Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Nutr., 12 May 2022
Sec. Nutrition, Psychology and Brain Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Mechanisms and Pathways of Action for Nutritional Effects on Brain Health View all 13 articles

Association Between Wine Consumption and Cognitive Decline in Older People: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies

\nMaribel Lucern-Lucas-TorresMaribel Lucerón-Lucas-Torres1Ivn Cavero-Redondo,
Iván Cavero-Redondo1,2*Vicente Martínez-Vizcaíno,Vicente Martínez-Vizcaíno1,2Alicia Saz-LaraAlicia Saz-Lara1Carlos Pascual-MorenaCarlos Pascual-Morena1Celia lvarez-Bueno,Celia Álvarez-Bueno1,3
  • 1Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha, Health and Social Research Center, Cuenca, Spain
  • 2Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Talca, Chile
  • 3Universidad Politécnica y Artística del Paraguay, Asunción, Paraguay

Background: Low-to-moderate alcohol consumption appears to have potential health benefits. Existing evidence concludes that wine may be associated with a lower incidence of certain diseases. This systematic review and meta-analysis aim to examine evidence on the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline and to analyze whether this association varies depending on the wine consumption level or is affected by individual and study characteristics, including mean age, percentage of women participants, and follow-up time.

Methods: In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we undertook a search in MEDLINE (via PubMed), Scopus, Cochrane, and Web of Science databases for longitudinal studies measuring the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline from their inception to May 2021. Effect sizes were calculated using the DerSimonian and Laird and Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman methods.

Results: The search retrieved 6,055 articles, 16 of which were included in this systematic review. In total, 12 studies were included in the meta-analysis. The studies were published between 1997 and 2019. They were conducted in nine different countries. The sample size of the included studies ranged from 360 to 10,308 with a mean age of 70 years old. Using the DerSimoniand and Laird method, the pooled RR for the effect of wine consumption on cognitive decline was 0.72 (95% CI 0.63–0.80; I2 = 82.4%; τ2: 0.0154). Using the Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman method, the RR was 0.65 (95% CI 0.52–0.79; I2 = 94,531%; τ2: 0.057).

Conclusions: This study may show a protective effect of wine consumption against cognitive decline. However, it would be important for future research to differentiate the types of wine within consumption.

Introduction

The consumption of alcohol and tobacco are considered unhealthy habits, harmful to health, and are related to the development of pathologies such as cardiovascular diseases (CVD), digestive system diseases, hypertension, diabetes mellitus, or cognitive deterioration among others, representing a serious problem for public health (13). Indeed, alcohol consumption increases the risk of dementia, especially early-onset dementia (410), indicating a negative impact of alcohol consumption on different areas of familial, social, and cultural wellbeing (1113).

The alcohol consumption recommendations established by the WHO are 30 g for men and 20 g for women, 3 standard drinking units (SBUs), and 2 SBUs, respectively, as 1 SBU corresponds to 10 g of pure alcohol (14). It has been suggested that low-to-moderate alcohol consumption could be beneficial to the health of middle-aged (15) and older subjects (1618), leading to a J-shaped or inverse U-shaped association between alcohol and cognitive function, heart disease (19, 20) and all-cause mortality (21). Furthermore, and according to previous systematic reviews, moderate alcohol consumption appears to be associated with a lower risk of cognitive impairment, dementia, Alzheimer's disease, and better cognition (9, 22), which may be linked to its effects on cardiovascular disease (22). These effects of alcohol consumption are not observed for vascular dementia (9), neither for heavy, chronic, and irregular alcohol consumption which are associated with an increased risk of cognitive impairment or dementia (13).

Further analysis of the data indicates that the effect of alcohol depends on the type of alcoholic beverage analyzed (20), distinguishing between beer, white wine, red wine, fortified wine, and spirits (23). Although wine consumption has been associated with a reduced risk of cerebrovascular disease and Alzheimer's disease (24), there is controversy as to whether these benefits are also reported for beer and other spirits (4, 25). The specific characteristics of wine could be the reason for its benefits. Wine is produced from the fermentation of grapes, and yeast is added, causing the sugars present in the grapes to be converted into ethanol, endowing wine with different nutritional properties. It has been reported that some components of wine, such as resveratrol, phenolic acids, and flavonoids, may exert positive health effects (26). Previous research has shown that these components reduce free cholesterol (27), have a cardioprotective effect (28), induce endothelial relaxation (29), activate NO synthase (30), inhibit platelet aggregation, and (31) prevent oxidation of low-density lipoproteins (LDL) cholesterol (32).

Regardless of previous research, the evidence on the association between wine consumption and the risk of cognitive decline remains inconclusive (9, 14, 22, 33). This systematic review and meta-analysis aim to examine the strength of the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline and to analyze whether this association varies depending on the wine consumption level or is affected by individual and study characteristics, including the mean age, percentage of women participants, and follow-up time.

