- 1Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Shengjing Hospital of China Medical University, Shenyang, China
- 2Tibet Military Region Centers for Disease Control and Prevention of PLA, Tibet, China
- 3The Third Central Hospital of Tianjin, Tianjin, China
Objectives: The aim of this meta-analysis was to assess the quantitative associations between fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive disorders in older adults.
Design: A meta-analysis.
Setting and Participants: We used the PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus databases for a literature search to 12 April 2022. We preliminarily retrieved 11,759 studies, 16 of which met the inclusion criteria including six cross-sectional studies, nine cohort studies and one case-control study, incorporating 64,348 participants and 9,879 cases.
Methods: Using the three databases, we identified observational studies exploring the association. The pooled odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated using a random effects model.
Results: Sixteen studies were included in the meta-analysis, and the results showed that increased fruit and vegetable consumption in older adults was associated with a decline in the prevalence of cognitive disorders (OR: 0.79, 95% CI: 0.76–0.83). Moreover, intake of fruits (OR: 0.83, 95% CI: 0.77–0.89) and vegetables (OR: 0.75, 95% CI: 0.70–0.80) alone were both associated with a lower prevalence of cognitive disorders. Subgroup analyses indicated that the intake of fruits and vegetables was associated with the prevalence of cognitive impairment (OR: 0.72, 95% CI: 0.76–0.80) and dementia (OR: 0.84, 95% CI: 0.78–0.91) but not Alzheimer’s disease (OR: 0.88, 95% CI: 0.76–1.01).
Conclusion and Implications: Our meta-analysis provides evidence that the intake of fruits and vegetables is inversely proportional and linearly associated with the prevalence of cognitive disorders in older adults. Future research is required to further investigate the preventive effects of the frequency, quantity, and duration of eating vegetables and fruits on cognitive disorders in older adults.
Introduction
With the rapid development of various countries, population aging is accelerating around the world, especially in developing countries (1). Alongside this, a series of age-related public health problems are growing in prominence, including dementia, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (2). Age-related cognitive disorders will become more widespread, mainly including delirium, mild cognitive impairment (MCI), and major neurocognitive disorders [such as dementia, Parkinson’s disease (PD), and AD] (3). Among them, MCI is a transitional or a gray area between normal aging and the onset of dementia in which memory, attention, and cognitive function are degraded, but functional independence remains intact (4). The incidence of MCI is 21.5–71.3 per 1,000 person-years for older groups (5). Progression of MCI over 2 years is marked by a gradual decline in intelligence, with personality changes of varying degrees (6). As reported previously, 46.8 million people worldwide suffered from dementia in 2015. By 2050, this figure is estimated to reach up to 131.5 million, increasing the burden on society and families, with AD being the most frequent cause (7). In 2006, the number of patients with AD reached 26.6 million worldwide. It is estimated that this number will reach 106.8 million by 2050 (8). Moreover, the latest research has shown that the prevalence of AD varies greatly worldwide and that the standardized prevalence (all age groups) is between 207 and 812 per 10,000 people according to door-to-door surveys (9). Hence, it is of great clinical and public health significance to find measures to reduce or prevent the incidence of dementia.
The impact of nutritional factors on cognition is very important, as it is a measure that can be changed easily. However, in the context of nutritional epidemiology, the factors that protect against cognitive disorders in older adults are unclear. Previous research has found that nutritional factors play an essential role in brain aging (10). A randomized controlled trial showed that the prevalence of cognitive disorders and related brain diseases can be reduced by consuming specific macronutrients and micronutrients in a balanced diet (11). Certain diets are very important to optimizing cognition and reducing the prevalence of cognitive disorders. For example, the consumption of strawberries, tofu, and edible mushrooms has been found to be associated with the abated prevalence of cognitive disorders (12–14). Overall, these studies demonstrate an association between the intake of fruits and vegetables and a lower prevalence of dementia and cognitive decline. However, published meta-analyses have paid less attention to older adults and have not evaluated the dose-response relationship thoroughly (15–17). Therefore, on this basis, we searched for observational studies published in recent years and carried out a meta-analysis specifically on the older population to assess quantitatively the relationship between fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive disorders.
Materials and Methods
Literature Search Strategy
We complied strictly with the Guidelines for Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews of Observational Studies when conducting the meta-analysis (18). Two researchers conducted a systematic literature search of the Web of Science, PubMed, and Scopus electronic databases (1970.01-2022.04) to find appropriate articles. Electronic searches included MeSH and free text searches. The following key words were used: (“vegetable” OR “vegetables” OR “fruit” OR “fruits” OR “vegetable products” OR “dried fruit”) combined with (“Alzheimer’s disease” OR “cognitive impairment” OR “Alzheimer’s type” OR “cognitive decline” OR “dementia” OR “MCI” OR “mild neurocognitive disorder” OR “mild cognitive impairment” OR “cognitive impair*” OR “memory impair*” OR “Alzheimer’s disease” OR “dementia*” OR “cognitive disorders” OR “cognitive disorder*” OR “cognitive defect” OR “memory disorder*” OR “AD” OR “executive function” OR “Alzheimer*”). Manuscripts published in languages other than English were excluded. In addition, we searched manually for a list of potential and related articles.
