in the present study are aligned with the literature, which shows that FoPNL indeed favors the understanding of nutritional information (12, 14, 51, 60, 63). The presence of FoPNL, in the three evaluated models, increased the correct answers regarding the presence of critical nutrients in excess in the three evaluated food categories. Bandeira et al. (51) indicated a similar result, in which the octagon, the triangle, the circle, and magnifying glass symbols increased the understanding of the nutritional content, compared to the control, in an equal way.

On the contrary, the presence of claims and the greater quantity of excessive critical nutrients (i.e., the presence of two excessive nutrients) reduced the percentage of correct answers, in the three evaluated food categories, indicating that such information hinders consumer understanding. Some studies have reported that the overload of information in food labels may discourage consumers from seeking more nutritional information on packages as a way to minimize cognitive effort (33, 64), jeopardizing choices and, consequently, the change in consumer behavior (65).

Effect on healthfulness perception and purchase intention

As hypothesized, the presence of nutrition claims on the evaluated labels generated a positivity bias, as it increased the participants’ healthfulness perceptions of food and purchase intentions. The results from the literature, concerning the influence of claims in foods with FoPNL on healthfulness perception and purchase intention, are mixed (32, 33, 39, 40, 50, 66), and some studies have observed a positivity bias of claims, as reported in the present study (39, 40). Such influence seems to be related to the type of claim presented on labels (39, 40, 67). For example, Mediano Stoltze et al. (39) observed, using breakfast cereal packages, that fiber-related claims led to more positive product ratings compared to having no claims or having fat-related claims. Furthermore, according to Talati et al. (44), claims may be less persuasive for foods considered “unhealthy,” which justifies the observed effect in the present study, especially in relation to cereal bars, which received the lowest health scores and their purchase intentions were not affected by the inclusion of nutrition claims.

Although such claims refer to positive nutritional properties, they are frequently presented in products with unbalanced nutritional profiles (6871). This is particularly worrisome, once, as in the present study, other studies have already shown that the presence of claims on food labels tends to increase their healthfulness perception (15, 39, 7274). Thus, some countries, such as Australia, New Zealand (75), and Mexico (19), forbid claims on foods that contain excessive content of critical nutrients.

The inclusion of FoPNL statistically reduced healthfulness perceptions and purchase intentions of the evaluated foods. In products with two critical nutrients, the octagon and the triangle had a greater effect than the magnifying glass in reducing the perception of healthfulness. As for the outcome of purchase intention, although all FoPNL models were greater than the control, for the cereal bars category and for the least healthy version of cookies, there was a better performance of the octagon and the triangle symbols in relation to the magnifying glass. These findings can be explained by the number of seals applied to the labels in the different FoPNL conditions. In the condition of the warning symbols (octagon and triangle), for each nutrient in excess, one seal was applied to the label. While in the magnifying glass condition, regardless of the number of critical nutrients, only one magnifying glass symbol was present on food labels.

This result can be discussed under the aspect of processing fluency, which concerns the ease or difficulty of processing new information (76). This fluency can be influenced by a large number of variables, such as the “figure-ground contrast, the clarity with which a stimulus is presented, the duration of its presentation, or the amount of previous exposure to the stimulus,” and can be objectively measured, through processing speed and accuracy (76). According to Delivett et al. (77), easily processed images and texts provide feelings of processing fluency that can shape people’s judgment of information. Some studies have already demonstrated this effect, where an increase in processing fluency has led to more positive product reviews (7779).

In the present study, probably, the presence of two warning seals vs. a single magnifying glass, contemplating the same nutritional information, improving the processing fluency, and reducing the healthfulness perceptions and purchase intention (in the cereal bars category and for the less healthy version of cookies) of the consumers. Deliza et al. (12) observed that participants needed less time to identify the high nutrient content in the presence of the black octagon and triangle, compared to the magnifying glass, which may also reflect greater processing fluency in the presence of the warning symbols, since this fluency can be measured by processing speed and accuracy (76). Those authors (12) attribute this finding to the fact that warning symbols are more familiar and this has also been discussed by other authors (51, 52). However, the results of the present study demonstrate that not only the FoPNL design but also the number of seals applied to the labels influence consumers’ perceptions. Furthermore, in the present study, FoPNL was applied according to the requirements of the legislation, so that the smallest labeling area for the magnifying glass could also contribute to the worst results of this symbol.

