Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 23 August 2022
Sec. Nutrition and Sustainable Diets
This article is part of the Research Topic What, How, and Where to Eat is More than an Individual Choice: New Ways to Achieve Healthy Eating View all 11 articles

Breakfast and psychosocial behavioural problems in young population: The role of status, place, and habits

  • 1Health and Social Research Center, Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha, Cuenca, Spain
  • 2Centre for Health, Performance and Wellbeing, Anglia Ruskin University, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Physical Medicine and Nursing, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
  • 4Departamento de Ciencias Médicas, Facultad de Medicina, Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha, Albacete, Spain

The aim of this study was to examine whether breakfast status, place and habits are associated with psychosocial behavioural problems in a nationally representative sample of young people aged 4–14 years residing in Spain. This study analysed secondary data from the Spanish National Health Survey (2017), including 3,772 Spanish children and adolescents. Breakfast status, place, and habits were assessed by ad hoc questions answered by parents/guardians. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) parents’ version form was applied to evaluate the psychosocial health of their children. Skipping breakfast and eating breakfast out of home were linked to greater odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (skipping breakfast: OR = 3.29; CI 95%, 1.47–7.35; breakfast out of home: OR = 2.06; CI 95%, 1.27–3.33) than eating breakfast at home. Similarly, not consuming coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc., for breakfast was related to greater odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (OR = 1.76; CI 95%, 1.21–2.55). This association was also found for those who did not eat bread, toast, cereals, pastries, etc., for breakfast (OR = 1.31; CI 95%, 1.01–1.73). Conversely, not consuming eggs, cheese, ham, etc., was associated with lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (OR = 0.56; CI 95%, 0.38–0.83). Our results show that eating breakfast (specifically at home) and breakfast habits related to the intake of certain food/beverages groups were associated with higher or lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems.

Introduction

Psychosocial health is broadly defined to include psychological and social psychological outcomes interlinked with socioeconomic factors (1). There is no accepted definition in the field, although it usually includes characteristics such as self-esteem and mood, as well as affect, such as anxiety (2). Importantly, affective disorders (e.g., anxiety) are the leading causes of illness and disability (3), as well as years lost due to disability (4) among children and adolescents (young people). Most affective disorders begin in childhood (3) and have been shown to be considerably stable over time (5). Therefore, early identification and treatment of such complications is essential during this life period (3). Owing to this, the assessment of affective disorders in childhood has expanded over the last two decades (3, 6, 7).

One potentially important factor associated with an increased risk of psychosocial complications is lower adherence to a healthy diet. In a meta-analysis (8) including 14 studies and 399,550 participants, skipping breakfast was related to a higher risk of stress, depression, and psychological distress in all age groups, as well as anxiety in adolescents. Likewise, another systematic review found similar findings (9).

Consuming a healthy breakfast on a daily basis has been observed to have multiple beneficial effects on psychosocial and health behaviours, such as improved memory recall and cognitive function, as well as higher levels of physical activity, among others (10). Furthermore, O’Sullivan et al. (11) indicated that breakfast quality is a key factor in the interaction between lifestyle and psychosocial health during early adolescence. Concluding that in children, a high-quality breakfast should include cereals, low-fat milk or other dairy products, and fruit or fruit juice (12). However, the intake of whole fruit rather than fruit juices has been discussed because of the more conclusive evidence of the health benefits of whole fruit (13). Thus, the Spanish Society of Community Nutrition (14) indicates that an adequate breakfast should be composed of the triad: (1) dairy products (one glass of milk, one fresh yogurt or cheese); (2) cereals (bread, cookies, whole wheat bread, homemade pastries or breakfast cereals); and (3) fruit or natural juice. Furthermore, it could also be complemented on some occasions with other protein foods, such as eggs, ham, nuts, etc. Thus, it is not surprising that daily breakfast consumption along with adequate breakfast selections have been highlighted as an important public health message (15).

The association between eating breakfast and psychosocial health in young people has been previously studied (8, 11, 16, 17). However, to date, the association between breakfast place (i.e., at home, out-of-home) and breakfast habits in relation to psychosocial behavioural problems in young people remains unknown. Interestingly, a systematic review suggested that the social context (e.g., breakfast at home) plays a key role in breakfast consumption (18). Similarly, eating at home favours the accessibility and availability of different foods, in addition to the key sociocultural scenario that family meals represent, since they provide a setting in which parents/guardians often control children’s behaviours, interact with them, and imposes rules and expectations on them (19). Supporting this notion, a systematic review by Lachat et al. (20) has pointed out that eating out of home is a risk factor for greater energy and fat consumption and lower consumption of micronutrients. More specifically, in children, the consumption out of home (e.g., coffee shops/restaurants) seems to favour a greater consumption of energy-dense foods and a lower consumption of nutrient-rich foods (21). This consumption of meals out of home could translate into a lower quality of individual meals (e.g., breakfast) and/or global diet, which could influence (as mentioned above) the psychosocial health complications of young people (22). Therefore, it could be suggested that eating breakfast away from the home or skipping breakfast per se, as well as consuming a breakfast that does not follow the Spanish recommendations, may be related to psychosocial behavioural problems in young people. Considering these premises, the aim of this study was to examine whether breakfast status, place and habits are associated with psychosocial behavioural problems in a nationally representative sample of young people aged 4–14 years residing in Spain.

Materials and methods

Population sample and study design

A cross-sectional study was carried out using nationwide data from the Spanish National Health Survey (2017) (23). This survey was conducted by the Ministry of Health, Consumer Affairs and Social Welfare and the National Statistics Institute (24). The sampling framework involved non-institutionalised Spanish individuals (i.e., people who live in group quarters other than institutions, such as college dormitories, rooming houses, religious group homes, communes, and halfway houses). A three-stage sampling design was applied. The census section was the first stage, the households were the second-stage units, and the individuals were the third-stage units. Within each household, an adult (aged 15 or older) was chosen to complete the Adult Questionnaire, and if there were minors (from 0 to 14 years of age), a minor was randomly chosen to complete the Minors Questionnaire. Data on the minors were reported by the parents/guardians. The participants were informed about the survey methodology through an informative letter from the Ministry of Health, Consumer Affairs and Social Welfare describing the aims of the survey, the anonymous and voluntary nature of participation, and the visit of a qualified and authorised interviewer.