Study Selection
The inclusion criteria for the selection of studies were as follows: (1) the manuscript reported an observational study; (2) the outcome of the study was associated with the occurrence of cognitive disorders; (3) the variables that explored in the study population included dietary intake of fruits and/or vegetables; (4) studies indicated an relationship between vegetable and fruit intake, and the association strength index odds ratio [(OR)/hazard ratio (HR)/relative risk (RR)] with cognitive disorders and the 95% confidence interval (CI) were included; and (5) participants in the original study were aged over 60 years. The exclusion criteria for the study were as follows: (1) research data included only nutrients within fruits and vegetables, such as protein, vitamins or fiber; or (2) the exposure variable was the intake of specific types of vegetables or fruits.
Data Extraction and Quality Assessment
A research assistant collected the following data from the literature: first author, type of study design, country, sample size, age, follow-up time, disease type (cognitive impairment, AD and dementia), disease determination, exposure variables (fruits and/or vegetables), number of cases, lowest and highest categories of fruit and/or vegetable intake, exposure assessment, risk estimates with CIs, and confounders. Disagreement during the analysis was solved through discussion with the third author. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (19) and the Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (20) were used for quality assessment.
Statistical Analysis
We conducted a meta-analysis of risk assessments for cognitive disorders; the highest and lowest categories of fruit and vegetable intake were compared. In a meta-analysis, we assumed that RRs and HRs were approximately equivalent to ORs when the prevalence of the outcome was low (21). Given the limited amount of research on each disease, we collected data associated with cognitive impairment, AD and dementia. Subsequently, we used a generic inverse-variance method (random-effects model) (22) to calculate the total association strength index. We used an Excel Macro file (23) to convert the effect quantity and 95% CI referring to groups other than the lowest dose group in the original study into the effect quantity and 95% CI referring to the lowest dose group, because these two types of studies cannot be directly combined. Moreover, we carried out a dose-response meta-analysis based on the daily intake of vegetables and fruits. Dose-response categorical meta-analyses require data for the distribution of exposures across ≥3 categories, and only four studies met this condition (24–27).
He et al. assessed the average weight of a series of common fruits and vegetables, using serving size weights for a 0.5 cup standard serving (28); it is estimated that the average daily intake of fruits is 80 g and that of vegetables is 77 g. According to a previous study (15), to obtain a better fit in the dose-effect model, for studies that reported results for daily consumption of fruit and/or vegetable in servings only, we derived grams by assuming that the average daily serving is 80 g for fruits and 77 g for vegetables. In the dose-response meta-analysis, the model was fitted in increments of 80 g of fruits, 77 g of vegetables, and 78.5 g of fruits and vegetables combined per day. We also conducted a sensitivity analysis to remove each individual study from the meta-analysis to determine the impact of each study on the overall results. Funnel plots, followed by Egger’s and Begg’s tests, were used to estimate publication bias. In addition, we carried out subgroup analyses using pre-specified characteristics (country, gender, exposure assessment method, disease type, design type, number of cases, and type of exposure). The statistical analysis was carried out using Stata version 17.0 software (Stata Corp., College Station, TX, United States).
Results
Search Result
Figure 1 presents the selection flow chart for this study. Briefly, 11,759 articles were identified in the PubMed, Web of Science and Scopus databases. Ultimately, 16 studies (including six cross-sectional studies, nine cohort studies, and one case-control study) conformed to the inclusion criteria and were included in the meta-analysis. A total of 64,348 participants and 9,879 patients with cognitive disorders, AD, or dementia were included, with an average follow-up time of 9.8 years. Table 1 describes the characteristics of each study. We recorded the different consumption frequencies of vegetables and fruits and the RR/HR/OR of cognitive disorders in accordance with the highest and lowest classification of fruit and vegetable intake. Supplementary Tables 1, 2 indicate the quality of the estimated results of the included studies. In accordance with the quality estimated by the Newcastle-Ottawa scale, the mean value for the one case-control and nine cohort studies was 7.6 stars. We considered a study awarded 5–9 stars to be of medium- or high-quality, because the criteria for medium or high quality have not been established. All documents were of medium or high quality, with scores of 5–9. The Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality scale was used to evaluate the cross-sectional studies, including 11 items. Each item was rated as “yes” (1 point), “no” or “unclear” (0 points); 0–3 points indicated low-quality literature, 4–7 points medium-quality literature, and 8–11 points high-quality literature. All documents were of medium or high quality, with scores of 6–8.