Methods

Search Strategy and Study Selection

This systematic review and meta-analysis were performed according to the Cochrane Collaboration Handbook (34) and reported following the MOOSE guidelines (Meta-analysis of Observational Studies in Epidemiology) (35) this systematic review and meta-analysis were registered on PROSPERO (registration number CRD42021232060).

A systematic search was conducted in the MEDLINE (via PubMed), Scopus, Cochrane, and Web of Science databases from their inception to 25 May 2021. The literature search was updated on 15 February 2022. The search strategy included the following relevant terms: (1) “dementia,” “mental deterioration,” “Alzheimer's disease,” “vascular dementia,” “predementia syndromes,” and “mild cognitive impairment”; (2) “alcohol,” “wine,” “alcohol consumption,” and “wine consumption” and (3) “older,” “elderly,” “elderly people,” and “older people.” Finally, the reference list of the studies included in this systematic review was examined to identify relevant studies. Supplementary Table 1 presents the complete search strategy for MEDLINE.

Eligibility

Eligible articles included longitudinal studies measuring the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline. The inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) subjects: general population without dementia at baseline aged 65 or over at the end of the study; (ii) outcomes: cognitive decline assessments using standardized and validated tests; (iii) study design: longitudinal studies; and (iv) studies reporting wine consumption separated from other alcoholic beverages. Studies were excluded if they: (i) were review articles, editorials, or patient case reports; or (ii) included patients with cognitive decline at baseline determined by a battery of psychometric tests or an examination by a neurologist. No language restrictions were applied to the search or study selection process.

Data Extraction and Quality Assessment

The following information was extracted from the included studies: study name; subject characteristics, including sample size, percentage of women and mean age at baseline; wine consumption; grams of ethanol consumed per day; type of wine consumed; test used to measure cognitive function and cognitive domains measured.

To evaluate the risk of bias of cohort studies, we used the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies from the United States National Institute of Health National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (36). This tool assesses 14 items for longitudinal studies in the following domains: (1) research question, (2) study population, (3) participation rate, (4) recruitment, (5) sample size, (6) timeframe for associations, (7) exposure levels, (8) exposure measures and (9) assessment, (10) repeated exposure assessment, (11) outcome measures, (12) blinding of exposure assessors, (13) loss to follow-up, and (14) statistical analyses.

For case-control studies, the Quality Assessment of Case-Control Studies (37) from the United States National Institute of Health National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute was used. This tool includes 12 domains, namely: (1) research question, (2) study population, (3) target population, (4) sample size, (5) recruitment, (6) inclusion and exclusion criteria, (7) case and control definitions, (8) random selection of study participants, (9) concurrent controls, (10) exposure measures and assessment, (11) blinding of exposure assessors, and (12) statistical analyses.

Finally, the overall risk of bias of each study was scored as “good” if most criteria were met; “fair” if some criteria were met; or “poor” if few criteria were met.

The search strategy, study selection, eligibility, data extraction, and quality assessment, were conducted by two independent reviewers (M.L.-L.-T and C.A.-B.). When the agreement was not reached, a third reviewer (I.C.-R.) was consulted.

Statistical Analysis and Data Synthesis

A meta-analysis was performed to determine the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline. The studies were classified according to wine consumption in the three subgroups, namely: (i) “within WHO recommendations” when studies reported wine consumption of <20 g in women and 30 g in men; (ii) “above the WHO recommendations” when studies reported higher wine consumption; and (iii) “unclassified” when studies did not report participants' wine consumption.

Some methodological considerations should be noted. When two studies reported data from the same population, we include in the meta-analysis the study with the largest sample size. The RR and odds ratio (OR) for the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline were jointly included in the meta-analysis (38). When studies reported the hazard ratio (HR), it was converted to RR using the following formula: RR=(1 – eHRln(1 – r))/r (38). In addition, the type and consumption of wine were collected as reported by the original studies and converted into grams using the equivalences of the SBU.

The DerSimonian and Lair and Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random-effects methods were used to compute the pooled estimate of the RR and their respective 95% Cis (39, 40). Following the Cochrane Handbook recommendations, the I2 statistic was used to examine the inconsistency, which ranges between 0 and 100% (41). According to the I2 values, inconsistency was considered not important (0–30%), moderate (≥30–50%), substantial (≥50–75%), or considerable (≥75–100%). The corresponding p-values were also considered. In addition, heterogeneity was evaluated using the τ2 statistic, which was interpreted as low when τ2 was lower than 0.04, moderate when τ2 was from ≥0.04 to 0.14, and substantial when τ2 was from ≥0.14 to 0.40 (42).

To assess the robustness of the summary estimates, sensitivity analyses were conducted by removing each study one at a time from the pooled estimations. Meta-regression analyses were performed to address whether mean age, percentage of women, and time of follow-up, as continuous variables, could modify the effect of the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline. Finally, publication bias was assessed through Egger's regression asymmetry test, where a p-value of <0.10 was used to determine if there was significant publication bias (43). Analyses were performed using Stata 15.0 (Stata, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

Study Selection

The search retrieved 6,055 articles. From them, 101 studies were selected by reviewing the title and abstract, and 16 of which were included in this systematic review (25, 26, 4458). Only 12 of these studies were included in the meta-analysis (Figure 1) (25, 26, 4448, 50, 52, 53, 55, 58).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases.