For the purpose of this study, the sample was restricted to individuals aged 0–14 years old (Minors Questionnaire). The sample originally consisted of 6,106 participants. As psychosocial behavioural problems were only evaluated in children and adolescents from 4 to 14 years, we excluded 1,502 participants under the age of 4 years. Moreover, 832 participants were removed due to missing data in relation to diet, weight or height or any covariate. Thus, the final sample included 3,772 (49.4% females) Spanish children and adolescents. Differences between the included and excluded samples can be found in Supplementary Table 1.

Anonymised data were obtained from the Ministry of Health, Consumer Affairs and Social Welfare (23). Following the Spanish regulations, no Ethics Committee approval was needed for this study due to the use of secondary data.

Procedures

Breakfast status and breakfast place

Using the original question from the Spanish National Health Survey (2017), we determined breakfast status (eating breakfast/skipping breakfast) and breakfast place (eating breakfast at home/eating breakfast out of home): “Where does your child usually eat breakfast?.” The options varied from (a) “at home”; (b) “out of home”; and (c) “no breakfast.”

Breakfast habits

Breakfast habits were determined by the following question: What does your child usually eat for breakfast? This question was applied for five different food/beverage groups: (a) “coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc.”; (b) “bread, toast, cookies, pastries, etc.”; (c) “fruit and/or juice”; (d) “eggs, cheese, ham, etc.”; (e) “other foods.” These categories correspond to those originally designed for the Spanish National Health Surveys and were designed to report what has been defined in Spain as a complete breakfast: hot drink (e.g., milk, cacao), accompanied by a solid food (e.g., bread, toasts), and fruit/fruit juice (25).

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) (26) parents’ version form was applied for the evaluation of different behavioural, emotional, and social problems related to the psychosocial health of children and adolescents.1 In this study, the Spanish version of the SDQ, which has been previously validated in other studies (7, 27), was applied. The SDQ contains a total of 25 items with five different subscales: (a) “emotional problems”, (b) “conduct problems”, (c) “hyperactivity”, (d) “peer problems”; and (e) “prosocial behaviour.” A Likert-scale with three possible options (0: “not true”; 1: “somewhat true”; 2: “certainly true”) was applied. Furthermore, the score on each subscale varies from 0 to 10 points. The first four subscales (hyperactivity, emotional problems, conduct problems, and peer problems) were used to establish a total score of psychosocial behavioural problems. The original 3-band categorisation of SDQ included normal (0–13 points), borderline (14–17 points), or “abnormal” (17–40 points). For additional analyses, the SDQ score was dichotomised into (a) no psychosocial behavioural problems (normal and borderline) and (b) psychosocial behavioural problems (abnormal).

Covariates

Age, sex, region, and immigrant status were declared by the parents/guardians. Social class was categorised according to the reference person’s occupation. Height and weight were also reported by parents/guardians. These values were used to determine the body mass index (BMI), which was transformed into BMI z score (zBMI) following the sex- and age-criteria from the International Obesity Task Force (IOTF) (28). According to the zBMI, participants were categorised as “no excess weight” (“underweight,” and “normal weight”) and “excess weight” (“overweight,” and “obesity”). Physical activity was evaluated by a modified short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (29), with only one specific question about engagement in physical activity during free time. The question has four possible options: (a) “no exercise” (free-time mainly engaged in sedentary behaviours such as watching television, reading, going to the cinema, etc.); (b) “occasional sport or physical activity”; (c) “physical activity several times a month”, and (d) “sports or physical training several times a week” (23). Recreational screen time was assessed by asking respondents for weekdays and weekends independently: How much time does your child typically spend on a weekday/weekend in front of a screen, including a computer, tablet, television, video, video game or cell phone screen?.” The possible options were (a) “nothing or almost nothing”; (b) “less than 1 h”; and (c) “1 h or more.” The proportion of the young population meeting the recreational screen time recommendation was determined through the World Health Organization international guidelines for children under 5 years old (30) and the Canadian guidelines on screen time for the young population (31). Sleep duration was assessed by the following question: “Approximately how many hours does your child usually sleep per day? (Including nap times).” The meeting of the sleep recommendation was determined by the National Sleep Foundation’s sleep duration guidelines (32). Global quality diet was assessed by the Spanish Health Eating Index (S-HEI) (33), which is an adapted version of the original Healthy Eating Index (HEI) (34). The S-HEI includes 10 food groups (vegetables, cereals, legumes, fruit, meat, dairy, sweets, soft drinks, cold meats, and variety of the diet) divided into five categories (“never or hardly ever,” “one time per week,” “from one to two times per week,” “more than three times per week, but not daily,” and “daily”) according to the frequency of food intake indicated within the guidelines of the Spanish Society of Community Nutrition (35). The score on each food group ranged from 0 to 10 points. The total score for the S-HEI was calculated by summing the frequency of intake from the different food groups (Supplementary Table 2). A higher S-HEI score denotes a greater adherence to the guidelines of the Spanish Society of Community Nutrition (i.e., global quality diet). The choice of these covariates was based on prior literature (2, 22, 3639).