Figure 1. Flowchart of study selection about the relationship between fruits and vegetables intake and cognitive disorders.
Association Between Fruit and Vegetable Intake and Cognitive Disorders
A total of 16 articles, including 31 effect groups, reported the relationship between fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive disorders in the older age group. The results of the meta-analysis of ORs in accordance with the highest and lowest categories of fruit and vegetable intake are shown in Figure 2. The pooled results demonstrated that a high intake of fruits and vegetables was associated with a declined in the prevalence of cognitive disorders (OR: 0.82, 95% CI: 0.75–0.90). Intake of fruits (OR: 0.83, 95% CI: 0.77–0.89) or vegetables (OR: 0.75, 95% CI: 0.70–0.80) separately was also associated with a reduced prevalence of disease in older adults. Remarkable heterogeneity was found in the pooled analysis (I2 = 35.3%, P < 0.05).
Figure 2. Meta-analysis of the association between fruits and vegetables consumption and the prevalence of cognitive disorders.
Subgroup Analysis and Sensitivity Analysis
The subgroup analyses (Table 2) showed that higher fruit and vegetable intake was negatively associated with the prevalence of cognitive disorders (OR: 0.76, 95% CI: 0.72–0.81) both in the Chinese population (OR: 0.76, 95% CI: 0.72–0.81), and other countries (OR: 0.84, 95% CI: 0.79–0.90). A gender-based subgroup analysis demonstrated that, in women (OR: 0.73, 95% CI: 0.54–0.99) and both men and women participants combined (OR: 0.79, 95% CI: 0.76–0.83), higher fruit and vegetable intake was associated with a lower prevalence of cognitive disorders when compared with lower intake. However, no significant association was found in the subgroups of men (OR: 1.09, 95% CI: 0.72–1.66). In the subgroup analysis of disease types, the intake of vegetables and fruits was significantly association with prevalence of cognitive impairment (OR: 0.76, 95% CI: 0.72–0.80) and dementia (OR: 0.84, 95% CI: 0.78–0.91), but there was no association with AD (OR: 0.88, 95% CI: 0.76–1.01). According to the type of study design, we divided all studies into three subgroups. However, all three subgroups, cohort studies (OR: 0.83, 95% CI: 0.79–0.87), cross-sectional studies (OR: 0.70, 95% CI: 0.61–0.82), and case-control studies (OR: 0.68, 95% CI: 0.61–0.76), indicated that intake of vegetables and fruits was negatively associated with the presence of cognitive disorders. A subgroup analysis based on exposure assessment methods showed that there was a negative association between vegetable and fruit intake and cognitive disorders as measured by the Food Frequency Questionnaire (OR: 0.79, 95% CI: 0.75–0.83) and other assessment methods (OR: 0.80, 95% CI: 0.75–0.86). In the subgroup analysis based on the number of cases, no remarkable heterogeneity was found among the subgroups. In the sensitivity analysis (Supplementary Figure 1), the comprehensive results did not change after excluding each article successively.
Table 2. Subgroup analyses of relationship between fruits and vegetables intake and cognitive disorders.
Dose-Response Association Analysis
Dose-response meta-analysis requires the inclusion of data for at least three exposure categories, and therefore only five estimates from four separate studies were analyzed. As shown in Figure 3, in older adults there was an obvious linear dose-response relationship between the intake of vegetables and fruits and the prevalence of cognitive disorders (P = 0.03).
Figure 3. Dose-response relationship between fruits and vegetables intake and prevalence of cognitive disorders.
Publication Bias
The funnel plot (Supplementary Figure 2) indicated that, in the meta-analysis and dose-response meta-analysis, the comprehensive effect size distribution of the study was relatively symmetrical, suggesting that there was no publication bias. Begg’s regression test also showed no potential publication bias in the meta-analysis (P = 0.51) and dose-response meta-analysis (P = 0.22).
Discussion
A total of 16 articles, including 31 effect groups, was used to explore the relationship between fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive disorders in older adults. Following the data analysis, we found that consumption of fruits and/or vegetables was associated with a decline in the prevalence of cognitive disorders. The results of the dose-response relationship indicated that an increase in the daily intake of fruits and/or vegetables was significantly associated with a reduction of cognitive impairment in older adults.