Study and Intervention Characteristics

The included longitudinal studies (25, 26, 4458) were published between 1997 and 2019. They were conducted in ten different countries, including China (1 study), Canada (1 study), Germany (1 study), Sweden (2 studies), the United States (4 studies), the Netherlands (2 studies), France (2 studies), the United Kingdom (1 study), Italy (1 study), and Denmark (1 study).

The sample size of the included studies ranged from 360 to 10,308, with a total of 46,472 participants (60% women) aged 70 years old. Their follow-up periods ranged from 2 to 43 years. All the studies included the general population without dementia at baseline (25, 26, 4458). One of the studies was conducted on twins born in Sweden between 1907 and 1925 (46), and another included only women (53). Only one study (54) did not report wine consumption and only three reported the type of wine consumed, distinguishing among white, red, and fortificated (Table 1) (41, 47, 51).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Main characteristics of the included studies.

Finally, the most used tests to assess the risk of cognitive decline or dementia were the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), the SIDAM test, the Global Deterioration Scale and the Blessed Dementia Rating Scale, and the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD) neuropsychological battery. Other tests used were the Battery of psychometric test, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-III-R Criteria), the Modified Mini-Mental State (3MSE), the Health Study-Cognition (CHS), and the Informant Questionnaire on the Cognitive Decline of the Elderly (IQCODE) (Table 1).

Risk of Bias

For cohort studies, the overall risk of bias was low in all studies (100%). All the studies included information on the domains related to exposure levels and the time frame to observe an effect. Only two studies (45, 48) provided information on the blinding of the assessors (13.32%). In addition, the participation rate was not reported in 42.86% of studies, and the follow-up rate was not achieved in 35.71%. Finally, only one study (48) did not provide information on the exposure measures (6.66%) and only 28.6% of studies assessed the exposure on more than one occasion (Supplementary Table 2).

For case-control studies, the overall risk of bias was low in all studies (100%) (25, 50). The studies provided information on all the domains, except one study (50) that did not report information about blinding of exposure assessors (50%) (Supplementary Table 3).

Meta-Analysis

Using the DerSimonian and Lair random effect models, the pooled RR for the association of wine consumption with cognitive decline was 0.72 (95% CI 0.63–0.80; I2 = 82.4%; τ2: 0.0154). For the subgroup analysis, the pooled RR in those cohorts within the WHO recommendations was 0.59 (95% CI: 0.33–0.85; I2 = 41.3%; τ2: 0.0348), the pooled RR for those cohorts above the WHO recommendations was 0.59 (95% CI: 0.34–0.85; I2 = 58.8%; τ2: 0.042), and the pooled RR for unclassified cohorts was 0.82 (95% CI: 0.73–0.92; I2 = 84.8%; τ2: 0.0088) (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Meta-analysis for the association of wine consumption and cognitive impairment.

Using the Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman random effect models, the pooled RR for the association of wine consumption with cognitive decline was 0.65 (95% CI 0.52–0.79; I2 = 94,531%; τ2: 0.057). For the subgroup analysis, the pooled RR for those cohorts within the WHO recommendations was 0.59 (95% CI: 0.32–0.87; I2 = 45.87%; τ2: 0.042), the pooled RR for those cohorts above the WHO recommendations was 0.59 (95% CI: 0.32–0.86; I2 = 63.9%; τ2: 0.060), and the pooled RR for unclassified cohorts was 0.68 (95% CI: 0.47–0.90; I2 = 98.27%; τ2: 0.091).

Sensitivity and Meta-Regression Analysis

The pooled RR estimates were not modified after removing one study at a time from the analyses (Supplementary Table 4). In addition, random-effects meta-regression models for the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline revealed that age, percentage of women, and duration of follow-up were not related to heterogeneity across studies (Supplementary Table 5).

Publication Bias

Publication bias was observed by Egger's test (43) for the overall effect of wine consumption on cognitive decline (p < 0.01). For subgroup analyses, publication bias was found for the unclassified cohorts (p < 0.01).

Discussion

This systematic review and meta-analysis provide an overview of the evidence on the association of wine consumption with cognitive decline. Our data support the association between wine consumption and a lower risk of cognitive decline, with this evidence being stronger for those populations within WHO recommendations (30 g for men and 20 g for women, i.e., 3 SBUs and 2 SBUs, respectively) (14). Finally, this association did not seem to be modified by mean age, percentage of women, or follow-up time. Our results are in accordance with previous research (9, 22, 59) reporting the effect of alcohol consumption, including wine in sub-analyses, and add to the evidence that a protective effect could be exerted by low-to-moderate wine consumption.