Statistical analysis

Continuous information was depicted as the mean (standard deviation), while categorical information was displayed as a number (percentage). Normality assumption was tested by statistical procedures (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test), as well as graphical procedures (normal probability plot). The SDQ score did not meet the assumption of normality. Consequently, we opted for the bootstrapping technique as a reliable method to determine confidence intervals for measures of both central tendency and association, as well as robust assessments of standard errors. To assess the differences between the mean values of the SDQ score (dependent variable) across breakfast habits (independent variable), analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) were applied. For the ANCOVAs performed, we used the following a priori parameters: effect size (f) = 0.10, alpha (α) error probability = 0.05, statistical power (1-β) = 0.95, number of dependent variables = 1, number of groups = 2, and number of covariates = 15. Thus, a sample size with 1,302 observations would be enough to reach high effect sizes. The effect sizes of different ANCOVAs performed were computed by omega squared (ω2). Preliminary analyses did not indicate significant interactions between sex or age group and breakfast status, place and habits and mean differences in SDQ (p > 0.05 for all). Thus, all analyses were performed including both sex (girls and boys) age groups (preschoolers, children, and adolescents) together to obtain increased statistical power. Binary logistic regression analyses were performed to determine the association of psychosocial behavioural problems (dependent variable) according to breakfast status, place, and habits (independent variables). Age, sex, region, social class, immigrant status, physical activity level, recreational screen time, sleep duration, and S-HEI score were included as potential covariates. Additionally, since for most participants, breakfast consisted of a combination of habits, the analyses were adjusted for the remaining different possible habits. For instance, the consumption of “coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc.” was further adjusted by the consumption of “bread, toast, cookies, pastries, etc.,” “fruit and/or juice,” “eggs, cheese, ham, etc.,” and “other foods.” Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS 25.0 (IBM Corp, Armonk, New York, NY, United States). A p-value lower than 0.050 was applied to determine statistical significance.

Results

Table 1 depicts the characteristics of the study participants. The final sample included 3,772 Spanish young people. According to parents/guardians, 98.9% of the analysed sample ate breakfast, of whom 95.8% ate breakfast at home. The consumption of bread, toast, cereals, pastries, etc., was the most reported habit for breakfast (94.5%) by parents/guardians. The SDQ mean score was 7.4 ± 5.1. Furthermore, 12.9% of the sample presented borderline/abnormal values for the SDQ score.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive data of the study participants (N = 3,772).

The mean differences in SDQ scores in relation to breakfast status and breakfast place are shown in Figure 1. A higher SDQ mean score was found for those who skipped breakfast in comparison with those eating breakfast (p < 0.001), with a small effect size (ω2 = 0.006). Similarly, this greater SDQ mean score was found in those who ate breakfast out of home compared to those who breakfast at home (p < 0.001), with a small effect (ω2 = 0.008).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Association between breakfast status and breakfast place and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire mean score in young population. Estimated mean (bars) and 95% CIs (lines) represent values after adjustment for age, sex, region, social class, immigrant status, excess weight, physical activity level, recreational screen time, sleep duration, Spanish Healthy Eating Index score, and breakfast habits.

In relation to the mean differences between the SDQ score according to the different foods/beverages, Figure 2 shows that those who do not consume coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc., or bread, toast, cereals, pastries, etc., for breakfast showed a greater SDQ mean score (p < 0.05 for both). The effect sizes for both associations were small (coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc.: ω2 = 0.006; bread, toast, cereals, pastries, etc.: ω2 = 0.003). In addition, those consuming eggs, cheese, ham, etc., for breakfast showed higher SDQ mean scores (p = 0.006), with a small effect (ω2 = 0.003).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Association between different breakfast habits and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire mean score in young population. Estimated mean (bars) and 95% CIs (lines) represent values after adjustment for age, sex, region, social class, immigrant status, excess weight, physical activity level, recreational screen time, sleep duration, and Spanish Healthy Eating Index score. Analyses were also adjusted for the remaining possible habits (i.e., the consumption of “coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yoghurt, etc.” was further adjusted for “bread, toast, biscuits, pastries, etc.,” “fruit and/or juice,” “eggs, cheese, ham, etc.”, and “other foods.”

On the other hand, Figure 3 shows the association between breakfast status, place or habits and psychosocial behavioural problems. Skipping breakfast and eating breakfast out of home were linked to greater odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (skipping breakfast: OR = 3.29; CI 95%, 1.47–7.35; breakfast out of home: OR = 2.06; CI 95%, 1.27–3.33) than eating breakfast at home. Similarly, not consuming coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc., for breakfast was related to greater odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (OR = 1.76; CI 95%, 1.21–2.55), with a small effect size ω2 = 0.006). This association was also found for those who did not consume bread, toast, cereals, pastries, etc., for breakfast (OR = 1.31; CI 95%, 1.01–1.73). Conversely, not consuming eggs, cheese, ham, etc., was associated with lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (OR = 0.56; CI 95%, 0.38–0.83), with a small effect size (ω2 = 0.003).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Odds of psychosocial behavioural problems according to breakfast status, place and habits. Data expressed as odds ratio (dots) and standard error (bars). No psychosocial behavioural problems group was considered as the reference category. Adjusted by age, sex, region, social class, immigrant status, excess weight, physical activity level, recreational screen time, sleep duration, and Spanish Healthy Eating Index score. aAnalyses were further adjusted by breakfast habits. bAnalyses were also adjusted for the remaining possible breakfast habits (i.e., the consumption of “coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yoghurt, etc.” was further adjusted for “bread, toast, biscuits, pastries, etc.,” “fruit and/or juice,” “eggs, cheese, ham, etc.,” and “other foods”).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, the present study is the first to assess the role of breakfast status, place and habits related to the intake of certain food/beverages groups and psychosocial behavioural problems in a young population. In the present study, we found that skipping breakfast, eating breakfast out of home, and eating some breakfast habits were associated with higher or lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems in the young population after adjustment for several sociodemographic, anthropometric, and lifestyle factors.