Several previous meta-analyses (15–17) have studied the impact of vegetable and fruit consumption on cognition among adults and found a negative association, which is consistent with our findings. Considering that cognitive decline is an age-related disease, we investigated its relationship with fruit and vegetable consumption among older adults. Consistently, consumption of fruits and/or vegetables showed a dose-response relationship with reduced prevalence of cognitive disorders. Furthermore, published meta-analyses have only reported that the combination of fruits and vegetables showed a beneficial effect on cognitive impairment and dementia. In addition to this, we found that fruit or vegetable intake alone was also negatively associated with cognitive disorders.
The beneficial effect of a diet containing a high proportion in fruits and vegetables on cognition in older adults is perhaps due to the rich supply of antioxidant nutrients (29, 30). Antioxidants may benefit cognitive function by influencing various metabolic pathways. In addition to participating in AD pathology, oxidative stress is also considered to be a crucial component of many steps in the progression of vascular cognitive impairment and dementia (31). In numerous studies on the abundance of antioxidant nutrients in fruits and vegetables (32–36), the results showed that older adults who took in more nutrients as food supplements or had higher concentrations in plasma had better cognitive status. Some epidemiological studies have reported that a greater intake of antioxidants can reduce cognitive disorders or dementia (37, 38). The consumption of specific kinds of fruits and vegetables, such as berries, soybeans and bean products, dark green leafy vegetables, nuts, cruciferous vegetables, root vegetables, fungi, and nuts, may also protect against the risk of cognitive disorders (12–14, 32–35, 39); however, because of limited data, these studies were not included in our meta-analysis. In addition, a previous study (40) indicated that nutrition regulates the immune system and changes the neuroinflammatory process associated with the pathogenesis of AD and the progression of neurodegeneration. Thus, the anti-inflammatory properties of fruits and vegetables may be another beneficial factor for cognitive function in older adults. In two cohort studies (41, 42), researchers investigated inflammation-related dietary patterns. Generally, an anti-inflammatory diet includes mainly vegetables, fruits, whole beans, and seafood. These studies found that more inflammatory diet patterns are associated with an accelerated decline in cognitive ability. Compared with the lowest quartile, overall cognitive and reasoning ability declined the most in those in the highest quartile, who consumed the most inflammatory diet. Moreover, in a recent review, researchers found a beneficial effect of an anti-inflammatory diet on cognitive health results in older adults (43).
Both stroke and AD have major socio-economic effects, and these two diseases can occur simultaneously and affect each other. The vascular factors predisposing people to cerebrovascular diseases are often associated with the occurrence of AD. Acute stroke is known to raise the risk of cognitive disorders in the older age group (44). A meta-analysis indicated that the intake of fruits and vegetables can reduce the incidence of stroke, which can partially explain its beneficial effect on the vascular components of dementia (45). Fruits and vegetables include high levels of polyphenols and phytoestrogens, which may also play a positive role in reducing the prevalence of cognitive disorders (46). The result of a cohort study in Spain indicated that the intake of lignans and stilbene could improve cognitive function in older adults (47). Some researchers have also found that the more fruit consumed, the better the cognitive performance of older adults (48). However, one cohort study found that eating more vegetables can reduce dementia or cognitive decline, but this was not true of fruits (36). Other studies have shown that different types of vegetables and fruits may have different effects on cognition, depending on the type of cognitive function assessed (such as verbal memory and executive function) (49, 50). Therefore, more studies are required to further evaluate the effect of fruits and vegetables individually on the risk of developing cognitive disorders.
Our country-based subgroup analysis showed that higher fruit and vegetable intake was negatively associated with the prevalence of cognitive impairment in the Chinese population and other countries, and it is worth noting that this negative association was more pronounced in the Chinese population. We speculated that this difference may be attributed to diverse dietary patterns and cooking methods. Specifically, the traditional Chinese diet pattern is characterized by whole grains, tea, soybeans, and vegetables (51), as well as a high content of phytochemicals (catechol and caffeine). In contrast, the Western diet, marked by the intake of large amounts of high-fat dairy products, meat, butter, eggs, and refined sugar, is associated with a greater decline in cognitive ability (52). In another study of the older adults in China (53), maintaining healthy eating habits reduced the risk of developing cognitive disorders. As a potential strategy to reduce brain diseases, a healthy diet has attracted increased attention in the scientific community recently. Many studies (54–56) have shown that there is an association between dietary patterns and cognitive function. Healthy dietary patterns are mostly based on eating rice and flour, red meat, seafood, vegetables, and fruits, which can prevent cognitive disorders (54). Among modern residents of New York, adherence to the Mediterranean diet has been associated with a slower decline in cognitive ability and a lower risk of AD (57), as measured by global scores, as well as higher language learning and memory abilities (58). This may partially explain the more pronounced negative association between the consumption of vegetables and fruits and the risk of cognitive decline in Chinese population.