Many lifestyle habits have been proposed to prevent or delay cognitive decline, including physical exercise, socialization, cognitive training, maintenance of good sleep hygiene, and adoption of the Mediterranean diet, among others. It has been reported that the Mediterranean diet could help to control cardiovascular parameters and oxidative stress. The Mediterranean diet contains a wide variety of foods rich in antioxidants, including a low-to-moderate wine consumption (6062). This moderate alcohol consumption has been associated with better cognitive performance (63) and may contribute to maintaining cognition levels, although these results should be interpreted with caution since some types of alcohol-associated dementia have been identified (64, 65).

It is usually accepted that the limits for healthy alcohol consumption are 30 g for men and 20 g for women (14). Within these limits, most literature supports the possible protective effect of wine consumption against cognitive decline, even after controlling for covariates, such as age and percentage of women. However, some studies found no difference in the association of wine consumption and cognitive decline between women and men (25, 26, 4458), Although, our data support no differences by sex, others have reported a higher association for women (45, 66) due to the differences in wine consumption between groups (45) and women's preference for white wine (33).

Wine consumption has been associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline due to some of the components present in wine that may have an antioxidant function or that could inhibit the lethal events of oxidative stress produced by nitric oxide (24, 32). Grape skins are made up of different types of polyphenols, namely, quercetin, myricetin, catechin, and epicatechin (flavonols), gallic acid, and polymeric anthocyanins (24). The concentration of these components, which varies among types of wine, is responsible for the promising antioxidant potential of wine, with some wines being more protective than others. Flavonoids are components present primarily in red wine that might explain the reduction in the incidence of Alzheimer's disease and cerebrovascular disease (32, 66, 67). Resveratrol, a plant compound found in red wine grapes, might appear to have a neuroprotective effect by protecting neuronal cells from β-amyloid, a neurotoxin involved in the creation of senile plaques detected in neurodegenerative diseases, such as Alzheimer's disease. These senile plaques contribute to cell death (68), although removal of these plaques does not lead to improved cognition (69), resveratrol has anti-neuroinflammatory properties that protect against cognitive decline (57). This substance also has cancer-inhibiting effects (70) and reduces the incidence of coronary heart disease (71).

Moreover, there are also phenolic compounds in wine that contribute to the sensory properties of wine and protect it from oxidation (72, 73). These compounds also have antioxidant effects, reducing the risk of degenerative diseases such as osteoporosis, diabetes, and cancer (74). Various in vitro and animal studies have shown that polyphenols such as resveratrol could exert a number of health benefits, including anti-inflammatory and anti-atherogenic effects (27), and suggest a possible use of these polyphenols as therapeutic agents for ischemic and neurodegenerative events (75). In humans, a randomized clinical trial in women using a resveratrol supplement, concluded that a low-dose resveratrol could be a preventive strategy to counteract aging factors such as cognitive decline (76). Furthermore, it has been reported that resveratrol consumption needed for improvements in cognitive functions is far from the daily intake associated with food and wine consumption (77), A very high wine consumption, that would be detrimental to health, would be necessary to achieve the cognitive healthy resveratrol intake (78). In the case of observational studies with polyphenol supplementation through diet, including polyphenol-rich foods such as vegetables, fruit, or wine consumption (79), a low-dose wine consumption could produce benefits for some pathologies (53). In addition, evidence suggests that carrying the apolipoprotein E (APOE) epsilon 4 allele increases the likelihood of developing Alzheimer's disease (80). The risk of Alzheimer's disease in APOE ε4 carriers increased with white wine consumption vs. red wine consumption (41). Furthermore, the possible benefits of moderate wine consumption are increased in APOE ε4 noncarriers (6, 81). These possible benefits of wine components could explain the famous French paradox, where it was observed that mortality from CVD was much lower in France than in other industrialized countries (81). Further studies should consider additional confounding variables, namely, age, sex, race, body mass index, smoking, marital status, education, hormone treatment, and some pathologies such as diabetes (23).

Our systematic review and meta-analysis have some limitations that should be mentioned. First, studies included a wide variety of scales for measuring cognitive decline, which can lead to bias. Second, each study measured wine consumption differently, and some of the included studies that did not quantify wine consumption. Third, there is no global consensus on maximum recommended intake or safe drinking limits, so the use of alcohol consumption limits, although recommended by the WHO, could represent a limit in addressing this issue. Fourth, most studies did not provide information on assessor blinding or whether exposure was assessed more than once over time. Fifth, we found evidence of publication bias using the Egger's test. Sixth, we only included studies in English and Spanish, in addition, gray literature was not included. Finally, due to the lack of data, we were unable to analyze the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline by type of wine.