One interesting finding is that skipping breakfast was associated with higher odds of psychosocial behavioural problems. Similarly, an association between an unhealthy diet and psychosocial behavioural problems has been suggested in the young population (8, 9), as well as in other age stages (8). Recently, a meta-analysis by Mullan and Singh (18) found that skipping breakfast was positively associated with odds of depression, stress and psychological distress in all age groups and anxiety in adolescence. Richards and Smith (40) showed that not eating breakfast daily was linked to psychosocial behavioural problems (e.g., anxiety, stress, depression). Despite this evidence, there are no well-defined mechanisms that explain why skipping breakfast might affect psychosocial health (8). Two possible explanations could partly justify this finding. First, a young population skipping breakfast might not be able to obtain the nutrients lost with the rest of the meals of the day (41). Second, children and adolescents who skip breakfast may compensate for their daily energy intake by consuming more energy-dense foods during the rest of the day or during lunch (42). These two factors could lead to an overall unhealthy/low-quality diet, which has been associated with depression or poorer psychosocial health in the young population (9, 22). Accordingly, breakfast consumption, as a part of healthy eating habits, may be promoted as a helpful approach to prevent psychosocial health problems (8).

Another interesting finding of this study is that young people who eat breakfast out of home showed higher odds of psychosocial behavioural problems than those who eat breakfast out of home. One reason explaining (at least partially) our results may be related to family meals. The social context (e.g., breakfast at home) may play a key role in breakfast intake, as it appears to be associated with higher quality breakfast intake (i.e., including dairy, cereal, and fruit groups) (18, 43). Children and adolescents who eat breakfast at home are more likely to do so in the presence of their family members. Thus, Kameyama et al. (44) showed that children (aged 7–12 years) who ate breakfast with their families less than once a week and those who ate breakfast alone on weekends showed a greater prevalence of borderline or abnormal psychosocial health status than those who ate breakfast seven times a week and those who ate breakfast on weekends with their families, respectively. Furthermore, one study by Videon and Manning (45) indicated that adolescents who regularly used to eat out of home/missed family meals show a greater prevalence of skipping breakfast, as well as a lower quality diet (e.g., low intake of dairy products). Similarly, Agathão et al. (46) pointed out the key protective role of regular family meals for psychosocial health. This is because family meals can facilitate parents to connect emotionally with children through feelings of closeness and belonging (47), as well as identify early changes in existing behavioural patterns (e.g., dress, friendships and academic performance) that may be associated with behavioural modification (48). In addition, family meals are a family time that provides an opportunity for families to connect despite the ongoing intense demands of modern life (49). Thus, current evidence indicates positive relations between diet quality and physical, emotional and mental strength in the young population, suggesting, as a promising strategy, the promotion of family-based meals, with a focus on breakfast. One possible reason justifying this relationship is that family meals (e.g., breakfast) could offer a formal/informal time in which parents/guardians could connect with their children’s emotional well-being (50), control children’s behaviours, or establish norms and restrictions (19). Another possible reason could lie in the relationship between breakfast out of home and a lower-quality breakfast/global diet. Eating out of home has been related to energy-dense and high-fat food consumption, as well as a lack of micronutrients (20). This association has also been shown in children (21). All of these aspects are associated with a lower quality breakfast/global diet, which has been associated with psychosocial behavioural problems among young people (22). Therefore, it seems reasonable to promote breakfast at home, preferably with the family and in a relaxed atmosphere, since family members have a great influence on the acquisition of habits among the young population (51).

On the other hand, we found that breakfast habits related to the intake of certain food/beverages groups were associated with lower (e.g., not consuming eggs, cheese, ham) or higher odds of psychosocial behavioural problems (e.g., not consuming coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, or not consuming bread, toast, cereals, pastries). With the questions provided by the Spanish National Health Survey, we are not able to know what and how many foods are consumed within a food group. For instance, it is possible that young people including in the group “coffee, milk, tea, chocolate, cocoa, yogurt, etc.” consume milk or dairy products and not necessarily coffee. In fact, coffee consumption is not recommended for Spanish young people (14), and its intake is low among this population (52). Supporting this idea, the latest breakfast recommendations in Spain separate coffee from the group “milk or dairy products” and include it in the group “other foods” (51). Given the impossibility of separating foods from the established groups, future editions of the national health survey should provide more specific questions regarding the breakfast composition of the young population. Nevertheless, breakfast habits can potentially impact psychosocial behavioural problems through several pathways. In this sense, there are dietary benefits of consuming breakfast, specifically if it includes cereals, grains, lower fat milk, and fruit/fruit juices, in comparison to the potential negative impact of skipping breakfast (53). This is mainly because they are suitable nutrient sources that may influence brain function, including carbohydrates, calcium, B-complex vitamins (including folate), dietary fibre, and iron (11). Supporting this notion, there is emerging evidence of the relationship between breakfast cereal intake and higher feelings of well-being (54) (although further research is necessary). Likewise, whole grains are rich in several macronutrients, including magnesium that may have beneficial effects on psychosocial health (55). In addition, Ferrer-Cascales et al. (56) revealed that a high-quality breakfast, characterised by the intake of cereal and dairy products, is related to a higher health-related quality of life and lower levels of perceived stress and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Similarly, adolescents eating a high-quality breakfast (e.g., cereals, milk) had an improved overall dietary pattern compared with their counterparts eating a low-quality breakfast (57). Similarly, children who ate breakfast had greater daily protein and energy consumption than children who skipped breakfast (58). Concerning psychosocial behavioural problems, it has been shown that the type of breakfast or lunch was associated with significant differences in well-being scores (59) and high levels of quality of life compared with those who eat a low-quality breakfast (in children) (60). Furthermore, O’Sullivan et al. (11) found in adolescents that for every additional food group eaten at breakfast, the associated total mental health score decreased after adjustment for potential confounding factors. The beneficial influence of an adequate quality breakfast (as a healthy lifestyle indicator) is especially important during childhood and adolescence, when dietary and other lifestyle habits begin to be acquired, resulting in long-term health and nutritional advantages in adulthood (60). Thus, the prevention of psychosocial behavioural problems in young populations supports the prevention of the onset in adulthood, suggesting that promoting a healthy breakfast (at home if possible) as a modifiable factor could be effective in preventing such problems (8).