The present study has some advantages over previous reports. First, our meta-analysis included numerous people with long-term follow-up in a few prospective cohort studies. Second, we evaluated the dose-response relationship and detected a linear association based on the intake of vegetables and fruits. Given the high incidence of cognitive disorders in older adults, this study will support the development of dietary guidelines for older adults and help to develop preventive interventions aimed at reducing the prevalence of dementia-related diseases. Finally, our results showed that eating vegetables alone was associated with a reduced prevalence of cognitive disorders, which was not found in previous meta-analyses. In addition, to detect potential sources of heterogeneity, we carried out subgroup analyses, used sensitivity analysis to check the robustness of the results, and measured publication bias. The limitations of this study were, first, that the evaluation of fruit and vegetable intake in the evaluated studies was mostly based on self-reported habits; as a result, the data were prone to recall bias. Second, in the quantitative response analysis, only four studies reported the consumption of more than three kinds of fruits and vegetables, so it may have lacked sufficient effectiveness in quantifying the relevant dose-response model. Third, a previous study (59) suggested that using the OR as an approximation of the RR produces progressively larger errors as the outcome rate rises above 1%, therefore, we treated HR/RR as equal to OR, which may have affected the results. Fourth, a meta-analysis cannot solve possible problems with inherent confounding in the included studies; however, most of the studies included had adjusted for the main confounding factors to reduce the potential bias caused by diet and lifestyle factors. Finally, no casual relationships could be established due to the observational nature of the studies. Future randomized controlled trials are necessary to assess the casual relationships. Following cognitive impairment, the incidence of AD and dementia rise sharply with age, and the positive effect of fruits and vegetables on cognition is more prominent among older adults. Therefore, further studies are warranted, which involve high-quality research with standardized measurement units, to investigate the potential dose-response effects of different kinds of fruits and vegetables on the risk of developing cognitive disorders and to determine whether the frequency, amount, and duration of vegetable and fruit consumption can effectively improve cognition or prevent the occurrence of cognitive disorders.
Conclusion and Implications
The results of the current meta-analysis of 16 studies showed that higher fruit and vegetable intake was negatively associated with the prevalence of cognitive disorders in older adults. It will be necessary to conduct further prospective studies to analyze the effects of different varieties of vegetables and fruits on cognitive disorders and accurately quantify the potential dose-response patterns of fruit and/or vegetable consumption.
Author Contributions
YZho: conceptualization, formal analysis, visualization, and writing – original draft. JW: writing – review and editing and supervision. LC, MS, HL, and YZha: writing – review and editing. YX: conceptualization, resources, writing – review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (grant number: 81903302), Young Elite Scientists Sponsorship Program by China Association for Science and Technology (grant number: YESS20200151), and 345 Talent Project of Shengjing Hospital of China Medical University (grant number: M0294).
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.871061/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Chen SL, Conwell Y, Chiu HFK. Loneliness and aging in China-a public health problem in need of solutions. Int Psychogeriatr. (2014) 26:1771–2. doi: 10.1017/S104161021400177X
2. Albert SM, Tabert MH, Dienstag A, Pelton G, Devanand D. The impact of mild cognitive impairment on functional abilities in the elderly. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2002) 4:64–8. doi: 10.1007/s11920-002-0015-8
3. Sachdev PS, Blacker D, Blazer DG, Ganguli M, Jeste DV, Paulsen JS, et al. Classifying neurocognitive disorders: the DSM-5 approach. Nat Rev Neurol. (2014) 10:634–42. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.181
4. Gillette Guyonnet S, Abellan Van Kan G, Andrieu S, Barberger Gateau P, Berr C, Bonnefoy M, et al. IANA task force on nutrition and cognitive decline with aging. J Nutr Health Aging. (2007) 11:132–52.
5. Ward A, Arrighi HM, Michels S, Cedarbaum JM. Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement. (2012) 8:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.01.002
7. Realdon O, Rossetto F, Nalin M, Baroni I, Cabinio M, Fioravanti R, et al. Technology-enhanced multi-domain at home continuum of care program with respect to usual care for people with cognitive impairment: the ability-TelerehABILITation study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2016) 16:425. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1132-y
8. Brookmeyer R, Johnson E, Ziegler-Graham K, Arrighi HM. Forecasting the global burden of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. (2007) 3:186–91.