Hodgkins et al. (80) suggest that labels with a data approach (nutrient-specific) require greater cognitive effort from the consumer for understanding, while the criteria-based approach (synthesis) requires less cognitive effort from the consumer. In this way, the use of two warning symbols—as a visual synthesis of two nutrition warnings, could help the consumer to understand the message more than just one symbol with two warning texts. However, a specific study would need to be designed for this.

Combined effect of front-of-package nutritional labeling and claims

In the present study, no interaction between nutrition claims and FoPNL was observed in the evaluated outcomes, contradicting hypothesis 2. Despite the positive effects of FoPNL, as the increase in understanding of nutritional information and the reduction of healthfulness perception and purchase intention, this information could not cancel the positivity bias generated by the claims, which seems to happen independently. Other studies have evaluated the relative impact of FoPNL information and claims on consumers, obtaining diverse results, mainly in relation to the effect of the claims. However, while some studies observe the occurrence of the positivity bias (32, 39, 67), others do not find it (33, 34). In general, the results indicate a stronger effect of FoPNL information on consumers, compared to the claims (32, 3941, 50, 67, 81). It was also observed in the present study since the F values for FoPNL were greater than the ones for claims in all the evaluated outcomes. Mediano Stoltze et al. (39) tested the co-occurrence of warning labels and nutrient content claims on consumers’ perception of products and behavioral intentions using breakfast cereal labels. They found similar results to those obtained in the present study—although the claims generated a positivity bias, the FoPNL presented a stronger effect, with no interaction with the claims (39). On the contrary, in the study by Talati et al. (82), the authors concluded that FoPNLs are more effective in improving food choices by consumers when nutrition and health claims are not present. These differences seem to be justified both by factors that influence the effect of claims on consumers and by differences in experimental design, such as the evaluated categories and types of food. The similarity between the results of this study and those found by Mediano Stoltze et al. (39), for example, may be due to the similarities between foods (foods with a “healthy connotation”) and the nutrition claims tested in both studies.

Such findings help support regulations on FoPNL, providing evidence concerning the influence of claims on FoPNL. According to McLean et al. (41), nutrition claims are only useful when they are consistent with the nutritional profile of foods, which is hardly ever a reality (6871). For instance, Duran et al. (69) evaluated a sample of 3,491 products available in the Brazilian market and observed that foods with nutrition claims were more likely to be high in critical nutrients when compared to those with low content of critical nutrients. The presence of claims in “unhealthy” foods has also been reported by other authors (71, 83).

Consumer perceptions of the front-of-package nutritional labeling models

With regards to participants’ opinions and perceptions about the respective evaluated FoPNL models, the “high grades” found and the lack of significant difference in the results support the efficacy of FoPNL concerning visibility, ease of understanding, and credibility of the information assessed through the questionnaire. Nonetheless, it is noteworthy that the octagon and the triangle models, as applied in this study, indicated better results than the magnifying glass, and all showed better results compared to the control in relation to healthfulness perception and purchase intention. According to Khandpur et al. (60), who evaluated consumers’ opinions about the triangle and the traffic light FoPNL models, although those opinions are important, label appeal may not mean that consumers will use labels in a better way. Likewise, their opinion about a piece of information may have a different impact on their behavior or behavior intention, which corroborates with the findings of the present study.

Strengths and limitations

It is worth mentioning that this research is the first to evaluate the influence of nutrition claims on FoPNL, including the magnifying glass model, in the Brazilian population. This study was also carefully designed by applying the FoPNL dimensions as defined in the Brazilian legislation for the magnifying glass model (23) and in the Chilean legislation for the octagon and triangle models (18), collecting results closer to reality. Besides, the sample was meticulously composed of age-diverse participants and represented the Brazilian population regarding gender, socioeconomic status, education, and country region.