Although the mechanisms through which breakfast habits contribute to decreased psychosocial behavioural problems remain unclear, certain mechanisms have been suggested. Thus, after eating breakfast, carbohydrates from foods (e.g., cereals, milk) are transformed into glucose, generating alterations in the levels of insulin, glutamate, acetylcholine, serotonin, and cortisol (11, 61). Carbohydrate intake is especially helpful for the brain after night fasting since it diminishes the production of cortisol levels, thereby reducing the “stress” signal (62). Furthermore, the transformation of carbohydrates into glucose is crucial for tryptophan formation, a precursor protein involved in the synthesis of serotonin, which regulates depressive symptoms, cognitive functioning, and irritable mood (63). Similarly, the consumption of tryptophan-rich foods has been noted as important to maintain a high quality of sleep and morning-type diurnal rhythm and indirectly improved psychosocial health, probably by the metabolism of tryptophan to serotonin in the daytime and melatonin at night in children (64). In contrast, the potentially beneficial effect of other vitamins (e.g., vitamin D) on psychosocial health in children has been pointed out in a recent systematic review (65). Additionally, it has been suggested that lower vitamin D levels may be linked to depression among children and adolescents (66). Thus, the consumption of milk and/or dairy products may provide greater amounts of vitamin D and, consequently, help to reduce the odds of psychosocial behavioural problems. In addition, B-complex vitamin deficiency (e.g., folate, B6, and B12) might also affect psychosocial behavioural problems, and it has been associated with mood and cognitive performance (67). Similarly, dietary patterns have been related to depression via alterations in folate and vitamin B12 serum levels (68). Moreover, Esnafoglu and Ozturan (66) has indicated that vitamin B12 and increased homocysteine may support the etiopathogenesis of depression. In addition, dietary fibre intake could also help in psychosocial behavioural problems. A meta-analysis of observational studies by Fatahi et al. (69) showed that a greater intake of total dietary fibre was linked to lower odds of depression, suggesting some possible mechanisms that could explain this association, such as the variations in the intestinal microbiome composition and the decrease in oxidative stress. Supporting this notion, one possible hypothesis is that a higher consumption in fibre-rich healthy foods (e.g., bread, cereals) may (to some degree) explain the association found. Another finding is the association found between the non-consumption of high-protein foods (e.g., eggs, cheese, ham) and lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems. One possible explanation of this finding may be related to the ratio of carbohydrates/protein in the dietary intake. A higher intake of high-protein foods (e.g., eggs, cheese, ham) might displace carbohydrate-rich foods and modify the ratio of protein to carbohydrate, which has been related to consistent and reciprocal changes in important regulatory factors (e.g., cortisol). Despite this fact, it must be considered that the intake of some protein-rich foods (e.g., eggs) contains high amounts of choline that is essential for producing neurotransmitters that can positively affect psychosocial health (70). Based on the above, recommending what is considered a healthy breakfast in Spain [i.e., hot drink (e.g., milk, cacao), accompanied by a solid food (e.g., bread, toasts), and fruit/fruit juice (25)], could be useful for the prevention of psychosocial problems in young people.

This current study includes some limitations that should be noted. First, due to the cross-sectional design of this study, we cannot establish whether the observed relationships imply cause and effect associations. Longitudinal studies are necessary to determine how breakfast status, place and habits related to the intake of certain food/beverages groups could exert an essential role on psychosocial behavioural problems. Notwithstanding, this cross-sectional analysis could serve as a helpful first step in detecting relations between breakfast patterns and psychosocial behavioural problems in youth. Second, this study did not consider the influence of both daily energy intake and parental feeding practices, since there was no information available on both variables for analyses. Future studies using 24-h dietary recall or dietary history are needed to obtain more accurate information. However, it is complex to use more specific methodologies in relation to dietary intake or parental feeding practices in national epidemiological studies. Third, we were not able to establish individual associations between the consumption of a specific food and psychosocial behavioural problems, since frequencies and serving sizes of consumption of different breakfast items were not assessed. However, the Spanish National Health Survey (2017), as well as previous waves, were designed to report information related to a typical Spanish complete breakfast (25). Fourth, we used parent-reported questionnaires. For this reason, both measurement and recall bias are still plausible. Nonetheless, information was reported by parents/guardians and not by young people, which may be closer to the reality of their children’s breakfasts. Likewise, the SDQ and HEI are validated and useful instruments that have been widely used in the scientific literature for both psychosocial behavioural problems and diet quality. Fifth, BMI and excess weight were determined through height and weight reported by the parents/guardians for children and adolescents, which could introduce measurement error. Conversely, the main strength of this study is that, to date, it is the first study to examine the relationship between breakfast status, place and habits related to the intake of certain food/beverages groups and the association of psychosocial behavioural problems among a young population. Another strength is the nationwide, large sample of children and adolescents analysed. It is noteworthy that the relationship between breakfast (as an isolated meal) and the odds of psychosocial behavioural problems has been assessed. Therefore, it should be considered that a child or adolescent may not consume some of the foods listed for breakfast (e.g., fruits) and consume them later in the day at other meals. To try to minimise this concern, we adjusted the analyses performed by global quality diet (through the S-HEI score). Despite this fact, caution is required to interpret our results.