In summary, this systematic review and meta-analysis identified a possible protective effect of wine consumption on the development of cognitive decline. This effect appears to be independent of age, the percentage of women, and follow-up time. These results do not suggest that the population should increase their wine consumption, as this could be harmful, especially in older persons who, with the loss of lean mass, polymedication and other factors, could suffer serious harm. However, considering our results, low wine consumption could be promoted within other lifestyles, including the Mediterranean diet, as an effective habit to prevent or delay cognitive deterioration in the healthy population. These results also support the international guidelines that suggest low-to-low-moderate alcohol consumption as the most acceptable level of consumption, both in the long and short term and for people who do not suffer any pathology such as liver cirrhosis or are not polymedicated or pregnant (8284). Finally, it would be important for future research to differentiate between white and red wine consumption, which could allow to determine the association between wine consumption and cognitive decline by type of wine.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

ML-L-T and CÁ-B: conceptualization, investigation, and writing—original draft preparation. ML-L-T, CÁ-B, and IC-R: methodology. IC-R and CÁ-B: software. AS-L and CP-M: validation and visualization. ML-L-T and IC-R: formal analysis. ML-L-T, AS-L, and CP-M: resources. CÁ-B and VM-V: data curation. VM-V: writing—review and editing. CÁ-B: supervision. All authors revised and approved the final version of the articles.

Funding

This work was funded by FEDER funds.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnut.2022.863059/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Chun BY, Dobson AJ, Heller RF. Smoking and the incidence of coronary heart disease in an Australian population. Med J Aust. (1993) 159:508–12. doi: 10.5694/j.1326-5377.1993.tb138002.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Choi SY, Kahyo H. Effect of cigarette smoking and alcohol consumption in the etiology of cancers of the digestive tract. Int J Cancer. (1991) 49:381–6. doi: 10.1002/ijc.2910490312

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Uchimoto S, Tsumura K, Hayashi T, Suematsu C, Endo G, Fujii S, et al. Impact of cigarette smoking on the incidence of Type 2 diabetes mellitus in middle-aged Japanese men: the Osaka Health Survey. Diabet Med. (1999) 16:951–5. doi: 10.1046/j.1464-5491.1999.00173.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. German JB, Walzem RL. The health benefits of wine. Annu Rev Nutr. (2000) 20:561–93. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nutr.20.1.561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rehm J, Mathers C, Popova S, Thavorncharoensap M, Teerawattananon Y, Patra J. Global burden of disease and injury and economic cost attributable to alcohol use and alcohol-use disorders. Lancet. (2009) 373:2223–33. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)60746-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. (2020) 396:413–46. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Anstey KJ, von Sanden C, Salim A, O'Kearney R. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Epidemiol. (2007) 166:367–78. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Agarwal DP. Cardioprotective effects of light-moderate consumption of alcohol: a review of putative mechanisms. Alcohol Alcohol. (2002) 37:409–15. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/37.5.409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Peters R, Peters J, Warner J, Beckett N, Bulpitt C. Alcohol, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly: a systematic review. Age Ageing. (2008) 37:505–12. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afn095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fratiglioni L, Wang HX, Ericsson K, Maytan M, Winblad B. Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: a community-based longitudinal study. Lancet. (2000) 355:1315–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02113-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Wilson RS, Krueger KR, Arnold SE, Schneider JA, Kelly JF, Barnes LL, et al. Loneliness and risk of Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2007) 64:234–40. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Van Gelder BM, Tijhuis M, Kalmijn S, Giampaoli S, Nissinen A, Kromhout D. Marital status and living situation during a 5-year period are associated with a subsequent 10-year cognitive decline in older men: the FINE Study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2006) 61:P213–9. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.4.P213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Rehm J, Hasan OSM, Black SE, Shield KD, Schwarzinger M. Alcohol use and dementia: a systematic scoping review. Alzheimers Res Ther. (2019) 11:1. doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0453-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Global Status Report on Alcohol and Health 2018. Geneva: World Health Organization (2018).