However, the present research has some limitations. The experimental design performed online may not represent the reality of purchases in physical stores and actual product labels. Nonetheless, it is believed that this limitation was minimized by excluding mobile devices (cell phones and tablets) from the study, allowing the visualization of food labels and their constituents in dimensions close to real ones on computer/laptop screens. In addition, online shopping has increased considerably, which requires understanding how consumers behave in this environment. Another aspect to be highlighted refers to the nutrition claims on the labels. For the three models, they were applied near the center of the front panel of the packages to promote similarity with the way claims appear on actual products. Thus, their application did not follow the parameter established by the new Brazilian legislation, which states that nutrition claims should be placed on the lower half of the frontal panel, with smaller characters than those used in the magnifying glass FoPNL (24). Moreover, the effect of the different types of nutrition claims that were included on labels was not evaluated, to assess possible differences in participants’ associations.

The question about participants’ weight before the application of the questionnaire and the order in which the outcomes were evaluated in the questionnaire may have triggered a response bias. The question about nutrient content, presented first, may have influenced the perception of healthfulness and this, in turn, influenced the purchase intention. Another issue that should be mentioned as a limitation is that changing both the design and the size of the FoPNL, based on the application criteria of Brazilian and Chilean legislation, makes it difficult to conclude which factor was responsible for the differences found in results. It is also worth noting that the results may not be generalizable to a larger variety of foods, once only three food categories were evaluated.

Conclusion

In general, the presence of FoPNL favored the understanding of nutritional information and reduced healthfulness perception and purchase intention. On the contrary, nutrition claims reduced the percentage of correct answers regarding excessive nutrients and increased healthfulness perception and purchase intention. Despite the positive effects of FoPNL, it did not cancel the positivity bias generated by the claims, which occurred independently. However, the effect of FoPNL varied according to the food category and the amount of critical nutrients in the product, with those with two nutrients in excess receiving the lowest health and purchase intentions scores.

Regarding the FoPNL models, all were more effective than the control. However, for the least healthy type of product (2 nutrients in excess), the octagon and the triangle models were superior to the magnifying glass, regarding the outcome of healthfulness perception. Finally, we consider that the results of this study constitute evidence that proves and reinforces the efficacy and benefits of including FoPNL for consumer understanding and judgment of the healthfulness perception and purchase intention of products. Future research that evaluates FoPNL effectiveness on consumer behavior changes, as well as research that evaluates the effect of different types of claims on consumers’ associations and specific research on the understanding of the symbols and attention to them, may be interesting to complement the body of evidence regarding FoPNL inclusion on food labels.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethics Committee of the Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais (Brazil Platform–CAAE 2395020.1.0000.5149). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

SP and LA: study conception and manuscript. CR and CS: label creation. SP, LA, and IR: statistical analysis. SP, IR, LA, and CR: data analysis and discussion. All authors reviewed and approved the final version submitted.

Funding

This study was funded by Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico—CNPq/Ministério da Saúde do Brasil (442990/2019-7), Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de Minas Gerais—FAPEMIG (APQ-00341-21), and Pró-Reitoria de Pesquisa da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Cowburn G, Stockley L. Consumer understanding and use of nutrition labelling: a systematic review. Public Health Nutr. (2005) 8:21–8. doi: 10.1079/phn2005666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Grunert KG, Wills JM. A review of European research on consumer response to nutrition information on food labels. J Public Health. (2007) 15:385–99. doi: 10.1007/s10389-007-0101-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Madilo FK, Owusu-Kwarteng J, Parry-Hanson Kunadu A, Tano-Debrah K. Self-reported use and understanding of food label information among tertiary education students in Ghana. Food Control. (2020) 108:106841. doi: 10.1016/J.FOODCONT.2019.106841