Conclusion

Our results show that skipping breakfast or eating breakfast out of home is associated with higher odds of psychosocial behavioural problems in a nationwide, large sample of Spanish children and adolescents. Similarly, some habits related to the consumption of certain foods/beverages are related to higher or lower odds of psychosocial behavioural problems. This finding is clinically meaningful, as psychosocial behavioural problems are one of the most important worldwide worries in the young population. It might be possible to underscore the significance of focussing not only on breakfast intake but also on habits related to the consumption of certain foods/beverages to try to reduce the deleterious effects of psychosocial behavioural problems in young populations. Nevertheless, further studies with different designs are needed to verify cause-effect associations.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analysed in this study. This data can be found here: https://www.sanidad.gob.es/estadisticas/microdatos.do.

Ethics statement

Ethical approval was not provided for this study on human participants because following the Spanish regulations, no ethics committee approval was needed for this study, due to the use of secondary data. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

JL-G: conceptualisation, software, validation, formal analysis, and data curation. JL-G and LS: writing—original draft preparation. LS, RL-B, and PT-L: writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Acknowledgments

JL-G is a Margarita Salas Fellow (Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha – 2021-MS-20563).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.871238/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.sdqinfo.org

References

1. Smith L, Hamer M. Sedentary behaviour and psychosocial health across the life course. In: MF Leitzmann, C Jochem, D Schmid editors. Sedentary behaviour epidemiology. Springer series on epidemiology and public health. Cham: Springer International Publishing (2018). p. 311–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. World Health Organization [WHO]. Social determinants of mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization (2014). p. 52.

Google Scholar

3. World Health Organization [WHO]. Adolescent mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization (2020).

Google Scholar

4. Guthold R, White Johansson E, Mathers CD, Ross DA. Global and regional levels and trends of child and adolescent morbidity from 2000 to 2016: An analysis of years lost due to disability (YLDs). BMJ Glob Health. (2021) 6:e004996. doi: 10.1136/bmjgh-2021-004996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Otto C, Reiss F, Voss C, Wüstner A, Meyrose A-K, Hölling H, et al. Mental health and well-being from childhood to adulthood: Design, methods and results of the 11-year follow-up of the BELLA study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 30:1559–77. doi: 10.1007/s00787-020-01630-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. World Health Organization [WHO]. The WHO special initiative for mental health (2019-2023): Universal health coverage for mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization (2019).

Google Scholar

7. Ortuño-Sierra J, Fonseca-Pedrero E, Paíno M, Aritio-Solana R. Prevalencia de síntomas emocionales y comportamentales en adolescentes españoles. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment. (2014) 7:121–30. doi: 10.1016/j.rpsm.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Zahedi H, Djalalinia S, Sadeghi O, Zare Garizi F, Asayesh H, Payab M, et al. Breakfast consumption and mental health: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Nutr Neurosci. (2022) 25:1250–64. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2020.1853411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Khalid S, Williams CM, Reynolds SA. Is there an association between diet and depression in children and adolescents? A systematic review. Br J Nutr. (2016) 116:2097–108. doi: 10.1017/S0007114516004359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rani R, Dharaiya CN, Singh B. Importance of not skipping breakfast: A review. Int J Food Sci Technol. (2021) 56:28–38. doi: 10.1111/ijfs.14742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. O’Sullivan TA, Robinson M, Kendall GE, Miller M, Jacoby P, Silburn SR, et al. A good-quality breakfast is associated with better mental health in adolescence. Public Health Nutr. (2009) 12:249–58. doi: 10.1017/S1368980008003935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Giovannini M, Verduci E, Scaglioni S, Salvatici E, Bonza M, Riva E, et al. Breakfast: A good habit, not a repetitive custom. J Int Med Res. (2008) 36:613–24. doi: 10.1177/147323000803600401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Scheffers FR, Boer JMA, Verschuren WMM, Verheus M, van der Schouw YT, Sluijs I, et al. Pure fruit juice and fruit consumption and the risk of CVD: The European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition–Netherlands (EPIC-NL) study. Br J Nutr. (2019) 121:351–9. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518003380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Aranceta-Bartrina J, Partearroyo T, López-Sobaler AM, Ortega RM, Varela-Moreiras G, Serra-Majem L, et al. Updating the food-based dietary guidelines for the Spanish population: The Spanish society of community nutrition (SENC) proposal. Nutrients. (2019) 11:2675. doi: 10.3390/nu11112675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Papoutsou S, Briassoulis G, Hadjigeorgiou C, Savva SC, Solea T, Hebestreit A, et al. The combination of daily breakfast consumption and optimal breakfast choices in childhood is an important public health message. Int J Food Sci Nutr. (2014) 65:273–9. doi: 10.3109/09637486.2013.854750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Sawa S, Hashizume K, Abe T, Kusaka Y, Fukazawa Y, Hiraku Y, et al. Pathway linking physical activity, sleep duration, and breakfast consumption with the physical/psychosocial health of schoolchildren. J Child Health Care. (2021) 25:5–17. doi: 10.1177/1367493519891019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ahadi Z, Kelishadi R, Qorbani M, Zahedi H, Aram M, Motlagh ME, et al. Association of breakfast intake with psychiatric distress and violent behaviors in iranian children and adolescents: The CASPIAN- IV study. Indian J Pediatr. (2016) 83:922–9. doi: 10.1007/s12098-016-2049-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mullan BA, Singh M. A systematic review of the quality, content, and context of breakfast consumption. Nutr Food Sci. (2010) 40:81–114. doi: 10.1108/00346651011015953

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Scaglioni S, De Cosmi V, Ciappolino V, Parazzini F, Brambilla P, Agostoni C. Factors influencing children’s eating behaviours. Nutrients. (2018) 10:706. doi: 10.3390/nu10060706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lachat C, Nago E, Verstraeten R, Roberfroid D, Van Camp J, Kolsteren P. Eating out of home and its association with dietary intake: A systematic review of the evidence: Eating out and diet. Obes Rev. (2012) 13:329–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00953.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Moreira T, Severo M, Oliveira A, Ramos E, Rodrigues S, Lopes C. Eating out of home and dietary adequacy in preschool children. Br J Nutr. (2015) 114:297–305. doi: 10.1017/S0007114515001713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. O’Neil A, Quirk SE, Housden S, Brennan SL, Williams LJ, Pasco JA, et al. Relationship between diet and mental health in children and adolescents: A systematic review. Am J Public Health. (2014) 104:e31–42. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ministry of Health, Consumer Affairs and Social Welfare. Spanish National Health Survey. Madrid: Ministry of Health, Consumer Affairs and Social Welfare (2018).