15. Cerhan JR, Folsom AR, Mortimer JA, Shahar E, Knopman DS, McGovern PG, et al. Correlates of cognitive function in middle-aged adults. Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study Investigators. Gerontology. (1998) 44:95–105. doi: 10.1159/000021991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Ngandu T, Helkala EL, Soininen H, Winblad B, Tuomilehto J, Nissinen A, et al. Alcohol drinking and cognitive functions: findings from the Cardiovascular Risk Factors Aging and Dementia (CAIDE) Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2007) 23:140–9. doi: 10.1159/000097995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Espeland MA, Coker LH, Wallace R, Rapp SR, Resnick SM, Limacher M, et al. Women's Health Initiative Study of Cognitive Aging. Association between alcohol intake and domain-specific cognitive function in older women. Neuroepidemiology. (2006) 27:1–12. doi: 10.1159/000093532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Vincze G, Almos P, Boda K, Döme P, Bódi N, Szlávik G, et al. Risk factors of cognitive decline in residential care in Hungary. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2007) 22:1208–16. doi: 10.1002/gps.1815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Duffy JC. Alcohol consumption and all-cause mortality. Int J Epidemiol. (1995) 24:100–5. doi: 10.1093/ije/24.1.100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Svärdsudd K. Moderate alcohol consumption and cardiovascular disease: is there evidence for a preventive effect? Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (1998) 22(7 Suppl):307S-314S. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb04383.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Andreasson S. Alcohol and J-shaped curves. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (1998) 22:359S−364S. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1998.tb04391.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Anstey KJ, Mack HA, Cherbuin N. Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2009) 17:542–55. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181a2fd07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Barberger-Gateau P, Raffaitin C, Letenneur L, Berr C, Tzourio C, Dartigues JF, et al. Dietary patterns and risk of dementia: the Three-City cohort study. Neurology. (2007) 69:1921–30. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000278116.37320.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Broustet JP. Wine and health. Heart. (1999) 81:459–60. doi: 10.1136/hrt.81.5.459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Truelsen T, Thudium D, Grønbaek M. Copenhagen City Heart Study. Amount and type of alcohol and risk of dementia: the Copenhagen City Heart Study. Neurology. (2002) 59:1313–9. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000031421.50369.E7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mukamal KJ, Kuller LH, Fitzpatrick AL, Longstreth WT Jr, Mittleman MA, Siscovick DS. Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults. JAMA. (2003) 289:1405–13. doi: 10.1001/jama.289.11.1405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Riccioni G, Gammone MA, Tettamanti G, Bergante S, Pluchinotta FR, D'Orazio N. Resveratrol and anti-atherogenic effects. Int J Food Sci Nutr. (2015) 66:603–10. doi: 10.3109/09637486.2015.1077796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Rakici O, Kiziltepe U, Coskun B, Aslamaci S, Akar F. Effects of resveratrol on vascular tone and endothelial function of human saphenous vein and internal mammary rtery. Int J Cardiol. (2005) 105:209–15. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2005.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fitzpatrick DF, Hirschfield SL, Coffey RG. Endothelium-dependent vasorelaxing activity of wine and other grape products. Am J Physiol. (1993) 265:H774–8. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1993.265.2.H774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Leikert JF, Rathel TR, Wohlfart P, Cheynier V, Vollmar AM, Dirsch VM, et al. Red wine polyphenols enhance endothelial nitric oxide release from endothelial cells. Circulation. (2002) 106:1614–7. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.0000034445.31543.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

31. Demrow HS, Slane PR, Folts JD. Administration of wine and grape juice inhibits in vivo platelet activity and thrombosis in stenosed canine coronary arteries. Circulation. (1995) 91:1182–8. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.91.4.1182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Frankel EN, Kanner J, German JB, Parks E, Kinsella JE. Inhibition of oxidation of human low-density lipoprotein by phenolic substances in red wine. Lancet. (1993) 341:454–7. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(93)90206-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Espeland MA, Gu L, Masaki KH, Langer RD, Coker LH, Stefanick ML, et al. Association between reported alcohol intake and cognition: results from the Women's Health Initiative Memory Study. Am J Epidemiol. (2005) 161:228–38. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwi043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Higgins JP, Green S editors. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell (2008).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

35. Stroup DF, Berlin JA, Morton SC, Olkin I, Williamson GD, Rennie D, et al. Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: a proposal for reporting. Meta-analysis Of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (MOOSE) group. JAMA. (2000) 283:2008–12. doi: 10.1001/jama.283.15.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. National Institutes of Health. Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Available online at: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools (accessed September 16, 2021).

37. National Institutes of Health. Quality Assessment of Case-Control Studies. National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Available online at: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools (accessed September 16, 2021).

38. Stare J, Maucort-Boulch D. Odds Ratio, Hazard Ratio and Relative Risk. Metodoloski zvezki. (2016) 13:59–67 doi: 10.51936/uwah2960

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. DerSimonian R, Kacker R. Random-effects model for meta-analysis of clinical trials: an update. Contemp Clin Trials. (2007) 28:105–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2006.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. IntHout J, Ioannidis JP, Borm GF. The Hartung-Knapp-Sidik-Jonkman method for random effects meta-analysis is straightforward and considerably outperforms the standard DerSimonian-Laird method. BMC Med Res Methodol. (2014) 14:1–12. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-14-25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Higgins JP, Thompson SG. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. (2002) 21:1539–58. doi: 10.1002/sim.1186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Stettler C, Allemann S, Wandel S, Kastrati A, Morice MC, Schömig A, et al. Drug eluting and bare metal stents in people with and without diabetes: Collaborative network meta-analysis. BMJ. (2008) 337:a1331. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a1331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sterne JA, Egger M, Smith GD. Systematic reviews in health care: Investigating and dealing with publication and other biases in meta-analysis. BMJ. (2001) 323:101–5 doi: 10.1136/bmj.323.7304.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Deng J, Zhou DH, Li J, Wang YJ, Gao C, Chen M. A 2-year follow-up study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. (2006) 108:378–83. doi: 10.1016/j.clineuro.2005.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fischer K. Melo van Lent D, Wolfsgruber S, Weinhold L, Kleineidam L, Bickel H, et al. Prospective Associations between Single Foods, Alzheimer's Dementia and Memory Decline in the Elderly. Nutrients. (2018) 10:852. doi: 10.3390/nu10070852