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hawley KL, Roberto CA, Bragg MA, Liu PJ, Schwartz MB, Brownell KD. The science on front-of-package food labels. Public Health Nutr. (2013) 16:430–9. doi: 10.1017/S1368980012000754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Hafner E, Pravst I. Evaluation of the ability of nutri-score to discriminate the nutritional quality of prepacked foods using a sale-weighting approach. Foods. (2021) 10:1689. doi: 10.3390/FOODS10081689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kanter R, Vanderlee L, Vandevijvere S. Front-of-package nutrition labelling policy: global progress and future directions. Public Health Nutr. (2018) 21:1399–408. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018000010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Jones A, Neal B, Reeve B, Ni Mhurchu C, Thow AM. Front-of-pack nutrition labelling to promote healthier diets: current practice and opportunities to strengthen regulation worldwide. BMJ Glob Heal. (2019) 4:e001882. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2019-001882

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. World Health Organization. Technical Meeting On Nutrition Labelling For Promotion Healthy Diets. Geneva: World Health Organization (2015).

Google Scholar

9. World Health Organization. Guiding Principles And Framework Manual For Front-Of-Pack Labelling For Promoting Healthy Diet. (2019). Available online at: https://apps.who.int/nutrition/publications/policies/guidingprinciples-labelling-promoting-healthydiet.pdf?ua=1 (accessed on Aug 31, 2021).

Google Scholar

10. World Cancer Research Fund International. Building Momentum: Lessons On Implementing A Robust Front-Of-Pack Food Label. (2019). Available online at: https://www.wcrf.org/policy/our-publications/building-momentum-series/lessons-implementing-robust-front-of-pack-food-label/ (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

11. Temple NJ. Front-of-package food labels: a narrative review. Appetite. (2020) 144:104485. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104485

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Deliza R, Alcantara M, Pereira R, Ares G. How do different warning signs compare with the guideline daily amount and traffic-light system? Food Qual Prefer. (2020) 80:103821. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103821

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pan American Health Organization. Front-of-Package Labeling As a Policy Tool for the Prevention of Noncommunicable Diseases In The Americas. (2020). Avaliable online at: https://iris.paho.org/bitstream/handle/10665.2/52740/PAHONMHRF200033_eng.pdf?sequence=6 (accessed on April 26, 2022).

Google Scholar

14. Arrúa A, Machín L, Curutchet MR, Martínez J, Antúnez L, Alcaire F, et al. Warnings as a directive front-of-pack nutrition labelling scheme: comparison with the guideline daily amount and traffic-light systems. Public Health Nutr. (2017) 20:2308–17. doi: 10.1017/S1368980017000866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Arrúa A, Curutchet MR, Rey N, Barreto P, Golovchenko N, Sellanes A, et al. Impact of front-of-pack nutrition information and label design on children’s choice of two snack foods: comparison of warnings and the traffic-light system. Appetite. (2017) 116:139–46. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ares G, Aschemann-Witzel J, Curutchet MR, Antúnez L, Machín L, Vidal L, et al. Nutritional warnings and product substitution or abandonment: policy implications derived from a repeated purchase simulation. Food Qual Prefer. (2018) 65:40–8. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Pan American Health Organization. Ultra-Processed Food And Drink Products In Latin America: Trends, Impact On Obesity, Policy Implications. Washington, DC: Pan American Health Organization (2015).

Google Scholar

18. Ministerio de Salud Chile. Decreto 13/2015 - Modifica decreto supremo n° 977, de 1996, reglamento sanitario de los alimentos. (2015). Available online at: https://www.bcn.cl/leychile/navegar?i=1078836 (acessed on April 13, 2022).

Google Scholar

19. Secretaria de Economia. MODIFICACIÓN a la Norma Oficial Mexicana NOM-051-SCFI/SSA1-2010, Especificaciones generales de etiquetado para alimentos y bebidas no alcohólicas preenvasados-Información comercial y sanitaria, publicada el 5 de abril de 2010. (2020). Available online at: https://www.dof.gob.mx/2020/SEECO/NOM_051.pdf (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

20. Ministerio de Salud Peru. Decreto Supremo No 012-2018-AS - Aprueban Manual de Advertencias Publicitarias en el marco de lo establecido en la Ley No 30021, Ley de promoción de la alimentación saludable para niños, niñas y adolescentes, y su Reglamento aprobado por Decreto Supremo No 017-2017-SA. (2018). Available online at: https://busquedas.elperuano.pe/download/url/aprueban-manual-de-advertencias-publicitarias-en-el-marco-de-decreto-supremo-n-012-2018-sa-1660606-1 (accessed on May 8, 2022).