9. Prince M, Ali GC, Guerchet M, Prina AM, Albanese E, Wu YT. Recent global trends in the prevalence and incidence of dementia, and survival with dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2016) 8:23. doi: 10.1186/s13195-016-0188-8
10. Scarmeas N, Anastasiou CA, Yannakoulia M. Nutrition and prevention of cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. (2018) 17:1006–15. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30338-7
11. Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. (2015) 385:2255–63. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5
12. Agarwal P, Holland TM, Wang Y, Bennett DA, Morris MC. Association of strawberries and anthocyanidin intake with Alzheimer’s dementia risk. Nutrients. (2019) 11:3060. doi: 10.3390/nu11123060
13. Zhang S, Tomata Y, Sugiyama K, Sugawara Y, Tsuji I. Mushroom consumption and incident dementia in elderly Japanese: the Ohsaki cohort 2006 study. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2017) 65:1462–9. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14812
14. Xu X, Xiao S, Rahardjo TB, Hogervorst E. Tofu intake is associated with poor cognitive performance among community-dwelling elderly in China. J Alzheimers Dis. (2015) 43:669–75. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141593
15. Jiang X, Huang J, Song D, Deng R, Wei J, Zhang Z. Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables is related to a reduced risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: meta-analysis. Front Aging Neurosci. (2017) 9:18.
16. Mottaghi T, Amirabdollahian F, Haghighatdoost F. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2018) 72:1336–44.
17. Wu L, Sun D, Tan Y. Intake of fruit and vegetables and the incident risk of cognitive disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Aging. (2017) 21:1284–90. doi: 10.1007/s12603-017-0875-6
18. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Rennie D, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA. (2000) 283:2008–12.
19. Stang A. Critical evaluation of the Newcastle-Ottawa scale for the assessment of the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses. Eur J Epidemiol. (2010) 25:603–5. doi: 10.1007/s10654-010-9491-z
20. Hu J, Dong Y, Chen X, Liu Y, Ma D, Liu X, et al. Prevalence of suicide attempts among Chinese adolescents: a meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies. Compr Psychiatry. (2015) 61:78–89. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.05.001
21. Cummings P. The relative merits of risk ratios and odds ratios. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2009) 163:438–45. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.31
22. Jackson D, White IR, Thompson SG. Extending DerSimonian and Laird’s methodology to perform multivariate random effects meta-analyses. Stat Med. (2010) 29:1282–97. doi: 10.1002/sim.3602
23. Hamling J, Lee P, Weitkunat R, Ambühl M. Facilitating meta-analyses by deriving relative effect and precision estimates for alternative comparisons from a set of estimates presented by exposure level or disease category. Stat Med. (2008) 27:954–70. doi: 10.1002/sim.3013
24. Wu MS, Lan TH, Chen CM, Chiu HC, Lan TY. Socio-demographic and health-related factors associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly in Taiwan. BMC Public Health. (2011) 11:22. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-22
25. Pastor-Valero M, Furlan-Viebig R, Menezes PR, da Silva SA, Vallada H, Scazufca M. Education and WHO recommendations for fruit and vegetable intake are associated with better cognitive function in a disadvantaged Brazilian elderly population: a population-based cross-sectional study. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e94042. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0094042
26. Lee J, Lam L, Woo J, Kwok T. Lower fluid and fruits/vegetable intake in questionable dementia among older Hong Kong Chinese. J Nutr Health Aging. (2010) 14:45–9. doi: 10.1007/s12603-010-0008-y
27. Roberts RO, Geda YE, Cerhan JR, Knopman DS, Cha RH, Christianson TJ, et al. Vegetables, unsaturated fats, moderate alcohol intake, and mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2010) 29:413–23. doi: 10.1159/000305099
28. He FJ, Nowson CA, Lucas M, MacGregor GA. Increased consumption of fruit and vegetables is related to a reduced risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of cohort studies. J Hum Hypertens. (2007) 21:717–28. doi: 10.1038/sj.jhh.1002212
29. Foley DJ, White LR. Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer disease: food for thought. JAMA. (2002) 287:3261–3. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.24.3261
30. Itoh Y, Hine K, Miura H, Uetake T, Nakano M, Takemura N, et al. Effect of the antioxidant supplement pyrroloquinoline quinone disodium salt (BioPQQ™) on cognitive functions. Adv Exp Med Biol. (2016) 876:319–25. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-3023-4_40
31. O’Brien JT, Erkinjuntti T, Reisberg B, Roman G, Sawada T, Pantoni L, et al. Vascular cognitive impairment. Lancet Neurol. (2003) 2:89–98.