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Handing EP, Andel R, Kadlecova P, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Midlife Alcohol Consumption and Risk of Dementia Over 43 Years of Follow-Up: A Population-Based Study From the Swedish Twin Registry. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. (2015) 70:1248–54. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glv038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Heymann D, Stern Y, Cosentino S, Tatarina-Nulman O, Dorrejo JN, Gu Y. The Association Between Alcohol Use and the Progression of Alzheimer's Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. (2016) 13:1356–62. doi: 10.2174/1567205013666160603005035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Leibovici D, Ritchie K, Ledésert B, Touchon J. The effects of wine and tobacco consumption on cognitive performance in the elderly: a longitudinal study of relative risk. Int J Epidemiol. (1999) 28:77–81. doi: 10.1093/ije/28.1.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lemeshow S, Letenneur L, Dartigues JF, Lafont S, Orgogozo JM, Commenges D. Illustration of analysis taking into account complex survey considerations: the association between wine consumption and dementia in the PAQUID study. Personnes Ages Quid Am J Epidemiol. (1998) 148:298–306. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Lindsay J, Laurin D, Verreault R, Hébert R, Helliwell B, Hill GB, et al. Risk factors for Alzheimer's disease: a prospective analysis from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Am J Epidemiol. (2002) 156:445–53. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwf074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Low DY, Lefèvre-Arbogast S, González-Domínguez R, Urpi-Sarda M, Micheau P, Petera M, et al. Diet-Related Metabolites Associated with Cognitive Decline Revealed by Untargeted Metabolomics in a Prospective Cohort. Mol Nutr Food Res. (2019) 63:e1900177. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.201900177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Luchsinger JA, Tang MX, Siddiqui M, Shea S, Mayeux R. Alcohol intake and risk of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2004) 52:540–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52159.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Mehlig K, Skoog I, Guo X, Schütze M, Gustafson D, Waern M, et al. Alcoholic beverages and incidence of dementia: 34-year follow-up of the prospective population study of women in Goteborg. Am J Epidemiol. (2008) 167:684–91. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Nooyens AC, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, van Gelder BM, van Boxtel MP, Verschuren WM. Consumption of alcoholic beverages and cognitive decline at middle age: the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Br J Nutr. (2014) 111:715–23. doi: 10.1017/S0007114513002845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Lafont S, Letenneur L, Commenges D, Salamon R, et al. Wine consumption and dementia in the elderly: a prospective community study in the Bordeaux area. Rev Neurol (Paris). (1997) 153:185–92.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

56. Ruitenberg A, van Swieten JC, Witteman JC, Mehta KM, van Duijn CM, Hofman, et al. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Lancet. (2002) 359:281–6. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07493-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sabia S, Fayosse A, Dumurgier J, Dugravot A, Akbaraly T, Britton A, et al. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia: 23 year follow-up of Whitehall II cohort study. BMJ. (2018) 362:k2927. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k2927

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Solfrizzi V, D'Introno A, Colacicco AM, Capurso C, Del Parigi A, Baldassarre G, et al. Italian Longitudinal Study on Aging Working Group. Alcohol consumption, mild cognitive impairment, and progression to dementia. Neurology. (2007) 68:1790–9. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000262035.87304.89