Google Scholar

21. Ministerio de Salud Uruguay. Decreto 34/021 - Sustitucion Del Anexo Del Decreto 246/020, Relativo Al Rotulado De Alimentos Y Creacion De Comision Interministerial, Integracion Y Funciones. (2021). Available online at: https://www.impo.com.uy/bases/decretos-reglamento/34-2021/1 (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

22. Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária. Gerência Geral de Alimentos. Relatório Preliminar de Análise de Impacto Regulatório sobre Rotulagem Nutricional. Brasília: Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária (2018).

Google Scholar

23. Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária. Instrução Normativa No 75, De 8 De Outubro De 2020 - Estabelece Os Requisitos Técnicos Para Declaração Da Rotulagem Nutricional Nos Alimentos Embalados. (2020). Available online at: https://www.in.gov.br/en/web/dou/-/instrucao-normativa-in-n-75-de-8-de-outubro-de-2020-282071143 (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

24. Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária. Resolução De Diretoria Colegiada - Rdc No 429, De 8 De Outubro De 2020 - Dispõe Sobre A Rotulagem Nutricional Dos Alimentos Embalados. (2020). Available online at: https://www.in.gov.br/en/web/dou/-/resolucao-de-diretoria-colegiada-rdc-n-429-de-8-de-outubro-de-2020-282070599 (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

25. Agência Nacional de Vigilância Sanitária. Gerência Geral de Alimentos. Relatório de Análise de Impacto Regulatório sobre Rotulagem Nutricional. (2019). Available online at: https://www.gov.br/anvisa/pt-br/assuntos/regulamentacao/air/analises-de-impacto-regulatorio/2019/relatorio-de-analise-de-impacto-regulatorio-sobre-rotulagem-nutricional.pdf/view (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

26. Cabrera M, Machín L, Arrúa A, Antúnez L, Curutchet MR, Giménez A, et al. Nutrition warnings as front-of-pack labels: influence of design features on healthfulness perception and attentional capture. Public Health Nutr. (2017) 20:3360–71. doi: 10.1017/S136898001700249X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Carrillo E, Varela P, Fiszman S. Effects of food package information and sensory characteristics on the perception of healthiness and the acceptability of enriched biscuits. Food Res Int. (2012) 48:209–16. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2012.03.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cavaliere A, De Marchi E, Banterle A. Investigation on the role of consumer health orientation in the use of food labels. Public Health. (2017) 147:119–27. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2017.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Grunert KG, Celemín LF, Storcksdieck S, Wills JM. Motivation and attention are the major bottlenecks in nutrition labelling. Food Sci Technol. (2012) 26:19–21.

Google Scholar

30. Visschers VHM, Hartmann C, Leins-Hess R, Dohle S, Siegrist M. A consumer segmentation of nutrition information use and its relation to food consumption behaviour. Food Policy. (2013) 42:71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.foodpol.2013.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Schermel A, Emrich TE, Arcand JA, Wong CL, L’Abbé M. Nutrition marketing on processed food packages in Canada: 2010 food label information program. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. (2013) 38:666–72. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2012-0386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Nobrega L, Ares G, Deliza R. Are nutritional warnings more efficient than claims in shaping consumers’ healthfulness perception? Food Qual Prefer. (2020) 79:103749. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Centurión M, Machín L, Ares G. Relative impact of nutritional warnings and other label features on cereal bar healthfulness evaluations. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2019) 51:850–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2019.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ares G, Antúnez L, Otterbring T, Curutchet MR, Galicia L, Moratorio X, et al. Sick, salient and full of salt, sugar and fat: understanding the impact of nutritional warnings on consumers’ associations through the salience bias. Food Qual Prefer. (2020) 86:103991. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.103991