32. Ozawa M, Ninomiya T, Ohara T, Doi Y, Uchida K, Shirota T, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia in an elderly Japanese population: the Hisayama Study. Am J Clin Nutr. (2013) 97:1076–82. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.045575
33. Boespflug EL, Eliassen JC, Dudley JA, Shidler MD, Kalt W, Summer SS, et al. Enhanced neural activation with blueberry supplementation in mild cognitive impairment. Nutr Neurosci. (2018) 21:297–305. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2017.1287833
34. Gu Y, Nieves JW, Stern Y, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N. Food combination and Alzheimer disease risk: a protective diet. Arch Neurol. (2010) 67:699–706. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.84
35. Nooyens AC, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, van Boxtel MP, van Gelder BM, Verhagen H, Verschuren WM. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive decline in middle-aged men and women: the Doetinchem cohort study. Br J Nutr. (2011) 106:752–61. doi: 10.1017/S0007114511001024
36. Loef M, Walach H. Fruit, vegetables and prevention of cognitive decline or dementia: a systematic review of cohort studies. J Nutr Health Aging. (2012) 16:626–30. doi: 10.1007/s12603-012-0097-x
37. Morris MC, Evans DA, Bienias JL, Tangney CC, Bennett DA, Aggarwal N, et al. Dietary intake of antioxidant nutrients and the risk of incident Alzheimer disease in a biracial community study. JAMA. (2002) 287:3230–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.287.24.3230
38. Zandi PP, Anthony JC, Khachaturian AS, Stone SV, Gustafson D, Tschanz JT, et al. Reduced risk of Alzheimer disease in users of antioxidant vitamin supplements: the Cache County study. Arch Neurol. (2004) 61:82–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.1.82
39. Kang JH, Ascherio A, Grodstein F. Fruit and vegetable consumption and cognitive decline in aging women. Ann Neurol. (2005) 57:713–20. doi: 10.1002/ana.20476
40. McGrattan AM, McGuinness B, McKinley MC, Kee F, Passmore P, Woodside JV, et al. Diet and inflammation in cognitive ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Nutr Rep. (2019) 8:53–65. doi: 10.1007/s13668-019-0271-4
41. Shivappa N, Steck SE, Hurley TG, Hussey JR, Hébert JR. Designing and developing a literature-derived, population-based dietary inflammatory index. Public Health Nutr. (2014) 17:1689–96. doi: 10.1017/S1368980013002115
42. Wärnberg J, Gomez-Martinez S, Romeo J, Díaz LE, Marcos A. Nutrition, inflammation, and cognitive function. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2009) 1153:164–75. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2008.03985.x
43. Chen X, Maguire B, Brodaty H, O’Leary F. Dietary patterns and cognitive health in older adults: a systematic review. J Alzheimers Dis. (2019) 67:583–619.
44. Goulay R, Mena Romo L, Hol EM, Dijkhuizen RM. From stroke to dementia: a comprehensive review exposing tight interactions between stroke and amyloid-β formation. Transl Stroke Res. (2020) 11:601–14. doi: 10.1007/s12975-019-00755-2
45. Hu D, Huang J, Wang Y, Zhang D, Qu Y. Fruits and vegetables consumption and risk of stroke: a meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Stroke. (2014) 45:1613–9. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.114.004836
46. Panickar KS, Jang S. Dietary and plant polyphenols exert neuroprotective effects and improve cognitive function in cerebral ischemia. Recent Pat Food Nutr Agric. (2013) 5:128–43. doi: 10.2174/1876142911305020003
47. Goni L, Fernández-Matarrubia M, Romanos-Nanclares A, Razquin C, Ruiz-Canela M, Martínez-González M, et al. Polyphenol intake and cognitive decline in the Seguimiento Universidad de Navarra (SUN) project. Br J Nutr. (2021) 126:43–52. doi: 10.1017/S000711452000392X
48. Gehlich KH, Beller J, Lange-Asschenfeldt B, Köcher W, Meinke MC, Lademann J. Fruit and vegetable consumption is associated with improved mental and cognitive health in older adults from non-western developing countries. Public Health Nutr. (2019) 22:689–96. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018002525
49. Péneau S, Galan P, Jeandel C, Ferry M, Andreeva V, Hercberg S, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake and cognitive function in the SU.VI.MAX 2 prospective study. Am J Clin Nutr. (2011) 94:1295–303. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.111.014712
50. Kesse-Guyot E, Amieva H, Castetbon K, Henegar A, Ferry M, Jeandel C, et al. Adherence to nutritional recommendations and subsequent cognitive performance: findings from the prospective supplementation with antioxidant vitamins and minerals 2 (SU.VI.MAX 2) study. Am J Clin Nutr. (2011) 93:200–10. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29761
51. Zheng PF, Shu L, Zhang XY, Si CJ, Yu XL, Gao W, et al. Association between dietary patterns and the risk of hypertension among Chinese: a cross-sectional study. Nutrients. (2016) 8:239. doi: 10.3390/nu8040239
52. Yu FN, Hu NQ, Huang XL, Shi YX, Zhao HZ, Cheng HY. Dietary patterns derived by factor analysis are associated with cognitive function among a middle-aged and elder Chinese population. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 269:640–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.09.004
53. Wang Z, Pang Y, Liu J, Wang J, Xie Z, Huang T. Association of healthy lifestyle with cognitive function among Chinese older adults. Eur J Clin Nutr. (2021) 75:325–34. doi: 10.1038/s41430-020-00785-2
54. Ding K, Zhou H, Gao T, Xu R, Chen L, Cai J, et al. Dietary patterns and cognitive function in older adults residing in rural China. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. (2021) 30:253–62. doi: 10.6133/apjcn.202106_30(2).0010
55. Kim KY, Yun JM. Dietary patterns and mild cognitive impairment risk in Korean adults over 50 years old. Prev Nutr Food Sci. (2021) 26:132–45.