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. (1999) 56:303–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Tipton PW, Graff-Radford NR. Prevention of late-life dementia: what works and what does not. Pol Arch Intern Med. (2018) 128:310–6. doi: 10.20452/pamw.4263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Shah H, Albanese E, Duggan C, Rudan I, Langa KM, Carrillo MC, et al. Research priorities to reduce the global burden of dementia by 2025. Lancet Neurol. (2016) 15:1285–94. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)30235-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Winblad B, Amouyel P, Andrieu S, Ballard C, Brayne C, Brodaty H, et al. Defeating Alzheimer's disease and other dementias: a priority for European science and society. Lancet Neurol. (2016) 15:455–532. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(16)00062-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Arntzen KA, Schirmer H, Wilsgaard T, Mathiesen EB. Moderate wine consumption is associated with better cognitive test results: a 7 year follow up of 5033 subjects in the Tromsø Study. Acta Neurol Scand Suppl. (2010) 190:23–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2010.01371.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Letenneur L. Risk of dementia and alcohol and wine consumption: a review of recent results. Biol Res. (2004) 37:189–93. doi: 10.4067/S0716-97602004000200003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Brust JC. A 74-year-old man with memory loss and neuropathy who enjoys alcoholic beverages. JAMA. (2008) 299:1046–54. doi: 10.1001/jama.299.5.jrr80000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Ott BR, Owens NJ. Complementary and alternative medicines for Alzheimer's disease. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (1998) 11:163–73. doi: 10.1177/089198879901100401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Savaskan E, Olivieri G, Meier F, Seifritz E, Wirz-Justice A, Müller-Spahn F. Red wine ingredient resveratrol protects from beta-amyloid neurotoxicity. Gerontology. (2003) 49:380–3. doi: 10.1159/000073766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Holmes C, Boche D, Wilkinson D, Yadegarfar G, Hopkins V, Bayer A, et al. Long-term effects of Abeta42 immunisation in Alzheimer's disease: follow-up of a randomised, placebo-controlled phase I trial. Lancet. (2008) 372:216–23. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61075-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Ranney A, Petro MS. Resveratrol protects spatial learning in middle-aged C57BL/6 mice from effects of ethanol. Behav Pharmacol. (2009) 20:330–6. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32832f0193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Sale S, Verschoyle RD, Boocock D, Jones DJ, Wilsher N, Ruparelia KC, et al. Pharmacokinetics in mice and growth-inhibitory properties of the putative cancer chemopreventive agent resveratrol and the synthetic analogue trans 3,4,5,4'-tetramethoxystilbene. Br J Cancer. (2004) 90:736–44. doi: 10.1038/sj.bjc.6601568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Labinskyy N, Csiszar A, Veress G, Stef G, Pacher P, Oroszi G, et al. Vascular dysfunction in aging: potential effects of resveratrol, an anti-inflammatory phytoestrogen. Curr Med Chem. (2006) 13:989–96. doi: 10.2174/092986706776360987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Vidal S, Francis L, Noble A, Kwiatkowski M, Cheynier V, Waters E. Taste and mouth-feel properties of different types of tannin-like polyphenolic compounds and anthocyanins in wine. Analytica Chimica Acta. (2004) 513:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.aca.2003.10.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Bimpilas A, Tsimogiannis D, Balta-Brouma K, Lymperopoulou T, Oreopoulou V. Evolution of phenolic compounds and metal content of wine during alcoholic fermentation and storage. Food Chem. (2015) 178:164–71. doi: 10.1016/j.foodchem.2015.01.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Scalbert A, Manach C, Morand C, Rémésy C, Jiménez L. Dietary polyphenols and the prevention of diseases. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. (2005) 45:287–306. doi: 10.1080/1040869059096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Wang Q, Xu J, Rottinghaus GE, Simonyi A, Lubahn D, Sun GY, et al. Resveratrol protects against global cerebral ischemic injury in gerbils. Brain Res. (2002) 958:439–47. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(02)03543-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Thaung Zaw JJ, Howe PR, Wong RH. Long-term effects of resveratrol on cognition, cerebrovascular function and cardio-metabolic markers in postmenopausal women: a 24-month randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Clin Nutr. (2021) 40:820–9. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2020.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Restani P, Fradera U, Ruf JC, Stockley C, Teissedre PL, Biella S, et al. Grapes and their derivatives in modulation of cognitive decline: a critical review of epidemiological and randomized-controlled trials in humans. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. (2021) 61:566–76. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2020.1740644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Evans HM, Howe PR, Wong RH. Effects of Resveratrol on Cognitive Performance, Mood and Cerebrovascular Function in Post-Menopausal Women; a 14-Week Randomised Placebo-Controlled Intervention Trial. Nutrients. (2017) 9:27. doi: 10.3390/nu9010027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Colizzi C. The protective effects of polyphenols on Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review. Alzheimers Dement (N Y). (2018) 5:184–96. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2018.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Panza F, Frisardi V, Seripa D, Logroscino G, Santamato A, Imbimbo BP, et al. Alcohol consumption in mild cognitive impairment and dementia: harmful or neuroprotective?. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2012) 27:1218–38. doi: 10.1002/gps.3772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Renaud S, de Lorgeril M. Wine, alcohol, platelets, and the French paradox for coronary heart disease. Lancet. (1992) 339:1523–6. doi: 10.1016/0140-6736(92)91277-F

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. World Health Organization. International Guide for Monitoring Alcohol Consumption and Related Harm. Geneva: World Health Organization (2000).

Google Scholar

83. European Medicines Agency. Guideline on the Development of Medicinal Products for the Treatment of Alcohol Dependence. London: European Medicines Agency (2010).

Google Scholar

84. Letenneur L, Larrieu S, Barberger-Gateau P. Alcohol and to- bacco consumption as risk factors of dementia: a review of epidemiological studies. Biomed Pharmacother. (2004) 58:95–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2003.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive decline, wine, older people, alcohol consumption, elderly

Citation: Lucerón-Lucas-Torres M, Cavero-Redondo I, Martínez-Vizcaíno V, Saz-Lara A, Pascual-Morena C and Álvarez-Bueno C (2022) Association Between Wine Consumption and Cognitive Decline in Older People: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Longitudinal Studies. Front. Nutr. 9:863059. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2022.863059

Received: 26 January 2022; Accepted: 18 March 2022;
Published: 12 May 2022.

Edited by:

Claire Williams, University of Reading, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Nenad Naumovski, University of Canberra, Australia
David Vauzour, University of East Anglia, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Lucerón-Lucas-Torres, Cavero-Redondo, Martínez-Vizcaíno, Saz-Lara, Pascual-Morena and Álvarez-Bueno. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Iván Cavero-Redondo, aXZhbi5jYXZlcm8mI3gwMDA0MDt1Y2xtLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.