Google Scholar

24. National Statistics Institute. National health survey. Results. Madrid, España. (2018). Available online at: https://www.ine.es/dyngs/INEbase/es/operacion.htm?c=Estadistica_C&cid=1254736176783&menu=resultados&idp=1254735573175#!tabs-1254736195650 (accessed November 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

25. Aranceta Bartrina J, Pérez Rodrigo C, Serra Majem L, Delgado Rubio A. Hábitos alimentarios de los alumnos usuarios de comedores escolares en España. Estudio «Dime Cómo Comes». Aten Primaria. (2004) 33:131–9.

Google Scholar

26. Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1997) 38:581–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ezpeleta L, Granero R, de la Osa N, Penelo E, Domènech JM. Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties Questionnaire3–4 in 3-year-old preschoolers. Compr Psychiatry. (2013) 54:282–91. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cole TJ, Lobstein T. Extended international (IOTF) body mass index cut-offs for thinness, overweight and obesity: Extended international BMI cut-offs. Pediatr Obes. (2012) 7:284–94. doi: 10.1111/j.2047-6310.2012.00064.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Roman-Viñas B, Serra-Majem L, Hagströmer M, Ribas-Barba L, Sjöström M, Segura-Cardona R. International physical activity questionnaire: Reliability and validity in a Spanish population. Eur J Sport Sci. (2010) 10:297–304. doi: 10.1080/17461390903426667

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. World Health Organization [WHO]. Guidelines on physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep for children under 5 years of age. (2019). Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK541170/ (accessed December 4, 2020).

Google Scholar

31. Tremblay MS, Carson V, Chaput J-PP, Connor Gorber S, Dinh T, Duggan M, et al. Canadian 24-hour movement guidelines for children and youth: An integration of physical activity, sedentary behaviour, and sleep. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. (2016) 41:S311–27. doi: 10.1139/apnm-2016-0203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Hirshkowitz M, Whiton K, Albert SM, Alessi C, Bruni O, DonCarlos L, et al. National Sleep Foundation’s updated sleep duration recommendations: Final report. Sleep Health. (2015) 1:233–43. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Norte Navarro AI, Ortiz Moncada R. [Spanish diet quality according to the healthy eating index]. Nutr Hosp. (2011) 26:330–6. doi: 10.1590/S0212-16112011000200014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kennedy ET, Ohls J, Carlson S, Fleming K. The healthy eating index. J Am Diet Assoc. (1995) 95:1103–8. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8223(95)00300-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Spanish Society of Community Nutrition (SSCN). Healthy dietary guidelines. (2014). Available online at: http://www.nutricioncomunitaria.org/es/otras-publicaciones (accessed November 1, 2021).

Google Scholar

36. Zhang J, Paksarian D, Lamers F, Hickie IB, He J, Merikangas KR. Sleep patterns and mental health correlates in US adolescents. J Pediatr. (2017) 182:137–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Lubans D, Richards J, Hillman C, Faulkner G, Beauchamp M, Nilsson M, et al. Physical activity for cognitive and mental health in youth: A systematic review of mechanisms. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20161642. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Stiglic N, Viner RM. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: A systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open. (2019) 9:e023191. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Sutaria S, Devakumar D, Yasuda SS, Das S, Saxena S. Is obesity associated with depression in children? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Dis Child. (2019) 104:64–74. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2017-314608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Richards G, Smith AP. Breakfast and energy drink consumption in secondary school children: Breakfast omission, in isolation or in combination with frequent energy drink use, is associated with stress, anxiety, and depression cross-sectionally, but not at 6-month follow-up. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:106. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ramsay SA, Bloch TD, Marriage B, Shriver LH, Spees CK, Taylor CA. Skipping breakfast is associated with lower diet quality in young US children. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2018) 72:548–56. doi: 10.1038/s41430-018-0084-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Barrett N, Riordan F, Michels N, Frost Andersen L, vant Veer P, Moreno LA, et al. Breakfast Skipping and overweight/obesity among European adolescents, a cross-sectional analysis of the HELENA dataset: A DEDIPAC study. HRB Open Res. (2018) 1:19. doi: 10.12688/hrbopenres.12847.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Aranceta J, Serra-Majem L, Ribas L, Pérez-Rodrigo C. Breakfast consumption in Spanish children and young people. Public Health Nutr. (2001) 4:1439–44. doi: 10.1079/PHN2001235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Kameyama N, Morimoto Y, Hashimoto A, Inoue H, Nagaya I, Nakamura K, et al. The relationship between family meals and mental health problems in Japanese elementary school children: A cross-sectional study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:9281. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18179281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Videon TM, Manning CK. Influences on adolescent eating patterns: The importance of family meals. J Adolesc Health. (2003) 32:365–73. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(02)00711-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Agathão BT, Cunha DB, Sichieri R, Lopes CS. The role of family meal frequency in common mental disorders in children and adolescents over eight months of follow-up. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0243793. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Jones BL. Making time for family meals: Parental influences, home eating environments, barriers and protective factors. Physiol Behav. (2018) 193:248–51. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Skeer MR, Ballard EL. Are family meals as good for youth as we think they are? A review of the literature on family meals as they pertain to adolescent risk prevention. J Youth Adolesc. (2013) 42:943–63. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-9963-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Smith SL, Ramey E, Sisson SB, Richardson S, DeGrace BW. The family meal model: Influences on family mealtime participation. OTJR Occup Particip Health. (2020) 40:138–46. doi: 10.1177/1539449219876878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Eisenberg ME, Olson RE, Neumark-Sztainer D, Story M, Bearinger LH. Correlations between family meals and psychosocial well-being among adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2004) 158:792. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.158.8.792

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Spanish Foundation for Nutrition [SFN]. Breakfast situation in Spain. Madrid: Spanish Foundation for Nutrition (2018).