56. Mumme KD, von Hurst PR, Conlon CA, Jones B, Haskell-Ramsay CF, Stonehouse W, et al. Study protocol: associations between dietary patterns, cognitive function and metabolic syndrome in older adults - a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:535. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6900-4
57. Miranda A, Gómez-Gaete CG, Mennickent S. [Role of mediterranean diet on the prevention of Alzheimer disease]. Rev Med Chil. (2017) 145:501–7. doi: 10.4067/S0034-98872017000400010
58. Parletta N, Milte CM, Meyer BJ. Nutritional modulation of cognitive function and mental health. J Nutr Biochem. (2013) 24:725–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2013.01.002
60. Dai Q, Borenstein AR, Wu Y, Jackson JC, Larson EB. Fruit and vegetable juices and Alzheimer’s disease: the Kame Project. Am J Med. (2006) 119:751–9.
61. Barberger-Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort study. Neurology. (2007) 69:1921–30.
62. Vercambre MN, Boutron-Ruault MC, Ritchie K, Clavel-Chapelon F, Berr C. Long-term association of food and nutrient intakes with cognitive and functional decline: a 13-year follow-up study of elderly French women. Br J Nutr. (2009) 102:419–27.
63. Ritchie K, Carrière I, Ritchie CW, Berr C, Artero S, Ancelin ML. Designing prevention programmes to reduce incidence of dementia: prospective cohort study of modifiable risk factors. BMJ. (2010) 341:c3885.
64. Chen X, Huang Y, Cheng HG. Lower intake of vegetables and legumes associated with cognitive decline among illiterate elderly Chinese: a 3-year cohort study. J Nutr Health Aging. (2012) 16:549–52.
65. Chan R, Chan D, Woo J. A cross sectional study to examine the association between dietary patterns and cognitive impairment in older Chinese people in Hong Kong. J Nutr Health Aging. (2013) 17:757–65.
66. Lee ATC, Richards M, Chan WC, Chiu HFK, Lee RSY, Lam LCW. Lower risk of incident dementia among Chinese older adults having three servings of vegetables and two servings of fruits a day. Age Ageing. (2017) 46:773–9.
67. Kimura Y, Yoshida D, Ohara T, Hata J, Honda T, Hirakawa Y, et al. Long-term association of vegetable and fruit intake with risk of dementia in Japanese older adults: the Hisayama study. BMC Geriatr. (2022) 22:257.
68. Fischer K, Melo van Lent D, Wolfsgruber S, Weinhold L, Kleineidam L, Bickel H, et al. Prospective associations between single foods, Alzheimer’s Dementia and memory decline in the elderly. Nutrients. (2018) 10:852. doi: 10.3390/nu10070852
69. An R, Liu G, Khan N, Yan H, Wang Y. Dietary habits and cognitive impairment risk among oldest-old Chinese. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2019) 74:474–83. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw170
70. Ngabirano L, Samieri C, Feart C, Gabelle A, Artero S, Duflos C, et al., Intake of meat, fish, fruits, and vegetables and long-term risk of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. (2019) 68:711–22.
Keywords: fruit, vegetable, cognitive disorders, meta-analysis, dose-response
Citation: Zhou Y, Wang J, Cao L, Shi M, Liu H, Zhao Y and Xia Y (2022) Fruit and Vegetable Consumption and Cognitive Disorders in Older Adults: A Meta-Analysis of Observational Studies. Front. Nutr. 9:871061. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.871061
Received: 07 February 2022; Accepted: 25 May 2022;
Published: 20 June 2022.
Edited by:
Mohd Razif Shahril, National University of Malaysia, MalaysiaReviewed by:
Naiman A. Khan, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, United StatesYu An, Capital Medical University, China
Ke Xiong, Qingdao University, China
Copyright © 2022 Zhou, Wang, Cao, Shi, Liu, Zhao and Xia. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Yang Xia, eHl0bXU1MDdAMTI2LmNvbQ==, eGlheTFAc2otaG9zcGl0YWwub3Jn
†These authors have contributed equally to this work