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gil-Pérez I, Rebollar R, Lidón I. Without words: the effects of packaging imagery on consumer perception and response. Curr Opin Food Sci. (2020) 33:69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cofs.2019.12.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Rayner M, Vandevijvere S. INFORMAS Protocol: Food Labelling Module. (2017). Available online at: https://figshare.com/articles/journal_contribution/INFORMAS_Protocol_Food_Labelling_Module/5673643 (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

37. Hamlin R, McNeill L. Does the Australasian “health star rating” front of pack nutritional label system work? Nutrients. (2016) 8:327. doi: 10.3390/NU8060327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hamlin RP, McNeill LS, Moore V. The impact of front-of-pack nutrition labels on consumer product evaluation and choice: an experimental study. Public Health Nutr. (2015) 18:2126–34. doi: 10.1017/S1368980014002997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Mediano Stoltze F, Busey E, Taillie LPS, Dillman Carpentier FR. Impact of warning labels on reducing health halo effects of nutrient content claims on breakfast cereal packages: a mixed-measures experiment. Appetite. (2021) 163:105229. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Franco-Arellano B, Vanderlee L, Ahmed M, Oh A, L’Abbé M. Influence of front-of-pack labelling and regulated nutrition claims on consumers’ perceptions of product healthfulness and purchase intentions: a randomized controlled trial. Appetite. (2020) 149:104629. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. McLean R, Hoek J, Hedderley D. Effects of alternative label formats on choice of high- and low-sodium products in a New Zealand population sample. Public Health Nutr. (2012) 15:783–91. doi: 10.1017/S1368980011003508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Andrews JC, Netemeyer RG, Burton S. Consumer generalization of nutrient content claims in advertising. J Market. (1998) 62:62–75. doi: 10.1177/002224299806200405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. McCann J, Woods J, Mohebbi M, Russell CG. Regulated nutrition claims increase perceived healthiness of an ultra-processed, discretionary toddler snack food and ultra-processed toddler milks: a discrete choice experiment. Appetite. (2022) 174:106044. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2022.106044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Talati Z, Pettigrew S, Dixon H, Neal B, Ball K, Hughes C. Do health claims and front-of-pack labels lead to a positivity bias in unhealthy foods? Nutrients. (2016) 8:787. doi: 10.3390/nu8120787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Steinhauser J, Hamm U. Consumer and product-specific characteristics influencing the effect of nutrition, health and risk reduction claims on preferences and purchase behavior – A systematic review. Appetite. (2018) 127:303–23. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2018.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Roe B, Levy AS, Derby BM. The impact of health claims on consumer search and product evaluation outcomes: results from FDA experimental data. J Public Policy Market. (1999) 18:89–105. doi: 10.1177/074391569901800110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Geyskens K, Pandelaere M, Dewitte S, Warlop L. The backdoor to overconsumption: the effect of associating “low-fat” food with health references. J Public Policy Market. (2007) 26:118–25. doi: 10.1509/JPPM.26.1.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Tórtora G, Machín L, Ares G. Influence of nutritional warnings and other label features on consumers’ choice: results from an eye-tracking study. Food Res Int. (2019) 119:605–11. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2018.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Acton RB, Hammond D. Do manufacturer ‘nutrient claims’ influence the efficacy of mandated front-of-package labels? Public Health Nutr. (2018) 21:3354–9. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018002550

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Talati Z, Pettigrew S, Hughes C, Dixon H, Kelly B, Ball K, et al. The combined effect of front-of-pack nutrition labels and health claims on consumers’ evaluation of food products. Food Qual Prefer. (2016) 53:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.05.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bandeira LM, Pedroso J, Toral N, Gubert MB. Performance and perception on front-of-package nutritional labeling models in Brazil. Rev Saude Publica. (2021) 55:19. doi: 10.11606/s1518-8787.2021055002395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Goodman S, Vanderlee L, Acton R, Mahamad S, Hammond D. The impact of front-of-package label design on consumer understanding of nutrient amounts. Nutrients. (2018) 10:1624. doi: 10.3390/nu10111624

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística.Estatísticas Sociais. (2022). Available online at: https://www.ibge.gov.br/ (accessed on March 21, 2022).