Google Scholar

52. Fernández-Alvira JM, Iglesia I, Ferreira-Pêgo C, Babio N, Salas-Salvadó J, Moreno LA. Fluid intake in Spanish children and adolescents; a cross-sectional study. Nutr Hosp. (2014) 29:1163–70. doi: 10.3305/nh.2014.29.5.7420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. O’Neil CE, Nicklas TA, Fulgoni VL III Nutrient intake, diet quality, and weight measures in breakfast patterns consumed by children compared with breakfast skippers: NHANES 2001-2008. AIMS Public Health. (2015) 2:441–68. doi: 10.3934/publichealth.2015.3.441

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Williams PG. The benefits of breakfast cereal consumption: A systematic review of the evidence base. Adv Nutr. (2014) 5:636S–73S. doi: 10.3945/an.114.006247

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Anjom-Shoae J, Sadeghi O, Hassanzadeh Keshteli A, Afshar H, Esmaillzadeh A, Adibi P. The association between dietary intake of magnesium and psychiatric disorders among Iranian adults: A cross-sectional study. Br J Nutr. (2018) 120:693–702. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518001782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ferrer-Cascales R, Sánchez-SanSegundo M, Ruiz-Robledillo N, Albaladejo-Blázquez N, Laguna-Pérez A, Zaragoza-Martí A. Eat or skip breakfast? The important role of breakfast quality for health-related quality of life, stress and depression in Spanish adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:1781. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15081781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Matthys C, De Henauw S, Bellemans M, De Maeyer M, De Backer G. Breakfast habits affect overall nutrient profiles in adolescents. Public Health Nutr. (2007) 10:413–21. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007248049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chitra U, Reddy CR. The role of breakfast in nutrient intake of urban schoolchildren. Public Health Nutr. (2007) 10:55–8. doi: 10.1017/S1368980007219640

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Hayhoe R, Rechel B, Clark AB, Gummerson C, Smith SJL, Welch AA. Cross-sectional associations of schoolchildren’s fruit and vegetable consumption, and meal choices, with their mental well-being: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Nutr Prev Health. (2021) 4:447. doi: 10.1136/bmjnph-2020-000205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Chen X, Sekine M, Hamanishi S, Wang H, Gaina A, Yamagami T, et al. Lifestyles and health-related quality of life in Japanese school children: A cross-sectional study. Prev Med. (2005) 40:668–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.09.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hoyland A, Dye L, Lawton CL. A systematic review of the effect of breakfast on the cognitive performance of children and adolescents. Nutr Res Rev. (2009) 22:220–43. doi: 10.1017/S0954422409990175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Lee SA, Park EC, Ju YJ, Lee TH, Han E, Kim TH. Breakfast consumption and depressive mood: A focus on socioeconomic status. Appetite. (2017) 114:313–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2017.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Miller AH, Maletic V, Raison CL. Inflammation and its discontents: The role of cytokines in the pathophysiology of major depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 65:732–41. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Harada T, Hirotani M, Maeda M, Nomura H, Takeuchi H. Correlation between breakfast tryptophan content and morningness–eveningness in japanese infants and students aged 0–15 yrs. J Physiol Anthropol. (2007) 26:201–7. doi: 10.2114/jpa2.26.201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Głąbska D, Kołota A, Lachowicz K, Skolmowska D, Stachoń M, Guzek D. The influence of vitamin D intake and status on mental health in children: A systematic review. Nutrients. (2021) 13:952. doi: 10.3390/nu13030952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Esnafoglu E, Ozturan DD. The relationship of severity of depression with homocysteine, folate, vitamin B12, and vitamin D levels in children and adolescents. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2020) 25:249–55. doi: 10.1111/camh.12387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lang UE, Beglinger C, Schweinfurth N, Walter M, Borgwardt S. Nutritional aspects of depression. Cell Physiol Biochem. (2015) 37:1029–43. doi: 10.1159/000430229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Khosravi M, Sotoudeh G, Amini M, Raisi F, Mansoori A, Hosseinzadeh M. The relationship between dietary patterns and depression mediated by serum levels of Folate and vitamin B12. BMC Psychiatry. (2020) 20:63. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-2455-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Fatahi S, Matin SS, Sohouli MH, Gãman M-A, Raee P, Olang B, et al. Association of dietary fiber and depression symptom: A systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Complement Ther Med. (2021) 56:102621. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2020.102621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. van Lee L, Quah PL, Saw SM, Yap FKP, Godfrey KM, Chong YS, et al. Maternal choline status during pregnancy, but not that of betaine, is related to antenatal mental well-being: The growing up in Singapore toward healthy outcomes cohort. Depress Anxiety. (2017) 34:877–87. doi: 10.1002/da.22637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: healthy diet, nutrition, lifestyle, mental health, preschoolers, children, adolescents

Citation: López-Gil JF, Smith L, López-Bueno R and Tárraga-López PJ (2022) Breakfast and psychosocial behavioural problems in young population: The role of status, place, and habits. Front. Nutr. 9:871238. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.871238

Received: 08 February 2022; Accepted: 15 July 2022;
Published: 23 August 2022.

Edited by:

Veronica Ginani, University of Brasilia, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Jagmeet Madan, SNDT Women’s University, India
Panchali Moitra, SNDT Women’s University, India

Copyright © 2022 López-Gil, Smith, López-Bueno and Tárraga-López. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: José Francisco López-Gil, am9zZWZyYW5jaXNjb2xvcGV6Z2lsQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.