Google Scholar

54. Montgomery DC. Design and Analysis of Experiments. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons (2019).

Google Scholar

55. Ares G, Varela F, Machin L, Antúnez L, Giménez A, Curutchet MR, et al. Comparative performance of three interpretative front-of-pack nutrition labelling schemes: insights for policy making. Food Qual Prefer. (2018) 68:215–25. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.03.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Correa T, Fierro C, Reyes M, Dillman Carpentier FR, Taillie LS, Corvalan C. Responses to the Chilean law of food labeling and advertising: exploring knowledge, perceptions and behaviors of mothers of young children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2019) 16:21. doi: 10.1186/s12966-019-0781-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lima M, Ares G, Deliza R. How do front of pack nutrition labels affect healthfulness perception of foods targeted at children? Insights from Brazilian children and parents. Food Qual Prefer. (2018) 64:111–9. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Machín L, Cabrera M, Curutchet MR, Martínez J, Giménez A, Ares G. Healthfulness perception of ultra-processed products featuring different front-of-pack nutritional information schemes across two income levels. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2017) 49:330–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2016.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Pan American Health Organization. Nutrient Profile Model. (2016). Available online at: https://iris.paho.org/bitstream/handle/10665.2/18621/9789275118733_eng.pdf?sequence=9&isAllowed=y (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

60. Khandpur N, de Morais Sato P, Mais LA, Bortoletto Martins AP, Spinillo CG, Garcia MT, et al. Are front-of-package warning labels more effective at communicating nutrition information than traffic-light labels? A randomized controlled experiment in a Brazilian sample. Nutrients. (2018) 10:688. doi: 10.3390/nu10060688

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Roininen K, Lähteenmäki L, Tuorila H. Quantification of consumer attitudes to health and hedonic characteristics of foods. Appetite. (1999) 33:71–88. doi: 10.1006/appe.1999.0232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Soares LLS, Deliza R, Gonçalves EB. Escalas atitudinais utilizadas em estudos de consumidor: tradução e validação para a língua portuguesa. Aliment e Nutr Araraquara. (2006) 17:51–64.

Google Scholar

63. Khandpur N, Mais LA, de Morais Sato P, Martins APB, Spinillo CG, Rojas CFU, et al. Choosing a front-of-package warning label for Brazil: a randomized, controlled comparison of three different label designs. Food Res Int. (2019) 121:854–61. doi: 10.1016/j.foodres.2019.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Williams P. Consumer understanding and use of health claims for foods. Nutr Rev. (2005) 63:256–64. doi: 10.1301/nr.2005.jul.256-264

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Rojas CFU, Spinillo CG. Avaliação de advertências: contribuições do design da informação para avaliação de eficácia comunicacional de rotulagem nutricional frontal. Rev Bras Des da Informação. (2021) 18:1–28.

Google Scholar

66. Hwang J, Lee K, Lin TN. Ingredient labeling and health claims influencing consumer perceptions, purchase intentions, and willingness to pay. J Foodserv Bus Res. (2016) 19:352–67. doi: 10.1080/15378020.2016.1181507

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Maubach N, Hoek J, Mather D. Interpretive front-of-pack nutrition labels. Comparing competing recommendations. Appetite. (2014) 82:67–77. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Choi H, Yoo K, Baek TH, Reid LN, Macias W. Presence and effects of health and nutrition-related (HNR) claims with benefit-seeking and risk-avoidance appeals in female-orientated magazine food advertisements. Int J Advert. (2013) 32:587–616. doi: 10.2501/IJA-32-4-587-616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Duran AC, Ricardo CZ, Mais LA, Martins APB, Taillie LS. Conflicting messages on food and beverage packages: front-of-package nutritional labeling, health and nutrition claims in Brazil. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2967. doi: 10.3390/nu11122967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Colby SE, Johnson L, Scheett A, Hoverson B. Nutrition marketing on food labels. J Nutr Educ Behav. (2010) 42:92–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2008.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Giménez A, Saldamando L, Curutchet MR, Ares G. Package design and nutritional profile of foods targeted at children in supermarkets in Montevideo, Uruguay. Cad Saude Publica. (2017) 33:e00032116. doi: 10.1590/0102-311X00032116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Saba A, Vassallo M, Shepherd R, Lampila P, Arvola A, Dean M, et al. Country-wise differences in perception of health-related messages in cereal-based food products. Food Qual Prefer. (2010) 21:385–93. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2009.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. van Trijp HCM, van der Lans IA. Consumer perceptions of nutrition and health claims. Appetite. (2007) 48:305–24. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Lyly M, Roininen K, Honkapää K, Poutanen K, Lähteenmäki L. Factors influencing consumers’ willingness to use beverages and ready-to-eat frozen soups containing oat β-glucan in Finland, France and Sweden. Food Qual Prefer. (2007) 18:242–55. doi: 10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Food Standards Australia New Zealand Calculation Method For Determining Foods Eligible To Make Health Claims. (2013). Available online at: https://www.foodstandards.gov.au/code/proposals/Documents/P293_PFAR_Att_7%20-_Health%20Claims%20Nutrient%20Profiling%20Calculator.pdf (accessed on March 31, 2022).

Google Scholar

76. Schwarz N. Metacognitive experiences in consumer judgment and decision making. J Consum Psychol. (2004) 14:332–48. doi: 10.1207/s15327663jcp1404_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Delivett CP, Klepacz NA, Farrow CV, Thomas JM, Raats MM, Nash RA. Front-of-pack images can boost the perceived health benefits of dietary products. Appetite. (2020) 155:104831. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Leonhardt JM, Catlin JR, Pirouz DM. Is your product facing the Ad’s center? Facing direction affects processing fluency and ad evaluation. J Advert. (2015) 44:315–25. doi: 10.1080/00913367.2015.1048911

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Song H, Schwarz N. If it’s hard to read, it’s hard to do: processing fluency affects effort prediction and motivation. Psychol Sci. (2008) 19:986–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02189.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Hodgkins C, Barnett J, Wasowicz-Kirylo G, Stysko-Kunkowska M, Gulcan Y, Kustepeli Y, et al. Understanding how consumers categorise nutritional labels: a consumer derived typology for front-of-pack nutrition labelling. Appetite. (2012) 59:806–17. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Eguren J, Antúnez L, Otterbring T, Curutchet MR, Ares G. Health gains through loss frames: testing the effectiveness of message framing on citizens’ use of nutritional warnings. Appetite. (2021) 166:105469. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2021.105469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Talati Z, Norman R, Kelly B, Dixon H, Neal B, Miller C, et al. A randomized trial assessing the effects of health claims on choice of foods in the presence of front-of-pack labels. Am J Clin Nutr. (2018) 108:1275–82. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/nqy248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Franco-Arellano B, Labonté MÈ, Bernstein JT, L’Abbé MR. Examining the nutritional quality of canadian packaged foods and beverages with and without nutrition claims. Nutrients. (2018) 10:832. doi: 10.3390/nu10070832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: food labeling, nutrition policy, FOP, warning labels, consumer research, consumer perception, health halo, positivity bias

Citation: Prates SMS, Reis IA, Rojas CFU, Spinillo CG and Anastácio LR (2022) Influence of nutrition claims on different models of front-of-package nutritional labeling in supposedly healthy foods: Impact on the understanding of nutritional information, healthfulness perception, and purchase intention of Brazilian consumers. Front. Nutr. 9:921065. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.921065

Received: 15 April 2022; Accepted: 22 August 2022;
Published: 23 September 2022.

Edited by:

Rosires Deliza, Embrapa Agroindústria de Alimentos, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Gaston Ares, Universidad de la República, Uruguay
Venkatesan Arul, Pondicherry University, India

Copyright © 2022 Prates, Reis, Rojas, Spinillo and Anastácio. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lucilene Rezende Anastácio, lucilene.rezende@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.